Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T14:54:03.026Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interaction Effects of the COMT and DRD4 Genes with Anxiety-Related Traits on Selective Attention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2014

Margarita Alfimova*
Affiliation:
The Mental Health Research Centre of the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (Russia)
Galina Korovaitseva
Affiliation:
The Mental Health Research Centre of the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (Russia)
Tatiana Lezheiko
Affiliation:
The Mental Health Research Centre of the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (Russia)
Vera Golimbet
Affiliation:
The Mental Health Research Centre of the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (Russia)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Margarita V. Alfimova. Principal Investigator. Department of Clinical Genetics. Mental Health Research Centre RAMS. MHRC RAMS. Zagorodnoe sh.2, k.2. 117152. Moscow (Russia). Phone: +7–4991320062, +7–9169195348. E-mail: m.alfimova@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The study investigated whether the DRD4 and COMT genes can modify relations between trait anxiety and selective attention. Two hundreds and sixty-six subjects performed a visual search task in which they had to find words looking through a sheet with rows of letters. After finishing the first sheet the subject was presented the second one, this time with an instruction to perform the task as quickly and accurate as possible. To study top-down attention, the number of correctly identified words (accuracy) and the time for completion of each trial were analyzed. To study bottom-up attention, the letters ‘o’ and ‘n’ were written in green, whilst the others were in black, and subjects were asked whether they had noticed that 2–3 minutes after the task completion. Genotypes for the COMT Val158Met and DRD4 VNTR-48 polymorphisms and TCI Harm Avoidance and MMPI Depression scales’ scores were obtained as well. High anxious individuals showed a more pronounced increase in accuracy in the second trial and more profound processing of irrelevant stimuli (colored letters). There was a significant interaction effect of DRD4 and Harm avoidance on the accuracy dynamics F(1, 210), = 7.65, p = .006, η2 = .04. Among DRD4 long allele carriers, high anxious subjects significantly improved accuracy (p = .013) and tended to slow speed, while those with lower Harm avoidance demonstrated the opposite trend. These effects were more robust in less educated individuals. It was concluded that the DRD4 polymorphism may modify the influence of trait anxiety on the speed-accuracy tradeoff.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

There is a wealth of studies documenting anxiety effects on cognitive functions. Anxiety has been shown to be associated with a tendency to interpret ambiguous information in a mood-congruent manner, which in turn may produce biases in estimating the likelihood of future events and finally in decision making. Behind such a cascade of events lay anxiety effects on basic cognitive processes. In particular, the central executive component of working memory, especially its inhibition and shifting functions, and spreading activation in semantic memory have been proposed to play a key mediating role in relations between anxiety and the higher level cognitive processes (see Blanchette & Richards, Reference Blanchette and Richards2010; Derakshan & Eysenck, Reference Derakshan and Eysenck2010; Gable & Harmon-Jones, Reference Gable and Harmon-Jones2010 for reviews). According to the attentional control theory (ACT) and its antecedent - the processing efficiency theory (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007; Eysenck & Calvo, Reference Eysenck and Calvo1992), anxiety alters the balance between goal-directed (top-down) and stimulus-driven (bottom-up) attention in favor of the latter. It is also assumed that anxiety diminishes processing efficiency, i.e. increases brain costs of task performance, but may not influence performance effectiveness, if individuals allocate additional processing resources (effort) and use compensatory strategies to maintain a desirable level of performance. However, there is a lack of empirical data concerning a difference between state and trait anxiety impact on cognitive processes. It has been recently argued that trait anxiety is related to deficiencies in the executive control attentional network, while state anxiety is associated with hyperactivity of the alerting and orienting networks (Pacheco-Unguetti, Acosta, Callejas, & Lupianez, Reference Pacheco-Unguetti, Acosta, Callejas and Lupianez2010).

Consistent with ACT, neuroimaging studies demonstrated that high-anxious individuals had to invest more cognitive (compensatory) efforts in task performance which manifested itself as an increased task-related activation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Ansari & Derakshan, Reference Ansari and Derakshan2011; Basten, Stelzel, & Fiebach, Reference Basten, Stelzel and Fiebach2011; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Satpute, Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Satpute2005). At the same time, the reduced sustained activity of the PFC in high-anxious individuals was reported. This was interpreted as an aversive effect of trait anxiety on brain mechanisms of attentional control (Bishop, Reference Bishop2009; Fales et al., Reference Fales, Barch, Burgess, Schaefer, Mennin and Braver2008). It may be assumed that the discrepancies between studies are explained partly by baseline individual differences in prefrontal functioning. These differences depend, to a considerable degree, on genetic polymorphism of the dopaminergic system regulating PFC neuronal activity. In particular, prefrontal functions are influenced by polymorphism of the gene coding for catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT). This enzyme catalyzes dopamine degradation to 3-methoxytyramine thereby inactivating dopamine after its release in the synaptic cleft. COMT is thought to be particularly critical for regulating dopamine signaling in the PFC due to the scarcity of dopamine transporter in this region.

The human COMT gene is located on the chromosomal region 22q11.1-q11.2. Among polymorphic markers lying within this site, a polymorphism commonly known as Val158Met (rs4680), which corresponds to a G to A transition at nucleotide 472 in exon 4 of the COMT gene and results in a valine to methionine substitution at the 158 locus of the peptide sequence, has received the most attention. The substitution causes alterations in COMT enzymatic activity by influencing enzyme thermostability. The Met allele is associated with lower COMT activity and is therefore thought to lead to higher extrasynaptic dopamine levels in the PFC than the Val allele (Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Papolos, Saito, Yu, Szumlanski and Weinshilboum1996; Tunbridge, Harrison, & Weinberger, Reference Tunbridge, Harrison and Weinberger2006). There is evidence that the Met allele is related to superior performance in PFC-dependent tasks including measures of working memory and sustained attention and is associated with higher IQ (Barnett, Scoriels, & Munafo, Reference Barnett, Scoriels and Munafo2008; Blasi et al., Reference Blasi, Mattay, Bertolino, Elvevag, Callicott, Das and Weinberger2005; Egan et al., Reference Egan, Goldberg, Kolachana, Callicott, Mazzanti, Straub and Weinberger2001; Tunbridge et al., Reference Tunbridge, Harrison and Weinberger2006). Neuroimaging data indicate that Met-carriers exhibit lower PFC activity during cognitive tasks compared to Val-carriers, suggesting more efficient processing in the former group (Blasi et al., Reference Blasi, Mattay, Bertolino, Elvevag, Callicott, Das and Weinberger2005; Egan et al., Reference Egan, Goldberg, Kolachana, Callicott, Mazzanti, Straub and Weinberger2001).

A DRD4 gene polymorphism may represent another important factor of variability of dopaminergic signaling in the brain. D4 receptors are widespread in the PFC, including regions of the anterior cingulate cortex related to error-monitoring and inhibition control, as well as in some limbic structures involved in motivation. The DRD4 gene is located on the chromosomal region 11р15.5. A VNTR-polymorphism in exon 3 of the gene is represented by a variable number of 48-bp repeated sequences. Allelic variants with 2–11 imperfect copies of the repeat have been reported. In European populations, the most prevalent alleles are those with 4 (67%) and 7 (12%) repeats (Petronis, Van Tol, Lichter, Livak, & Kennedy, 1993). Shorter alleles (2–5 repeats) are considered to be more efficient with regard to transcription and DRD4-mediated inhibitory postsynaptic effects (Ebstein, Reference Ebstein2006). Association studies indicate that longer alleles (6 or more repeats), and the 7R allele in particular, are related to extraversion, novelty seeking, and impulsivity, i.e. to domain of approach-related traits, although a recent meta-analysis does not provide support for significant associations between the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism and this personality dimension (Munafo, Yalcin, Willis-Owe, & Flint, Reference Munafò, Yalcin, Willis-Owe and Flint2008). Moreover, there is evidence that the 7R allele is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Li, Sham, Owen, & He, Reference Li, Sham, Owen and He2006). It is therefore expected that carriers of the 7R allele compared to those without this allele should show greater cognitive impulsiveness manifesting as faster and less accurate responses in attention tasks (Kieling, Roman, Doyle, Hutz, & Rohde, Reference Kieling, Roman, Doyle, Hutz and Rohde2006; Langley et al., Reference Langley, Marshall, Van den Bree, Thomas, Owen, O'Donovan and Thapar2004).

The data reviewed above suggest that the COMT and DRD4 genes could modify anxiety impact on attentional control. We hypothesized that their influence would show itself in effects of interaction with anxiety-related traits on top-down and bottom-up attention during visual search.

Method

Participants

A total of 266 participants, aged between 16 and 67 years (M = 30.8 years; SD = 11.3; 157 females and 109 males, 184 subjects having or receiving higher education and 82 subjects with secondary education), took part in the study. All subjects were ethnically Russians from Moscow and the surrounding region. Exclusion criteria were psychiatric, neurologic and other serious medical conditions, and education less than 9 grades. Each subject gave written informed consent to take part in the study. The research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Mental Health Research Centre.

Anxiety and attention measures

Subjects were asked to donate biological samples for DNA extraction and to undergo examination which included assessment of cognition and personality (see Alfimova et al., Reference Alfimova, Abramova, Barhatova, Yumatova, Lyachenko and Golimbet2009 for more detailed description of the assessment battery). As part of the cognitive session, subjects completed a selective attention task, which was a modified Munsterberg test. In this test, the subject had to find and name words looking through sheets with rows of letters, initially at any desired rate and then at the highest possible one (Figure 1). In contrast to the original version of the task, each row contained a different number of words, and words’ length varied from 2 to 8 letters. The letters ‘o’ and ‘n’ were written in a dark-green color, whilst the others were in black. After finishing the first sheet (Trial 1), the subject was shown 2–3 words he/she had missed as a negative feedback. Then he/she was presented the second sheet with an instruction to perform the task as quickly and accurate as possible (Trial 2). Two-three minutes after completion of the second sheet, the subject was asked if he/she had noticed what colors the letters were. Where there was a correct answer, he/she was asked to remember which letters exactly were in green.

Figure 1. The modified version of Munsterberg’s test, in which subjects have to search for words among letters.

To assess top-down attention, we used the number of correctly identified words during each trial (Accuracy 1 and Accuracy 2) and the time for completion of each trial (Time 1 and Time 2). Errors were not analyzed as they were rare. To study bottom-up attention, answers about green letters were assessed in points (0 - did not notice the color, 1- noticed that some letters were green, 2 - named the color and one of the green letters, 3- named the color and both letters).

Anxiety related traits were assessed with the Harm Avoidance (HA) scale of Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory-125 (TCI-125) and the Depression scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). According to Cloninger, individuals who score higher on HA manifest anticipatory worry and pessimism, shyness with strangers, fear of uncertainty, fatigability and asthenia (versus Optimism, Confidence, and Vigor; Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, Reference Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic and Wetzel1994). Twenty hundreds and eighteen of the subjects completed the TCI-125. The MMPI Depression scale reflects symptoms of depression or anxiety depending on a whole MMPI-profile configuration. Its items concern a lack of confidence and pessimism (Berezin, Miroshnikov, & Rozhanec, Reference Berezin, Miroshnikov and Rozhanec1976). Valid MMPI data were available for 259 of the subjects.

DNA extraction and Genotyping. DNA was extracted from blood or mouthwash samples using phenol-chloroform method. The primer sequences for the COMT Val158Met polymorphism were: forward 5’-CTG ACA ACG GGT CAG GCA TG- 3’ and reverse 5’-CTG ACA ACG GGT CAG GCA TG- 3’. PCR reaction was carried out in a reaction volume of 15 μL containing 100 ng of genomic DNA, 10 pmoles of each primer, 0.5 U Taq polymerase (Helicon, Russia), 200 μM of each dNTP and 1M betaine hydrochloride. After an initial denaturation at 94°C for 2 min, amplification was performed as follows: 30 cycles of denaturation at 94°C for 30 sec, annealing at 57°C for 30 sec and extension at 72°C for 30 sec with a final extension for 4 min at 72°C. PCR products were cleaved by the restriction endonuclease Fat I (Sibenzyme, Russia) and separated on an 8%-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (PAGE). The resulting fragments were 87 and 22 bp for the Val allele and 69, 18 and 22 bp for the Met allele.The primer sequences for the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism were: forward 5'- CGACTACGTGGTCTACTCG -3’and reverse 5'- AGGACCCTCATGGCCTTG -3’. PCR conditions were the same as for the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. The reaction mixture was electrophoresed on an 8%-polyacrilamide gel with ethidium bromide to screen for alleles. Fragments representing different numbers of repeats (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9) were identified.

Statistical Analysis

Distribution of the accuracy and anxiety measures did not substantially deviate from normality. The time measures were skewed to the right and were subjected to square-root transformation. Then the anxiety and top-down attention measures were analyzed with parametric statistics (t-test for independent samples, Pearson’s correlations, ANCOVA). To test the study hypothesis, eight 2x2x2x2 repeated-measures ANCOVAs were conducted, where genotype, anxiety and education were the between-subjects factors, Trial was the within-subject factor, and age served as a covariate. Dependent variables were either Accuracy or Time. To run ANCOVAs, genotypes were grouped and encoded as follows: COMT ValVal (1) vs. at least one Met allele (2); at least one DRD4 L allele (6 or more repeats) (1) vs. SS (2). Genotype groups for both polymorphisms did not differ with regard to sex, age and education. Participants were divided into low-anxious and high-anxious individuals using a median split on the HA and Depression scales. Belonging to the lower anxiety group (HA < 11 scores; Depression < 49 Т-scores) was encoded as 1, and to the higher anxiety group as 2; secondary education was encoded as 1, higher education was encoded as 2. A separate ANCOVA was run for each gene and each anxiety measure. Where significant main or interaction effects of genes and anxiety took place, post hoc analyses were conducted using Newman-Keuls test.

The bottom-up attention, measured with ordering scale, was analyzed using nonparametric statistics (Mann–Whitney U-test, Spearman’s correlations). To assess influence of genotypes and anxiety on the bottom-up attention variable, logistic regressions were conducted. For the logistic regression analysis, the subjects were classified into those who did not identified color letters (0) and those who identified at least one of the letters (1). Age and education were entered in the equations as additional independent variables. Data analysis was performed using Statistica 6.0 for Windows.

Results

Allele and genotype frequencies

Genotyping was successful in 250 subjects for COMT and in 266 subjects for DRD4. The relative frequencies of the COMT ValVal, ValMet, and MetMet genotypes were .24, .52, and .24; the relative frequency of each allele was .5. The distribution of COMT genotypes did not deviate from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, χ 2 (1, N=250) = 0.4, p > .5. Among DRD4 alleles, the four-repeat allele was the most common (.72), followed by the seven-repeat (.16) allele. Frequencies of the other alleles were .7 for 2R, .3 for 3R, less than .2 for 5R, less than .1 for 6R, 8R, and 9R. These were similar to allele frequencies reported for other white samples.

Influence of demographic factors on attention and anxiety

The attention measures did not differ by sex. Accuracy was related to education, being higher in individuals with higher education (Accuracy 1, M = 17.09, SD = 4.43; Accuracy 2, M = 17.74, SD = 3.40) than in those with secondary education (Accuracy 1, M = 15.27, SD = 4.31; Accuracy 2, M = 16.30, SD = 3.50), t(264) = 3.12, р = .002, d = 0.41 and t(264) = 3.16, р = .002, d = 0.41 for Accuracy 1 and Accuracy 2 respectively. Time was positively correlated with age (r = .13, p = .030 and r = .14, p = .023 for Time 1 and Time 2 respectively), while the bottom-up attention measure was negatively correlated with age (r s = –.23, p = .001). Analysis of relations between the top-down and bottom-up attention measures revealed a negative correlation of bottom-up attention with Time 1 (r s = –.16, р = .009).

HA and Depression were correlated with each other (r = .45, p = .001), but were related differently to demographic variables. Women had higher НA scores (M = 10.38, SD = 4.34) compared to men (M = 8.72, SD = 4.61), t(216) = 2.65, р = .009, d = 0.37. Individuals with secondary education had higher Depression scores (M = 52, SD = 11) than those with higher education (M = 48, SD = 12), t(259) = 2.36, р = .019, d = 0.31. In addition, Depression scores were positively correlated with age (r = .21, p = .001). These data were taken into account when selecting independent variables for the ANCOVAs and logistic regression models.

Influence of genotypes and anxiety on top-down attention

We did not reveal any significant main effect of genotypes and anxiety on Accuracy and Time. However, there were significant interaction effects of Trial*HA, F(1, 210) = 10.43, р = .001, η2 = .05, Trial*HA*DRD4, F(1, 210) = 7.65, p = .006, η2 = .04, and Trial*HA*DRD4*education, F(1, 210) = 7.61, p = .006, η2 = .04, on Accuracy and that of Trial*DRD4* HA *education on Time, F(1, 210) = 10.14, p = .002, η2 = .05. Higher HA individuals were significantly more accurate in Trial 2 than in Trial 1 (р = .001). In this group, the accuracy increased by 15%, while in the lower HA group it did so by 5% only. This anxiety effect was dependent on the DRD4 genotype (fig. 2). DRD4 SS homozygotes showed a moderate increase in the accuracy (by 8–12%) from Trial 1 to Trial 2 regardless of HA level. Anxiety affected accuracy changes in the presence of the DRD4 L allele (р = .013). A marked increase in the accuracy (by 23%) was seen in higher HA carriers of the L allele; in the lower HA*DRD4 L group accuracy remained constant (0%). Moreover, the high anxious carriers of the DRD4 L allele showed a trend toward lengthening of scanning time (by 18%), while the other groups tended to reduce it (Trial*HA*DRD4 interaction effect on Time, F(1, 210) = 3.73, p = .055, η2 = .02).

Figure 2. Dynamics of performance accuracy (Munsterberg’s test) as a function of DRD4 VNTR genotype and TCI Harm Avoidance. HA – Harm Avoidance. DRD4 SS – genotypes with two short alleles (2–5 repeats), DRD4 L+ - genotypes with at least one long allele (6–9 repeats). Trial 1 – results for the first sheet, Trial 2- results for the second sheet under instruction to perform the task as quickly and accurate as possible.

Post hoc analysis of the four-way interaction effect (Trial*HA*DRD4*education) on Accuracy revealed a significant increase in the accuracy from Trial 1 to Trial 2 (р = .0002) in the group of higher HA carriers of the DRD4 L alleles with secondary education. These individuals differed from the others significantly or at a trend level on the number of words they identified during the first trial. As a whole, they showed the low initial accuracy and the shortest initial scanning time and then improved their performance by 52% by means of extending the scanning period by 70%. However, they did not approach the accuracy level of the most successful groups (Table 1). In contrast, in the group of individuals with the same genotype and education level but lower anxiety both the accuracy and time decreased (by 13% and 23% respectively). It should be noted that in the post hoc analysis differences between groups on time changes did not reach statistical significance.

Table 1. Means (SD) of top-down attention measures as a function of DRD4 genotype, TCI Harm Avoidance and education

Note: Sec – secondary education, High – higher education, S – DRD4 alleles with 2–5 repeats, L – DRD4 alleles with 6–9 repeats; negative values mean a decrease in accuracy or time; accuracy and time changes from the first trial to the second one were initially calculated for each individual as follows: ∆accuracy =(accuracy 2– accuracy 1)/ accuracy 1 *100% and ∆time= (time 2– time 1)/ time 1 *100%, and then were averaged for each group.

Influence of genotypes and anxiety on bottom-up attention

The logistic regression analysis revealed a significant effect of HA, measured as a continuous variable, on bottom-up attention, β = –0.07 (SE = 0.03), Wald (1) = 4.13, p = .042, while controlling for effects of age, sex and education. A higher HA was related to an increased likelihood of letter identification. A similar trend took place when HA was entered in the regression model as a dichotomous variable, β = 0.31 (SE = 0.18), Wald (1) = 3.07, p = .080. The lower HA group was divided approximately equally on those who identified at least one color letter and those who did not identified any letter (49 and 51%). In the higher HA group, percentage of those who identified at least one color letter was higher (63%). Controlling for age and education we did not reveal significant effects of genotypes or genotype*anxiety interactions on bottom-up attention.

Discussion

The main results of the present investigation can be summarized as follows. Trait anxiety, measured with the TCI Harm Avoidance scale, impacted both top-down and bottom-up attention. A higher HA level was related to the more prominent response to instruction to perform the task as accurate as possible. In addition, higher anxiety was linked to a trend toward more profound processing of irrelevant stimuli. The DRD4 gene modified the HA effect solely on the top-down attention. The anxiety effect was largest in carriers of the DRD4 long allele. In this group, higher HA was associated with a marked increase in performance accuracy from the first trial to the second one, while in case of lower anxiety accuracy remained practically constant. Furthermore, this effect was dependent on the education level with the largest HA effect on the accuracy increment taking place in the group of the DRD4 long allele carriers with secondary education. According to the widely accepted interpretation (e.g., Ansari & Derakshan, Reference Ansari and Derakshan2011), accuracy reflects performance effectiveness, while time to complete the task may represent an indirect indicator of efficiency. Thus, our findings are in line with ACT statements (Eysenck et al., Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007), suggesting that under certain conditions high trait anxiety: 1) causes more profound processing of irrelevant stimuli increasing the influence of stimulus-driven attention system and 2) promotes the investment of additional resources to performance effectiveness.

It should be stressed that although the DRD4*anxiety interaction effects were significant solely for accuracy, the whole pattern of results suggests that this interaction may influence changes in accuracy-speed tradeoff caused by motivation to perform the task as quickly and accurate as possible. Carriers of the DRD4 long allele with higher anxiety demonstrated not only an accuracy increment but also lengthening of the scanning period, whereas those with lower anxiety showed an accuracy decrease and shortening of the scanning period. It has been previously suggested that when motivated people react by increasing the amount of effort or resources they expend to perform the task; these efforts may be translated into accuracy or speed. In most cases, one has to find balance between speed and accuracy to maximize reward outcomes, unless circumstances put absolute importance on the one of these response parameters (Gold & Shadlen, Reference Gold and Shadlen2002). The default setting seems to be that people become faster without sacrificing accuracy (Bijleveld, Custers, & Aarts, Reference Bijleveld, Custers and Aarts2010). This pattern of changes indeed took place in most of the groups in our investigation. However, our findings suggest that speed–accuracy tradeoffs are dependent on interactions of a number of factors including education level, HA and DRD4 genotypes. Education level presumably reflects amount of cognitive reserve, i.e. internal (neuronal) processing resources, that an individual can invest in performance to improve its effectiveness (Stern, Reference Stern2002). Secondary education level thus may indicate limited cognitive reserve. So in less educated people, a response to motivation might manifest itself behaviorally in the form of changes of searching time. Higher anxiety led to an increase in accuracy at the expense of a longer scanning (efficiency) in our study. Such a relationship has been previously interpreted as a sign that high anxiety leads to enhanced motivation to be more accurate and to invest additional resources to task performance (Ansari & Derakshan, Reference Ansari and Derakshan2011; Richards, French, Keogh, & Carter, Reference Richards, French, Keogh and Carter2000).

Two hypotheses may represent the framework for considering mechanisms whereby the interaction of the DRD4 long allele and trait anxiety influences attention. The first one postulates that the gene is associated with extraversion, novelty seeking, and impulsivity in such a way that the DRD4 L allele leads to faster and less accurate reactions. Within this framework our results suggest that the long allele effects resulting in impulsive behavior and hyperactivity of behavioral activation system are evident only in low anxious individuals, while higher anxiety, an indicator of hyperactivity of behavioral inhibition system, counterbalances the long allele influence.

Alternatively, our findings may be considered in the framework of another hypothesis, according to which rather than predisposing to development of certain traits (novelty seeking, impulsivity) the DRD4 long allele is associated with greater responsiveness to environment, thereby providing high developmental and behavioral plasticity (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Rende, Reference Rende2012). This hypothesis has emerged from investigations of DRD4 interactions with exogenous environmental factors, but it can apparently be extended to endogenous environment including trait anxiety. Previously Schmidt, Fox, Perez-Edgar, and Hamer (Reference Schmidt, Fox, Perez-Edgar and Hamer2009) investigated whether the interaction of DRD4 with such endogenous environmental factor as frontal brain EEG asymmetry predicted child temperament. They found that the EEG asymmetry influenced temperament only in the presence of the DRD4 long allele. Among the long allele carriers, the right frontal asymmetry presumably reflecting vulnerability to negative affect predicted poorer ability to focus and sustain attention as well as lower soothability scores. Thus, the results of Schmidt et al. (Reference Schmidt, Fox, Perez-Edgar and Hamer2009) indicate that the long allele compared to the short one is more sensitive to brain characteristics underlying differences in emotionality, and this is in line with our findings.

The interactions between HA, DRD4 and education revealed in the present study may account for contradictory data concerning detrimental effects of high anxiety and the DRD4 long allele on attention, which were obtained when anxiety and the gene were investigated separately. That is, some authors reported that in healthy individuals and in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, the DRD4 7R allele was associated with less accurate and faster reactions while others found the opposite pattern (Congdon, Lesch, & Canli, Reference Congdon, Lesch and Canli2008; Kebir, Tabbane, Sengupta, & Joober, Reference Kebir, Tabbane, Sengupta and Joober2009; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Rojo, Schule, Cunillera, Schols, Marco-Pallares and Munte2009; Szekely et al., Reference Szekely, Balota, Duchek, Nemoda, Vereczkei and Sasvari-Szekely2011). Data on speed-accuracy tradeoffs in individuals with high trait anxiety are also inconsistent. There is evidence that trait anxiety does not impact accuracy or enhances it, but slows reaction time down, suggesting the negative effect of anxiety on performance efficiency (Coombes, Higgins, Gamble, Cauraugh, & Janelle, Reference Coombes, Higgins, Gamble, Cauraugh and Janelle2009; Sadeh & Bredemeier, Reference Sadeh and Bredemeier2011). In contrast, others have found that individuals with high neuroticism (an anxiety-related trait) compared to those with low neuroticism are characterized by lower performance effectiveness preferring speed to accuracy (Flehmig, Steinborn, Westhoff, & Langner, Reference Flehmig, Steinborn, Westhoff and Langner2010).

As mentioned above, our findings are consistent with the ACT hypothesis that anxiety favors bottom-up attention enhancing processing of irrelevant distractors (Eysenck et al. Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007; Sadeh & Bredemeier, Reference Sadeh and Bredemeier2011). However, the DRD4 and COMT genes seem not to modify this influence. It is possible that the lack of dopaminergic genes effect on the balance between top-down and bottom-up attention in the present study is accounted for by simplicity of the task used or distractor characteristics which do not imply intense control of central executive component on interference. It is also possible that bottom-up attention is modified mostly by genes affecting other neurotransmission systems. For instance, cholinergic genes were shown to be associated with such domains of visual attention as orienting and spatial scaling without influencing working memory, whereas for genes modulating dopaminergic neurotransmission the opposite pattern of results was obtained (Greenwood, Lin, Sundararajan, Fryxell, & Parasuraman, Reference Greenwood, Lin, Sundararajan, Fryxell and Parasuraman2009; Parasuraman, Greenwood, Kumar, & Fossella, Reference Parasuraman, Greenwood, Kumar and Fossella2005). It is important to note that in the present study the enhanced processing of colored letter did not result from longer scanning; on the contrary, it was correlated with a shorter searching period.

At present, a great diversity of data concerning nature and power of COMT Val158Met polymorphism effects on cognition exists. This diversity may be accounted for by variation in baseline levels of prefrontal dopamine, sex dimorphism and COMT impact on both executive functions and emotion regulation. In particular, some authors related the Met allele with enhanced emotional reactivity and anxiety (Eley et al., Reference Eley, Tahir, Angleitner, Harriss, McClay, Plomin and Craig2003; Enoch, Xu, Ferro, Harris, & Goldman, Reference Enoch, Xu, Ferro, Harris and Goldman2003; Yeh et al., Reference Yeh, Chang, Hu, Yeh and Lin2009). On this basis, it was suggested that the COMT polymorphism could partly explain individual differences in anxiety effects on cognitive processes implicated in working memory and decision-making (Goldman, Oroszi, & Ducci, Reference Goldman, Oroszi and Ducci2005; Parasuraman & Jiang, Reference Parasuraman and Jiang2012; Posner & Rothbart, Reference Posner and Rothbart2009). However, like several other researchers, (Baekken, Skorpen, Stordal, Zwart, & Hagen, Reference Baekken, Skorpen, Stordal, Zwart and Hagen2008; Barnett, Scoriels, & Munafo, Reference Barnett, Scoriels and Munafo2008; Wray et al., Reference Wray, James, Dumenil, Handoko, Lind, Montgomery and Martin2008), we did not reveal associations between COMT and either anxiety-related traits or cognitive variables and did not find effects of COMT*anxiety interaction on attention. This negative result may reflect a true lack of COMT effects on relations between anxiety and attention. Alternatively, behavioral level of analysis may be insufficient for recovering a role of COMT in task performance. Existing data (Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Need, LaBar, Waters-Metenier, Cirulli, Kragel and Cabeza2010) suggest that influence of COMT on cognitive processes and its relation to anxiety manifest themselves at the level of brain activity and are not observable at the behavioral level.

Given high probability of type 1 and type 2 errors in association studies, present positive and negative results should be interpreted with caution. Further, large values of standard deviations of the accuracy and time measures deserve consideration as they point to a high variability in magnitude and directions of the dynamics within groups. This variability suggests that beside anxiety, education and DRD4, many other factors may influence speed-accuracy tradeoffs.

In sum, the findings are in accordance with some ACT statements regarding anxiety influence on top-down and bottom-up attention. In addition, they suggest that individual differences in trait anxiety effects on top-down attention are related to the DRD4 genotypes and education level.

References

Alfimova, M. V., Abramova, L. I., Barhatova, A. I., Yumatova, P. E., Lyachenko, G. L., & Golimbet, V. E. (2009). Facial affect recognition deficit as a marker of genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12, 4655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ansari, T. L., & Derakshan, N. (2011). The neural correlates of cognitive effort in anxiety: Effects on processing efficiency. Biological Psychology, 86, 337348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baekken, P. M., Skorpen, F., Stordal, E., Zwart, J. A., & Hagen, K. (2008). Depression and anxiety in relation to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype in the general population: the Nord-Trondelag Health Study (HUNT). BMC Psychiatry, 8, 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-8-48 Google Scholar
Barnett, J. H., Scoriels, L., & Munafo, M. R. (2008). Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 137144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basten, U., Stelzel, C., & Fiebach, C. J. (2011). Trait anxiety modulates the neural efficiency of inhibitory control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 31323145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00003 Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., & Williams, R. (2009). Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 746754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.44 Google Scholar
Berezin, F. B., Miroshnikov, M. P., & Rozhanec, R. V. (1976). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory in clinical medicine and psychohygiene. Moscow, Russia: Medicine.Google Scholar
Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). Unconscious reward cues increase invested effort, but do not change speed–accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition, 115, 330335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 9298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2242 Google Scholar
Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgment, decision making and reasoning. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 561595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930903132496 Google Scholar
Blasi, G., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., Elvevag, B., Callicott, J. H., Das, S., … Weinberger, D. R. (2005). Effect of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype on attentional control. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 50385045. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0476-05.2005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Wetzel, R. D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality.Google Scholar
Congdon, M. A. E., Lesch, K. P., & Canli, T. (2008). Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: Implications for impulsivity. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 2732. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30557 Google Scholar
Coombes, S. A., Higgins, T., Gamble, K. M., Cauraugh, J. H., & Janelle, C. M. (2009). Attentional control theory: Anxiety, emotion, and motor planning. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 10721079. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.009 Google Scholar
Dennis, N. A., Need, A. C., LaBar, K. S., Waters-Metenier, S., Cirulli, E. T., Kragel, J., … Cabeza, R. (2010). COMT Val 108/158 Met genotype affects neural but not cognitive processing in healthy individuals. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 672683. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp132 Google Scholar
Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2010). Introduction to the special issue: Emotional states, attention, and working memory. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 189199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930903412120 Google Scholar
Ebstein, R. P. (2006). The molecular genetic architecture of human personality: Beyond self-report questionnaires. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 427445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001814 Google Scholar
Egan, M. F., Goldberg, T. E., Kolachana, B. S., Callicott, J. H., Mazzanti, C. M., Straub, R. E., … Weinberger, D. R. (2001). Effect of COMT Val108/158met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 69176922. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.111134598 Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 169181. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.2.169 Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Tahir, E., Angleitner, A., Harriss, K., McClay, J., Plomin, R., … Craig, I. (2003). Association analysis of MAOA and COMT with neuroticism assessed by peers. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 120B, 9096. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.20046 Google Scholar
Enoch, M. A., Xu, K., Ferro, E., Harris, C. R., & Goldman, D. (2003). Genetic origins of anxiety in women: A role for a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism. Psychiatric Genetics, 13, 3341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00041444-200303000-00006 Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition & Emotion, 6, 409434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336353. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 Google Scholar
Fales, C. L., Barch, D. M., Burgess, G. C., Schaefer, A., Mennin, D. S., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: Differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 239253. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/CABN.8.3.239 Google Scholar
Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M. B., Westhoff, K., & Langner, R. (2010). Neuroticism and speed-accuracy tradeoff in self-paced speeded mental addition and comparison. Journal of Individual Differences, 31, 130137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000021 Google Scholar
Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 322337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930903378305 Google Scholar
Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2002). Banburismus and the brain: Decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward. Neuron, 36, 299308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00971-6 Google Scholar
Goldman, D., Oroszi, G., & Ducci, F. M. (2005). The genetics of addictions: Uncovering the genes. Nature Review Genetics, 6, 521532. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrg1635 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, P. M., Lin, M.-K., Sundararajan, R., Fryxell, K. J., & Parasuraman, R. (2009). Synergistic effects of genetic variation in nicotinic and muscarinic receptors on visual attention but not working memory. The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 36333638. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0807891106 Google Scholar
Kebir, O., Tabbane, K., Sengupta, S., & Joober, R. (2009). Candidate genes and neuropsychological phenotypes in children with ADHD: Review of association studies. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 34, 88101.Google Scholar
Kieling, C., Roman, T., Doyle, A. E., Hutz, M. H., & Rohde, L. A. (2006). Association between DRD4 gene and performance of children with ADHD in a test of sustained attention. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 11631165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.027 Google Scholar
Kramer, U. M., Rojo, N., Schule, R., Cunillera, T., Schols, L., Marco-Pallares, J., … Munte, T. F. (2009). ADHD candidate gene (DRD4 exon III) affects inhibitory control in a healthy sample. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-150 Google Scholar
Lachman, H. M., Papolos, D. F., Saito, T., Yu, Y. M., Szumlanski, C. L., & Weinshilboum, R. M. (1996). Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: Description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics, 6, 243250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00008571-199606000-00007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langley, K., Marshall, L., Van den Bree, M., Thomas, H., Owen, M., O'Donovan, M., & Thapar, A. (2004). Association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene 7-repeat allele with neuropsychological test performance of children with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 133138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.1.133 Google Scholar
Li, D., Sham, P. C., Owen, M. J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Human Molecular Genetics, 15, 22762284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddl152 Google Scholar
Munafò, M. R., Yalcin, B., Willis-Owe, S. A., & Flint, J. (2008). Association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene and approach-related personality traits: Meta-analysis and new data. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 197206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.006 Google Scholar
Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupianez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety: Different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359624 Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M., Kumar, R., & Fossella, J. (2005). Beyond heritability: Neurotransmitter genes differentially modulate visuospatial attention and working memory. Psychological Science, 16, 200207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00804.x Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R., & Jiang, Y. (2012). Individual differences in cognition, affect, and performance: Behavioral, neuroimaging, and molecular genetic approaches. NeuroImage, 59, 7082. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.040 Google Scholar
Petronis, A., Van Tol, H. H., Lichter, J. B., Livak, K. J., & Kennedy, J. L. (1993). The D4 dopamine receptor gene maps on 11p proximal to HRAS. Genomics, 18, 161163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/geno.1993.1445 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2009). Toward a physical basis of attention and self regulation. Physics of Life Reviews, 6, 103120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2009.02.001 Google Scholar
Rende, R. (2012). Behavioral resilience in the post-genomic era: Emerging models linking genes with environment. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 50. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00050 Google Scholar
Richards, A., French, C. C., Keogh, E., & Carter, C. (2000). Test anxiety, inferential reasoning and working memory load. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 13, 87109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615800008248335 Google Scholar
Sadeh, N., & Bredemeier, K. (2011). Individual differences at high perceptual load: The relation between trait anxiety and selective attention. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 747755. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.500566 Google Scholar
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., & Hamer, D. H. (2009). Linking gene, brain, and behavior: DRD4, frontal asymmetry, and temperament. Psychological Science, 20, 831837. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02374.x Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702813248 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szekely, A., Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., Nemoda, Z., Vereczkei, A., & Sasvari-Szekely, M. (2011). Genetic factors of reaction time performance: DRD4 7-repeat allele associated with slower responses. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 129136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00645.x Google Scholar
Tunbridge, E. M., Harrison, P. J., &Weinberger, D. R. (2006). Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 141151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.024 Google Scholar
Wray, N. R., James, M. R., Dumenil, T., Handoko, H. Y., Lind, P. A., Montgomery, G. W., & Martin, N. G. (2008). Association study of candidate variants of COMT with neuroticism, anxiety and depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B, 13141318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30744 Google Scholar
Yeh, T. K., Chang, C. Y., Hu, C. Y., Yeh, T. C., & Lin, M. Y. (2009). Association of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) polymorphism and academic achievement in a Chinese cohort. Brain & Cognition, 71, 300305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.011 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The modified version of Munsterberg’s test, in which subjects have to search for words among letters.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Dynamics of performance accuracy (Munsterberg’s test) as a function of DRD4 VNTR genotype and TCI Harm Avoidance. HA – Harm Avoidance. DRD4 SS – genotypes with two short alleles (2–5 repeats), DRD4 L+ - genotypes with at least one long allele (6–9 repeats). Trial 1 – results for the first sheet, Trial 2- results for the second sheet under instruction to perform the task as quickly and accurate as possible.

Figure 2

Table 1. Means (SD) of top-down attention measures as a function of DRD4 genotype, TCI Harm Avoidance and education