Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T03:04:48.603Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longevity returns to political office

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2020

Sebastian Barfort
Affiliation:
Independent Researcher, Copenhagen, Denmark
Robert Klemmensen
Affiliation:
Political Science, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
Erik Gahner Larsen*
Affiliation:
School of Politics and International Relations, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: E.G.Larsen@kent.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Does political office cause worse or better longevity prospects? Two perspectives in the literature offer contradicting answers. First, increased income, social status, and political connections obtained through holding office can increase longevity. Second, increased stress and working hours associated with holding office can have detrimental effects on longevity. To provide causal evidence, we exploit a regression discontinuity design with unique data on the longevity of candidates for US gubernatorial office. The results show that politicians winning a close election live 5–10 years longer than candidates who lose.

Type
Research Note
Copyright
Copyright © The European Political Science Association 2020

Holding political office entails stress, long working hours, and an irregular work schedule. However, the demanding life in office also comes with a series of perks such as the significant financial return to political office (Eggers and Hainmueller, Reference Eggers and Hainmueller2009; Fisman et al., Reference Fisman, Schulz and Vig2014; Palmer and Schneer, Reference Palmer and Schneer2016; Fahey, Reference Fahey2018). While studies have emphasized the costs and benefits of holding political office, there is one key return to office that has so far received little attention in the literature: politicians' health. Specifically, among the few studies on the topic, the evidence is pointing towards different conclusions on the impact of political office on longevity, namely a negative effect (Olenski et al., Reference Olenski, Abola and Jena2015), no effect (Shavelle et al., Reference Shavelle, Kush, Paculdo, Strauss and Day2008; Olshansky, Reference Olshansky2011), and a positive effect (Dennis and Crayford, Reference Dennis and Crayford2015).

The limited attention to this question can partially be explained by the methodological challenges in testing such causal dynamics, including factors related to reverse causation and confounders. First, healthy politicians might be more likely to win elections, e.g. if voters are more likely to reward attractive politicians (Berggren et al., Reference Berggren, Jordahl and Poutvaara2017). Second, economic resources might explain both election wins and health, resulting in a spurious relationship. Both of these challenges put serious limitations to our knowledge of the health implications of winning political office. Accordingly, the correlation between holding political office and longevity can be biased for multiple reasons leading to different effects in the literature. In order to accommodate these challenges, we rely on a regression discontinuity design and collect a unique dataset on the longevity of all now-deceased candidates from US gubernatorial elections from 1945 to 2012. This large dataset allows us to estimate the causal effect of holding office on candidates' life expectancy taking reverse causation and confounders into account.

The results presented here are in line with a positive effect of holding office on longevity. Politicians gaining office live 5–10 years longer compared to politicians not gaining office. These results are substantial and hold across a series of specifications and does not in any models provide indication of a negative effect. In sum, the findings help adjudicate on the implications of gaining office for health outcomes. In the next section, we briefly introduce the past research on the topic before we introduce our empirical approach and estimate the return of holding office on longevity.

1. Longevity returns to political office

The adverse effects of holding political office on life expectancy have been a frequent subject of debate in epidemiology and demography (Olshansky, Reference Olshansky2011; Goldbaum, Reference Goldbaum2012), with some studies suggesting substantial negative effects on longevity. Olenski et al. (Reference Olenski, Abola and Jena2015), for example, compare the longevity of elected leaders in 17 Western countries with runner ups who never served in office and find that elected candidates lived on average 4 fewer years after their last election compared to candidates who never served. Link et al. (Reference Link, Carpiano and Weden2013) find that US presidents and vice presidents lived on average 5 years shorter than candidates for who did not serve in these offices. Similarly, Jones and Jones (Reference Jones and Jones2006) shows that US presidents are more likely than the average American to suffer from stress-related diseases.

These findings underscore that public office might lead to accelerated aging and reduced longevity due to the inherent pressure and stress of holding political leadership positions. Stress has direct psychological and biochemical effects, but has also been found to affect health outcomes indirectly by increasing the likelihood that an individual engages in harmful health behaviors (Schneiderman et al., Reference Schneiderman, Ironson and Siegel2005). This resonates with the fact that political life entails stress, long working hours and an irregular work schedule which are associated with negative health outcomes. Accordingly, this is used as an explanation for why politicians staying longer in office, on average, should live shorter lives.

However, this perspective is contested by evidence showing that parliamentarians have lower mortality rates than the general population (Dennis and Crayford, Reference Dennis and Crayford2015). Elected politicians experience an increase in status and lifetime earnings, both of which have been found to be positively related to health outcomes (Marmot, Reference Marmot2004; Cutler et al., Reference Cutler, Deaton and Lleras-Muney2006; Cristia, Reference Cristia2009; Chetty et al., Reference Chetty, Stepner, Abraham, Lin, Scuderi, Turner, Bergeron and Cutler2016; Deaton, Reference Deaton2016). Chetty et al. (Reference Chetty, Stepner, Abraham, Lin, Scuderi, Turner, Bergeron and Cutler2016) use administrative population tax records in the US to show that higher income is associated with greater longevity. Importantly, holding political office has been shown to entail sizable pecuniary benefits. Palmer and Schneer (Reference Palmer and Schneer2016), for example, find that winning a senate seat or a gubernatorial election increases the likelihood of serving on a board of directors with approximately 30 percentage points. In the UK, Eggers and Hainmueller (Reference Eggers and Hainmueller2009) find that winning a parliamentary seat in Britain almost doubled the wealth for Conservative MPs.

In addition, in sociology and social epidemiology, social status has been found to be associated with lower mortality and better health outcomes (Marmot, Reference Marmot2004; Wilkinson, Reference Wilkinson2005). Rablen and Oswald (Reference Rablen and Oswald2008), for example, compare scientists who won the Nobel prize in Chemistry and Physics to scientists who were nominated, finding a causal effect of winning a Nobel prize on longevity. Liu et al. (Reference Liu, Kwon, Xue and Fleisher2017) estimate the effect among scientists of being elected to the Chinese Academy of Science and Engineering and find that becoming an academician increases longevity with approximately 1.2 years.

The different studies suggest distinct and contradicting ways through which political office can affect longevity. The epidemiological literature suggests that political life entails stress, long working hours, and sleep deprivation which are associated with negative life expectancy. However, elected politicians also experience increases in status, wealth, power, and beneficial connections which all could lead to health advantages. Which of these effects dominate is ultimately an empirical question and the current evidence on holding political office on longevity is inconclusive.

Politicians often have better socioeconomic backgrounds than the average citizen, and for this reason, comparing elected politicians to the general population risks conflating the effect of holding office with unobserved factors such as wealth or education (Goldbaum, Reference Goldbaum2012). From the point of establishing a causal relationship between political office and longevity, the observational approaches used in the current studies are inadequate for several reasons. Comparing politicians to the general population is problematic not just because politicians are more affluent than the average citizen (Carnes, Reference Carnes2012, Reference Carnes2013), but also because unhealthy citizens might decide not to run for office if they fear that political life will affect their health negatively. Even observational approaches that compare candidates who compete for the same position risk selection bias whenever candidates' health profile at the time of the election correlates with attributes that affect citizens' voting behavior. In the next sections, we outline our data collection and describe our strategy for estimating the causal effects of political office on longevity.

2. Method and data

To shed further light on the conflicting empirical evidence in the literature, we examine the effect of holding gubernatorial office on longevity in the US. In addition to being the ceremonial head of state, governors are central figures in the political landscape with important budgeting, planning, and managerial powers and responsibilities. Governors nominate and appoint local judges, enact state legislation, prepare state budgets, and are responsible in many symbolic roles (Sabato, Reference Sabato1978; Ransone, Reference Ransone1982).

We collect data on all gubernatorial elections from the CQ Voting and Elections Collection (Press, Reference Press2010). These data contain names and votes for all candidates running in a gubernatorial election from 1945 to 2012. We limit attention to the two candidates who received the highest number of votes. We use these data to generate an assignment variable which is defined as the fraction of all votes received by the winning candidate less the vote share of the closest losing candidate.

Next, we link this data to information on birth and death dates. We collected information on birth and death dates of all gubernatorial candidates running for office from 1945 to 2012 from a variety of sources. Birth and death dates for winning candidates are available in Glashan (Reference Glashan1979) and Mullaney and Glashan (Reference Mullaney and Glashan1988). For losing candidates, we use information gathered from several online sources, including Wikipedia, The Political Graveyard (a website that contains comprehensive biographies of US politicians), Find a Grave (a website that allows family members to upload pictures of tombstones and biographical information about the deceased) and Our Campaigns (a crowdsourced website that documents the political careers of over 350,000 people who have held or sought political office, obituaries in various national and local newspapers). We use the death date to calculate the outcome variable, namely how many days each candidate lives after the election.

From these sources, we are able to identify biographical information for approximately 97 percent of all candidates who ran for gubernatorial office from 1945–2012. In our data, 772 are still alive as of September 2019. When these observations are removed we are left with a total of 1092 candidate-year observations and 676 unique elections. Of these candidates, 557 are Democrats, 527 are Republicans, and eight are from a third party. A majority of the candidates in our sample ran for office one time (67%), 22% ran for office twice and 11% ran for office between three and six times. We further obtained the life expectancy for an average US citizen with the same age as the candidate at the time of the election. These data are available from the Human Mortality Database (http://www.mortality.org).

In a few cases, we are only able to identify the year of birth or death, not the exact date of the event. For these candidates, we impute the date as July 1 of the given year. This introduces some degree of nonsystematic measurement error which should bias our parameter estimates toward zero. The results are robust when observations with incomplete information are dropped from the analysis.

Last, we merge our candidate-level dataset with additional information at the state level. This includes annual (real) per capita income, population, and total state expenditure, available from Jordan and Grossmann (Reference Jordan and Grossmann2016), as well as the region of the state as defined by the US Census Bureau, and information on whether the state has gubernatorial term limits. Summary statistics are available in the online appendix.

We estimate the causal effect of winning the gubernatorial election using a sharp regression discontinuity design based on close elections. In doing this, we compare the longevity of candidates who narrowly win to candidates who narrowly lose the election. The underlying identification assumption is that candidates within this narrow margin are similar across all other characteristics that might affect longevity. Because election outcomes within this narrow bandwidth can be considered essentially random, the setup allows us to use candidates who narrowly lose the election as a counterfactual for the longevity of candidates who narrowly win—had they instead lost the election.

We estimate the local average treatment effect of winning office nonparametrically using local polynomial inference. We rely on linear regression in the main specifications, but the online appendix shows robustness of our results to using a second order polynomial (Gelman and Imbens, Reference Gelman and Imbens2014). In all models, we use the optimal bandwidth, bias correction, and robust standard errors initially proposed by Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo and Titiunik2014) and refined in Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo, Farrell and Titiunik2016).

We used the auxiliary information from our covariates in a series of placebo regressions to assess the validity of our regression discontinuity design. The idea is to test whether there are systematic differences in baseline covariates on either side of the cutoff. Any discontinuity would indicate that narrow winners were systematically different from narrow losers before the election, which would violate the validity of our design (Lee and Lemieux, Reference Lee and Lemieux2010). We perform the placebo test using the estimation strategy on the baseline covariates. The results, available in the online appendix, show no systematic baseline differences between narrow winners and losers. Accordingly, we are confident that any significant differences in longevity cannot be attributed to the covariate differences.

3. Results

Table 1 reports the main results. Column one presents the raw estimate without control variables. In the next three columns we include various covariates. Including these controls may generate more precise estimates of the causal effect (Lee and Lemieux, Reference Lee and Lemieux2010; Calonico et al., Reference Calonico, Cattaneo, Farrell and Titiunik2016). In column two, we add state level controls. These include indicators for the Census region of the state, and information on whether the state had gubernatorial term limits at the time of the election. Column three includes controls for candidate characteristics such as gender, life expectancy at the time of the election, and political party. Column four includes all controls simultaneously.

Table 1. The causal effect of election win on longevity, main results.

Regression discontinuity results. We use Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo, Farrell and Titiunik2016) optimal bandwidth and triangular kernel weights in all columns. All models use local linear regression and include the bias correction and robust standard errors of Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo, Farrell and Titiunik2016). State controls add indicator variables for the Census region of the state, and an indicator variable for whether the state has gubernatorial term limits. Candidate controls include life expectancy at the time of the election, gender, and political party. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, $^\ast p< 0.1$.

Across all models, the effect of winning the gubernatorial election on the longevity of the candidate is positive and statistically significant. According to the estimates, winning a gubernatorial election leads to an increase in longevity around 2000 and 3000 days conditional upon the specific model. This effect is substantively important and represents an increase in longevity of approximately 20 percent for an average candidate for gubernatorial office.

Figure 1 presents the main findings graphically (based on a model without controls). Observations within the optimal bandwidth and the associated first degree polynomial are displayed. Each point represents the average days a candidate is alive within each bin, selected using the mimicking variance number of evenly spaced bins described in Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo and Titiunik2015). A jump in the longevity of candidates across the cutoff is directly evident in the plot.

Figure 1. The causal effect of winning on longevity.

In the online appendix, we report heterogeneous results based on partisanship. If elected politicians live longer due to higher wealth, then we might observe a larger effect for Republicans than Democrats (Eggers and Hainmueller, Reference Eggers and Hainmueller2009; Palmer and Schneer, Reference Palmer and Schneer2015, Reference Palmer and Schneer2016). The evidence here is inconclusive. While we do find evidence that the point estimates are larger for Republicans than Democrats, the difference between these estimates is not statistically significant.

Next, it is standard to test for discontinuity in the density. In our setting, where losers and winners are both included, the only plausible reason for a discontinuity in the density would be a difference in the probability of locating the date of death. We are able to obtain birth and death dates for 97 percent of all gubernatorial elections and while the assumption often holds in electoral settings (Eggers et al., Reference Eggers, Fowler, Hainmueller, Hall and Snyder2015), it could still be that we are more likely to be able to detect a candidate's birth and death date if they narrowly win the election. Due to this concern, we test for non-random sorting around the cutoff using the robust approach of Cattaneo et al. (Reference Cattaneo, Jansson and Ma2016).Footnote 1 Reassuringly, the test detects no evidence of sorting around the cutoff (p=0.56).

In the online appendix, we present a series of additional robustness checks. First, we investigate the robustness of the estimated effect to different bandwidths. The results remain positive and statistically significant for a wide range of bandwidths. Second, we further investigate the validity of the identifying assumption, namely that election outcomes are as good as random within the bandwidth, by testing for treatment effects at alternative cutoffs. Reassuringly, we find no evidence of significant treatment effects at these synthetic cutoffs. Third, we investigate the robustness of the results to various sample restrictions. Specifically, we test the validity of our estimates when we censor the outcome variable at the 2nd and the 98th percentile. This seems to only increase the point estimates, although not to a point where they are statistically different from our main estimates. Last, we also limit the sample to first time candidates. This reduces the sample size, but the estimates are similar to those reported above.

4. Concluding remarks

There is conflicting evidence in the literature linking political office to longevity. Building on a novel dataset with birth and death dates for close to all deceased gubernatorial candidates in elections in the period from 1945 to 2012, we provide evidence that holding political office has a positive causal effect on the longevity of the candidate. The identification strategy relies on a regression discontinuity design, and the parameter estimates imply that holding gubernatorial office increases longevity with 5–10 years. Our paper is the first to document a positive causal effect on longevity of holding political office and these results are robust to a wide variety of robustness checks and sample restrictions.

A common limitation of regression discontinuity designs that focus on close elections is external validity. Our results are by definition ‘local’, and they do not directly travel to other types of political office, such as the US presidency. One could argue that the job of president is inherently more stressful than that of US governor. On the other hand, the benefits in terms of status and lifetime income are arguably also larger. However, most types of political office are much more likely to resemble the job of governor than that of US president. For this reason, while we expect the health effects of political office to vary based on institutional settings, we consider it likely that our results have relevance to many other types of political office such as that of senator and parliamentarian. In addition, the candidates in the study are not necessarily representative of other candidates running in less narrow elections and one should be cautious with making strong inferences to all winning US governors.

As both winning and losing politicians in our sample on average live longer than their life expectancy, we can argue that the effect of holding office is most likely beneficial for office holders rather than causing a premature death for the losing candidates. That being said, additional fine-grained data on socio-economic characteristics of the politicians will enable better counterfactual inferences for the exact difference between actual longevity and life expectancy.

Last, our findings highlight the importance of considering effects of political office beyond the directly monetary advantages, and we consider this a fruitful avenue for future research.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2019.63

Footnotes

1 Results are similar when we use the test proposed in McCrary (Reference McCrary2008).

References

Berggren, N, Jordahl, H and Poutvaara, P (2017) The right look: conservative politicians look better and voters reward it. Journal of Public Economics 146, 7986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calonico, S, Cattaneo, MD and Titiunik, R (2014) Robust nonparametric confidence intervals for regression-discontinuity designs. Econometrica 82, 22952326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calonico, S, Cattaneo, MD and Titiunik, R (2015) Optimal data-driven regression discontinuity plots. Journal of the American Statistical Association 110, 17531769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calonico, S, Cattaneo, MD, Farrell, MH and Titiunik, R (2016) Regression discontinuity designs using covariates. University of Michigan, Working Paper.Google Scholar
Carnes, N (2012) Does the numerical underrepresentation of the working class in congress matter? Legislative Studies Quarterly 37, 534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnes, N (2013) White-Collar Government: The Hidden Role of Class in Economic Policy Making. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattaneo, MD, Jansson, M and Ma, X (2016) Simple local regression distribution estimators with an application to manipulation testing. University of Michigan, Working paper. http://www-personal.umich.edu/~cattaneo/papers/Cattaneo-Jansson-Ma_2017_LocPolDensity.pdf.Google Scholar
Chetty, R, Stepner, M, Abraham, S, Lin, S, Scuderi, B, Turner, N, Bergeron, A and Cutler, D (2016) The association between income and life expectancy in the united states, 2001–2014. JAMA 315, 17501766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, JP (2009) Rising mortality and life expectancy differentials by lifetime earnings in the united states. Journal of Health Economics 28, 984995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutler, D, Deaton, A and Lleras-Muney, A (2006) The determinants of mortality. The Journal of Economic Perspectives 20, 97120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaton, A (2016) On death and money: history, facts, and explanations. JAMA 315, 17031705. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.4072.eprint/data/journals/jama/935237/jed160028.pdf. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.4072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, J and Crayford, T (2015) Parliamentary privilege–mortality in members of the houses of parliament compared with the UK general population: retrospective cohort analysis, 1945–2011. BMJ 351, h6563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eggers, AC and Hainmueller, J (2009) MPs for sale? Returns to office in postwar British politics. American Political Science Review 103, 513533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eggers, AC, Fowler, A, Hainmueller, J, Hall, AB and Snyder, JM (2015) On the validity of the regression discontinuity design for estimating electoral effects: new evidence from over 40,000 close races. American Journal of Political Science 59, 259274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahey, K (2018) The perks of being a lawmaker: returns to office as a legislative goal. Legislative Studies Quarterly 41, 3768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisman, R, Schulz, F and Vig, V (2014) The private returns to public office. Journal of Political Economy 122, 806862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A and Imbens, G (2014) Why high-order polynomials should not be used in regression discontinuity designs. Technical Report, National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glashan, RR (1979) American Governors and Gubernatorial Elections, 1775–1978. Vol. 1. Westport, CT: Meckler Books.Google Scholar
Goldbaum, G (2012) Accelerated aging of us presidents. JAMA 307, 12541254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, JM and Jones, JL (2006) Presidential stroke: United States presidents and cerebrovascular disease. CNS Spectrums 11, 674678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, MP and Grossmann, M (2016) The Correlates of State Policy Project. East Lansing, MI: Institute for Public Policy and Social Research (IPPSR).Google Scholar
Lee, DS and Lemieux, T (2010) Regression discontinuity designs in economics. Journal of economic literature 48, 281355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Link, BG, Carpiano, RM and Weden, MM (2013) Can honorific awards give us clues about the connection between socioeconomic status and mortality? American Sociological Review 78, 192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, GG, Kwon, O, Xue, X and Fleisher, BM (2017) How much does social status matter to longevity?—Evidence from China's academician election. Health Economics 26, 292304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marmot, M (2004) The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Henry Holt and Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrary, J (2008) Manipulation of the running variable in the regression discontinuity design: a density test. Journal of Econometrics 142, 698714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullaney, MM and Glashan, RR (1988) American Governors and Gubernatorial Elections, 1979–1987. Westport, CT: Meckler.Google Scholar
Olenski, AR, Abola, MV and Jena, AB (2015) Do heads of government age more quickly? Observational study comparing mortality between elected leaders and runners-up in national elections of 17 countries. BMJ 351, h6424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olshansky, SJ (2011) Aging of us presidents. JAMA 306, 23282329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, M and Schneer, B (2015) The corporate boardroom's revolving door. Working paper.Google Scholar
Palmer, M and Schneer, B (2016) Capitol gains: the returns to elected office from corporate board directorships. The Journal of Politics 78, 181196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Press, CQ (2010) CQ Press voting and elections collection. Online, http://library.cqpress.com/elections/.Google Scholar
Rablen, MD and Oswald, AJ (2008) Mortality and immortality: the Nobel prize as an experiment into the effect of status upon longevity. Journal of Health Economics 27, 14621471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ransone, CB (1982) The American Governorship. Connecticut: Greenwood Press Westport. ISBN 0313229775.Google Scholar
Sabato, L (1978) Goodbye to good-time Charlie: The American governor transformed, 1950–1975. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, N, Ironson, G and Siegel, SD (2005) Stress and health: psychological, behavioral, and biological determinants. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 1, 607628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shavelle, RM, Kush, SJ, Paculdo, DR, Strauss, DJ and Day, SM (2008) Underwriting the presidents. Journal of Insurance Medicine 40, 120123.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, RG (2005) The Impact of Inequality: How to Make Sick Societies Healthier. New York, NY: The New Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. The causal effect of election win on longevity, main results.

Figure 1

Figure 1. The causal effect of winning on longevity.

Supplementary material: Link

Barfort et al. Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Barfort et al. supplementary material

Online Appendix
Download Barfort et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 175.5 KB