Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:04:20.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of post-training modafinil administration in a discriminative avoidance task in mice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2015

Helaine Arrais Fernandes
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Karina Agustini Zanin
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Camilla de Lima Patti*
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Leonardo Brito Lopes-Silva
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Carolina Souza Bizerra
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Lia Rita Azeredo Bittencourt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Sergio Tufik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Roberto Frussa-Filho
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, SP, Brazil
*
Camilla L. Patti, Departamento de Farmacologia – UNIFESP, Rua Botucatu, 862, Ed. Leal Prado, 1º andar, 04023062, São Paulo, SP, Brazil. Tel/Fax: +55 011 5549 4122; E-mail: camila.patti@unifesp.br; clpatti@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Although the cognitive-enhancing abilities after modafinil have been demonstrated, its effects on memory consolidation remain overlooked. We investigated the effects of repeated modafinil administration on consolidation of a discriminative avoidance task.

Methods

Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. After training, mice received intraperitonial modafinil (doses of 32, 64 or 128 mg/kg). Animals were treated for more 9 consecutive days; 30 min after the last injection, testing was performed. In addition, the effects of 32 mg/kg modafinil on consolidation at different time points were examined.

Results

The smaller dose of modafinil (32 mg/kg) impaired memory consolidation, without modifying anxiety or locomotion. Still, modafinil post-training administration at 1 or 2 h impaired memory persistence.

Conclusions

Modafinil impaired memory consolidation in a dose- and time-dependent fashion.

Type
Short Communications
Copyright
© Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2015 

Significant outcomes

  • Repeated administration of 32 mg/kg (but not 64 or 128 mg/kg) modafinil impaired consolidation of a discriminative avoidance task in mice.

  • Modafinil, at the dose of 32 mg/kg, impaired memory persistence when given 1 or 2 h after training.

  • The memory impairment was not accompanied by anxiety alterations.

Limitations

  • The effects of pre-test administration of modafinil may have influenced performance during testing; later post-training time points intervals were not evaluated.

Introduction

Modafinil is a psychostimulant that acts as a wake-promoting drug and has been approved unanimously for the treatment of excessive daytime sleepiness in narcolepsy and, in some European countries and in the United States of America, for obstructive sleep apnea, and shift work syndrome. Clinical studies have pointed modafinil positive effects when used to treat Parkinson’s disease (Reference Ferraro, Antonelli, O’Connor, Tanganelli, Rambert and Fuxe1), multiple sclerosis (Reference Kraft and Bowen2), schizophrenia (Reference Turner, Clark, Pomarol-Clotet, Mckenna, Robbins and Sahakian3) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Reference Taylor and Russo4). In addition, modafinil seems to be widely prescribed off-label to enhance alertness, attention, memory for dementia and depression (Reference Joos, Docx, Schmaal, Sabbe and Dom5). Of note, an illicit market exists for academic doping as well (Reference Cakic6).

It has been demonstrated that modafinil alters the activity of brain areas involved with memory, such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (Reference Béracochéa, Celerier, Peres and Piérard7). Within this context, studies indicate that modafinil has cognitive-enhancing abilities in rodents performing a variety of learning/memory exercises in the T-maze based on spontaneous alternation behavior (Reference Béracochéa, Cagnard, Celerier, Merrer, Peres and Piérard8), and also enhanced learning (Reference Béracochéa, Celerier, Peres and Piérard7).

Concerning its repeated administration, modafinil was reported to improve learning (Reference Béracochéa, Celerier, Borde, Valleau, Peres and Piérard9). Although many studies have reported cognitive-enhancing abilities of this wake-promoting drug (Reference Shuman, Wood and Anagnostaras10), the possible facilitative effects of modafinil on memory have been overlooked when memory consolidation was specifically investigated. In this way, a study conducted by Shuman et al. (Reference Shuman, Wood and Anagnostaras10) reported that the administration of modafinil immediately after training had no effects on either cued or contextual fear paradigms. In contrast, we have recently demonstrated that the post-training acute administration of modafinil impaired memory consolidation in mice subjected to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (Reference Fernandes, Zanin and Patti11).

Aims of the study

The investigation of the effects of the post-training administration of modafinil on the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task can be interesting because this animal model can concurrently evaluate learning and memory, anxiety and locomotion (Reference Patti, Zanin and Sanday12Reference Claro, Silva and Frussa-Filho17). The present findings provide evidence of potential amnestic proprieties of modafinil when repeatedly administered or administrated in distinct time points after training.

Material and methods

Subjects

Three month-old Swiss EPM-M1 male mice (outbred, raised and maintained in the Center for Development of Experimental Models in Medicine and Biology of the Universidade Federal de São Paulo) were used. Animals weighing 30–35 g were housed under controlled temperature (22–23°C) and lighting (12 h light, 12 h dark; lights on at 06:45 a.m.) conditions. Food and water were available ad libitum throughout the experiments. Animals used in this study were maintained in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals (NIH Publications N° 8023), revised 2011. The Institutional Ethical Committee of UNIFESP approved the experimental procedures under protocol #1162/08.

Drug

Modafinil (Cephalon®) was dissolved in 0.5% Arabic gum and intraperitoneally (i.p.) administered in a volume of 10 ml/kg body weight at doses of 32, 64 or 128 mg/kg. Modafinil vehicle was used as the control solution and administered i.p. This dose range was selected based on previous work of our group (Reference Fernandes, Zanin and Patti11,Reference Wuo-Silva, Fukushiro and Borçoi18).

Plus-maze discriminative avoidance task

The apparatus employed was a modified elevated plus-maze, made of wood, containing two enclosed arms with sidewalls and no top (28.5×7×14 cm, 03 lx at the floor level), opposite to two open arms (28.5×7 cm, 9 lx at the floor level). A 100-W lamp was placed exactly over the middle of one of the enclosed arms (aversive enclosed arm, 660 lx at the floor level). In the training, each mouse was placed in the centre of the apparatus and, over a period of 10 min, every time the animal entered the enclosed arm containing the lamp, an aversive situation was produced until the animal left the arm. The aversive stimuli were the 100-W light and a cold air blow produced by a hair dryer placed over the aversive enclosed arm. In the testing (performed in the same room with the observer in the same position), the mice were again placed in the centre of the apparatus and observed for 3 min without receiving the aversive stimulation. In all experiments, the animals were observed in a random order and in a blind manner, and the apparatus was cleaned with a 5% alcohol solution after each behavioural session.

The total number of entries into any of the arms (an entry was defined as the entry of all four paws into one arm), percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm (time spent in aversive enclosed arm/time spent in both enclosed arms) and percent time spent in the open arms (time spent in open arms/time spent in both open and enclosed arms) were calculated. Learning and memory were evaluated by the percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm during the training and testing sessions, respectively. Anxiety-like behavior was evaluated by the percent time spent in the open arms of the apparatus. Total number of entries into any of the arms was used to evaluate motor activity.

Experimental design

Experiment I Effects of repeated modafinil administration on consolidation in mice submitted to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task.

A total of 48 animals were randomly assigned to one of the following groups: vehicle (VEH, n=12), 32 mg/kg modafinil (MOD32, n=12), 64 mg/kg modafinil (MOD64, n=12) or 128 mg/kg modafinil (MOD128, n=12). Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Immediately after, they received an acute i.p. injection of vehicle or modafinil at the different doses. Mice also received nine subsequent daily injections of vehicle or different modafinil doses; 30 min after the last injection (10th day after training), animals were submitted to testing.

Experiment II Effects of 32 mg/Kg modafinil administered in specific intervals after training in mice submitted to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task.

A total of 50 mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. After being trained, they were allocated into five groups (n=10), which received 32 mg/kg MOD at different time points (1, 2, 3 or 6 h after training). Vehicle group (VEH) received i.p. vehicle injections at all the time points; MOD groups received MOD 1, 2, 3 or 6 h after training and vehicle injections at all the remaining time points; 10 days after training the testing was performed.

Statistical analysis

Total number of entries in any of the arms, percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm (time spent in aversive enclosed arm/time spent in both enclosed arms), and percent time spent in open arms (time spent in open arms/time spent in both open and enclosed arms) were calculated and compared by the analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Tukey’s test. Significance was accepted at p values below 0.05.

Results

Experiment I Effects of repeated modafinil administration on consolidation in mice submitted to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task.

During the training, the ANOVA for time spent in the aversive enclosed arm did not reveal any statically significant differences among the groups [F(3,44)=0.35; p>0.05] (Fig. 1a), demonstrating similar basal learning of the task. In addition, ANOVA did not reveal significant differences among the groups regarding the percent time spent in the open arms [F(3,44)=0.42; p>0.05] (Fig. 1b) or the number of entries [F(3,44)=0.53; p>0.05] (Fig. 1c), as expected.

Fig. 1 Effects of repeated post-training modafinil administration on memory of mice subjected to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task without receiving any experimental manipulation. Immediately after training, mice were intraperitoneally treated with vehicle (VEH, n=12) or modafinil at the doses of 32 (MOD32, n=12), 64 (MOD64, n=12) or 128 mg/kg (MOD128) for 10 consecutive days. Results are presented as the mean±SE of percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm in the training (a) and testing (d), percent time spent in the open arms in the training (b) and testing (e) and number of entries in the training (c) and testing (f). ● p<0.05 compared with the other groups (analysis of variance and Tukey’s test).

During testing, the ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test showed that mice repeatedly treated with 32 mg/kg modafinil (the MOD32 group) displayed an increased percent time in the aversive enclosed arm compared with mice treated with vehicle or with the higher doses of modafinil (the VEH, MOD64 and MOD128 groups) [F(3,44)=3.61; p<0.05] (Fig. 1d). Finally, ANOVA did not reveal any significant differences in the percent time spent in the open arms [F(3,44)=0.29; p>0.05] (Fig. 1e) or in the total number of entries [F(3,44)=0.80; p<0.05] (Fig. 1f).

Experiment II Effects of 32 mg/Kg modafinil administered in specific intervals after training in mice submitted to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task.

Data from the training demonstrated that there were no significant differences in the basal performance of the different groups. Indeed, ANOVA did not reveal significant effects for the percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm, time spent in the open arms and total number of entries in the training session (Figs 2a, b and c).

Fig. 2 Effects of 32 mg/kg modafinil administration on memory consolidation at different time points of mice subjected to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task without receiving any experimental manipulation. After training, mice were intraperitoneally treated with vehicle (VEH, n=10) or modafinil at 1- (n=10), 2- (n=10), 3- (n=10) or 6-h interval (n=10). Results are presented as the mean±SE of percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm in the training (a) and testing (d), percent time spent in the open arms in the training (b) and testing (e) and number of entries in the training (c) and testing (f). ● p<0.05 compared with the other groups (analysis of variance and Tukey’s test). MOD, modafinil.

In the testing, ANOVA for percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm revealed significant effects of treatment [F(4,45)=3.83; p=0.009]. Tukey’s post-hoc demonstrated that mice treated with 32 mg/Kg MOD, 1 or 2 h after training, presented an enhancement on this parameter compared with the other groups (Fig. 2d).

When the percent time spent in the open arms and the number of entries of the testing were analysed, ANOVA did not reveal significant effect of treatment on both parameters (Figs 2e and f, respectively).

Discussion

Here, we verified that when repeatedly administered, 32 mg/kg (but not 64 or 128 mg/kg) modafinil induced amnesia in a discriminative avoidance task in mice. Such impairment was not followed by modification in anxiety or motor activity. In addition, we demonstrated that this dose of modafinil promoted consolidation deficits when administered 1 or 2 h after training (but not 3 or 6 h).

Several behavioural changes can influence the processes of acquisition, processing, storage and retrieval of various memory systems. In this scenario, the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task is a behavioural model able to evaluate the interactions among these mnemonic processes, anxiety and locomotor activity in rodents in an integrative and concomitant manner. In this paradigm, learning can be assessed by the magnitude of the discrimination of both enclosed arms (Reference Patti, Zanin and Sanday12). The storage of the task (and, consequently, the processes of consolidation and recall) is detected in the testing by the percent time in the aversive enclosed arm. In this context, the avoidance of the aversive enclosed arm upon testing has been validated as a measurement of retention, because amnestic manipulations decrease this effect (Reference Patti, Zanin and Sanday12Reference Carvalho, Patti and Takatsu-Coleman15). In contrast, memory-improving treatments increase this effect (Reference Silva, Bellot, Vital and Frussa-Filho16,Reference Claro, Silva and Frussa-Filho17). Furthermore, this behavioural model allows simultaneous and independent assessment of anxiety levels (through the avoidance of the open arms of the apparatus) and locomotor activity (through the number of entries in all arms of the apparatus). Thus, the model’s effectiveness in detecting the effects of factors known anxiolytics (Reference Patti, Zanin and Sanday12) and anxiogenic (Reference Gulick and Gould13) has been repeatedly demonstrated. In parallel, manipulations known to increase or decrease the locomotor activity were able to increase (Reference Patti, Zanin and Sanday12) or decrease (Reference Zanin, Patti and Sanday14,Reference Carvalho, Patti and Takatsu-Coleman15) the total number of entries into the arms of the apparatus, respectively.

Previous study of our group (Reference Fernandes, Zanin and Patti11) has demonstrated that post-training acute administration of 64 and 128 mg/kg modafinil promoted amnesia, since the animals treated with these doses presented an increased percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm. Of note, in the present study, when modafinil was repeatedly given (10 consecutive days) such amnestic effect is no longer observed. Conversely, although post-training acute of 32 mg/kg modafinil was ineffective in promoting amnesia, the repeated administration of this dose impaired retention. These results indicate that the consolidation deficits induced by modafinil could be tolerated or sensitised depending on the dose (64 and 32 mg/kg, respectively). From the best of our knowledge, only the study of Shuman et al. (Reference Shuman, Wood and Anagnostaras10) investigated the effects of modafinil on memory consolidation. In this way, these authors have reported that the acute modafinil administration was ineffective in modifying the consolidation of context- or cued-conditioned fear tasks in mice. These discrepant findings could lie on the memory tasks employed and the modafinil doses used in both studies (Reference Fernandes, Zanin and Patti11).

Concerning the effects of repeated administration of modafinil on memory, Burgos et al. (Reference Burgos, Castillo and Flores19) showed that the repeated administration of 64 mg/kg modafinil before daily conditioning in rats induced different effects on learning depending on the type of task or the type of memory involved. These authors demonstrated that modafinil did not modify working memory but decreased long-term memory on the Olton 4×4 maze, that is, the drug can enhance memory on hippocampus-dependent tasks when chronically administered. On the other hand, the same treatment decreased successful responses in a complex operant conditioning task, suggesting that the repeated administration of the drug impaired a prefrontal cortex-dependent task. In line with that, Béracochéa et al. (Reference Béracochéa, Celerier, Borde, Valleau, Peres and Piérard9) showed that chronic modafinil administration daily before conditioning (at 64 mg/kg but not at 32 mg/kg) enhanced performance in the spatial discrimination reversals in a T-maze. Together, these studies demonstrated that the repeated treatment with modafinil can negatively or positively modulate memory depending on the task and the dose employed. Notwithstanding, both studies evaluated only the pre-training administration of modafinil.

One possible explanation that could be raised is that the consolidation process has critical time points after training in which the memory trace can become labile again (Reference Bekinschtein, Cammarota and Igaz20Reference Bekinschtein, Katche and Slipczuk22) and can be modified. We designed experiment II to evaluate the effects of 32 mg/kg modafinil (which induced deficits in experiment I) on consolidation at different time points after the acquisition of the task. When administered 1 or 2 h after training, modafinil resulted in consolidation deficits, which were not detected when the administration occurred 3 or 6 h after training. Collectively, we have demonstrated that this specific modafinil dose did not induce any memory effects when administered immediately after training (Reference Fernandes, Zanin and Patti11) but did impair memory persistence when administered 1–2 h after it. Indeed, most manipulations of consolidation are effective immediately after learning, even if they also have later effects. Within this context, Bekinschtein et al. (Reference Bekinschtein, Cammarota and Igaz20) demonstrated that the protein-synthesis inhibitor, anisomycin, administered 12 h (but not 9 or 24 h) after training in the inhibitory-avoidance or contextual fear-conditioning tasks hindered memory 7 days after acquisition but left it intact at 2 days post-training, showing that there is a novel protein-synthesis-dependent phase in the rat hippocampus that is critical for the memory persistence. From the best of our knowledge this is the first study that demonstrated negative effects of modafinil on memory consolidation and that this deficits are critically influenced by the timing of the drug administration.

Concerning emotionality, we did not observe alterations on anxiety or locomotion. One could argue that modafinil should have increased motor activity in the testing, taking into account that animals received the last injection shortly before the exposure to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (PM-DAT). In fact, the acute administration of modafinil was able to promote an increase in the spontaneous activity of mice exposed to an open-field in a dose-dependent manner (Reference Wuo-Silva, Fukushiro and Borçoi18). A speculative explanation for these behavioural differences could be the interaction between the drug and the experimental environment. In other words, the aversive environment could be unfavourable for exploration as opposed to the open-field, a neutral environment. Thus, this environmental component could promote an inhibition of exploration, abolishing the hyperlocomotion effect of the drug.

It has been demonstrated that modafinil inhibits the dopamine transporter with exceptional selectivity (Reference Mignot, Nishino, Guilleminault and Dement23). In addition, recent evidence indicates that modafinil increases extracellular dopamine in the rat (Reference Zolkowska, Jain and Rothman24), monkey (Reference Andersen, Kessler, Murnane, McClung, Tufik and Howell25) and human brain (Reference Volkow, Fowler and Logan26). Still, this drug can interfere with some critical component required for long-term potentiation in the prefrontal cortex, thereby altering neuroplastic capabilities (Reference Burgos, Castillo and Flores19). As hippocampal dopamine modulates long-term memory encoding and consolidation (Reference TM27,Reference O’Carroll, Martin, Sandin, Frenguelli and Morris28), it could be hypothesised that after modafinil administration, the dopamine-dependent mechanisms in the hippocampus that favour memory encoding early after training might be attenuated, thereby leading to memory deficits in the present emotional discriminative avoidance task.

Collectively, our data suggest that modafinil has potential amnestic effects depending on dose and duration of the treatment. These results points to the need of caution in the prescription of this drug.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Ms. Teotila R. R. Amaral, Ms. Claudenice M. dos Santos, Mr. Cleomar S. Ferreira and Mr. Antonio Rodrigues dos Santos Ferreira for capable technical assistance. Dr. Roberto Frussa-Filho is no longer with us but he actively participated in the study concept, analyses of data and interpretation as well as in the manuscript draft. Authors’ contribution: All the authors provided substantial contributions to conception and design or acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting the article or revising it critically for important intellectual content and approved the final version to be published.

Financial Support

This work was founded by fellowships from Fundação de Amparo a Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq), Associação Fundo de Incentivo à Pesquisa (AFIP). L.B. and S.T. are recipients of CNPq fellowship.

Conflicts of Interest

All authors disclose any actual or potential conflict of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other people or organisations within 3 years of beginning the work that could inappropriately influence (bias) the present work.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional guides on the care and use of laboratory animals.

Footnotes

*

The first two authors contributed equally to this work.

In memoriam.

References

1.Ferraro, L, Antonelli, T, O’Connor, WT, Tanganelli, S, Rambert, FA, Fuxe, K. The effects of modafinil on striatal, pallidal and nigral GABA and glutamate release in the conscious rat: evidence for a preferential inhibition of striato-pallidal GABA transmission. Neurosci Lett 2008;253:1518.Google Scholar
2.Kraft, GH, Bowen, J. Modafinil for fatigue in MS: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind study. Neurology 2005;65:19951997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3.Turner, DC, Clark, L, Pomarol-Clotet, E, Mckenna, P, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ. Modafinil improves cognition and attentional set shifting in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29:13631373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Taylor, FB, Russo, J. Efficacy of modafinil compared to dextroamphetamine for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2000;10:311320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Joos, L, Docx, L, Schmaal, L, Sabbe, BG, Dom, G. Modafinil in psychiatric disorders: the promising state reconsidered. Tijdschr Psychiatr 2010;52:763773.Google ScholarPubMed
6.Cakic, V. Smart drugs for cognitive enhancement: ethical and pragmatic considerations in the era of cosmetic neurology. J Med Ethics 2009;35:611615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Béracochéa, D, Celerier, A, Peres, M, Piérard, C. Enhancement of learning processes following an acute modafinil injection in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;76:473479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Béracochéa, D, Cagnard, B, Celerier, A, Merrer, J, Peres, M, Piérard, C. First evidence of a delay-dependent working memory – enhancing effect of modafinil in mice. Neuroreport 2001;12:375378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Béracochéa, D, Celerier, A, Borde, N, Valleau, M, Peres, M, Piérard, C. Improvement of learning processes following chronic systemic administration of modafinil in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2002;73:723728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Shuman, T, Wood, SC, Anagnostaras, SG. Modafinil and memory: effects of modafinil on Morris water maze learning and Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 2009;123:257266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Fernandes, HA, Zanin, KA, Patti, CLet al. Inhibitory effects of modafinil on emotional memory in mice. Neuropharmacol 2013;64:365370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Patti, CL, Zanin, KA, Sanday, Let al. Effects of sleep deprivation on memory in mice: role of state-dependent learning. Sleep 2010;33:16691679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Gulick, D, Gould, TJ. Nicotine acts in the anterior cingulate, but not dorsal or ventral hippocampus, to reverse ethanol-induced learning impairments in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Addict Biol 2011;16:176188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Zanin, KA, Patti, CL, Sanday, Let al. Effects of zolpidem on sedation, anxiety, and memory in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013;226:459474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Carvalho, RC, Patti, CL, Takatsu-Coleman, ALet al. Effects of reserpine on the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task: dissociation between memory and motor impairments. Brain Res 2006;29:179183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Silva, RH, Bellot, RG, Vital, MA, Frussa-Filho, R. Effects of long-term ganglioside GM1 administration on a new discriminative avoidance test in normal adult mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;129:322328.Google ScholarPubMed
17.Claro, FT, Silva, RH, Frussa-Filho, R. Bovine brain phosphatidylserine attenuates scopolamine-induced amnesia. Physiol Behav 1999;67:551554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Wuo-Silva, R, Fukushiro, DF, Borçoi, ARet al. Addictive potential of modafinil and cross-sensitization with cocaine: a pre-clinical study. Addict Biol 2011;16:565579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Burgos, H, Castillo, A, Flores, Oet al. Effect of modafinil on learning performance and neocortical long-term potentiation in rats. Brain Res Bull 2010;83:238244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Bekinschtein, P, Cammarota, M, Igaz, LMet al. Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis- and BDNF-dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron 2007;53:261277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Bekinschtein, P, Cammarota, M, Katche, Cet al. BDNF is essential to promote persistence of long-term memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008;105:27112716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Bekinschtein, P, Katche, C, Slipczuk, Let al. Persistence of long-term memory storage: new insights into its molecular signatures in the hippocampus and related structures. Neurotox Res 2010;18:377385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Mignot, E, Nishino, S, Guilleminault, C, Dement, WC. Modafinil binds to the dopamine uptake carrier site with low affinity. Sleep 1994;17:436437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24.Zolkowska, D, Jain, R, Rothman, RBet al. Evidence for the involvement of dopamine transporters in behavioral stimulant effects of modafinil. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2009;329:738746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Andersen, ML, Kessler, E, Murnane, KS, McClung, JC, Tufik, S, Howell, LL. Dopamine transporter-related effects of modafinil in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;210:439448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Volkow, ND, Fowler, JS, Logan, Jet al. Effects of modafinil on dopamine and dopamine transporters in the male human brain: clinical implications. JAMA 2009;301:11481154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.TM, Jay. Dopamine: a potential substrate for synaptic plasticity and memory mechanisms. Prog Neurobiol 2003;69:375390.Google Scholar
28.O’Carroll, CM, Martin, SJ, Sandin, J, Frenguelli, B, Morris, RG. Dopaminergic modulation of the persistence of one-trial hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn Mem 2006;13:760769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Effects of repeated post-training modafinil administration on memory of mice subjected to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task without receiving any experimental manipulation. Immediately after training, mice were intraperitoneally treated with vehicle (VEH, n=12) or modafinil at the doses of 32 (MOD32, n=12), 64 (MOD64, n=12) or 128 mg/kg (MOD128) for 10 consecutive days. Results are presented as the mean±SE of percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm in the training (a) and testing (d), percent time spent in the open arms in the training (b) and testing (e) and number of entries in the training (c) and testing (f). ● p<0.05 compared with the other groups (analysis of variance and Tukey’s test).

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Effects of 32 mg/kg modafinil administration on memory consolidation at different time points of mice subjected to the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task. Mice were trained in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task without receiving any experimental manipulation. After training, mice were intraperitoneally treated with vehicle (VEH, n=10) or modafinil at 1- (n=10), 2- (n=10), 3- (n=10) or 6-h interval (n=10). Results are presented as the mean±SE of percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm in the training (a) and testing (d), percent time spent in the open arms in the training (b) and testing (e) and number of entries in the training (c) and testing (f). ● p<0.05 compared with the other groups (analysis of variance and Tukey’s test). MOD, modafinil.