Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T12:36:40.481Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal neglect and the serotonin system are associated with daytime sleep in infant rhesus monkeys

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2019

Alexander Baxter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
Elizabeth K. Wood
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
Christina S. Barr
Affiliation:
Section of Comparative Behavioral Genomics, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, National Institutes of Health Rockville, MD, USA
Daniel B. Kay
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
Stephen J. Suomi
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Poolesville, MD, USA
J. Dee Higley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
*
Address for Correspondence: J. Dee Higley, Department of Psychology, 1042 KMBL, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT84602. E-mail: james_higley@byu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Environmental and biological factors contribute to sleep development during infancy. Parenting plays a particularly important role in modulating infant sleep, potentially via the serotonin system, which is itself involved in regulating infant sleep. We hypothesized that maternal neglect and serotonin system dysregulation would be associated with daytime sleep in infant rhesus monkeys. Subjects were nursery-reared infant rhesus macaques (n = 287). During the first month of life, daytime sleep-wake states were rated bihourly (0800–2100). Infants were considered neglected (n = 16) if before nursery-rearing, their mother repeatedly failed to retrieve them. Serotonin transporter genotype and concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) were used as markers of central serotonin system functioning. t tests showed that neglected infants were observed sleeping less frequently, weighed less, and had higher 5-HIAA than non-neglected nursery-reared infants. Regression revealed that serotonin transporter genotype moderated the relationship between 5-HIAA and daytime sleep: in subjects possessing the Ls genotype, there was a positive correlation between 5-HIAA and daytime sleep, whereas in subjects possessing the LL genotype there was no association. These results highlight the pivotal roles that parents and the serotonin system play in sleep development. Daytime sleep alterations observed in neglected infants may partially derive from serotonin system dysregulation.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Sleep is an important part of early-life development, and is crucial for normative neurological development and functioning (Ednick, Cohen, McPhail, Beebe, & Simakajornboon, Reference Ednick, Cohen, McPhail, Beebe and Simakajornboon2009; Graven & Browne, Reference Graven and Browne2008), as well as psychiatric health. At birth, infants typically spend 15–16 hrs per day sleeping, although there are substantial individual differences centered on this mean. As they develop, their nighttime sleep consolidates, and they gradually sleep less during daylight hours (Parmelee, Wenner, & Schulz, Reference Parmelee, Wenner and Schulz1964). Although many changes in sleep occur in the first 6 months of life, individual differences in daytime sleep duration remain stable across this period (Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto, & Field, Reference Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto and Field2016) and are thought to be windows to the developing brain (Kohyama, Reference Kohyama1998). Studies suggest that neonatal sleep patterns are endogenously mediated and consequently relatively immune to disruptions from family routines (McGraw, Hoffmann, Harker, & Herman, Reference McGraw, Hoffmann, Harker and Herman1999). Its daily rhythm shows a 4-hr ultraradian sleep-wake, light-initiated sequence that extends to both nighttime sleep and daytime naps (Scher & Loparo, Reference Scher and Loparo2009), likely an evolutionary adaptation to feeding. Studies show that during the first month, infants sleep as much during the day as they do at night (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto and Field2016), possibly reflecting the endogenous nature of neonatal regulation. Daytime sleep (napping) appears to be an extension of overall sleep, because studies have shown that during the first month of life, the amount of time infants spend sleeping during the day and during the night is positively correlated (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto and Field2016; Iwata et al., Reference Iwata, Fujita, Kinoshita, Unno, Horinouchi, Morokuma and Iwata2017), as is the amount of time it takes for them to fall asleep during the day and at night (Iwata et al., Reference Iwata, Fujita, Kinoshita, Unno, Horinouchi, Morokuma and Iwata2017). Daytime sleep is a useful marker of infant development because it is easily measured and readily observable; for full-term infants, it shows a similar architecture to night sleep (Biagioni et al., Reference Biagioni, Boldrini, Giganti, Guzzetta, Salzarulo and Cioni2005). Studies suggest that during infancy, it is equally as important for brain development as is nighttime sleep (Fagioli & Salzarulo, Reference Fagioli and Salzarulo1982; Spruyt et al., Reference Spruyt, Aitken, So, Charlton, Adamson and Horne2008), largely because neonatal and early infant sleep patterns are not fully consolidated into adult-like diurnal patterns (Ednick et al., Reference Ednick, Cohen, McPhail, Beebe and Simakajornboon2009). Hence, aberrations in neonatal daytime sleep patterns likely reflect general sleep disturbances to the same degree as aberrations in nighttime sleep patterns during early infancy.

Although these studies suggest that sleep is largely immune to typical environmental influences during the neonatal period, sleep problems later in infancy and childhood are risk factors for future psychopathology, including anxiety and mood disorders (Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Caspi, Eley, Moffitt, O'connor and Poulton2005; Lam, Hiscock, & Wake, Reference Lam, Hiscock and Wake2003; Ong, Wickramaratne, Tang, & Weissman, Reference Ong, Wickramaratne, Tang and Weissman2006). Aberrant sleep patterns in infancy often correlate with other physical and mental impairments (Ednick et al., Reference Ednick, Cohen, McPhail, Beebe and Simakajornboon2009); hence, research devoted to understanding neonatal daytime sleep development may have therapeutic applications. To this end, translational research investigating daytime sleep in neonatal rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), a primate species with similar sleep-wake patterns as humans (Bowman, Wolf, & Sackett, Reference Bowman, Wolf and Sackett1970; Reite, Rhodes, Kavan, & Adey, Reference Reite, Rhodes, Kavan and Adey1965) and parallels in their underlying central nervous system (CNS) and developmental sequences (Gibbs et al., Reference Gibbs, Rogers, Katze, Bumgarner, Weinstock, Mardis and Wilson2007), has shed light on the environmental and biological factors associated with daytime sleep in infants.

Although neonates are relatively immune to parental sleep-entrainment with consolidation occurring later in infancy, caregiving during the neonatal period is especially important because neonates show inefficient thermoregulation (Knobel, Reference Knobel2014; Waldron & MacKinnon, Reference Waldron and MacKinnon2007); without access to their parents as a source of warmth, infants may suffer cold stress, eliciting cortisol release, and a cascade of other stress markers (Scaramuzzo et al., Reference Scaramuzzo, Giampietri, Fiorentini, Bartalena, Fiori, Guzzetta and Ghirri2015), which may be life-threatening when severe (McCall, Alderdice, Halliday, Jenkins, & Vohra, Reference McCall, Alderdice, Halliday, Jenkins and Vohra2006). In hospital settings, studies show that human neonates who are not kept warm exhibit increased levels of epinephrine and norepinephrine, as well as dysregulated metabolism (Knobel, Reference Knobel2014). Hunger and dehydration are also major stressors for neonates, leading to changes in energy expenditure and cortisol regulation (see Welberg & Seckl, Reference Welberg and Seckl2001). Although these stressors lead to aberrant homeostatic states, once normal care returns, the aberrant states are thought to dissipate. Nevertheless, studies have not longitudinally followed neonates that experienced hypothermia and hunger during the neonatal period; thus, little is known about the long-term effects of neonatal hypothermia on the development of the brain and its long-term effect on sleep development.

Sleep and serotonin: A complex relationship

Serotonin has long been held as a CNS modulator of sleep, and dysregulation of central serotonin functioning can have profound effects on sleep patterns (Jouvet, Reference Jouvet1999). By interacting with other neurotransmitters, serotonin has indirect effects on circadian rhythmicity, sleep patterns, and sleep development (Monti & Jantos, Reference Monti and Jantos2008). More specifically, however, serotonin can act directly on the reticular activating system to modulate wakefulness and sleep (Jones, Reference Jones2003). CNS serotonin functioning is commonly measured by assaying cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) for concentrations of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), a proxy measure of CNS serotonin turnover. This measure of serotonin function is especially useful because in humans and in monkeys, it can be directly measured from the cistern at the base of the brain (Higley et al., Reference Higley, Bennett, Helis, Lesch, Shoaf, White and Linnoila1998; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Marchuk, Gadde, Barefoot, Grichnik, Helms and Siegler2003). In a laboratory-based investigation of juvenile rhesus monkeys, Zajicek, Higley, Suomi, and Linnoila (Reference Zajicek, Higley, Suomi and Linnoila1997) found that low concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA were related to increased sleep disturbances (i.e., falling asleep later at night). In another study investigating free-ranging adult male monkeys, low CSF 5-HIAA was associated with less nighttime sleep and more daytime naps (Mehlman et al., Reference Mehlman, Westergaard, Hoos, Sallee, Marsh, Suomi and Higley2000). Although serotonin serves as a prenatal neurodevelopmental guide (Kepser & Homberg, Reference Kepser and Homberg2015) and is one of the first monoaminergic systems to develop in the CNS (Daws & Gould, Reference Daws and Gould2011), relatively little is known concerning the role of serotonin in the development of infant sleep. To our knowledge, no studies have assessed the relationship between serotonin and neonatal sleep, although studies of human neonates suggest serotonin can affect sleep development: as early as 2 weeks after birth, neonates prenatally exposed to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors that their mothers used while pregnant show sleep aberrations (see Olivier et al., Reference Olivier, Åkerud, Kaihola, Pawluski, Skalkidou, Högberg and Sundström-Poromaa2013), spending disproportionately more sleep time in the rapid eye movement stage and waking up more often when compared with healthy controls (Zeskind & Stephens, Reference Zeskind and Stephens2004). Developmentally, CSF 5-HIAA shows a large decline from the neonatal period to middle infancy, although individual differences remain highly stable over this early period (Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005), the time when infant sleep patterns begin to consolidate and circadian sleep patterns emerge (Bathory & Tomopoulos, Reference Bathory and Tomopoulos2017). To the degree that sleep is modulated by CNS serotonin during these formative months, this stability may be reflected in the stability of sleeping patterns; to our knowledge, however, this has not been investigated. This study will explore the nature of this association in infant monkeys.

As discussed previously, the conditions of the sleep environment and early experiences play a role in sleep development. Genetics may also play an important role in the development of sleep by shaping underlying CNS mechanisms, such as the serotonin system. The serotonin transporter (5HTT) gene is believed to be the sole gene encoding serotonin reuptake transporters (Daws & Gould, Reference Daws and Gould2011; Lesch et al., Reference Lesch, Balling, Gross, Strauss, Wolozin, Murphy and Riederer1994). Consequently, variations in this gene can affect CNS serotonin transport and functioning (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Marchuk, Gadde, Barefoot, Grichnik, Helms and Siegler2003). The 5HTT gene has two common allelic variants: the ancestral long (L) allele, and the low-expressing short (s) allele, a 44-bp insertion or deletion in the gene's promoter region (Heils et al., Reference Heils, Teufel, Petri, Stober, Riederer, Bengel and Lesch1996). In early investigations of 5HTT and sleep, adults possessing at least one copy of the s allele were more likely to suffer from insomnia (Deuschle et al., Reference Deuschle, Schredl, Schilling, Wüst, Frank, Witt and Schulze2010). As observed in many other studies of the 5HTT gene (see Caspi, Hariri, Holmes, Uher, & Moffitt, Reference Caspi, Hariri, Holmes, Uher and Moffitt2010; Suomi, Reference Suomi2006), its effects on sleep appear to be modulated by gene-by-environment interactions, with the s allele modulating sleep, but only in subjects experiencing chronic stress. In one study, researchers showed that chronically stressed caregivers carrying the s allele experienced poorer sleep quality than individuals in the same stressful condition that were homozygous for the L allele (Brummett et al., Reference Brummett, Krystal, Ashley-Koch, Kuhn, Züchner, Siegler and Williams2007). Those that were not under the stress of being a caregiver were undifferentiated by genotype. Not all studies have found this relationship, however (e.g., see Barclay et al., Reference Barclay, Eley, Mill, Wong, Zavos, Archer and Gregory2011). Other studies suggest that possession of the s allele is linked with sleeping longer at night and going to bed earlier (Koga, Fukushima, Sakuma, & Kagawa, Reference Koga, Fukushima, Sakuma and Kagawa2016). To our knowledge, no studies have investigated the relationship between neonatal and infant sleep and the 5HTT gene. Suggestive of the role of 5HTT genotype in infant sleep development are studies linking variation in 5HTT genotype with sudden infant death syndrome; the L allele is found more frequently in infants that die in their sleep from this disorder (Filonzi et al., Reference Filonzi, Magnani, Nosetti, Nespoli, Borghi, Vaghi and Marzano2012; Lavezzi, Casale, Oneda, Weese-Mayer, & Matturri, Reference Lavezzi, Casale, Oneda, Weese-Mayer and Matturri2009). Research investigating the association between 5HTT genotype and sleep may inform future research aimed at developing personalized medicine to prevent and treat developmental disorders.

Hypotheses

A central purpose of this study was to investigate the association between serotonin functioning and sleep development. We were also interested in the association between early life experiences and sleep development, in particular the consequences of neonatal maternal neglect. Rhesus monkeys are commonly used to investigate the role that mothers play in infant development because maternal parenting behaviors are similar to humans (Hinde & Spencer-Booth, Reference Hinde and Spencer-Booth1967; McCormack et al., Reference McCormack, Howell, Guzman, Villongco, Pears, Kim and Sanchez2015). For example, rhesus mothers can profoundly affect their infant's developing serotonin system (Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005), and they play a primary role in regulating their infant's arousal and internal temperature (Harlow, Reference Harlow1958; Reite et al., Reference Reite, Short, Kaufman, Stynes and Pauley1978). Rhesus monkeys generally give birth in the early spring (March and April), when nights are longer and cooler than during summer months. At the National Institute of Child Health and Development nonhuman primate facility where data were collected, the average night-time temperature in March and April is between 0.5° and 6.7°C. At birth, rhesus monkey neonates are cradled and held tightly as they nurse, preventing hypothermia. A mother's failure to exhibit normative maternal care can be life-threatening to the infant. Consequently, those few infants that do not receive proper maternal care are typically placed in an incubator and raised in the neonatal nursery.

The conditions in the National Institute of Child Health and Development neonatal nursery allow a homogeneous environment for the subjects, and variables such as maternal neglect can be opportunistically studied by comparing neglected infants with those randomly removed from their mother for experimental purposes (hereafter referred to as non-neglected nursery-reared infants). The mother- and nursery-reared subjects were on the same 24-hr day-night cycle, with exposure to the same ambient sunlight and human activity schedule. Neonates were handled routinely for feeding, changing of soiled blankets, and weighing. Except for the time spent with their neglecting mothers, the experiences of the neglected infants were identical to those of the non-neglected nursery-reared infants once they were placed in the neonatal nursery. Hence, any differences in daytime sleep between these two groups of nursery-reared infants are likely due to maternal neglect.

In the nursery, sleep-wake states were collected every 2 hr during the day across infants’ first month of life (approximately equivalent to the first 3–4 months of life for human infants in terms of development and maturation). Although nighttime sleep measures were not collected, Meier and Berger (Reference Meier and Berger1965) investigated sleep in five neonatal nursery-reared monkeys during their first month of life and found little evidence of circadian rhythmicity in sleep-wake patterns, a finding that parallels research in human infants cited previously (McGraw et al., Reference McGraw, Hoffmann, Harker and Herman1999). To the extent that daytime sleep reflects overall sleep in infant rhesus monkeys, we hypothesized that neonatal rhesus monkeys experiencing early maternal neglect would exhibit differences in the frequency of observed daytime sleep compared to non-neglected nursery-reared infants.

Although nursery-rearing has been shown to alter older monkeys’ sleep patterns (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Noble, Hanson, Pine, Winslow and Nelson2009; Kaemingk & Reite, Reference Kaemingk and Reite1987), little is known concerning neonatal sleep in nonhuman primates. In the nursery, all neonates are given the same treatment, and, although they are reared without their mother, many components of maternal care can be artificially supplemented with heating pads to keep them warm, consistent access to milk, and a soft surrogate mother that rocks or moves for cuddling and comfort (Lubach & Coe, Reference Lubach, Coe, Sackett, Ruppenthal and Elias2006). Hence, the controlled environment in the neonatal nursery was ideal for assessing the effects of maternal neglect and the developing serotonin system on daytime sleep without the variability of maternal care and social group influences that occurs when monkeys are raised by their mothers. CSF can also be removed repeatedly at different developmental time points with little to no adverse medical effects (Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005). Using this technique, a study from our laboratory showed that the serotonin system of infants reared in the neonatal nursery is modulated by the 5HTT genotype, as measured by concentrations of 5-HIAA in cisternal CSF (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Lesch, Heils, Long, Lorenz, Shoaf and Higley2002). Because the precise mechanism by which 5HTT genotype affects sleep as well as the direction of the effect are still unclear, another purpose of this study was to explore the complex relationship between 5HTT genotype, CNS serotonin functioning, and sleep. Rhesus monkeys possess a human 5HTT ortholog (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Kaplan, Garcia Iv, Shelledy, Nair and Cameron2006), have similar sleep architecture to humans (Deley, Turner, Freeman, Bliwise, & Rye, Reference Deley, Turner, Freeman, Bliwise and Rye2006; Reite et al., Reference Reite, Rhodes, Kavan and Adey1965), and, unlike in human infants, it is possible to safely collect CSF samples during infancy. We hypothesized that for infant rhesus monkeys, dysregulation of the serotonin system would be associated with dysregulated sleep. Specifically, we hypothesized that an interaction between the 5HTT genotype and CSF 5-HIAA would predict daytime sleep occurrence.

Methods and Materials

Subjects and rearing procedures

Subjects were 287 infant rhesus monkeys in a nursery-rearing program at the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Data were collected between 1994 and 2006 for each birth-year cohort. Because of technical problems and project availability, some subjects were missing data, leaving n = 116 subjects with a complete battery of data (see Appendix A in the online supplementary material for a summary of subjects with available data).

Details of housing conditions and nursery protocols are explained in detail elsewhere (see Shannon, Champoux, & Suomi, Reference Shannon, Champoux and Suomi1998). Briefly, randomly selected infants were separated from their mothers between 1 and 3 days postpartum and relocated to the neonatal nursery where they were individually housed with a heated “surrogate mother” that had a bottle attached for feeding (see Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005, for a detailed description of the assignment procedure). Subjects were housed in a room maintained at approximately 27°C, where they could see and hear other infants but could not physically contact them. Infants were hand-fed a 50–50 mixture of Similac (Ross Laboratories, Columbus, OH) and Primilac (Bio-Serv, Frenchtown, NJ), enriched with docosahexaenoic acid formula ad libitum until they could feed themselves from the bottle fastened to the surrogate. When subjects were about 2 weeks of age (between 11 and 17 days old), infants were removed from their neonatal cage and administered a developmental assessment, following which blood and cisternal CSF samples were obtained.

Following testing, infants were relocated to a different room and housed individually in larger nursery cages. Subjects retained their surrogate mother and blanket, but the electric heating pad was removed. The room was maintained at 22–26°C with the lights on from 0700 to 2100. Because the room was connected to a central room with windows open to the outside environment, subjects were also exposed to outside light and noise. When subjects were approximately 4 weeks of age (between 26 and 31 days old), they were administered another developmental assessment, following which additional blood and cisternal CSF samples were obtained. All procedures conformed to standards set forth in the National Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Neglected infants

In some cases, an infant originally assigned to remain with its mother was reassigned to the nursery because of maternal neglect (n = 16). Decisions to reassign infant rearing conditions were made by nursery and veterinary staff, who observed the animals daily to assure that the mother-reared infants were on their mothers’ ventrum, suckling, and otherwise healthy. If the staff discovered an infant left alone on the floor, they approached the cage to determine if the mother would actively retrieve the infant. If the mother consistently failed to retrieve the infant, the infant was considered neglected. Before neglected infants were placed in the nursery, the mother and neglected infant were temporarily separated from the larger social group and placed together in a small cage where they could be more closely observed to determine lactation and patterns of maternal behavior. If there were no other lactating females to which the neglected infant could be fostered, it was removed from its mother and reassigned to the neonatal nursery. Infants that had been relocated to the neonatal nursery because they or their mothers were sick (n = 6) were not considered neglected and were not used in analyses. None of the neglected infants included in analyses showed subsequent severe pathology over the next year.

Before being placed in the neonatal nursery, neglected infants were housed with their mothers in connected indoor-outdoor enclosures with 8–10 adults and 2–5 other infants (see description of mother-reared monkeys in Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Champoux and Suomi1998). Similar to the nursery, the indoor pens were maintained on a 12-hr light-dark schedule, with lights on from 0700 to 1900. In both the neonatal nursery and the indoor pens, external windows or wire mesh allowed ambient light from sunrise to sunset. Unless temperatures were below freezing, mother-infant pairs were allowed to sleep outside as they pleased. Mothers were provided with monkey chow ad libitum. About one-half of the neglected infants were identified within the first 48 hr after birth and remained with their mothers for the same amount of time as non-neglected infants randomly assigned to the nursery. In other cases, some neglected infants remained with their mother for a longer period. In these cases, mothers retrieved their infant when observed by animal care staff, but later would drop the infant when the staff left. These infants were typically discovered as being neglected because they either appeared ill or dehydrated, lethargic, and unresponsive. Of the 16 neglected infants, 7 were determined to be neglected and relocated on the first or second day of life and 9 were determined to be neglected and relocated after the first week of life (7–19 days old). Preliminary analyses showed no differences between neglected infants placed in the nursery on the first or second day of life when compared with neglected infants placed in the nursery after the first week of life in 5HTT genotype or sex, nor in sleep, weight, or CSF 5-HIAA at either the week 2 assessment, the week 4 assessment, or when the average of week 2 and week 4 was used (p > .42). For this reason, all neglected infants were combined into a single group and compared with the non-neglected nursery-reared infants (n = 271).

Daytime sleep measurements

While infants were in the nursery, trained observers recorded daily sleep states 7 times a day, every 2 hr, between 0800 and 2000 hr. Scores were initially recorded on a 6-point Likert-type scale: 0 = quiet sleep, 1 = rapid eye movement sleep, 2 = drowsy (transitional state between sleep and wake), 3 = quiet wake, 4 = active wake, 5 = distressed awake. To improve face validity of measuring sleep-wake states based on observation, this scale was recoded by combining quiet sleep with rapid eye movement sleep, and combining quiet awake with active awake and distressed awake. These values were then reverse-scored, resulting in a 3-point scale: 1 = awake, 2 = drowsy, 3 = sleep. To determine average sleep during the first month of life unbiased by neonatal assessment and anesthesia for CSF sampling, each subject's sleep scores for the 7 days before their week 2 and week 4 neonatal assessments were aggregated. These time points were selected to create corresponding measures of daytime sleep that were just before and temporally associated with CSF sampling. Sleep data for days on which infants were tested as part of other projects were excluded from this average. The average sleep score showed good overall reliability across the 14 days (Cronbach reliability α = .71).

CSF 5-HIAA metabolite assay

CSF 5-HIAA assays were completed for a subset of 176 subjects (see Appendix A). Procedures for CSF extraction and assays are explained in detail elsewhere (see Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005). Briefly, subjects were anesthetized using 10 mg/kg of ketamine hydrochloride, delivered intramuscularly. Within 15 min of administering the anesthesia, a 22-gauge needle and 5-mL syringe were inserted into the cisterna magna, and 1 mL of CSF was obtained. CSF samples were assayed for 5-HIAA with high-performance liquid chromatography and electrochemical detection (see Scheinin, Chang, Kirk, & Linnoila, Reference Scheinin, Chang, Kirk and Linnoila1983; Seppala, Scheinin, Capone, & Linnoila, Reference Seppala, Scheinin, Capone and Linnoila1984). Inter- and intra-assay variability was <10%.

5HTT Genotyping

Genotyping for the 5HTT gene was completed for a subset of 220 subjects (see Appendix A). The procedures for 5HTT genotyping are explained in detail elsewhere (see Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Lesch, Heils, Long, Lorenz, Shoaf and Higley2002). DNA was extracted from blood samples obtained during testing at weeks 2 and 4 of life, and segments were amplified using standard polymerase chain reaction methods. Allelic sizes and identities for the L allele (419 bp) and s allele (398 bp) were confirmed using electrophoresis visualized by ethidium bromide staining. Three subjects were excluded from analyses of 5HTT genotype because of their genotype's infrequent occurrence: two subjects were homozygous for the s allele and one subject had a copy of the L allele and a copy of an extra-long allele. For the remaining 217 subjects with available 5HTT genotype data, there were 174 that were homozygous for the L allele and 43 that were heterozygous with the Ls genotype. Subjects from the neglected group were not different in 5HTT genotype frequency compared with non-neglected nursery-reared subjects (see Appendix A). Within the 116 subjects that had no missing data, the genotyping found 91 subjects were homozygous for the L allele and 25 subjects were heterozygous.

Weight, cohort, and sex variables

Subjects were weighed each morning as a part of veterinary rounds. Because weight is a proxy measure for gestational age and corresponding brain development (Vohr et al., Reference Vohr, Wright, Dusick, Mele, Verter, Steichen and Bauer2000), and because human infants born preterm exhibit sleep impairments (Huang, Paiva, Hsu, Kuo, & Guilleminault, Reference Huang, Paiva, Hsu, Kuo and Guilleminault2014), weight was included as a covariate in all analyses to control for age since conception. Because preliminary t tests showed that neglected infants’ weight did not differ from non-neglected infants’ weight during the first week of life (p > .14), average weight was used in analyses, which was computed using the day before subjects’ week 2 assessment and subjects’ week 4 assessment.

Sleep and weight data were recorded for 13 different birth-year cohorts. CSF 5-HIAA and 5HTT genotype data were not available for the last two cohort years, and were collected for only 11 birth-year cohorts. As is commonly done in studies spanning multiple years, cohort was considered in analyses (Zajicek et al., Reference Zajicek, Higley, Suomi and Linnoila1997). Preliminary one-way analyses of variance revealed cohort-differences in sleep, F (12, 256) = 6.36, p < .001; and CSF 5-HIAA, F (10, 166) = 2.85, p = .003. Dummy codes for cohort were included in subsequent analyses, with the cohort of infants born in 1994 (the first year that data were collected) set as the reference group.

Although there was not a significant sex difference in daytime sleep (p = .15), because some studies indicate a sex difference in sleep in human infants (Moss, Reference Moss1967) and nonhuman primates (Champoux & Suomi, Reference Champoux and Suomi1988; Sackett, Fahrenbruch, & Ruppenthal, Reference Sackett, Fahrenbruch, Ruppenthal, Ruppenthal and Reese1979), sex was included as a covariate in analyses. A t test revealed sex differences in average weight, such that males weighed more than females, M = 601 g and 579 g, respectively, t (260) = -2.44, p = .02. Although preliminary analyses did not reveal sex differences in CSF 5-HIAA (p = .50), sex was also considered because other investigations commonly report sex differences in CSF 5-HIAA (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Marchuk, Gadde, Barefoot, Grichnik, Helms and Siegler2003; Young, Gauthier, Anderson, & Purdy, Reference Young, Gauthier, Anderson and Purdy1980).

Analyses

All analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM, V.23). Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests of normality revealed that all variables were normally distributed, excluding subjects if their data included outliers >3 standard deviations from the group mean. To investigate the effect of maternal neglect, independent t tests were performed, with neglect status as the independent variable and separate tests performed for the dependent variables of sleep, CSF 5-HIAA, and weight. For each comparison, Cohen's d was calculated as a measure of the effect size. Because there were few subjects considered neglected, and not all subjects had available data for all three dependent variables (see Appendix A), analyses were performed in a pair-wise fashion to maximize available data for these analyses. To verify that the amount of time that neglected infants spent with their mothers was not a confound, all analyses were repeated with number of days with mother as a covariate; however, it was not significant in any analysis, and the results are not reported.

To investigate the combined effects of maternal neglect, CSF 5-HIAA, and 5HTT genotype on daytime sleep, multiple regression was performed using the PROCESS add-in for SPSS (Hayes, Reference Hayes2013). Fifteen variables were used in base regression models, including CSF 5-HIAA, 5HTT genotype (coded as 0 = LL, 1 = Ls), the interaction between CSF 5-HIAA and 5HTT genotype (calculated by multiplying subjects’ mean-centered CSF 5-HIAA by their mean-centered 5HTT code), neglect status (0 = non-neglected nursery-reared, 1 = neglected nursery-reared), weight, sex, and nine dummy codes for cohort. To investigate how interactional effects within the serotonin system affect daytime sleep, Model 1 of the PROCESS add-in was used to test for a moderation effect of 5HTT genotype on CSF 5-HIAA, with all other variables included as covariates. A posteriori simple slopes analyses were performed separately for subjects with the LL genotype and subjects with the Ls genotype.

Results

Effect of maternal neglect on daytime sleep, the serotonin system, and weight

Independent samples t tests showed that, when compared with the non-neglected nursery-reared infants, neglected infants slept less during the day, t (265) = 2.19, p = .030, d = 0.65 (Figure 1), had higher concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA, t (174) = -2.45, p = .015, d = 0.76 (Figure 2), and, on average, weighed less, t (260) = 2.87, p = .004, d = 0.88 (Figure 3).

Figure 1. Effect of maternal neglect on daytime sleep. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 255), neglected infants (black bar, n = 12) were less frequently observed sleeping during the day. Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Figure 2. Effect of maternal neglect on cerebral serotonin turnover. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 165), neglected infants (black bar, n = 11) exhibited higher cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (CSF 5-HIAA). Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Figure 3. Effect of maternal neglect on maturational weight. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 251), neglected infants (black bar, n = 11) on average weighed less during the first month of life. Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Interaction between 5HTT genotype and levels of CSF 5-HIAA predicts sleep

Regression analyses with daytime sleep as the dependent variable showed that the interaction between 5HTT genotype and CSF 5-HIAA significantly predicted daytime sleep (p = .002; Table 1). Simple slopes analyses revealed that there was a positive association between daytime sleep and CSF 5-HIAA, but only among infants that were heterozygous with the Ls genotype (p = .019, β = .56), an association not present among infants that were homozygous for the L allele (p = .63; Figure 4). Analyses also showed that daytime sleep varied negatively with weight (p < .001, β = -.44), and occurred less frequently in neglected nursery-reared infants compared with non-neglected nursery-reared infants (p = .001, β = -.27). Three of the nine cohort dummy codes were significant in the model (p < .04; β 1996 = .25; β 2002 = .33; β 2003 = -.18). Although sex did not achieve significance (p = .072, β = .14), it was retained in the model because it approached significance and because of its association with weight and CSF 5-HIAA.

Figure 4. Concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (CSF 5-HIAA) predict daytime sleep in infants heterozygous with the Ls genotype. The x-axis represents concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA, and the y-axis represents average daytime sleep. The white dots represent subjects that were homozygous for the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT) L allele (n = 91), and the black triangles represent subjects that were heterozygous for the 5HTT Ls genotype (n = 25). The dotted line is the line of best fit for subjects that were homozygous for the L allele, and the solid black line is the line of best fit for subjects that were heterozygous for the Ls genotype. There was a positive correlation between concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep among infants that were heterozygous with the Ls genotype. There was no association between concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep among infants that were homozygous for the L allele.

Table 1. Multiple regression model showing dysregulation in the serotonin system is associated with daytime sleep

Note: Bold indicates variables that were significant in the model at p < .05. Nine dummy codes for cohort were also included in the model, but are not reported. CSF 5-HIAA, cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid; 5HTT, serotonin transporter; Ls, heterozygous genotype with one long allele and one short allele.

aCSF 5-HIAA and 5HTT genotype were mean centered.

Discussion

Maternal neglect, neonatal stress, and sleep patterns

We found that early maternal neglect was associated with decreased daytime sleep and serotonin system functioning in infant rhesus monkeys. These findings add to a growing literature suggesting that early experiences have profound consequences for neurological and behavioral development (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Lesch, Heils, Long, Lorenz, Shoaf and Higley2002; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Ames, Chisholm and Savoie1997; Spinelli et al., Reference Spinelli, Chefer, Suomi, Higley, Barr and Stein2009) and that these early experiences interact with genotype to produce differences in developmental outcome. As hypothesized, neglected infants exhibited dysregulated daytime sleep: whereas infants typically slept frequently during the daytime, neglected infants were observed sleeping less often during the day than the non-neglected nursery-reared infants. Although it is widely held that serotonin plays a primary function in sleep (Jouvet, Reference Jouvet1999), to our knowledge, this is the first study to show that aberrations in the CNS serotonin system are correlated with variation in infant sleep development. Our study also showed that infants possessing a copy of the s allele exhibited a stronger coupling of CNS serotonin functioning with sleep when compared with infants with the LL genotype, as measured by the positive correlation between CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and daytime sleep. This finding is of interest because some theoretical models suggest that the s allele of the 5HTT gene may be more sensitive to experiences and settings that are outside the range of the species-typical environment (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009).

These findings suggest that the early experiences neglected neonates had before they were placed in the nursery contributed to dysregulated sleep. Infants are born with a specific set of needs that must be met for normative development to occur, something often referred to as experience-expectant brain development. One possible explanation for our findings is that the neglected neonates did not receive the adequate “experience-expectant” mothering necessary for proper brain development, resulting in decreased daytime sleep during their first month of life, potentially because of hypothermia resulting from poor maternal care immediately following birth. In humans, stressful events, such as circumcision, acutely increase sleep duration and depth in neonates, a process attributed to adaptations allowing recovery from stress (Emde, Harmon, Metcalf, Koenig, & Wagonfeld, Reference Emde, Harmon, Metcalf, Koenig and Wagonfeld1971; Gunnar, Malone, Vance, & Fisch, Reference Gunnar, Malone, Vance and Fisch1985). Although all infants in this study eventually received identical nursery-rearing, the neglected infants experienced the additional stress of a neglecting mother in the critical first few days of life. Mothers are the primary source of heat for neonatal rhesus monkeys (Harlow, Reference Harlow1958), and without a mother to hold an infant close, hypothermia can result, which is a major stressor for a newborn and can lead to elevated cortisol (Scaramuzzo et al., Reference Scaramuzzo, Giampietri, Fiorentini, Bartalena, Fiori, Guzzetta and Ghirri2015) and dysregulate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (Knobel, Reference Knobel2014). The non-neglected nursery-reared infants had constant access to a heat source from their first day of life. Although we could not observe the neglected infants constantly, it is likely that they were left on the cold floor and not held by their mothers for extended periods, potentially impeding brain development and resulting in decreased daytime sleep and weight. Our findings suggest that the lack of maternal care and hypothermia during the first days of life had a lasting effect on the developing infant across the first month of life.

Although one alternative explanation for the differences between the neglected and non-neglected nursery-reared infants is that the time with the neglecting mothers entrained the sleep differences seen in the neglected infants (see Lubach, Kittrell, & Coe, Reference Lubach, Kittrell and Coe1992), there are reasons to think this unlikely. First, when the early-removed infants (within the first 2 days of life) and late-removed infants (after the first week of life) were compared, there were no differences in sleep between the two groups; when the number of days infants spent with their mother before being placed in the nursery was considered in the analyses, days on mother was not statistically related or trending. Second, because of the natural light that entered the pens and the nursery, the light-dark cycle was identical for all infants. Also, as noted in the introduction, in humans and macaques, neonatal sleep-wake rhythms show ultraradian patterns that are relatively robust to routine disturbances (in humans: McGraw et al., Reference McGraw, Hoffmann, Harker and Herman1999; in macaques: Meier & Berger, Reference Meier and Berger1965; Sackett et al., Reference Sackett, Fahrenbruch, Ruppenthal, Ruppenthal and Reese1979), and sleep does not fully consolidate until later in infancy (in humans: Ednick et al., Reference Ednick, Cohen, McPhail, Beebe and Simakajornboon2009; in macaques: Sackett et al., Reference Sackett, Fahrenbruch, Ruppenthal, Ruppenthal and Reese1979). It is clear that the early neglect had deleterious effects, and although it is possible that maternal entrainment contributed to the sleep differences, its effects were likely secondary to the traumatic effects of neglect, hypothermia, and potential dehydration and malnutrition.

Besides maternal entrainment, it could be argued that compared with the mothers’ breast milk, the formula the nursery-reared monkeys were fed affected their sleep (Ball, Reference Ball2003) and weight (Dewey, Reference Dewey1998; Kelleher, Chatterton, Nielsen, & Lönnerdal, Reference Kelleher, Chatterton, Nielsen and Lönnerdal2003). Weight measurements from normally reared infants raised by their biological mother were available (see Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Champoux and Suomi1998) and could be used as reference data. Although the neglected infants’ birth weights were identical to the mother-reared infants, by the time the neglected infants were placed in the nursery, they weighed less than mother-reared infants of the same age and sex. This analysis suggests that the differences in maternal treatment were responsible for the neglected infants’ low weight, which was low even when compared with non-neglected infants that had never consumed formula. Although sleep data were not collected for mother-reared infants, considering the extreme effects of maternal neglect on neonates’ weight, it is likely that formula alone does not explain the differences in sleep between the neglected and non-neglected nursery-reared infants, and maternal neglect likely contributed. Because infant weight is considered a window to postconception age and maturational brain development (Vohr et al., Reference Vohr, Wright, Dusick, Mele, Verter, Steichen and Bauer2000), this finding suggests that neonatal maternal neglect can have consequences for physical and neurological development. Nonetheless, further investigation of neonatal daytime sleep in mother-reared monkeys is needed to confirm this interpretation. Regardless of the etiological reason for the differences between the neglected infants and the nursery controls, it is clear that maternal neglect is associated with dysregulated sleep in infants. In further support of this interpretation, neglected infants had impaired CNS serotonin functioning (i.e., higher concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA), suggesting that neglect-mediated impairments in central serotonin functioning may be, at least in part, a potential biological mechanism by which the neonates’ sleep was dysregulated. Taken together, the findings of this study illustrate the critical role that primary caregivers play in shaping their infants’ sleep and physiological development.

Effect of 5HTT genotype on neonatal sleep and its relationship with CNS serotonin

We hypothesized that dysregulation of the serotonin system would be associated with daytime sleep. In support of this hypothesis, we found that for infant monkeys with the s allele, there was a positive correlation between average frequency of daytime sleep and concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA. There was no association between CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep among infants that were homozygous for the L allele. Studies of adult rhesus monkeys tend to show negative correlations between CSF 5-HIAA and measures of nighttime sleep, including sleep onset (Zajicek et al., Reference Zajicek, Higley, Suomi and Linnoila1997), nighttime activity, and daytime naps (Mehlman et al., Reference Mehlman, Westergaard, Hoos, Sallee, Marsh, Suomi and Higley2000). Unlike those studies of adult monkeys, we found a positive correlation between CSF 5-HIAA and average frequency of daytime sleep in this study. These studies, however, did not assess CNS serotonin functioning in the context of 5HTT genotype, and in this case the association was only seen in subjects with the s allele. Moreover, our subjects were neonates reared without mothers, and early rearing conditions can profoundly shape the expression of the serotonin system (Maestripieri et al., Reference Maestripieri, Higley, Lindell, Newman, McCormack and Sanchez2006; Shannon et al., Reference Shannon, Schwandt, Champoux, Shoaf, Suomi, Linnoila and Higley2005). The discrepancy between our serotonin-mediated sleep findings and previous research could also be due to differences in the age or environmental conditions of the subjects in these studies. Although Zajicek et al. (Reference Zajicek, Higley, Suomi and Linnoila1997) investigated laboratory-housed juvenile monkeys and Mehlman et al. (Reference Mehlman, Westergaard, Hoos, Sallee, Marsh, Suomi and Higley2000) investigated free-ranging adult males, the current study investigated laboratory-housed male and female infants. Thus, the positive correlation between CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep reported in this study suggests that there may be developmental changes in the relationship between serotonin system functioning and daytime sleep, meriting further investigation.

As briefly noted, the moderating effects of 5HTT genotype found in this study coincide with reports that the s allele confers greater individual sensitivity to the effects of the environment on a variety of outcomes (see Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009), including sleep (Brummett et al., Reference Brummett, Krystal, Ashley-Koch, Kuhn, Züchner, Siegler and Williams2007), possibly leading to a closer coupling of CNS serotonin with sleep behavior, at least as the s allele genotype is phenotypically expressed in this environment. It is possible that in this study, the infants with the s allele were biologically more sensitive to the effects of their own serotonin system functioning on daytime sleep. The positive correlation between sleep and CSF 5-HIAA may be explained by serotonin's inhibitory function in the reticular activating system, which is primarily involved in arousal (Jones, Reference Jones2003) and is one of the first systems in the brain to develop (Barkovich, Kjos, Jackson, & Norman, Reference Barkovich, Kjos, Jackson and Norman1988). Infants with the s allele and lower CSF 5-HIAA may have had insufficient serotonin binding in their reticular formation to inhibit arousal, resulting in reduced daytime sleep. This interpretation remains speculative, because it is not possible to determine region-specific serotonin binding from concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA, a measure of total CNS serotonin functioning and turnover. Further research integrating neuroimaging is needed to confirm this interpretation. Nonetheless, the results of this study are consistent with research in humans demonstrating that variation in 5HTT genotype can moderate differences in sleeping patterns (Barclay et al., Reference Barclay, Eley, Mill, Wong, Zavos, Archer and Gregory2011; Koga et al., Reference Koga, Fukushima, Sakuma and Kagawa2016) and concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Marchuk, Gadde, Barefoot, Grichnik, Helms and Siegler2003). Ultimately, the results of this study suggest that the serotonin system has a complex association with sleep during early development.

Limitations and conclusion

Sleep was assessed behaviorally (bihourly) during the day only, and although a variety of studies show that systematic behavioral observation of sleep-wake states are a viable measure of sleep development (Cohen-Mansfield, Waldhorn, Werner, & Billig, Reference Cohen-Mansfield, Waldhorn, Werner and Billig1990; Papailiou, Sullivan, & Cameron, Reference Papailiou, Sullivan and Cameron2008), 24-hr monitoring would allow a better assessment of the relationship between daytime and nighttime sleep. Although nursery-rearing closely controls the environment, leading to homogenous treatments, considering the important role that mothers play in regulating their infant's development and physiology (Hofer, Reference Hofer and Rosenblum1983), having a mother-reared control group would facilitate our understanding of how mothers affect neonatal sleep, which should be considered in future studies.

The results of this study are the first to study the role of serotonin in the development of sleep patterns in neonatal primates and to link maternal neglect and 5HTT genotype effects on the functioning of the serotonin system, specifically because the serotonin system is related to infant daytime sleep. We found that early stressful experiences profoundly affect infants’ sleep and physiological development, a finding highlighting the pivotal role that caregivers play in infant sleep development. The results of this study also illustrate the complex role that the serotonin system plays in sleep and highlight the need for more research to elucidate how serotonin affects sleep and sleep development. The strong coupling of CSF 5-HIAA concentrations to daytime sleep in the subjects with the s allele but not in the subjects with the LL genotype further suggests that the possession of the s allele may lead to increased sensitivity of the serotonin system to aberrations in the brain, resulting in a close link between CNS serotonin functioning and sleep behaviors. This may have clinical implications when considering individual differences in the serotonin system when conducting sleep interventions in early development.

Supplementary material

To view the supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001359

Author ORCIDs

Alexander Baxter, 0000-0003-4109-6179; Elizabeth K. Wood, 0000-0001-6407-295X.

Acknowledgments

We thank Maribeth Champoux and Courtney Shannon for supervising the neonatal nursery and collecting these data. Annika Paukner also assisted in the data collection.

Financial support

This work was supported by Brigham Young University mentoring grants and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Intramural Research Programs.

Footnotes

*

A.B. and E.K.W. are equal contributors.

D.B.K., S.J.S., and J.D.H. are equal contributors.

References

Ball, H. L. (2003). Breastfeeding, bed-sharing, and infant sleep. Birth, 30, 181188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barclay, N. L., Eley, T. C., Mill, J., Wong, C. C. Y., Zavos, H. M. S., Archer, S. N., & Gregory, A. M. (2011). Sleep quality and diurnal preference in a sample of young adults: Associations with 5HTTLPR, PER3, and CLOCK 3111. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics: The Official Publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics, 156 B, 681690. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.31210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkovich, A., Kjos, B., Jackson, D., & Norman, D. (1988). Normal maturation of the neonatal and infant brain: MR imaging at 1.5 T. Radiology, 166, 173180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, C. E., Noble, P., Hanson, E., Pine, D. S., Winslow, J. T., & Nelson, E. E. (2009). Early adverse rearing experiences alter sleep-wake patterns and plasma cortisol levels in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 10291040. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bathory, E., & Tomopoulos, S. (2017). Sleep regulation, physiology and development, sleep duration and patterns, and sleep hygiene in infants, toddlers, and preschool-age children. Current Problems in Pediatric and Adolescent Health Care, 47, 2942. doi:10.1016/j.cppeds.2016.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., & Williams, R. (2009). Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 746754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, A. J., Lesch, K. P., Heils, A., Long, J. C., Lorenz, J. G., Shoaf, S. E., … Higley, J. (2002). Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biagioni, E., Boldrini, A., Giganti, F., Guzzetta, A., Salzarulo, P., & Cioni, G. (2005). Distribution of sleep and wakefulness EEG patterns in 24-h recordings of preterm and full-term newborns. Early Human Development, 81, 333339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, R. E., Wolf, R. C., & Sackett, G. P. (1970). Circadian rhythms of plasma 17-hydroxycorticosteroids in the infant monkey. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 133, 342344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brummett, B. H., Krystal, A. D., Ashley-Koch, A., Kuhn, C. M., Züchner, S., Siegler, I. C., … Williams, R. B. (2007). Sleep quality varies as a function of 5-HTTLPR genotype and stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Hariri, A. R., Holmes, A., Uher, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010). Genetic sensitivity to the environment: The case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 509527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champoux, M., & Suomi, S. J. (1988). Behavioral development of nursery-reared rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 363367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen-Mansfield, J., Waldhorn, R., Werner, P., & Billig, N. (1990). Validation of sleep observations in a nursing home. Sleep, 13, 512525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daws, L. C., & Gould, G. G. (2011). Ontogeny and regulation of the serotonin transporter: Providing insights into human disorders. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 131, 6179. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.03.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deley, J. T., Turner, R. S., Freeman, A., Bliwise, D. L., & Rye, D. B. (2006). Prolonged assessment of sleep and daytime sleepiness in unrestrained Macaca mulatta. Sleep, 29, 221231.Google Scholar
Deuschle, M., Schredl, M., Schilling, C., Wüst, S., Frank, J., Witt, S. H., … Schulze, T. G. (2010). Association between a serotonin transporter length polymorphism and primary insomnia. Sleep, 33, 343347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, K. G. (1998). Growth characteristics of breast-fed compared to formula-fed infants. Neonatology, 74, 94105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ednick, M., Cohen, A., McPhail, G., Beebe, D., & Simakajornboon, N. (2009). A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep, 32, 14491458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emde, R. N., Harmon, R. J., Metcalf, D., Koenig, K. L., & Wagonfeld, S. (1971). Stress and neonatal sleep. Psychosomatic Medicine, 33, 491497. doi:10.1097/00006842-197111000-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagioli, I., & Salzarulo, P. (1982). Sleep states development in the first year of life assessed through 24-h recordings. Early Human Development, 6, 215228. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-3782(82)90109-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueiredo, B., Dias, C. C., Pinto, T. M., & Field, T. (2016). Infant sleep-wake behaviors at two weeks, three and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 169178. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filonzi, L., Magnani, C., Nosetti, L., Nespoli, L., Borghi, C., Vaghi, M., & Marzano, F. N. (2012). Serotonin transporter role in identifying similarities between SIDS and idiopathic ALTE. Pediatrics, 130(e138e144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., & Savoie, L. (1997). Problems reported by parents of Romanian orphans adopted to British Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 6782. doi:10.1080/016502597385441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. A., Rogers, J., Katze, M. G., Bumgarner, R., Weinstock, G. M., Mardis, E. R., … Wilson, R. K. (2007). Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science, 316, 222234.Google ScholarPubMed
Graven, S. N., & Browne, J. V. (2008). Sleep and brain development: The critical role of sleep in fetal and early neonatal brain development. Newborn and Infant Nursing Reviews, 8, 173179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, A. M., Caspi, A., Eley, T. C., Moffitt, T. E., O'connor, T. G., & Poulton, R. (2005). Prospective longitudinal associations between persistent sleep problems in childhood and anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 157163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Malone, S., Vance, G., & Fisch, R. O. (1985). Coping with aversive stimulation in the neonatal period: Quiet sleep and plasma cortisol levels during recovery from circumcision. Child Development, 56, 824834. doi:10.2307/1130094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Heils, A., Teufel, A., Petri, S., Stober, G., Riederer, P., Bengel, D., & Lesch, K. P. (1996). Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression. Journal of Neurochemistry, 66, 26212624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higley, J. D., Bennett, A. J., Helis, A., Lesch, K. P., Shoaf, S. E., White, I. M., … Linnoila, M. (1998). Serotonin transporter gene variation is associated with CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in rhesus monkeys. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 24, 1113.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A., & Spencer-Booth, Y. (1967). The behaviour of socially living rhesus monkeys in their first two and a half years. Animal Behaviour, 15, 169196. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(67)80029-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, M. A. (1983). The mother-infant interaction as a regulator of infant physiology and behavior. In Rosenblum, L. (Ed.), Symbiosis in parent-offspring interactions (pp. 6175). Springer, Boston, MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y.-S., Paiva, T., Hsu, J.-F., Kuo, M.-C., & Guilleminault, C. (2014). Sleep and breathing in premature infants at 6 months post-natal age. BMC Pediatrics, 14, 303. doi:10.1186/s12887-014-0303-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iwata, S., Fujita, F., Kinoshita, M., Unno, M., Horinouchi, T., Morokuma, S., & Iwata, O. (2017). Dependence of nighttime sleep duration in one-month-old infants on alterations in natural and artificial photoperiod. Scientific Reports, 7, 44749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, B. E. (2003). Arousal systems. Frontiers in Bioscience, 8, s438s451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jouvet, M. (1999). Sleep and serotonin: An unfinished story. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 24S27S.Google Scholar
Kaemingk, K., & Reite, M. (1987). Social environment and nocturnal sleep: Studies in peer-reared monkeys. Sleep, 10, 542550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelleher, S. L., Chatterton, D., Nielsen, K., & Lönnerdal, B. (2003). Glycomacropeptide and α-lactalbumin supplementation of infant formula affects growth and nutritional status in infant rhesus monkeys. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 77, 12611268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kepser, L.-J., & Homberg, J. R. (2015). The neurodevelopmental effects of serotonin: A behavioural perspective. Behavioural Brain Research, 277, 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knobel, R. B. (2014). Fetal and neonatal thermal physiology. Newborn and Infant Nursing Reviews, 14, 4549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koga, A., Fukushima, A., Sakuma, K., & Kagawa, Y. (2016). Association between sleep duration and personality-gene variants: Sleep duration is longer in S/S homozygotes of serotonin transporter than in L allele genotypes. Journal of Sleep Disorders: Treatment and Care, 4.Google Scholar
Kohyama, J. (1998). Sleep as a window on the developing brain. Current Problems in Pediatrics, 28, 7392. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0045-9380(98)80054-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, P., Hiscock, H., & Wake, M. (2003). Outcomes of infant sleep problems: A longitudinal study of sleep, behavior, and maternal well-being. Pediatrics, 111, e203e207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavezzi, A. M., Casale, V., Oneda, R., Weese-Mayer, D. E., & Matturri, L. (2009). Sudden infant death syndrome and sudden intrauterine unexplained death: Correlation between hypoplasia of raphe nuclei and serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism. Pediatric Research, 66, 2227. doi:10.1203/PDR.0b013e3181a7bb73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesch, K.-P., Balling, U., Gross, J., Strauss, K., Wolozin, B. L., Murphy, D. L., & Riederer, P. (1994). Organization of the human serotonin transporter gene. Journal of Neural Transmission General Section, 95, 157162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubach, G. R., & Coe, C. L. (2006). Immunological consequences of nursery rearing. In Sackett, G. P., Ruppenthal, G., & Elias, K. (Eds.), Nursery rearing of nonhuman primates in the 21st century (pp. 135168). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubach, G. R., Kittrell, E. M. W., & Coe, C. L. (1992). Maternal influences on body temperature in the infant primate. Physiology & Behavior, 51, 987994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestripieri, D., Higley, J. D., Lindell, S. G., Newman, T. K., McCormack, K. M., & Sanchez, M. M. (2006). Early maternal rejection affects the development of monoaminergic systems and adult abusive parenting in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 10171024. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, E. M., Alderdice, F. A., Halliday, H. L., Jenkins, J. G., & Vohra, S. (2006). Interventions to prevent hypothermia at birth in preterm and/or low birthweight babies. Evidence-Based Child Health: A Cochrane Review Journal, 1, 287324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormack, K., Howell, B., Guzman, D., Villongco, C., Pears, K., Kim, H., … Sanchez, M. (2015). The development of an instrument to measure global dimensions of maternal care in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 77, 2033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGraw, K., Hoffmann, R., Harker, C., & Herman, J. H. (1999). The development of circadian rhythms in a human infant. Sleep, 22, 303310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehlman, P. T., Westergaard, G. C., Hoos, B. J., Sallee, F. R., Marsh, S., Suomi, S. J., … Higley, J. D. (2000). CSF5-HIAA and nighttime activity in free-ranging primates. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 210218. doi:10.1016/s0893-133x(99)00101-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, G. W., & Berger, R. J. (1965). Development of sleep and wakefulness patterns in the infant rhesus monkey. Experimental Neurology, 12, 257277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monti, J. M., & Jantos, H. (2008). The roles of dopamine and serotonin, and of their receptors, in regulating sleep and waking. Progress in Brain Research, 172, 625646. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00929-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, H. A. (1967). Sex, age, and state as determinants of mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 13, 1936.Google Scholar
Olivier, J. D., Åkerud, H., Kaihola, H., Pawluski, J. L., Skalkidou, A., Högberg, U., & Sundström-Poromaa, I. (2013). The effects of maternal depression and maternal selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor exposure on offspring. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 7, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ong, S. H., Wickramaratne, P., Tang, M., & Weissman, M. M. (2006). Early childhood sleep and eating problems as predictors of adolescent and adult mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 96, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papailiou, A., Sullivan, E., & Cameron, J. L. (2008). Behaviors in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) associated with activity counts measured by accelerometer. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 185190. doi:10.1002/ajp.20476CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parmelee, A. H., Wenner, W. H., & Schulz, H. R. (1964). Infant sleep patterns: From birth to 16 weeks of age. The Journal of Pediatrics, 65, 576582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reite, M., Rhodes, J., Kavan, E., & Adey, W. (1965). Normal sleep patterns in macaque monkey. Archives of Neurology, 12, 133144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reite, M., Short, R., Kaufman, I., Stynes, A., & Pauley, J. (1978). Heart rate and body temperature in separated monkey infants. Biological Psychiatry, 13, 91105.Google ScholarPubMed
Rogers, J., Kaplan, J., Garcia Iv, R., Shelledy, W., Nair, S., & Cameron, J. (2006). Mapping of the serotonin transporter locus (SLC6A4) to rhesus chromosome 16 using genetic linkage. Cytogenetic and Genome Research, 112, 341A341A.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackett, G., Fahrenbruch, C., & Ruppenthal, G. (1979). Development of basic physiological parameters and sleep-wakefulness patterns in normal and at-risk neonatal pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). In Ruppenthal, G. & Reese, D. (Eds.), Nursery care of nonhuman primates. Advances in primatology. (pp. 125142). Boston, MA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scaramuzzo, R. T., Giampietri, M., Fiorentini, E., Bartalena, L., Fiori, S., Guzzetta, A., … Ghirri, P. (2015). Serum cortisol concentrations during induced hypothermia for perinatal asphyxia are associated with neurological outcome in human infants. Stress: The International Journal on the Biology of Stress, 18, 129133. doi:10.3109/10253890.2014.987120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheinin, M., Chang, W.-H., Kirk, K. L., & Linnoila, M. (1983). Simultaneous determination of 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, and homovanillic acid in cerebrospinal fluid with high-performance liquid chromatography using electrochemical detection. Analytical Biochemistry, 131, 246253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scher, M. S., & Loparo, K. A. (2009). Neonatal EEG/sleep state analyses: A complex phenotype of developmental neural plasticity. Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 259275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seppala, T., Scheinin, M., Capone, A., & Linnoila, M. (1984). Liquid chromatographic assay for CSF catecholamines using electrochemical detection. Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicologica, 55, 8187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shannon, C., Champoux, M., & Suomi, S. J. (1998). Rearing condition and plasma cortisol in rhesus monkey infants. American Journal of Primatology, 46, 311321.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shannon, C., Schwandt, M. L., Champoux, M., Shoaf, S. E., Suomi, S. J., Linnoila, M., & Higley, J. D. (2005). Maternal absence and stability of individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in rhesus monkey infants. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 16581664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinelli, S., Chefer, S., Suomi, S. J., Higley, J. D., Barr, C. S., & Stein, E. (2009). Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 658665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spruyt, K., Aitken, R. J., So, K., Charlton, M., Adamson, T. M., & Horne, R. S. C. (2008). Relationship between sleep/wake patterns, temperament and overall development in term infants over the first year of life. Early Human Development, 84, 289296. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, S. J. (2006). Risk, resilience, and gene × environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 5262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohr, B. R., Wright, L. L., Dusick, A. M., Mele, L., Verter, J., Steichen, J. J., … Bauer, C. R. (2000). Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network, 1993–1994. Pediatrics, 105, 12161226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldron, S., & MacKinnon, R. (2007). Neonatal thermoregulation. Infant, 3, 101104.Google Scholar
Welberg, L. M., & Seckl, J. R. (2001). Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of the brain. Journal Of Neuroendocrinology, 13, 113128. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00601.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, R. B., Marchuk, D. A., Gadde, K. M., Barefoot, J. C., Grichnik, K., Helms, M. J., … Siegler, I. C. (2003). Serotonin-related gene polymorphisms and central nervous system serotonin function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 533541. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, S. N., Gauthier, S., Anderson, G. M., & Purdy, W. C. (1980). Tryptophan, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid and indoleacetic acid in human cerebrospinal fluid: interrelationships and the influence of age, sex, epilepsy and anticonvulsant drugs. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 43, 438445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajicek, K. B., Higley, J. D., Suomi, S. J., & Linnoila, M. (1997). Rhesus macaques with high CSF 5-HIAA concentrations exhibit early sleep onset. Psychiatry Research, 73, 1525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeskind, P. S., & Stephens, L. E. (2004). Maternal selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use during pregnancy and newborn neurobehavior. Pediatrics, 113, 368375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Effect of maternal neglect on daytime sleep. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 255), neglected infants (black bar, n = 12) were less frequently observed sleeping during the day. Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Effect of maternal neglect on cerebral serotonin turnover. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 165), neglected infants (black bar, n = 11) exhibited higher cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (CSF 5-HIAA). Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Effect of maternal neglect on maturational weight. Compared to non-neglected nursery infants (gray bar, n = 251), neglected infants (black bar, n = 11) on average weighed less during the first month of life. Asterisk indicates p < .05. Error bars show ± 1 standard error.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (CSF 5-HIAA) predict daytime sleep in infants heterozygous with the Ls genotype. The x-axis represents concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA, and the y-axis represents average daytime sleep. The white dots represent subjects that were homozygous for the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT) L allele (n = 91), and the black triangles represent subjects that were heterozygous for the 5HTT Ls genotype (n = 25). The dotted line is the line of best fit for subjects that were homozygous for the L allele, and the solid black line is the line of best fit for subjects that were heterozygous for the Ls genotype. There was a positive correlation between concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep among infants that were heterozygous with the Ls genotype. There was no association between concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA and daytime sleep among infants that were homozygous for the L allele.

Figure 4

Table 1. Multiple regression model showing dysregulation in the serotonin system is associated with daytime sleep

Supplementary material: File

Baxter et al. supplementary material

Appendix A

Download Baxter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 16.2 KB