Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:21:58.573Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

HTR2A gene polymorphisms and Inward and Outward Personal Meaning Organisations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2014

Bernardo Nardi
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Francesco Piva*
Affiliation:
Department of Specialized Clinical Sciences and Odontostomatology, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Chiara Turchi
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences and Public Health, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Matteo Giulietti
Affiliation:
Department of Specialized Clinical Sciences and Odontostomatology, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Gianni Castellucci
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Emidio Arimatea
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
David Rocchetti
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Gianfranco Rocchetti
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Giovanni Principato
Affiliation:
Department of Specialized Clinical Sciences and Odontostomatology, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Adriano Tagliabracci
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences and Public Health, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
Cesario Bellantuono
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona, Italy
*
Francesco Piva Department of Specialized Clinical Sciences and Odontostomatology, Polytechnic University of Marche, Ancona 60131, Italy. Tel: +390712204641; Fax: +390712204609; E-mail: f.piva@univpm.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Nardi B, Piva F, Turchi C, Giulietti M, Castellucci G, Arimatea E, Rocchetti D, Rocchetti G, Principato G, Tagliabracci A, Bellantuono C. HTR2A gene polymorphisms and Inward and Outward Personal Meaning Organisations.

Objective: Caregiver behaviours and emotional expressions may induce development of two basic categories of constructing identity and of regulating cognitive and emotional processes: an Inward or an Outward Personal Meaning Organisation (PMO). Inwards read environmental signals through their internal activations. Their emotions are more distinct, and reciprocity is more based on physical distance (protection, loneliness). Outwards read internal activations through the environment. Their emotions are more blurred, and reciprocity is more based on a semantic sight of relations (approval, rules).

It has recently been shown that PMO development may also have physiological and genetic bases. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, Inward and Outward subjects showed different amygdala activation patterns and an association with the SLC6A4 serotonin transporter gene 5-HTTLPR polymorphism.

Methods: In this work, 149 healthy subjects were examined with respect to Inward and Outward PMOs. We explored the association with 10 serotonin receptor 2A (HTR2A) gene single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) selected by bioinformatics methods.

Results: An intronic SNP (rs55948462) was found to be significantly associated with an Inward and an Outward PMOs development. However, after statistical adjustments, these results did not remain significant.

Conclusion: We did not find associations between considered SNPs and Inward/Outward PMOs. However, the role of HTR2A polymorphisms was not considered in this study and that of the other serotonin-related genes should be valued.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Significant outcomes

  1. Ten HTR2A polymorphisms are not associated with Inward/Outward Personal Meaning Organisations.

Limitations

  • False-negative results could be due to the small sample size.

Introduction

The construct of Personal Meaning Organisation (PMO) has been described as ‘the specific arrangement of personal meaning processes by which each individual is provided with a sense of oneness and historical continuity in the course of his/her lifespan'(Reference Guidano1).

Constancy and predictability of the caregiver behaviours and emotional expressions allow early de- coding of his/her basic feelings (Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall2Reference Bretherton4). For example, the child begins to perceive situations in which he/she feels secure or in danger, protected or lonely and reads the environmental signals through his/her internal activations (e.g. anger, fear or calmness, sadness or happiness). These conditions promote development of an internal focus with an ‘Inward' reading of experience. In contrast, when caregiver behaviours and expressions are perceived as being more complex, and to change as a function of external situations, thus being less predictable and more difficult to decode, the child needs to store, and update, a greater amount of information. His/her emotional activations are connected to a self-evaluating cognitive schema (guilt, shame or serenity, sense of self-adequacy or inadequacy, dubiousness or certainty). In these conditions, the self develops beginning from a preliminary evaluation of the environment, which orients the recognition of internal activations and self-perception, leading to the development of an external focus with an ‘Outward' reading of experience (Reference Arciero, Gaetano, Maselli, Gentili, Freeman, Mahoney, Devito and Martin5Reference Guidano7). Inward/Outward patterns are more or less evident in different subjects, but one prevails in each individual, at least within specific categories of experience (Reference Nardi and Bellantuono8).

However, the PMO model, whose value in clinical practice is well established (Reference Dobson9Reference Neimeyer and Mahoney12), and attachment theories, which generally refer to learned behaviours, tend not to be investigated from the genetic viewpoint (Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall2Reference Bretherton4). In the last few years, advances in neuroscience have suggested possible physiological and adaptive bases for PMO (Reference Nardi and Bellantuono8). A recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study has documented different amygdala activation patterns in relation to personality style (Reference Bertolino, Arciero and Rubino13). Inward subjects asked to look at facial expressions of fear, showed a greater activation of amygdala, hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex compared with Outward subjects, who exhibited greater activation at the level of the fusiform gyrus, dorsal–lateral frontal cortex and occipital cortex. Another investigation by the same group showed greater activation of the medial prefrontal cortex in Inwards exposed to threatening stimuli, very probably due to greater neuronal recruitment occurring during cognitive evaluation of primary emotions (Reference Rubino, Blasi and Latorre14). In an fMRI study by our group, where we investigated the activation induced by standardised emotional stimuli, Inward subjects were seen to respond to anger with a more intense and univocal pattern, activation being mainly detected in the right amygdala and less in cortical areas compared with Outwards. As regards happiness, greater activation of the verbal hemisphere was observed in Outwards, in line with the clinical observation that they construct relations on semantic reciprocity, while Inwards construct relations on physical reciprocity. Activation in both groups was greater when shown the facial expressions of an unknown person (third person experience) with respect to their own (first person experience), probably as a result of a ‘surprise effect' and to a keener attention to unfamiliar aesthetic details (Reference Nardi, Capecci and Fabri15).

In this constructivistic framework, a recent work examined the correlation between genotype and Inward/Outward PMO, showing that the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene is associated with Inward/Outward personality styles (Reference Bertolino, Arciero and Rubino13). These findings are in line with the hypothesis that the Inward/Outward PMO also has genetic bases and is correlated to serotonergic system. This correlation was expected because serotonin regulates activity of the amygdala that interprets the environmental stimulations and therefore, it has a role in construction of personality and of the sense of self (Reference Bertolino, Arciero and Rubino13).

In this work, we chose another serotonin-related gene, HTR2A. It is associated with personality and behaviour (Reference Giegling, Hartmann, Moller and Rujescu16Reference Unschuld, Ising and Erhardt18) as well as with psychiatric conditions, e.g. schizophrenia, mood disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), suicidal behaviour, anxiety, obsessive-compulsive behaviour, eating disorders and Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Reference Celada, Puig, Amargos-Bosch, Adell and Artigas19Reference Serretti, Drago and de Ronchi21). The aim of this study was to investigate whether 10 HTR2A polymorphisms are associated with an Inward or an Outward PMO.

Material and methods

Subjects

Subjects were assessed for PMO using a Post-rationalist clinical interview and two questionnaires: Personal Meaning Questionnaire (PMQ) and Mini Questionnaire of Personal Organisation (MQPO), the validated self-rating questionnaires for PMO. In the clinical interview, the Inward/Outward PMO was blindly diagnosed by two trained psychotherapists from the Psychiatric Unit of the Neuroscience Department with at least 10-year experience in cognitive post-rationalist psychotherapy (BN, EA). The interview was conducted in line with a recent work (Reference Bertolino, Arciero and Rubino13), through three steps: (a) a detailed account of two meaningful episodes involving fear or anger, (b) a detailed description of emotional experience of fear or anger to assess the personal style of emotional activation and regulation and (c) a reconstruction of the onset, expression and extinction of the emotional experience. The PMQ questionnaire (Reference Picardia22) includes 68 items addressing two Inward (phobic-prone and depressive-prone) and two Outward (eating disorders-prone and obsessive-prone) personality styles. The MQPO questionnaire (Reference Arimatea, Giovagnoli, Blasi, Bellantuono and Nardi23) has 20 items and addresses two Inward (controller-prone and detached-prone) and two Outward (contextualised-prone and principles oriented-prone) personality styles. A diagnosis of PMO was made when the score in one PMO scale was at least 10% higher than the other scores, according to author's guidelines (Reference Picardia22,Reference Arimatea, Giovagnoli, Blasi, Bellantuono and Nardi23). Subjects were enrolled if the separate clinical evaluations by the two psychiatrists were also concordant.

In total, 149 healthy unrelated volunteers (65 males and 84 females) were recruited by call. All worked or studied at the School of Medicine of the Polytechnic University of Marche and at the Health Public Services of Ancona. All subjects were Italian, to minimise genetic heterogeneity. There were 58 Inwards and 91 Outwards (mean age 42.1 ± 10.3 and 37.6 ± 8.7, respectively, Student's t-test p-value = 0.005). Males were 60.3% of Inwards and 32.9% of Outwards (chi-squared p-value = 0.001). All subjects having any psychiatric diagnosis (assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I and II) were excluded. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Azienda Universitaria-Ospedaliera Ospedali Riuniti, Ancona. All subjects gave their written informed consent to participate.

Markers selection

The NCBI dbSNP (release 131) and HapMap (www.hapmap.org) databases were searched for data regarding HTR2A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNPs). Potentially phenotype-affecting polymorphisms were selected using the most accurate bioinformatics tools according to the literature (Reference Zhu, Hoffman and Wu24), or the most recent ones in case of lack of literature data.

Prediction of the effects of the SNPs lying on the promoter region was accomplished using the rVISTA 2.0 software (Reference Loots and Ovcharenko25), which is based on the recent 10.2 version of the TRANSFAC database (Reference Matys, Fricke and Geffers26). The software can predict the gain or loss of transcription factor binding sites related to polymorphisms. The extension of the window straddling the transcription start site (TSS) to be analysed is debated. Some researchers use sequences of 5000 bp upstream the TSS (Reference Mao and Zheng27), others consider a region encompassing −1750 to +250 bp (Reference Blais, Tsikitis, Acosta-Alvear, Sharan, Kluger and Dynlacht28), and others still a region from −500 to +500 bp (Reference Ishii, Washio, Uechi, Yoshihama, Kenmochi and Tomita29). We used an intermediate stretch of 2000 bp upstream the TSS. Promoter sequences were extracted using the UCSC Genome Browser (version hg18).

rBLOSUM64 (Reference Styczynski, Jensen, Rigoutsos and Stephanopoulos30), the recently revised BLOSUM matrix, and the SIFT (Reference Ng and Henikoff31) and PolyPhen (Reference Ramensk, Bork and Sunyaev32) tools were applied to determine whether SNPs negatively affected protein function. NNSPLICE (Reference Reese, Eeckman, Kulp and Haussler33) was used to predict the 5′ and 3′ splice sites, and SpliceAid (www.introni.it/splicing.html) (Reference Piva, Giulietti, Nocchi and Principato34) to predict the binding sites of splicing regulatory proteins. Because SpliceAid uses a database collecting all experimentally assessed target RNA sequences in humans, it avoids the false positives that are obtained with tools using score matrices.

Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from buccal swabs using the Maxwell™ 16 Blood DNA Purification Kit on an automated Maxwell™ 16 system (Promega Corporation, Madison, Wisconsin, USA).

Selected SNPs were genotyped using a minisequencing assay with the SnaPshot™ Multiplex Kit [Applied Biosystems Inc., Carlsbad, CA, USA (ABI)] and the capillary electrophoresis discrimination assay in an automated ABI 3130 machine (ABI).

The minisequencing method involved PCR amplification and extension. Multiplex PCR primers and extension primers were designed using PrimerExpress™ v2.0 (ABI); secondary structure and primer–dimer interactions were tested with the Autodimer software (www.cstl.nist.gov/biotech/strbase/AutoDimerHomepage/AutoDimerProgramHomepage.htm). Given the large number of primers, their sequences are available on request. Multiplex PCR amplification was performed using the QIAGEN Multiplex PCR kit following the manufacturer's recommendations. The PCR product was purified by enzymatic digestion with ExoI and CIP (Calf Intestinal Alkaline Phosphatase, New England BioLabs Inc., Ipswich, MA, USA) to inactivate unincorporated dNTPs and remove excess primer.

The purified PCR product was subjected to single-base extension of unlabelled oligonucleotide primers using the SnaPshot™ Multiplex Kit (ABI) under the thermal cycling conditions specified by the manufacturer. A further purification step with 0.5 U of CIP enzyme was run to inactivate unincorporated ddNTPs, by removing 5′ phosphoryl groups. Then, 0.5 µl of purified minisequencing product was mixed with 0.25 µl of the internal size standard LIZ120 (ABI) and 9.25 µl Hi-Di Formamide (ABI), and injected in an automated ABI 3130 5-colour sequencer (ABI). Raw data were processed using the GeneMapper ID software, v3.2.1 (ABI), which automatically assigned the allelic state to all SNPs.

Statistical analysis

The pairwise linkage disequilibrium (LD) between any two markers and the haplotype block structure was evaluated using the Haploview software (Reference Barrett, Fry, Maller and Daly35). Haplotye blocks (segments with strong LD) were defined according to validated criteria (Reference Gabriel, Schaffner and Nguyen36).

Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) was tested in the Inward and Outward groups, and within the whole population. For each marker, HWE was tested using the goodness-of-fit implemented in the PowerMarker software (Reference Liu and Muse37).

Associations between alleles, genotype and the two PMO phenotypes were tested by comparing allele and genotype frequency distributions in Inwards and Outwards with the chi-squared test and an exact test using PowerMarker. The software automatically selects the suitable exact test depending on sample numerousness. In particular it can choose between the Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) approach (Reference Guo and Thompson38,Reference Rousset and Raymond39), the permutation approach and the asymptotic chi-squared test. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CI 95%) were calculated to assess the relative risk conferred by a given allele or genotype. To assess the strength of the combined evidence from multiple tests for the association between the HTR2A gene and the PMOs, the significance level α of the tests was corrected for multiple comparisons. The software SNPSpD was used to generate the experiment-wide significance threshold required to keep Type I error below 5%. This approach consists in a simple correction for multiple testing of SNPs in LD with each other, on the basis of the spectral decomposition (SpD) of matrices of pairwise LD between SNPs (Reference Nyholt40).

Potential confounding effects due to differences in demographic characteristics (age and sex) between the PMO phenotypes may occur. Therefore, logistic regression analysis was performed to estimate the effect of HTR2A markers on PMO development adjusted for demographic covariates. A backward stepwise logistic regression method was conducted using the program PASW 17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), with the PMO phenotypes as the dependent variable, and the age, sex and alleles as the independent variables. For allele data, only the information of the minor allele from each SNP was entered in the regression model. Minor alleles were coded as 1, 0.5 or 0, respectively, for the minor allele homozygote, the heterozygote or the major allele homozygote.

Results

Ten HTR2A SNPs were genotyped to assess their association with the Inward/Outward PMO. Among these, nine SNPs were selected because they could severely affect one or more biological functions, i.e. transcription, protein structure and RNA splicing (Table 1). rs6312 is included, although it was not predicted to influence any biological process, because it has been reported to decrease promoter activity in combination with rs6311 (Reference Myers, Airey, Manier, Shelton and Sanders-Bush41).

Table 1 Polymorphisms predicted to be capable of affecting important biological processes

Transcription: SNP may enhance or undermine transcription, giving rise to a greater or smaller amount of pre-mRNA, respectively; Splicing: SNP may increase or decrease the amount of correctly processed mRNA; Protein: SNP may damage protein folding.

* rs6312 was included, although it was not predicted to influence any biological process, because it has been reported to decrease promoter activity in combination with rs6311.

In general, an SNP lying in the gene promoter region can strengthen, weaken or even create or destroy a DNA binding site for a transcription factor, therefore up or downregulate the transcription level of a gene, thus altering in this case, the amount of synthesised receptor. In Table 1, the three selected SNPs predicted to affect transcription process, according to rVISTA tool, are reported.

Generally, an SNP lying in a gene-coding region and causing an amino acid substitution could affect the protein structure by enhancing or reducing its ligand affinity. We selected one SNP using rBLOSUM64 matrix, the SIFT and PolyPhen, software able to predict possible protein structure changes.

Both exonic and intronic SNPs could affect the splicing process by altering 5′ and 3′ splice sites or regulative elements such as exonic splicing enhancers (ESEs), exonic splicing silencers (ESSs), intronic splicing enhancers (ISEs) or intronic splicing silencers (ISSs). We selected seven SNPs using NNSPLICE and SpliceAid, tools able to predict the alteration of splicing regulatory motifs.

Pairwise LD analysis in the 149 subjects showed that some markers lay in a haplotype block (Fig. 1). Markers rs1058576 and rs7997977 were not polymorphic in our sample and were excluded by LD calculation. rs6311, rs6312 and rs6313 were in complete LD (D′ = 1) and belong to the same haplotype block (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Pairwise LD between HTR2A markers in unrelated Italians subjects. (D′ = 1 in blank squares; numbers inside the grey squares are D′× 100%; white squares represent low D' and low r 2 values).

The genotype frequency distribution of the markers was in HWE, except for rs594242, which significantly departed from it in the whole sample and in the Outward group (p < 0.01), but was in HWE in the Inward group. The HWE test results are reported in Table 2.

Table 2 Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium test

The chi-squared test was used to assess compliance with the HWE. The SNP not in equilibrium is shown in bold.

The allele and genotype frequencies of the HTR2A SNPs are shown in Table 3. Allele, genotype and phenotype associations were tested by comparing allele and genotype frequency distributions in Inwards and Outwards using the chi-squared and exact tests. Allele and genotype frequencies in Inwards and Outwards were not significantly different except for marker rs55948462, whose genotypes differed significantly (p < 0.05) between Inwards and Outwards (chi-squared and exact p-values 0.029 and 0.022, respectively).

Table 3 Statistical analysis results

Allele and genotype occurrences (n) and frequencies (F), chi-squared and exact p-values in Inwards and Outwards. Significant results are in bold and underlined.

Analysis of Odd ratios (ORs) (Table 4) shows that the association of rs55948462 with PMO was due to the significantly greater number (2.5 times) of heterozygotes in Inwards compared with Outward subjects (OR = 2.500; CI (95%) = 1.16–5.38).

Table 4 OR results

ORs and CI 95% in Inwards and Outwards. Significant results are in bold and underlined. – : no subjects with this genotype (Table 3).

As significant differences in sex and age between Inward and Outward subjects were observed (p-values < 0.05 for both variables), we tested whether the association between HTR2A variants and PMO development was attributable to differences in age- and/or sex-related penetrance. Although our data might suggest an association between HTR2A polymorphisms and PMOs, the results of logistic regression analysis, performed to detect the effect of alleles adjusted for demographic covariates, showed that none of the allele considered significantly affected the probability to develop an Inward or an Outward PMO.

A further regression analysis, performed by grouping AG and GG genotypes of rs55948462 within the same group, showed that G allele increases the probability to develop an Inward PMO (β = 0.75), although with a poor significance (p = 0.09).

Taking into account that some of the SNPs were in LD and therefore not independent, the experiment-wide corrected significance threshold was p corr = 0.007. Although the rs55948462 SNP was associated with development of an Inward PMO, after adjustments it followed that these results did not remain significant.

Discussion

Knowledge of the genetic bases of PMO development would permit a greater understanding of the psychotherapeutic process and its epistemological paradigms. New technologies now make it possible to observe that valuable approaches, like post-rationalist constructivism, not only have solid theoretical and epistemological bases but are also supported by specific neural patterns (e.g. biological differences in emotional activation in Inward and Outward subjects) and by polymorphism studies. This can explain how attachment processes have both learned and genetic bases.

We report the first association study of 10 serotonin receptor 2A (HTR2A) gene SNPs in Inward and Outward PMOs. The approach used to select phenotype-associated SNPs was a typical case-control test, a valuable method to identify polymorphisms involved in the development of a particular phenotype.

HWE analyses showed that the genotype frequency distributions of all HTR2A tested markers were in HWE, except for rs594242, which remained in HWD in the whole population sample and in the Outward group. In general, Hardy-Weinberg disequilibrium (HWD) data can indicate a sound disease-locus association (Reference Luo, Kranzler, Zuo, Lappalainen, Yang and Gelernter42,Reference Nielsen, Ehm and Weir43), and in our study, it can suggest that rs594242 affects the development of Outward PMO. Deviation from HWE in a population sample could provide information on whether genotyping errors may have occurred at particular marker loci, but it also could be due to other genetic factors, e.g. protective alleles, genetic drift, population admixture, population stratification, inbreeding or deletions (Reference Hosking, Lumsden, Lewis, Yeo, McCarthy, Bansal, Riley, Purvis and Xu44) and can be related to de novo mutations and deletions in cases, or to benign polymorphisms that are in LD with the risk locus (Reference Leal45). In this work, pairwise LD analysis showed a moderate LD of rs594242 with the Block 1 found in the gene region (Fig. 1). Because deviations from HWE can be detected for polymorphisms that are in LD with the risk locus (Reference Leal45), the HWD at rs594242 found in the whole population sample and in the Outward group could indicate a gene affecting PMOs development lying in the region. However, the low r 2 value indicates that a larger sample could be required to achieve a similar power to detect association at this locus that would be afforded by a causal locus.

We found that rs55948462 SNP alone is associated with Inward/Outward PMOs development. This SNP lies in the second intron of the HTR2A gene. According to our predictions, the rs55948462 G allele can create an ectopic 5′ splice site. This change could cause an alternative splicing event, resulting in an additional receptor form, to the detriment of the more abundant one. This modified receptor structure could affect serotonin response, i.e. receptor sensitivity and selectivity. Given that the G allele frequency is higher than about 10% in all populations according to NCBI dbSNP, a pathological effect of this allele is not expected. In this study, we only have indications that G allele could affect the PMO development. Unfortunately in our sample, there are no GG Inwards so we are not able to show an association with this genotype. However, after correction for multiple comparisons and adjustment for age and sex using logistic regression, the association between HTR2A gene region and PMOs is lost.

HTR2A gene variants have been investigated in several gene association studies in relation to various psychiatric disorders, but their findings are conflicting and generally negative. The most consistent data point to the C allele of rs6313 as a risk factor for psychosis and as affecting the antipsychotic response. rs6311 and rs6313, also analysed in this study, are among the most widely investigated SNPs of the HTR2A gene, but we have not found to be associated with Inward/Outward PMO. Besides, we confirm the complete LD between rs6311 and rs6313 (D′ = 1) previously reported (Reference Ni, Bismil and Chan46,Reference Spurlock, Heils and Holmans47), and they belong to the same haplotype block (Block 1, Fig. 1). Moreover, rs55948462 belongs to no haplotype block and is not in LD with other SNPs.

However, these data do not exclude the involvement of serotonin receptor 2A (HTR2A) SNPs in Inward and Outward PMO development. In fact, it is well established that besides psychiatric disorders, also temperamental predisposition and behaviour are influenced by alterations in the serotonergic system (Reference Lucki48,Reference Mathew and Paulose49), and, in particular, with regard to behavioural traits, serotonin receptors are known to be involved in anxiety, impulsiveness and aggression (Reference Roth50). Furthermore, serotonin neurotransmission interferes with the endocrine stress reaction. Indeed, increased adrenocorticotropic hormone and corticosterone secretion have been described in rat limbic cortex following HTR2A-agonist administration (Reference Van de Kar, Javed, Zhang, Serres, Raap and Gray51). Clinical observations predict that the reaction will be different, depending on the Inward or Outward focus. Therefore, a polymorphism that can alter receptor specificity or efficiency (i.e. by reducing the firing rate) and thus the activation of neuroendocrine systems in stress situations can play a role in an individual's coping strategies. In other words, not only attachment patterns but also upstream polymorphism arrangement can induce different behavioural skills depending on the Inward or Outward PMO.

Unfortunately, there are no literature data on rs55948462 polymorphism. A recent work found an association between avoidant attachment and the HTR2A rs6313 (T102C) SNP (Reference Gillath, Shaver, Baek and Chun52), but our data do not show its association to PMO.

In conclusion, our study has potential limitations. One concern is reduced power because of the limited sample size. Also, the possible selection bias that might have been present in the academic-based, case-control study is a relevant issue. Finally, as our study was performed on Italian subjects, these gene SNPs may have significant effects in other ethnic populations. Therefore, rs55948462 should be further investigated to assess that the Inward and Outward PMOs have a genetic basis. Also the role of HTR2A polymorphisms not considered in this study and that of the other serotonin related genes should be valued.

References

1.Guidano, VF.Complexity of the self. New York: The Guilford Press, 1987.Google Scholar
2.Ainsworth, MDS, Blehar, MC, Waters, E, Wall, S.Patterns of attachment: assessed in the strange situation and at home. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978.Google Scholar
3.Bowlby, J.Attachment and loss: volume 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1982.Google Scholar
4.Bretherton, I.A communication perspective on attachment relationships and internal working models. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1995;60:310329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5.Arciero, G, Gaetano, P, Maselli, P, Gentili, N. Identity, personality and emotional regulation. In: Freeman, A, Mahoney, M, Devito, P, Martin, M, eds. Cognition and psychotherapy, 2nd edn. New York: Springer Publishing Company, 2004.Google Scholar
6.Arciero, G, Guidano, VF.Experience, explanation and the quest for coherence. Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 2000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7.Guidano, VF.The self in process. New York: The Guilford Press, 1991.Google Scholar
8.Nardi, B, Bellantuono, C.A new adaptive and evolutionary conceptualization of the Personal Meaning Organization (PMO) framework. Eur Psychother 2008;8:516.Google Scholar
9.Dobson, KS.Handbook of cognitive-behavioural therapies. London: Hutchinson, 1988.Google Scholar
10.Mahoney, MJ.Cognitive and constructive psychotherapies: theory, research and practice. New York: Springer, 1995.Google Scholar
11.Mahoney, MJ, Freeman, A.Cognition and psychotherapy. New York: Plenum Press, 1985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12.Neimeyer, RA, Mahoney, MJ.Constructivism in psychotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13.Bertolino, A, Arciero, G, Rubino, V et al. Variation of human amygdala response during threatening stimuli as a function of 5′HTTLPR genotype and personality style. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:15171525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.Rubino, V, Blasi, G, Latorre, V et al. Activity in medial prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of threatening stimuli as a function of personality style. Brain Res Bull 2007;74:250257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Nardi, B, Capecci, I, Fabri, M et al. fMRI investigation of emotional activations during visual processing of other or own facial expressions in subjects with inward or outward personality. Rivista di Psichiatria 2008;43:233241.Google Scholar
16.Giegling, I, Hartmann, AM, Moller, HJ, Rujescu, D.Anger- and aggression-related traits are associated with polymorphisms in the 5-HT-2A gene. J Affect Disord 2006;96:7581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Jokela, M, Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L, Kivimaki, M et al. Serotonin receptor 2A gene and the influence of childhood maternal nurturance on adulthood depressive symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64:356360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Unschuld, PG, Ising, M, Erhardt, A et al. Polymorphisms in the serotonin receptor gene HTR2A are associated with quantitative traits in panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2007;144B:424429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Celada, P, Puig, M, Amargos-Bosch, M, Adell, A, Artigas, F.The therapeutic role of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors in depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2004;29:252265.Google ScholarPubMed
20.Norton, N, Owen, MJ.HTR2A: association and expression studies in neuropsychiatric genetics. Ann Med 2005;37:121129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Serretti, A, Drago, A, de Ronchi, D.HTR2A gene variants and psychiatric disorders: a review of current literature and selection of SNPs for future studies. Curr Med Chem 2007;14:20532069.Google ScholarPubMed
22.Picardia, A.First steps in the assessment of cognitive-emotional organisation within the framework of Guidano's model of the self. Psychother Psychosom 2003;72:363365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23.Arimatea, E, Giovagnoli, S, Blasi, S, Bellantuono, C, Nardi, B.Il mini questionario sulla organizzazione personale (MQOP): studio preliminare di validazione. Quaderni di Psicoterapia Cognitiva 2009;25:178201.Google Scholar
24.Zhu, Y, Hoffman, A, Wu, X et al. Correlating observed odds ratios from lung cancer case-control studies to SNP functional scores predicted by bioinformatic tools. Mutat Res 2008;639:8088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Loots, GG, Ovcharenko, I.rVISTA 2.0: evolutionary analysis of transcription factor binding sites. Nucleic Acids Res 2004;32:W217W221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Matys, V, Fricke, E, Geffers, R et al. TRANSFAC: transcriptional regulation, from patterns to profiles. Nucleic Acids Res 2003;31:374378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Mao, L, Zheng, WJ.Combining comparative genomics with de novo motif discovery to identify human transcription factor DNA-binding motifs. BMC Bioinformatics 2006;7:S21S28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Blais, A, Tsikitis, M, Acosta-Alvear, D, Sharan, R, Kluger, Y, Dynlacht, BD.An initial blueprint for myogenic differentiation. Genes Dev 2005;19:553569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Ishii, K, Washio, T, Uechi, T, Yoshihama, M, Kenmochi, N, Tomita, M.Characteristics and clustering of human ribosomal protein genes. BMC Genomics 2006;7:3753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Styczynski, MP, Jensen, KL, Rigoutsos, I, Stephanopoulos, GBLOSUM62 miscalculations improve search performance. Nat Biotechnol 2008;26:274275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Ng, PC, Henikoff, S.Predicting deleterious amino acid substitutions. Genome Res 2001;11:863874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Ramensk, YV, Bork, P, Sunyaev, S.Human non-synonymous SNPs: server and survey. Nucleic Acids Res 2002;30:38943900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33.Reese, MG, Eeckman, FH, Kulp, D, Haussler, D.Improved splice site detection in Genie. J Comput Biol 1997;4:311323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Piva, F, Giulietti, M, Nocchi, L, Principato, G.SpliceAid: a database of experimental RNA target motifs bound by splicing proteins in humans. Bioinformatics 2009;25:12111213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Barrett, JC, Fry, B, Maller, J, Daly, MJ.Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics 2005;21:263265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Gabriel, SB, Schaffner, SF, Nguyen, H et al. The structure of haplotype blocks in the human genome. Science 2002;296:22252229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37.Liu, K, Muse, SV.PowerMarker: an integrated analysis environment for genetic marker analysis. Bioinformatics 2005;21:21282129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38.Guo, SW, Thompson, EA.A Monte Carlo method for combined segregation and linkage analysis. Am J Hum Genet 1992;51:11111126.Google ScholarPubMed
39.Rousset, F, Raymond, M.Testing heterozygote excess and deficiency. Genetics 1995;140:14131419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Nyholt, DR.A simple correction for multiple testing for single-nucleotide polymorphisms in linkage disequilibrium with each other. Am J Hum Genet 2004;74:765769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Myers, RL, Airey, DC, Manier, DH, Shelton, RC, Sanders-Bush, E.Polymorphisms in the regulatory region of the human serotonin 5-HT2A receptor gene (HTR2A) influence gene expression. Biol Psychiatry 2007;61:167173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Luo, X, Kranzler, HR, Zuo, L, Lappalainen, J, Yang, BZ, Gelernter, J.ADH4 gene variation is associated with alcohol dependence and drug dependence in European Americans: results from HWD tests and case-control association studies. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31:10851095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Nielsen, DM, Ehm, MG, Weir, BS.Detecting marker-disease association by testing for Hardy–Weinberg disequilibrium at a marker locus. Am J Hum Genet 1998;63:15311540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44.Hosking, L, Lumsden, S, Lewis, K, Yeo, A, McCarthy, L, Bansal, A, Riley, J, Purvis, I, Xu, CF.Detection of genotyping errors by Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium testing. Eur J Hum Genet 2004;12:395399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Leal, SM.Detection of genotyping errors and pseudo-SNPs via deviations from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. Genet Epidemiol 2005;29:204214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Ni, X, Bismil, R, Chan, K et al. Serotonin 2A receptor gene is associated with personality traits, but not to disorder, in patients with borderline personality disorder. Neurosci Lett 2006;408:214219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47.Spurlock, G, Heils, A, Holmans, P et al. A family based association study of T102C polymorphism in 5HT2A and schizophrenia plus identification of new polymorphisms in the promoter. Mol Psychiatry 1998;3:4249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Lucki, I.The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biol Psychiatry 1998;44:151162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Mathew, J, Paulose, CSThe healing power of well-being. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2011;23:145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Roth, BL.Multiple serotonin receptors: clinical and experimental aspects. Ann Clin Psychiatry 1994;6:6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.Van de Kar, LD, Javed, A, Zhang, Y, Serres, F, Raap, DK, Gray, TS.5-HT2A receptors stimulate ACTH, corticosterone, oxytocin, renin, and prolactin release and activate hypothalamic CRF and oxytocin-expressing cells. J Neurosci 2001;21:35723579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Gillath, O, Shaver, PR, Baek, JM, Chun, DS.Genetic correlates of adult attachment style. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2008;34:13961405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Polymorphisms predicted to be capable of affecting important biological processes

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Pairwise LD between HTR2A markers in unrelated Italians subjects. (D′ = 1 in blank squares; numbers inside the grey squares are D′× 100%; white squares represent low D' and low r2 values).

Figure 2

Table 2 Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium test

Figure 3

Table 3 Statistical analysis results

Figure 4

Table 4 OR results