Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T12:00:41.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genetic overlap between episodic memory deficits and schizophrenia: results from The Maudsley Twin Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2010

S. F. Owens*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, UK
M. M. Picchioni
Affiliation:
St Andrew's Academic Centre, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Northampton, UK
F. V. Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, UK
D. Stahl
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, UK
E. Vassos
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, UK
A. K. Rodger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, UK
D. A. Collier
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, UK
R. M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, UK
T. Toulopoulou
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Ms. S. F. Owens, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, King's Health Partners, De Crespigny Park, LondonSE5 8AF, UK. (Email: sheena.owens@kcl.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Visual and verbal episodic memory deficits are putative endophenotypes for schizophrenia; however, the extent of any genetic overlap of these with schizophrenia is unclear. In this study, we set out to quantify the genetic and environmental contributions to variance in visual and verbal memory performance, and to quantify their genetic relationship with schizophrenia.

Method

We applied bivariate genetic modelling to 280 twins in a classic twin study design, including monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) pairs concordant and discordant for schizophrenia, and healthy control twins. We assessed episodic memory using subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (WMS-R).

Results

Genetic influences (i.e. heritability) contributed significantly to variance in immediate recall of both verbal memory and visual learning, and the delayed recall of verbal and visual memory. Liability to schizophrenia was associated with memory impairment, with evidence of significant phenotypic correlations between all episodic memory measures and schizophrenia. Genetic factors were the main source of the phenotypic correlations for immediate recall of visual learning material; both immediate and delayed recall of verbal memory; and delayed recall of visual memory that, for example, shared genetic variance with schizophrenia, which accounted for 88% of the phenotypic correlation (rph=0.41) between the two.

Conclusions

Verbal memory and visual learning and memory are moderately heritable, share a genetic overlap with schizophrenia and are valid endophenotypes for the condition. The inclusion of these endophenotypes in genetic association studies may improve the power to detect susceptibility genes for schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Introduction

Endophenotypes could facilitate the endeavours of molecular geneticists to dissect the complex disease mechanisms of illnesses such as schizophrenia (Tan et al. Reference Tan, Callicott and Weinberger2008). Intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes are discrete genetically determined disease-related phenotypes that gauge genetic predisposition (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). They may offer insights into the genetic transmission of psychiatric illness because they are more proximal to the gene effects than the heterogeneous and subjective symptoms that define clinical syndromes; and because of their quantitative and genetically simpler nature offer improved statistical power for genetic analyses (Cannon & Keller, Reference Cannon and Keller2006). Gottesman & Gould (Reference Gottesman and Gould2003) have suggested five criteria for the identification of endophenotypes: an endophenotype is associated with illness in the population; it is heritable; it is primarily state independent (manifests in an individual whether or not illness is active); within families, endophenotype and illness co-segregate; and it is found in non-affected family members at a higher rate than in the general population.

Neurocognitive impairments are putative endophenotypes, uncorrelated with illness subtype (Elvevag & Goldberg, Reference Elvevag and Goldberg2000), show limited improvement from typical or atypical antipsychotics (Blyler & Gold, Reference Blyler, Gold, Sharma and Harvey2000), are present before clinical onset (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Rodgers, Murray and Marmot1994; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor, Murray and Poulton2002), and persist throughout the course of illness (Heaton et al. Reference Heaton, Gladsjo, Palmer, Kuck, Marcotte and Jeste2001). Further support for neurocognitive endophenotypes has been suggested by genetic association studies, suggesting genetic effects linking both schizophrenia and episodic memory (Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Egan, Gscheidle, Coppola, Weickert, Kolachana, Goldman and Weinberger2003; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Hennah, van Erp, Thompson, Lonnqvist, Huttunen, Gasperoni, Tuulio-Henriksson, Pirkola, Toga, Kaprio, Mazziotta and Peltonen2005; Prasad et al. Reference Prasad, Chowdari, Nimgaonkar, Talkowski, Lewis and Keshavan2005; Toulopoulou et al. Reference Toulopoulou, Quraishi, McDonald and Murray2006), such as the α7 neural nicotinic acid receptor (CHRNA7) (Dempster et al. Reference Dempster, Toulopoulou, McDonald, Bramon, Walshe, Wickham, Sham, Murray and Collier2006). Deletion of a region of chromosome 15q13.3, which includes CHRNA7, is associated with schizophrenia (Stefansson et al. Reference Stefansson, Rujescu, Cichon, Pietilainen, Ingason, Steinberg, Fossdal, Sigurdsson, Sigmundsson, Buizer-Voskamp, Hansen, Jakobsen, Muglia, Francks, Matthews, Gylfason, Halldorsson, Gudbjartsson, Thorgeirsson, Sigurdsson, Jonasdottir, Jonasdottir, Bjornsson, Mattiasdottir, Blondal, Haraldsson, Magnusdottir, Giegling, Moller, Hartmann, Shianna, Ge, Need, Crombie, Fraser, Walker, Lonnqvist, Suvisaari, Tuulio-Henriksson, Paunio, Toulopoulou, Bramon, Di Forti, Murray, Ruggeri, Vassos, Tosato, Walshe, Li, Vasilescu, Muhleisen, Wang, Ullum, Djurovic, Melle, Olesen, Kiemeney, Franke, Sabatti, Freimer, Gulcher, Thorsteinsdottir, Kong, Andreassen, Ophoff, Georgi, Rietschel, Werge, Petursson, Goldstein, Nothen, Peltonen, Collier, St Clair and Stefansson2008).

Episodic memory (EM), together with deficits in attention and executive function, is among the most promising neurocognitive endophenotypic markers (Sitskoorn et al. Reference Sitskoorn, Aleman, Ebisch, Appels and Kahn2004). Meta-analyses consistently report severe impairments in immediate and delayed verbal and non-verbal memory in patients (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998; Aleman et al. Reference Aleman, Hijman, De Haan and Kahn1999; Fioravanti et al. Reference Fioravanti, Carlone, Vitale, Cinti and Clare2005; Dickinson et al. Reference Dickinson, Ramsey and Gold2007). Cirillo & Seidman (Reference Cirillo and Seidman2003) concluded that memory deficits are probably due to an encoding failure, with some evidence for mild but significantly impaired rate of forgetting.

The most compelling support for the validation of EM as a marker for schizophrenia has come from studies of unaffected relatives, where memory deficits are hypothetically determined by familial effects, attributable to either common environmental or genetic effects. Meta-analyses concur that unaffected relatives have moderate memory deficits with verbal material and mild to moderate with non-verbal material (for a review see Reichenberg & Harvey, Reference Reichenberg and Harvey2007). Twin studies are now needed to confirm the heritability of EM and its genetic relationship with schizophrenia, as the earlier family studies could not discriminate between genetic and common environmental effects (Cannon, Reference Cannon2005). Studying monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) discordant pairs in a classic twin design offers a natural experiment to untangle these competing roles (Martin et al. Reference Martin, Boomsma and Machin1997; Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002).

Very few twin studies have examined EM in schizophrenia (Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Ragland, Torrey, Gold, Bigelow and Weinberger1990, Reference Goldberg, Torrey, Gold, Ragland, Bigelow and Weinberger1993; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Huttunen, Lonnqvist, Tuulio-Henriksson, Pirkola, Glahn, Finkelstein, Hietanen, Kaprio and Koskenvuo2000). Those that have formally quantified the heritability of EM in schizophrenia have tended to use the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), with small to moderate estimates (Tuulio-Henriksson et al. Reference Tuulio-Henriksson, Haukka, Partonen, Varilo, Paunio, Ekelund, Cannon, Meyer and Jouko2002; Glahn et al. Reference Glahn, Almasy, Blangero, Burk, Estrada, Peralta, Meyenberg, Castro, Barrett, Nicolini, Raventós and Escamilla2007; Greenwood et al. Reference Greenwood, Braff, Light, Cadenhead, Calkins, Dobie, Freedman, Green, Gur, Gur, Mintz, Nuechterlein, Olincy, Radant, Seidman, Siever, Silverman, Stone, Swerdlow, Tsuang, Tsuang, Turetsky and Schork2007; Gur et al. Reference Gur, Nimgaonkar, Almasy, Calkins, Ragland, Pogue-Geile, Kanes, Blangero and Gur2007; Husted et al. Reference Husted, Lim, Chow, Greenwood and Bassett2009). Goldberg et al. (Reference Goldberg, Torrey, Gold, Ragland, Bigelow and Weinberger1993), studying MZ discordant pairs, found that the unaffected co-twins were also impaired on EM, in both immediate and delayed recall, and in verbal and non-verbal modalities. Cannon et al. (Reference Cannon, Huttunen, Lonnqvist, Tuulio-Henriksson, Pirkola, Glahn, Finkelstein, Hietanen, Kaprio and Koskenvuo2000) used a broad neuropsychological battery and identified four tests, including verbal memory recall errors, to contribute to the discrimination of genetic loading for schizophrenia. However, to our knowledge no study has so far used formal genetic model fitting to investigate the heritability of EM and its genetic overlap with schizophrenia. We have used this model to quantify the heritability and genetic and environmental contributions to the covariation of schizophrenia and candidate endophenotypes, including intellectual functioning and working memory (Toulopoulou et al. Reference Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Hua-Hall, Ettinger, Sham and Murray2007); brain volumes (Rijsdijk et al. Reference Rijsdijk, van Haren, Picchioni, McDonald, Toulopoulou, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn, Murray and Sham2005); and event-related potential components (Hall et al. Reference Hall, Rijsdijk, Picchioni, Schulze, Ettinger, Toulopoulou, Bramon, Murray and Sham2007). Genetic modelling separates the covariance between two variables (e.g. schizophrenia and memory) into genetic and environmental components, indicating the degree to which the same genes influence the two traits, and the latter reflects the level to which their covariance is induced by the environment (Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002).

Our aim was to validate EM as a candidate endophenotype, specifically its heritability and the extent to which EM's dysfunction overlaps genetically with schizophrenia. We used regression analysis to test the hypothesis that (1) EM impairment in schizophrenia would be genetically influenced, with EM dysfunction increasing linearly with the degree of genetic loading for schizophrenia, such that an increased genetic risk for schizophrenia (in the healthy co-twins) predicts poor memory performance compared to controls; that is, patients<healthy co-twins<controls. We then used bivariate genetic modelling to quantify the genetic (i.e. heritability) and environmental contributions to variance on EM and also to estimate the genetic covariance between EM and liability to schizophrenia. We hypothesized that (2) EM would be moderately heritable; and (3) shared genetic influences would account for the majority of the covariance between EM and liability to schizophrenia. To investigate whether any covariance between memory and liability to schizophrenia was due to memory performance being an indicator of general intelligence, we also estimated the genetic covariance between intelligence and episodic memory.

Method

Participants

Twin patients were referred through National Health Service (NHS) treatment centres across the UK by their treating psychiatrists. The NHS cares for most patients with schizophrenia, and thus is a representative system from which to recruit. Control twins were recruited from the Institute of Psychiatry twin volunteer register and through newspaper advertisements. Exclusion criteria applied to all groups were: age under 18 years, a neurological disorder or a systemic illness with known neurological complications, any head injury associated with loss of consciousness for more than 1 minute, and current harmful substance use or dependency (within the past 12 months). No participants had a psychotic illness attributable to the use of illicit substances. All patients were clinically stable at the time of testing. All participants fulfilling study criteria provided written consent, which was approved by the UK Multicentre Research Ethics Committee.

Clinical assessment

DSM-IV diagnoses for all participants were made using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia – Lifetime Version (Spitzer & Endicott, Reference Spitzer and Endicott1978) and/or by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1997), supplemented by medical notes where necessary. Patients' psychotic symptoms in the month before testing were assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1984b) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1984a). Zygosity was confirmed by assessment of 12 highly polymorphic microsatellite markers and/or a standardized twin likeness questionnaire (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Dibble, Grawe and Pollin1975).

In concordant pairs, both members fulfilled criteria for DSM-IV schizophrenia or schizo-affective disorder. In discordant pairs, one member met diagnostic criteria whereas the co-twin was free of any psychotic illness. In control pairs, both members had no personal or family history of psychosis or schizophrenia spectrum disorder (to second-degree relatives).

Neurocognitive assessments

We used selected measures from the UK version of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1987) to assess modality-specific EM. Immediate and delayed versions of each subtest were administered. Specifically, verbal paired associates and logical memory were used to assess verbal learning and verbal memory respectively. Visual paired associates were used to evaluate visual learning and visual reproduction was selected to assess visual memory. Administration and scoring of the WMS-R was conducted in a standardized fashion. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale version 3 was used as a measure of intelligence (full-scale IQ) (WAIS FSIQ; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997).

Statistical analysis

Mean comparisons between patients, healthy co-twins and controls

We investigated whether patients and healthy co-twins performed significantly worse than controls on EM, and whether co-twins performed intermediate between patients and controls, using a regression analysis in Stata version 10 statistical software (Stata Corp., USA). Planned contrasts were performed to test our hypotheses by comparing MZ concordant patients, MZ discordant patients, and MZ discordant non-psychotic co-twins with MZ control twins. Similarly, affected and unaffected members of DZ discordant pairs were compared with DZ control twins. Age, years of education and sex were included as covariates.

Familial correlations violate the assumption of independence made in standard regression models. Generalized estimating equations (GEEs) were used to account for the lack of independence, specifically an exchangeable correlation structure was assumed to account for the within-family correlation. GEEs provide unbiased estimates of the marginal effects, even if the assumed correlation structure is misspecified (Hardin & Hilbe, Reference Hardin and Hilbe2003; Rabe-Hesketh & Skrondal, Reference Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal2005). To safeguard against a possible misspecification in the variance/covariance matrix, we used robust Hubert White sandwich estimators to adjust standard errors, hence confidence intervals (CIs) and p values (Williams, Reference Williams2000).

Genetic model fitting

Maximum-likelihood genetic model fitting was used to estimate the model parameters directly (additive genetic effects, A; common environmental effects shared between twins, C; and unique influences that twins do not share, E) from the observed raw MZ and DZ twin data. Using the program Mx (Neale, Reference Neale1999), we applied liability threshold models that assume that the risk for schizophrenia is distributed normally and that the disorder occurs only when a certain threshold is exceeded. The partitioning of the correlation between schizophrenia and each cognitive measure into the different sources of covariation yields genetic (r g), common environmental (r c) and individual-specific environmental (r e) correlations. As the r g, r c and r e correlations do not take into account the heritability of either trait, it is possible for a large genetic correlation to in fact explain very little of the observed covariation between these two traits. Therefore, the model also combines the information from r g, r c and r e with the heritabilities a2, c2 and e2 of each trait to calculate that part of the phenotypic correlation (r ph) due to genetic effects (r ph-a) by \lsqb\! {\sqrt \lpar{{\rm h}_{\rm Sz}^{\setnum{2}} }\rpar \times r_{\rm g} \times \sqrt \lpar{{\rm h}_{\rm memory}^{\setnum{2}} } } \rpar\rsqb, to common environment (r ph-c) by \lsqb\! {\sqrt \lpar{{\rm c}_{\rm Sz}^{\setnum{2}} }\rpar \times r_{\rm c} \times &#x003C;$&#x003E; &#x003C;$&#x003E;\sqrt \lpar{{\rm c}_{{\rm memory}}^{\setnum{2}} } } \rpar\rsqb and to unique environment effects (r ph-e) \lsqb \!{\sqrt \lpar{{\rm e}_{\rm Sz}^{\setnum{2}} } \rpar\times r_{\rm e} \times \sqrt \lpar{{\rm e}_{\rm memory}^{\setnum{2}} } } \rpar\rsqb.

The model parameters for schizophrenia were fixed to the point estimates derived by meta-analysis (Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Kendler and Neale2003) as follows: h2=0.81, c2=0.11, e2=0.08. In addition, the threshold on the liability to schizophrenia was fixed to a lifetime population prevalence of 1%. Before model fitting, the effects of age, sex and education on the neuropsychology variables were regressed out and ordinalized into five equal classes to facilitate raw ordinal data analysis in Mx. More information on this model can be found in the articles by Rijsdijk et al. (Reference Rijsdijk, van Haren, Picchioni, McDonald, Toulopoulou, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn, Murray and Sham2005), Hall et al. (Reference Hall, Rijsdijk, Picchioni, Schulze, Ettinger, Toulopoulou, Bramon, Murray and Sham2007) and Toulopoulou et al. (Reference Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Hua-Hall, Ettinger, Sham and Murray2007).

The genetic overlap between intelligence and memory was also estimated using the above model; however, the model parameters did not need to be fixed as was the case for schizophrenia. This analysis was used to investigate whether EM is an indicator of general intelligence.

In some cases the second member of a twin pair did not complete the neuropsychology assessment due to attrition. However, data from one twin can still be used in regression analyses and in estimating variances in structural equation modelling. Therefore, to optimize the data set we included the first twin in our analyses.

Results

Demographic and clinical variables

Of the 297 participants (52 DZ pairs, 96 MZ pairs and one set of MZ triplets), 17 did not complete the neuropsychology assessment. When this occurred, their co-twin who did complete the assessment was retained in the analysis. The final sample of 280 included MZ concordant twins (22 twin pairs, one set of triplets and three patients whose co-twin did not complete the assessment), MZ discordant twins (12 pairs and one healthy co-twin), DZ discordant twins (10 pairs, two patients and three healthy co-twins), MZ controls (52 pairs and four co-twins) and DZ control (34 pairs and four co-twins). Table 1 displays the demographic data.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics for patients, well co-twins and controls (memory)

CC, Concordant; DC, discordant; MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic; s.d., standard deviation.

Among the 26 unaffected co-twins from the discordant pairs, 12 met criteria for historical DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis: one depression only, one depression and phobia, two depression and panic disorder, one depression, panic disorder and agoraphobia, one depression and generalized anxiety disorder, one depression, alcohol and substance misuse and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), one drug-induced psychosis, one depression substance misuse and panic disorder, one a manic episode, panic disorder and OCD, one depression, panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder, and one alcohol misuse only.

Among the 180 control twins, 13 individuals met criteria for historical DSM-IV Axis I diagnoses: eight depression only, one depression, mania, panic disorder and OCD, two depression, panic disorder, OCD and phobia, one panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder, and one anorexia nervosa. None of the unaffected co-twins or controls were unwell at the time of testing, under medical supervision, or taking any psychotropic medication.

Results from the mean comparisons between patients, co-twins and controls

Summary statistics are presented in Table 2, and Table 3 presents the results of the GEE analyses. Group had a significant effect on all memory variables. MZ twin patients, concordant and discordant, performed worse than controls on every WMS-R measure. Importantly, the non-psychotic members of the MZ pairs discordant for schizophrenia also performed significantly worse than the control subjects on five of the WMS-R subtests. DZ patients performed worse than controls on four of the eight measures whereas their co-twins did not differ from controls on any of the measures. Both patients and unaffected co-twins performed significantly worse than controls on full-scale intelligence. All significant results remained significant after adjustment for multiple testing using the Simes method (Reference Simes1986).

Table 2. Summary statistics of means and standard deviations on WMS-R memory variables for patients, co-twins and controls

CC, Concordant; DC, discordant; MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic; WMS-R, Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised; WAIS FSIQ, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Full-Scale IQ.

Table 3. Age-, education- and gender-adjusted generalized estimator equations show the marginal mean differences of planned comparisons between groups on memory

CC, Concordant; DC, discordant; MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic; WAIS FSIQ, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Full-Scale IQ.

All significant results remain significant after Simes correction for multiple testing.

Results from the bivariate twin modelling analysis

Heritability of EM

Table 4 shows the additive genetic effects (h2), shared environmental effects (c2) and unique environmental effects (e2) for WMS-R measures of EM. Genetic factors accounted for a significant proportion of total variance in four of the eight memory measures. Verbal memory recall demonstrated a moderate amount of variance due to genetic factors in both immediate (h2=0.45) and delayed conditions (h2=0.41), whereas immediate recall of visual learning (h2=0.31) and delayed visual memory (h2=0.37) were explained by small to moderate genetic influences. Shared environmental influences did not significantly account for inter-individual differences on any subtest. Unique environmental effects and error accounted for the remaining variances.

Table 4. Additive genetic, common and specific environmental estimates (with 95% CI) of full ACE genetic model

CI, Confidence interval; WAIS FSIQ, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Full-Scale IQ.

Parameters for schizophrenia are fixed based on a prevalence of 1% and the following genetic model: h2=0.81, c2=0.11, e2=0.08, where h2, c2 and e2 indicate heritability, shared environment and non-shared environment respectively. CIs not including 0 indicate statistical significance, given in bold.

A, C and E overlap between episodic memory and schizophrenia

Significant phenotypic correlations (r ph) suggested that increased liability to schizophrenia was associated with poorer performance on all the EM tests (see Table 5). Verbal memory delayed had the highest phenotypic correlation with schizophrenia (r ph=−0.48), followed by visual learning immediate (r ph=−0.41), verbal memory immediate (r ph=−0.40) and visual memory delayed (r ph=−0.37).

Table 5. The phenotypic correlations between schizophrenia and memory variables (rph), the decomposed sources of these correlations (rph-a, rph-c, rph-e) as predicted by the full ACE models and A, C and E correlation estimates (with 95% CI)

CI, Confidence interval; WAIS FSIQ, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Full-Scale IQ.

r ph-a, r ph-c and r ph-e indicate the phenotypic correlations due to additive genetic, shared environmental, and specific environmental influence respectively. r ph indicates the total phenotypic correlation. r g, r c and r e indicate the genetic, shared environmental, and specific environmental correlations respectively. The fixed genetic model for schizophrenia used the following parameters: h2=0.81, c2=0.11, e2=0.08. The 95% CIs not including 0 indicate statistical significance, given in bold.

The extent to which two traits share genetic, common environmental and unique environmental effects is given by the correlations r g, r c and r e respectively (Table 5). The same four subtests as above had significant genetic correlations (r g) with schizophrenia, visual learning immediate recall (r g=−0.71), and both verbal memory delayed (r g=−0.58) and immediate recall (r g=−0.58), and visual memory delayed (r g=−0.55). No common environmental (r c) or shared unique environment (r e) correlations were significant.

The model also combines the information from r g, r c and r e with the heritabilities of each trait to calculate that part of the phenotypic correlation (r ph) due to genetic effects (r ph-a), common environmental (r ph-c) and unique environmental effects (r ph-e).

Further examination of the four measures mentioned above revealed that 88% [(r pha/rph)×100] of the phenotypic correlation between visual learning immediate and schizophrenia (r ph=−0.41) was due to shared genetic influences (r ph-a=−0.36); 88% of the phenotypic correlation with verbal memory immediate recall (r ph=−0.40) was due to genetic overlap (r ph-a=−0.35), whereas 69% for verbal memory delayed recall (r ph=−0.48, r ph-a=−0.33) and 81% of visual memory delayed (r ph=−0.37, r ph-a=−0.30) was due to the same shared genetic influences. The proportion of the phenotypic correlations due to shared unique environment or shared common environment was not significant for all subtests.

A, C and E overlap between EM and intelligence

As expected, significant phenotypic correlations were observed between intelligence and EM ranging from 0.39 to 0.62. However, no significant genetic correlations were observed, instead common environment correlations (r c) and unique environment (r e) correlations were significant. (See Supplementary Table 1, available online.)

Discussion

This study aimed to test the validity of EM as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. We first tested whether EM impairment increased linearly with level of genetic risk for schizophrenia to demonstrate that memory deficits are associated with the disorder's aetiological influences. MZ patients, whether concordant or discordant for schizophrenia, scored significantly lower than controls on all EM subtests, whereas DZ discordant patients performed worse than controls on only four of the EM subtests. No previous studies have found MZ and DZ patients to differ (Cannon, Reference Cannon2005). This finding is probably a power issue or due to our sampling method. Because of their high genetic risk (sharing all of their genes with their ill co-twins), non-psychotic MZ discordant co-twins should show a similar level of impairment as their ill co-twins, if memory impairment is indeed part of a genetic causal pathway. As we hypothesized, the non-psychotic MZ discordant co-twins performed very similarly to their ill co-twins.

We hypothesized that DZ co-twins would score intermediately between their ill co-twins and controls, as they share approximately half of their genes with their ill co-twin. DZ non-psychotic co-twins did not differ from controls, which could be anticipated given their ill co-twins' relatively good memory performance and their hypothesized intermediate status between patient and controls. Overall, the regression analysis suggested that higher genetic loading for schizophrenia predicts poorer memory performance.

Bivariate genetic modelling estimated that some EM measures were moderately heritable, a requirement of endophenotype validity (Cannon & Keller, Reference Cannon and Keller2006). The tests with the greatest proportion of variance due to genetic effects (i.e. the greatest heritability) were verbal memory (immediate and delayed), followed by immediate visual learning and delayed visual memory. Shared environment did not contribute significantly on any tests. The remainder of the variance on test performance was due to unique environmental effects and measurement error.

Our findings support the few studies that have estimated the heritability of EM from Wechsler memory scales. Husted et al. (Reference Husted, Lim, Chow, Greenwood and Bassett2009) measured verbal and visual memory (but not visual or verbal learning) using the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition (WMS-III), in multiplex families, and found immediate verbal memory (h2=0.49) to be moderately heritable. Using sibling pairs that included discordant and control pairs, Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Rice, Thompson, Barch and Csernansky2009) estimated the heritability of an EM composite score, which combined verbal memory and family pictures of the WMS-III and the CVLT, to be 0.57.

We used bivariate genetic modelling to formally quantify the hypothetical genetic relationship between EM deficits and schizophrenia. We found evidence of significant phenotypic correlations (r ph) between the increased liability to schizophrenia and poorer performance on all the EM tests. We found significant genetic correlations (r g) between schizophrenia and four tests: visual learning immediate recall (r g=−0.78), both verbal memory delayed (r g=−0.58) and immediate recall (r g=−0.58), and delayed visual memory (r g=−0.55). Although these moderate to strong genetic correlations suggest that a high proportion of the same genes are involved in both phenotypes, the statistic (r g) does not consider the heritability of either trait. Partitioning the phenotypic correlation (r ph) into the proportions due to genetic, shared environment and unique environment revealed that genetic influences were indeed responsible for the majority of the phenotypic correlations for these four subtests. For example, 88% of the phenotypic correlation between immediate visual learning and liability to schizophrenia (r ph=−0.41) is due to shared genetic influences. We can conclude that verbal memory (immediate and delayed), immediate visual learning and delayed visual memory performance are heritable and share a substantial genetic overlap with schizophrenia. We have previously demonstrated that intelligence and its subcomponents, in particular working memory, are both heritable and share genetic variance with schizophrenia (Toulopoulou et al. Reference Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Hua-Hall, Ettinger, Sham and Murray2007). These findings for episodic memory are unlikely to be caused by memory being correlated with general intelligence. Our analysis revealed no significant genetic correlation between memory and intelligence. These findings support the hypothesis that initial encoding of information is the core memory deficit in schizophrenia (Cirillo & Seidman, Reference Cirillo and Seidman2003). There is emerging evidence that genetic risk for schizophrenia is not disorder specific. For example, Genome-Wide Association Studies (GWAS) data show that many of the same risk variants are associated with bipolar disorder (O'Donovan et al. Reference O'Donovan, Craddock, Norton, Williams, Peirce, Moskvina, Nikolov, Hamshere, Carroll, Georgieva, Dwyer, Holmans, Marchini, Spencer, Howie, Leung, Hartmann, Moller, Morris, Shi, Feng, Hoffmann, Propping, Vasilescu, Maier, Rietschel, Zammit, Schumacher, Quinn, Schulze, Williams, Giegling, Iwata, Ikeda, Darvasi, Shifman, He, Duan, Sanders, Levinson, Gejman, Cichon, Nothen, Gill, Corvin, Rujescu, Kirov and Owen2008) and a recent Swedish populations study shows the two disorders share a common genetic aetiology (Lichtenstein et al. Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon, Sullivan and Hultman2009). The presence of memory deficits in other psychiatric and developmental disorders could be due to common genetic risk variants.

Limitations

There are several limitations to our study. First, the study was underpowered to detect shared common environmental effects; however, these are unlikely to be greater than the common environmental effects for schizophrenia, which are small. The heritability estimates of several subtests were moderate yet failed to reach significance, suggesting a lack of power. In addition, the large 95% CIs of the genetic correlations demonstrate that large twin samples are needed for bivariate genetic modelling studies. Second, although the classic twin design is a powerful method of determining the sources of phenotypic correlations, it is dependent on several assumptions, namely that: MZ and DZ twin pairs share their environments to the same extent; gene–environment correlations and interactions are minimal for the trait; twins are no different from the general population in terms of the trait; and matings in the population occur at random (no assortment). For a detailed review of the implications of the assumption, see Rijsdijk & Sham (Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002). Genetic modelling of twin data is also subject to parameter indeterminacy (Keller & Coventry, Reference Keller and Coventry2005), whereby different combinations of the common environment/assortative mating and additive, dominant and epistatic genetic effects can lead to the same observed covariation between twin pairs. For example, intermediate levels of an endophenotype in DZ co-twins, that is between MZ co-twins and controls, may not be due to additive genetic effects, but alternatively could be due to non-additive genetic effects in combination with common environment (Keller & Coventry, Reference Keller and Coventry2005). Future studies can reduce this parameter indeterminacy, by also modelling the similarities among other family members, that is an extended twin-family design.

Finally, although the sample was recruited nationally, this is not a population-based study and therefore the heritability of schizophrenia could not be estimated. To account for this, our model parameters for schizophrenia were fixed to values estimated by a meta-analysis and the threshold to the population prevalence. Our results on the heritability of memory performance were compatible with those in healthy populations, supporting the findings of our study (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Rice, Thompson, Barch and Csernansky2009; Husted et al. Reference Husted, Lim, Chow, Greenwood and Bassett2009).

Conclusions

This is the first study to estimate the genetic overlap between learning and memory performance and liability to schizophrenia, using genetic modelling in the classic twin design. Verbal memory, visual memory and visual learning performance were moderately heritable and shared a substantial genetic overlap with schizophrenia. Although caution is needed because of our large CIs and relatively small sample, our findings nevertheless suggest that EM is a valid endophenotype. Further investigation of both the neural correlates and the genetic association of cognitive phenotypes is needed to identify the mechanisms by which genetic susceptibility impacts brain function in schizophrenia.

Note

Supplementary material accompanies this paper on the Journal's website (http://journals.cambridge.org/psm).

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the European Community's Sixth Framework Programme through a Marie Curie Training Network called the European Twin Study Network on Schizophrenia, NARSAD (through a Young Investigator Award to Dr Toulopoulou), grant 064971 from the Wellcome Trust, grant PTA-037-27-0002 from the Economic and Social Research Council/Medical Research Council, and the Psychiatry Research Trust. We acknowledge support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Specialist Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health award to the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and the Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London.

Declaration of Interest

Professor Murray has received honoraria for lectures from Lilly, Janssen, BMS, AstraZenenca. Dr Picchioni has received travel awards from Pfizer, Janssen-Cilag, and Eli Lilly.

References

Aleman, A, Hijman, R, De Haan, EHF, Kahn, RS (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13581366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC (1984 a). Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. University of Iowa Press: Iowa City.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC (1984 b). Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. University of Iowa Press: Iowa City.Google Scholar
Blyler, CR, Gold, JM (2000). Cognitive effects of typical antipsychotic treatment: another look. In Cognition in Schizophrenia: Impairments, Importance and Treatment Strategies (ed. Sharma, T. and Harvey, P. D.), pp. 241265. Oxford University Press: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, M, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Harrington, H, Taylor, A, Murray, RM, Poulton, R (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 449456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, TD (2005). The inheritance of intermediate phenotypes for schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 18, 135140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, TD, Huttunen, MO, Lonnqvist, J, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Pirkola, T, Glahn, D, Finkelstein, J, Hietanen, M, Kaprio, J, Koskenvuo, M (2000). The inheritance of neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for schizophrenia. American Journal of Human Genetics 67, 369382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, TD, Keller, MC (2006). Endophenotypes in the genetic analyses of mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2, 267290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, TD, Hennah, W, van Erp, TG, Thompson, PM, Lonnqvist, J, Huttunen, M, Gasperoni, T, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Pirkola, T, Toga, AW, Kaprio, J, Mazziotta, J, Peltonen, L (2005). Association of DISC1/TRAX haplotypes with schizophrenia, reduced prefrontal gray matter, and impaired short- and long-term memory. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 12051213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, L-S, Rice, TK, Thompson, PA, Barch, DM, Csernansky, JG (2009). Familial aggregation of clinical and neurocognitive features in sibling pairs with and without schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 111, 159166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirillo, MA, Seidman, LJ (2003). Verbal declarative memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: from clinical assessment to genetics and brain mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review 13, 4377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, DJ, Dibble, E, Grawe, JM, Pollin, W (1975). Reliably separating identical from fraternal twins. Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 13711375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dempster, EL, Toulopoulou, T, McDonald, C, Bramon, E, Walshe, M, Wickham, H, Sham, PC, Murray, RM, Collier, DA (2006). Episodic memory performance predicted by the 2bp deletion in exon 6 of the ‘alpha 7-like’ nicotinic receptor subunit gene. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 18321834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D, Ramsey, ME, Gold, JM (2007). Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 532542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvevag, B, Goldberg, TE (2000). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology 14, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fioravanti, M, Carlone, O, Vitale, B, Cinti, ME, Clare, L (2005). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review 15, 7395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBM (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Glahn, DC, Almasy, L, Blangero, J, Burk, GM, Estrada, J, Peralta, JM, Meyenberg, N, Castro, MP, Barrett, J, Nicolini, H, Raventós, H, Escamilla, MA (2007). Adjudicating neurocognitive endophenotypes for schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics 144B, 242249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, TE, Egan, MF, Gscheidle, T, Coppola, R, Weickert, T, Kolachana, BS, Goldman, D, Weinberger, DR (2003). Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 889896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, TE, Ragland, JD, Torrey, EF, Gold, JM, Bigelow, LB, Weinberger, DR (1990). Neuropsychological assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 47, 10661072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, TE, Torrey, EF, Gold, JM, Ragland, JD, Bigelow, LB, Weinberger, DR (1993). Learning and memory in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 23, 7185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, II, Gould, TD (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, TA, Braff, DL, Light, GA, Cadenhead, KS, Calkins, ME, Dobie, DJ, Freedman, R, Green, MF, Gur, RE, Gur, RC, Mintz, J, Nuechterlein, KH, Olincy, A, Radant, AD, Seidman, LJ, Siever, LJ, Silverman, JM, Stone, WS, Swerdlow, NR, Tsuang, DW, Tsuang, MT, Turetsky, BI, Schork, NJ (2007). Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 12421250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gur, RE, Nimgaonkar, VL, Almasy, L, Calkins, ME, Ragland, JD, Pogue-Geile, MF, Kanes, S, Blangero, J, Gur, RC (2007). Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 813819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, M-H, Rijsdijk, F, Picchioni, M, Schulze, K, Ettinger, U, Toulopoulou, T, Bramon, E, Murray, RM, Sham, P (2007). Substantial shared genetic influences on schizophrenia and event-related potentials. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 804812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardin, JW, Hilbe, JM (2003). Generalized Estimating Equations. Chapman & Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL.Google Scholar
Heaton, RK, Gladsjo, JA, Palmer, BW, Kuck, J, Marcotte, TD, Jeste, DV (2001). Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 2432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, RW, Zakzanis, KK (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Husted, JA, Lim, S, Chow, EWC, Greenwood, C, Bassett, AS (2009). Heritability of neurocognitive traits in familial schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics 150B, 845853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P, Rodgers, B, Murray, R, Marmot, M (1994). Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344, 13981402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, MC, Coventry, WL (2005). Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Research and Human Genetics 8, 201213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P, Yip, BH, Björk, C, Pawitan, Y, Cannon, TD, Sullivan, PF, Hultman, CM (2009). Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. Lancet 373, 234239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, N, Boomsma, D, Machin, G (1997). A twin-pronged attack on complex traits. Nature Genetics 17, 387392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, MC (1999). Mx: Statistical Modelling. Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia: Richmond, VA.Google Scholar
O'Donovan, MC, Craddock, N, Norton, N, Williams, H, Peirce, T, Moskvina, V, Nikolov, I, Hamshere, M, Carroll, L, Georgieva, L, Dwyer, S, Holmans, P, Marchini, JL, Spencer, CCA, Howie, B, Leung, H-T, Hartmann, AM, Moller, H-J, Morris, DW, Shi, Y, Feng, G, Hoffmann, P, Propping, P, Vasilescu, C, Maier, W, Rietschel, M, Zammit, S, Schumacher, J, Quinn, EM, Schulze, TG, Williams, NM, Giegling, I, Iwata, N, Ikeda, M, Darvasi, A, Shifman, S, He, L, Duan, J, Sanders, AR, Levinson, DF, Gejman, PV, Cichon, S, Nothen, MM, Gill, M, Corvin, A, Rujescu, D, Kirov, G, Owen, MJ (2008). Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up. Nature Genetics 40, 10531055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prasad, KM, Chowdari, KV, Nimgaonkar, VL, Talkowski, ME, Lewis, DA, Keshavan, MS (2005). Genetic polymorphisms of the RGS4 and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex morphometry among first episode schizophrenia patients. Molecular Psychiatry 10, 213219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabe-Hesketh, S, Skrondal, A (2005). Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling using Stata. Stata Press: College Station, TX.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A, Harvey, PD (2007). Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin 133, 833858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, FV, Sham, PC (2002). Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics 3, 119133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, FV, van Haren, NEM, Picchioni, MM, McDonald, C, Toulopoulou, T, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Kahn, RS, Murray, R, Sham, PC (2005). Brain MRI abnormalities in schizophrenia: same genes or same environment? Psychological Medicine 35, 13991409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simes, RJ (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika 73, 751754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sitskoorn, MM, Aleman, A, Ebisch, SJH, Appels, MCM, Kahn, RS (2004). Cognitive deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 71, 285295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, RL, Endicott, J (1978). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia – Lifetime Version. New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Stefansson, H, Rujescu, D, Cichon, S, Pietilainen, OPH, Ingason, A, Steinberg, S, Fossdal, R, Sigurdsson, E, Sigmundsson, T, Buizer-Voskamp, JE, Hansen, T, Jakobsen, KD, Muglia, P, Francks, C, Matthews, PM, Gylfason, A, Halldorsson, BV, Gudbjartsson, D, Thorgeirsson, TE, Sigurdsson, A, Jonasdottir, A, Jonasdottir, A, Bjornsson, A, Mattiasdottir, S, Blondal, T, Haraldsson, M, Magnusdottir, BB, Giegling, I, Moller, H-J, Hartmann, A, Shianna, KV, Ge, D, Need, AC, Crombie, C, Fraser, G, Walker, N, Lonnqvist, J, Suvisaari, J, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Paunio, T, Toulopoulou, T, Bramon, E, Di Forti, M, Murray, R, Ruggeri, M, Vassos, E, Tosato, S, Walshe, M, Li, T, Vasilescu, C, Muhleisen, TW, Wang, AG, Ullum, H, Djurovic, S, Melle, I, Olesen, J, Kiemeney, LA, Franke, B, Sabatti, C, Freimer, NB, Gulcher, JR, Thorsteinsdottir, U, Kong, A, Andreassen, OA, Ophoff, RA, Georgi, A, Rietschel, M, Werge, T, Petursson, H, Goldstein, DB, Nothen, MM, Peltonen, L, Collier, DA, St Clair, D, Stefansson, K (2008). Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia. Nature 455, 232236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, PF, Kendler, KS, Neale, MC (2003). Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 11871192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, HY, Callicott, JH, Weinberger, DR (2008). Intermediate phenotypes in schizophrenia genetics redux: is it a no brainer? Molecular Psychiatry 13, 233238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T, Picchioni, M, Rijsdijk, F, Hua-Hall, M, Ettinger, U, Sham, P, Murray, R (2007). Substantial genetic overlap between neurocognition and schizophrenia: genetic modeling in twin samples. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 13481355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T, Quraishi, S, McDonald, C, Murray, RM (2006). The Maudsley Family Study: premorbid and current general intellectual function levels in familial bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 28, 243259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Haukka, J, Partonen, T, Varilo, T, Paunio, T, Ekelund, J, Cannon, TD, Meyer, JM, Jouko, L (2002). Heritability and number of quantitative trait loci of neurocognitive functions in families with schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics 114, 483490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1987). Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition: Administration and Scoring Manual. The Psychological Corporation: London, UK.Google Scholar
Williams, RL (2000). A note on robust variance estimation for cluster-correlated data. Biometrics 56, 645646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics for patients, well co-twins and controls (memory)

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary statistics of means and standard deviations on WMS-R memory variables for patients, co-twins and controls

Figure 2

Table 3. Age-, education- and gender-adjusted generalized estimator equations show the marginal mean differences of planned comparisons between groups on memory

Figure 3

Table 4. Additive genetic, common and specific environmental estimates (with 95% CI) of full ACE genetic model

Figure 4

Table 5. The phenotypic correlations between schizophrenia and memory variables (rph), the decomposed sources of these correlations (rph-a, rph-c, rph-e) as predicted by the full ACE models and A, C and E correlation estimates (with 95% CI)

Supplementary material: File

Owens Supplementary Material

Table.doc

Download Owens Supplementary Material(File)
File 68.1 KB