Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:43:24.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Enjoying flow in primary school music making: does family musical practice matter?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2020

Rafael Valenzuela
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Nuria Codina*
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
José Vicente Pestana
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
*
Corresponding author. Email: ncodina@ub.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Given the differences in continuing musical after primary school, this study works with the idea that the development of music-making habits requires flow, which, in turn, depends on family musical practice. Flow is the state of mind of immersion in an intrinsically rewarding activity, which represents an optimal challenge fostering persistence and skill growth. Based on 164 students (82 girls) from six schools in Catalonia, we compared flow in primary school musical practice between students with high and low levels of family musical practice, concluding that family context should be considered in adapting musical activities to students’ previous experiences.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2020

Introduction

Musical practice has been part of every culture (Brown, Merker, & Wallin, Reference BROWN, MERKER, WALLIN, Brown, Merker and Wallin2000) and can thus be considered the quintessential human activity (Fautley, Reference FAUTLEY2017). Although most school students participate in music making (Valenzuela & Codina, Reference VALENZUELA and CODINA2014; Valenzuela, Codina, & Pestana, Reference VALENZUELA, CODINA and PESTANA2018), few adults enjoy an active musical practice (European Music Council, 2011). Consequently, inclusion needs problematisation, as music education can contain exclusory practices, especially for non-western non-instrumental musical expressions (Fautley & Daubney, Reference FAUTLEY and DAUBNEY2018).

In this regard, family context may condition individuals’ musical practices, given that an early start in learning music provides more time to incorporate cultural capital (knowledge and skills indispensable for cultural practice: Bourdieu, Reference BOURDIEU, Karable and Halsey1977, Reference BOURDIEU1990). Early acquisition of cultural capital facilitates faster and easier access to more of the same type of knowledge and skills. As a result, people tend to reproduce patterns of cultural participation previously present in their families (cultural reproduction) – even though sometimes they incorporate new cultural practices (cultural mobility: Bourdieu & Passeron, Reference BOURDIEU and PASSERON1990). Consequently, having close relatives who practise music increases the chances of early access to music making and may provide advantages in facing the demands of primary school music education.

Family context conditions the incorporation of beliefs, preferences, knowledge and even musical abilities (Ilari, Moura, & Bourscheidt, Reference ILARI, MOURA and BOURSCHEIDT2011), which promotes intrinsic motivation in learning music: the autonomous enjoyment or interest in the activity per se (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011). Intrinsic motivation is critical for learning music and is positively associated with family musical practice, interest in music and willingness to practice (Valenzuela & Codina, Reference VALENZUELA and CODINA2014). Families with greater musical practice usually provide their younger members with private training – compatible with the standards of dominant institutions in the field of music – in stages prior to regulated education (Burnard, Reference BURNARD2012).

These provisions favour the development of a musical sense of self (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011) and a sense of agency in learning music (Wiggins, Reference WIGGINS2011), which may be advantageous for children when facing the demands of school musical practice – given that they may consider success in music making likely and achievable, facilitating feelings of entitlement (Moore, Reference MOORE2012). By contrast, intrinsic motivation deficits are associated with anxiety – observed in many music students – which may lead to withdrawing from music studies (Osborne & Kenny, Reference OSBORNE and KENNY2005; Wristen, Reference WRISTEN2013).

Given these antecedents, educational institutions have been criticised for establishing homogeneous teaching policies (Törnqvist, Reference TÖRNQVIST2004; Johansen, Reference JOHANSEN2009). Additionally, given that the subject area receives subpar validation within a greater education framework, in Spain (Rodríguez-Quiles, Reference RODRÍGUEZ-QUILES2017) and other countries, gaps have been observed in music teacher training, and these are sometimes compensated for only by teachers’ individual creativity and efforts (Rusinek, Reference RUSINEK2004).

Based on the aforementioned, it is essential to establish if the development of a musical sense of self and agency in learning music form the foundations for enjoyment and perseverance in musical practice. If so, family context could condition this enjoyment. In order to investigate this issue, the construct of flow is useful (Custodero, Reference CUSTODERO2002, Reference CUSTODERO2005), given that it is characterised by high levels of intrinsic motivation and absorption (Bakker, Reference BAKKER2005), variables that can be theoretically related to the senses of self and agency in musical practice.

Flow is a psychological state of satisfaction, which results from success in an intrinsically motivating activity that represents a balanced level of demand with the individual’s present skill level (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference CSIKSZENTMIHALYI1988). Activities that are too difficult generate frustration, those that are too easy lead to boredom; enjoyment and the desire to persevere are found in the relative balance between skills and demands. Moreover, flow triggers learning, given that one same task does not produce equally intense experiences twice, and task demands need to increase gradually to maintain optimal challenge, motivation and enjoyment. In this way, improvements in practice generate a virtuous cycle, leading to the consolidation of participation habits (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference CSIKSZENTMIHALYI1991).

The three definitory aspects of flow are intrinsic motivation (practising for the task’s intrinsic value), absorption (being immersed and ‘forgetting everything else’) and enjoyment (enjoying a deep sense of reward) (Rhodes & Kates, Reference RHODES and KATES2015; Swann et al., Reference SWANN, PIGGOTT, SCHWEICKLE and VELLA2018). These aspects reveal the relationships between a musical sense of self, comprising positive attitudes and dispositions towards music, and intrinsic motivation (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011), and between a sense of agency in learning music and absorption (Wiggins, Reference WIGGINS2011), which is closely related with flow in music making (Amado et al., Reference AMADO, SÁNCHEZ-MIGUEL, LEO, SÁNCHEZ-OLIVA and GARCÍA-CALVO2013). Additionally, a robust sense of agency in learning music allows an independent assessment of one’s own skills and the selection of tasks with optimal demand levels (Keller & Landhäußer, Reference KELLER, LANDHÄUßER and Engeser2012) thus facilitating concentration, absorption and flow (Bakker, Reference BAKKER2005).

In the hypothetical case that flow in primary school music-making activities was equivalent between students from families with, respectively, high and low levels of musical practice, the effectiveness of primary school music education could be considered independent of family factors and could contribute to universal access to musical practice. By contrast, if flow were significantly different depending on family musical background (even in the case that all students had access to optimal music education conditions, teacher training, resources and infrastructure), it would be indispensable to consider family context in the pedagogical design.

Taking into account these antecedents, the following question was asked: is flow in primary school musical practice equivalent between students with and without relatives who practice music? In answering this question, socio-economic profile is considered as control variable given that cultural capital and socio-economics appear to be linked to the above-mentioned other variables (Bourdieu & Passeron, Reference BOURDIEU and PASSERON1990; Fautley & Daubney, Reference FAUTLEY and DAUBNEY2018).

Method

Participants

Participants were students from six primary schools which participated in a high-quality music education programme in Catalonia (Spain). The programme included complementary training for teachers and pedagogical and logistical coordination, so that, during one year in music class, children learned music through the songs of a 45-min long musical piece composed ad hoc for children’s choir and professional orchestra. The experience gave each student the opportunity to feel like a professional musician, appearing in front of an audience of relatives in a professional auditorium, singing the songs learned in school in a choir accompanied by symphonic orchestra musicians. The programme sought to be inclusive; thus, it was based on singing (a bodily and intuitive activity), rather than on instrumental practice. Furthermore, it considered that singing in a choir, rather than individually, would trigger less performance anxiety. Catalonia has a longstanding vocal tradition, where choirs and vocal ensembles are present in many towns and cities. Thus, we considered this programme a best-case scenario and – if differences in enjoyment were found between children due to family contexts – these differences would have been expected to be greater in other less-optimal pedagogical settings.

Consequently, an intentional sampling method was aimed at ensuring participation of students experiencing flow. Six schools were selected, according to the programme director’s indications regarding the best-performing teachers and those students (and relatives) with highest satisfaction with the programme. Participants completed anonymous questionnaires in their music class, as part of the final activity evaluation (coordinated between the programme and the schools’ principals).

The ethical requirements of the Ethics Committee of the University of Barcelona (University of Barcelona’s Bioethics Commission, CBUB – Institutional Review Board IRB00003099) were applied to the current study, which meant that additional approval for the research was not required because the data obtained did not involve animal or clinical experimentation. Additionally, this study complies with the recommendations of the General Council of Spanish Psychological Associations (Consejo General de Colegios de Psicólogos), the Spanish Organic Law on Data Protection (15/1999: Jefatura del Estado, 1999) and the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013).

Of the six selected schools, from diverse districts in Barcelona, one had a low socio-economic profile, being located in a poor neighbourhood, and included students stemming from socio-economically disadvantaged families (one classroom, n = 21); another school had a very high socio-economic profile, was located in the richest part of town, and included students from wealthy families (two classrooms, n = 46) and the other four schools had a middle-class socio-economic profile (four classrooms, n = 93).

Of all participants (N = 164), 67.7% were 5th graders and 32.3% were 6th graders and 49.4% were boys and 50.6% were girls. Their ages (M = 11.37; SD = 0.53) ranged from 11 to 13 years: 11 (65.8%), 12 (31.7%), and 13 (2.50%).

Measures

Given their practical usefulness for assessing optimal participation experiences within limited time frames, short scales have been created to evaluate flow reliably in several activities, including school musical practice (Jackson, Martin, & Eklund, Reference JACKSON, MARTIN and EKLUND2008; Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008). Similarly, in this investigation, flow was evaluated through ad hoc adaptations of the core flow and short flow scales (Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008).

The core flow scale uses ten items to assess the core phenomenological experience of an optimal state of mind, including aspects like being ‘in the zone’, feeling like ‘everything clicks’, ‘totally involved’ and ‘totally focused’ (Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008). The short flow scale uses nine items to evaluate the defining characteristics of flow experience (Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008): balance, goals, feedback, concentration, control, automaticity, lack of self-consciousness, time distortion and intrinsic reward.

Following established criteria, both scales were translated and adapted to the context of primary school musical practice (Hambleton, Reference HAMBLETON2001; Van de Vijver & Tanzer, Reference VAN DE VIJVER and TANZER2004). Items’ content validity was evaluated by three experts working in music education: a teaching methodologist, a music school principal and a children’s music teacher. Experts received definitions of core flow and short flow and evaluated whether each item adequately represented each construct or if they presented deficiencies impeding comprehension. Agreement was high regarding the correct comprehension and representation of each construct’s content. Two pilot applications were carried out with small samples in order to ensure that items were understood.

Regarding the core flow scale, experts and pilot application signalled two items (‘I’m in the zone’ and ‘I’m in the Groove’) with translation and comprehension problems. Thus, the final adapted core flow scale included eight items. With regard to the short flow scale, according to expert advice, given that the notion of ‘competence-situation balance’ could limit children’s comprehension, an additional item assessing skill-task balance was included (‘I have enough skills to adequately perform the tasks’), adapted from French, Caplan, and Harrison (Reference FRENCH, CAPLAN and HARRISON1982), as reported by Bakker (Reference BAKKER2008). All flow items were evaluated on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree.

The adapted core flow scale (8 items) presented a high alpha (α = .89) and the adapted short flow scale (10 items) presented a satisfactory alpha (α = .74). These reliability results coincide with previous studies that found alphas between α = .63 and α = .90 (Bakker, Reference BAKKER2008; Martin et al., Reference MARTIN, TIPLER, MARSH, RICHARDS and WILLIAMS2006). Aspects of convergent validity were analysed through the correlation between core and short flow, which was robust (r = .67; p < .001), similar to previous studies (r = .72: Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008).

Family musical practice – as expression of musical cultural capital – was evaluated with the item ‘the person has relatives who practice music (play instruments, sing in a choir, or compose)’. Participants reported their similarity to this portrait, on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from ‘not at all similar to me’ to ‘very similar to me’ (following previous research that has shown that intrinsic motivation for musical practice is positively associated with family musical practice and willingness to practice music: Valenzuela & Codina, Reference VALENZUELA and CODINA2014).

School socio-economic profile was assessed as control variable, taking into account the schools location (district). Four schools were middle-class socio-economic level; only two schools were, respectively, above (two classrooms) and below middle-class socio-economic level (one classroom).

Data analyses

Results were analysed with SPSS 25. Using independent group comparisons, core flow and short flow measures were compared between children from high and low musical practice families. Reports of family musical practice were compared between participants from schools with low, middle and high socio-economic levels.

Results

Most of participants experienced flow during school musical practice (Table 1), both in core flow (M = 4.65, SD = 0.93) and short flow (M = 4.66, SD = 0.73). The items with higher means (close to 5 points) were those including the aspects: ‘totally involved’, ‘switched on’ and ‘totally focused’ (core flow scale) and notions like ‘strong sense of what I want to do’, ‘how well I’m doing’, ‘focused on the task’, ‘having enough skills’ and ‘experience is extremely rewarding’ (short flow scale). Items with the lowest means included expressions such as ‘I feel in control’, ‘nothing else matters’ (core flow) and ‘I do things spontaneously and automatically’ (short flow).

Table 1. Core Flow and Short Flow: Means, Standard Deviations and Differences According to Family Musical Practice

Of all participants, 45.6% (hereinafter referred to as high musical practice families) reported perceiving themselves as similar to a ‘person with relatives who practice music’: the response alternative ‘very similar to me’ was chosen by 23.1% of students; ‘similar to me’ by 7.5%, and ‘somewhat similar to me’ by 15.0%. The remaining 54.4% of participants (hereinafter referred to as low musical practice families) answered that they did not perceive themselves as similar to this personal portrait: ‘has nothing in common with me’ yielded 39.4% of responses; ‘not similar to me’ was chosen by 8.1%; and ‘not very similar to me’ by 6.9%.

Comparisons made with Student’s t test (Table 1) showed that the participants stemming from high musical practice families gave significantly higher responses than their peers from low musical practice families on the items that contained expressions such as ‘totally involved’, ‘everything clicks’, ‘tuned in’ and ‘in the flow’ (core flow) as well as ‘ enough skills’, ‘competent enough’, ‘not worried about what others might think of me’ and ‘experience is extremely rewarding’ (short flow). As a result, the former scored higher on the measures of core flow (t (152) = −3.118; p = .002) and short flow (t (156) = −3.515; p = .001).

As means of control, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess differences in family musical practice according to school socio-economic level (data not tabulated). Children from schools with middle-class socio-economic profiles scored higher in family musical practice (M = 3.73; SD = 2.02) than children from schools above (M = 2.15; SD = 1.86) and below (M = 2.76; SD = 1.70) middle-class socio-economic profiles. However, post hoc comparisons based on Bonferonni method showed that only the difference between middle-class and above middle-class socio-economic profiles reached significance levels as regards family musical practice (D = 1.58; p < .001). These results suggest that the association between socio-economics and family musical practice follows an inverse-U pattern, favouring families with children in middle-class schools.

Discussion and conclusions

The introduction of music education in primary school in order to guarantee universal access to musical practice seemingly generates different continuity patterns: some individuals continue to be interested in musical practice and others do not. One cause for these differences is that, in order to continue musical practice, individuals need to experience flow in this activity (otherwise, boredom or frustration may take over and lead to abandonment). However, flow experience is conditioned by children’s family musical practice. In this regard, our study found differences depending on whether students came from families with high or low musical practice: the former experienced higher levels of core flow and short flow during school musical activities. We should warn that these differences were found in a context of optimal and standardised music education conditions, that is, with structured activities, trained teachers, adequate infrastructures and proper resources, motivating activities and the opportunity to sing in a choir (a more accessible practice than playing an instrument). So, it would be expected that under conditions of weaker music education, these differences would be similar or even greater.

Most participants, regardless of their family context, coincided in agreeing with items containing the expressions ‘strong sense of what I want to do’, ‘focused’, ‘switched on’ and ‘I have a good idea about how well I’m doing’. These findings show that the designed activities were successful in achieving participation with high motivation levels, clear goals, immediate feedback and concentration. In other words, these students’ answers indicate that the programme managed to provide all participants (regardless of their families’ high or low musical practice) with foundations for flow in school musical practice.

Additionally, students also generally coincided in manifesting low levels of agreement with items containing notions like ‘I’m in control’, ‘nothing else matters’, ‘I act automatically’ and ‘time passes differently from normal’. This response tendency shows that activities were structured and that students were not in control and did not act automatically. Instead, they followed instructions from their teachers, which made absorption impossible, regardless of their family’s musical practice. However, it would also be possible to argue that the cause for this could be the age of the participants, given that at young ages, musical practice is still quite driven by extrinsic motivations, such as approval from teachers and relatives (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011), which restricts the feelings of control, absorption and spontaneity.

In spite of these coincidences – probably based, on the one hand, on a successful pedagogical design and, on the other, on the natural restrictions of children’s absorption experience – students from high musical practice families showed higher scores on core flow than their classmates from low musical practice families. These differences could be explained by the fact that early contact with music contributes to perceiving it as something natural and every day (Burnard, Reference BURNARD2012), through the incorporation of positive attitudes and dispositions (Ilari et al., Reference ILARI, MOURA and BOURSCHEIDT2011), thus promoting intrinsic motivation (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011), flow and perseverance in musical practice.

Students from high musical practice families also reported (short flow) ‘having sufficient skills’ and ‘feeling competent enough’ to a greater degree. These findings converge with the perspective that the families’ socio-cultural context and previous experience in formal and informal music education can significantly condition music learning and practices (Moore, Reference MOORE2012; Wiggins, Reference WIGGINS2011). Furthermore, beliefs about one’s own ability and perceived competence (Deci, Reference DECI1975; Losier & Vallerand, Reference LOSIER and VALLERAND1994; Schüler, Sheldon, & Fröhlich, Reference SCHÜLER, SHELDON and FRÖHLICH2010) have been shown to play important roles in the processes that lead to the maintenance of intrinsic motivation over time. Children from socio-cultural contexts with higher musical practice can have more and earlier opportunities to acquire music skills (Ilari et al., Reference ILARI, MOURA and BOURSCHEIDT2011) that facilitate their intrinsic motivation and enjoyment of musical practice in elementary school.

Lastly, during school musical practice, students from high musical practice families showed greater ‘lack of concern about what others might think of them’ and ‘feeling of reward’ (short flow), as well as being ‘totally involved’, and ‘flowing with the activities’ (core flow). So, the experience of the students from high musical practice families presented more typical signs of flow – such as absorption, fusion between awareness and action and autotelic enjoyment.

These results support the idea that musical cultural capital and the level of musical practice of each family condition the experience of the younger members when learning music. This idea coincides with the cultural reproduction process (Bourdieu, Reference BOURDIEU, Karable and Halsey1977; DiMaggio, Reference DIMAGGIO1982), where children from families with high levels of cultural capital in a certain field generally obtain greater benefits from their cultural practice in that field. Taking into consideration the participants in the current study, this argument is upheld, among other things, if we consider flow to be one of the benefits of participating in musical activities. An integration of the concepts presented reveals that a high level of family musical practice can promote the sense of one’s musical self and the sense of one’s agency in learning music; these two senses can operate as a basis for flow in primary school musical practice – which, in turn, promotes perseverance.

In conclusion, it can be observed in our study that family musical cultural capital conditions childhood music participation experiences. Thus, it is a priority for primary school music education to acknowledge that in one same classroom there are students with different levels of attitudes, motivation and even aptitudes towards learning music. Considering these differences should allow music teachers to offer tasks adapted to each child, without making a deterministic interpretation of the capacity to achieve flow in school musical practice.

Based on the results obtained in this study on children’s music training, we point out some psycho-pedagogical guidelines in order to facilitate access to enjoyment of musical practice: some directed to the school and others to the teachers.

Regarding the school, efficiently promoting musical practice requires a detailed study of the effect of family context the students’ music education experiences in primary school, in order to optimise the music education processes. For example, taking into account the relationship between beliefs and attitudes towards music present in the family and the development of intrinsic motivation in learning music (Lamont, Reference LAMONT2011), a school music education programme designed to create atmospheres of significative learning (Díaz, Morales, & Díaz, Reference DÍAZ, MORALES and DÍAZ2014) can, among other actions, invite families of the youngest children to participate in fun musical activities in school. This type of pedagogically inclusive design would allow parents to offer their children the possibility of identifying with them in an active role of musical practitioners, creating a rich atmosphere of opportunities for the consolidation of children’s music participation (Kröner & Dickhäuser, Reference KRÖNER and DICKHÄUSER2009).

Regarding music teachers, it is necessary that they provide each student with tasks of fitting demand levels, considering their skills, at each moment, thus, providing the balance needed for absorption. Given that balance is personally determined – depending on previous experience with musical practice – in order to stimulate flow in every student, teachers will need to get acquainted with the musical practice situation of each family. Therefore, through the promotion of a sustained and personalised balance between demands and skills, music teachers will avoid students’ frustration and boredom, as well as potential anxiety and desertion. In this way, regardless of the family’s previous musical practice, most of the students will be able to achieve flow in school musical practice, which will allow them to acquire knowledge and musical abilities, thus, enabling them to consolidate their music-making habits.

Beyond the obtained results regarding processes to consider in children’s music education, this investigation makes contributions to the study of flow. On the one hand, the study provides very useful instruments for studies on musical practice in Spanish-speaking contexts, the core flow and short flow scales (Martin & Jackson, Reference MARTIN and JACKSON2008). These instruments have been successfully translated and adapted to the context of children’s musical practice and, in turn, are adaptable to other geographical and artistic learning contexts where flow processes are consubstantial to the activity. On the other hand, family and school contexts, as primary socialising agents, predispose students to certain sensitivities and abilities, so that family and the teachers can proactively work towards the continuity of certain cultural capitals or the incorporation of new ones.

References

AMADO, D., SÁNCHEZ-MIGUEL, P. A., LEO, F. M., SÁNCHEZ-OLIVA, D. & GARCÍA-CALVO, T. (2013). Desarrollo de un modelo de motivación para explicar el flow disposicional y la ansiedad en bailarines profesionales [Development of a motivation model to explain dispositional flow and anxiety in professional dancers]. Universitas Psychologica, 12, 457470. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-2.dmmfGoogle Scholar
BAKKER, A. B. (2005). Flow among music teachers and their pupils: The crossover of peak experiences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66, 2644. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2003.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BAKKER, A. B. (2008). The work-related flow inventory: Construction and initial validation of the WOLF. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72, 400414. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2007.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BOURDIEU, P. (1977). Cultural reproduction and social reproduction. In Karable, J. & Halsey, A. (eds.), Power and Ideology in Education (pp. 487511). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BOURDIEU, P. (1990). In Other Words: Essays towards a Reflexive Sociology (M. Adamson, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge Polity Press.Google Scholar
BOURDIEU, P. & PASSERON, J. C. (1990). Reproduction in Education, Society, and Culture. London: Sage.Google Scholar
BROWN, S., MERKER, B. & WALLIN, N. L. (2000). An introduction to evolutionary musicology. In Brown, S., Merker, B., & Wallin, N. L. (eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 324). Boston, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
BURNARD, P. (2012). Musical Creativities in Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CSIKSZENTMIHALYI, M. (1988). Optimal Experience: Psychological Studies of Flow in Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CSIKSZENTMIHALYI, M. (1991). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. San Francisco, CA: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
CUSTODERO, L. A. (2002) Seeking challenge, finding skill: Flow experience and music education. Arts Education Policy Review, 103, 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CUSTODERO, L. A. (2005). Observable indicators of flow experience: A developmental perspective on musical engagement in young children from infancy to school age. Music Education Research, 7, 185209. doi: 10.1080/14613800500169431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DECI, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DÍAZ, M., MORALES, R. & DÍAZ, W. (2014). La música como recurso pedagógico en la edad preescolar. Revista Infancias Imágenes, 13, 102108.Google Scholar
DIMAGGIO, P. (1982). Cultural capital and school success: The impact of status culture participation on the grades of U.S. high school pupils. American Sociological Review, 47, 189201. doi: 10.2307/2094962CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EUROPEAN MUSIC COUNCIL. (2011, May). Bonn Declaration on Music education in Europe. http://www.emc-imc.org/fileadmin/user_upload/Cultural_Policy/Bonn_Declaration.pdf.Google Scholar
FAUTLEY, M. (2017) Editorial: Music education: Why bother? – Erratum. British Journal of Music Education, 34, 237239. doi: 10.1017/S0265051717000043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FAUTLEY, M. & DAUBNEY, A. (2018). Editorial: Inclusion, music education, and what it might mean. British Journal of Music Education, 35, 219221. doi: 10.1017/S0265051718000219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FRENCH, J. R., CAPLAN, R. D. & HARRISON, R. V. (1982). The Mechanisms of Job Stress and Strain. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
HAMBLETON, R. K. (2001). The next generation of the ITC test translation and adaptation guidelines. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17, 164172. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.17.3.164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ILARI, B., MOURA, A. & BOURSCHEIDT, L. (2011). Between interactions and commodities: Musical parenting of infants and toddlers in Brazil. Music Education Research, 13, 5167. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2011.553277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
JACKSON, S. A., MARTIN, A. J. & EKLUND, R. C. (2008). Long and short measures of flow: The construct validity of the FSS-2, DFS-2, and new brief counterparts. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 561587. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.5.561CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
JEFATURA DEL ESTADO. (1999, December 14). Ley Orgánica 15/1999, de 13 de diciembre, de Protección de Datos de Carácter Personal [Organic Law 15/1999, of December 13, on the Protection of personal data]. Boletín Oficial del Estado, 298(23750), 43.08843.099. https://www.boe.es/boe/dias/1999/12/14/pdfs/A43088-43099.pdf.Google Scholar
JOHANSEN, G. (ed.). (2009). An Education Politics of the Particular: Promises and Opportunities for the Quality of Higher Music Education. New York: Taylor & Francis Ltd.Google Scholar
KELLER, J. & LANDHÄUßER, A. (2012). The flow model revisited. In Engeser, S. (ed.), Advances in Flow Research (pp. 5164). New York: Springer Science+Business Media. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KRÖNER, S., & DICKHÄUSER, O. (2009). Die Rolle Von Eltern, Peers Und Intrinsischem Wert Für Die Rezeptive Hochkulturelle Praxis Von Gymnasiasten Der Sekundarstufe II [The role of parents, peers and intrinsic value for the receptive high cultural practice of secondary school students]. Zeitschrift Für Pädagogische Psychologie, 23, 5363. doi: 10.1024/1010-0652.23.1.53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LAMONT, A. (2011). The beat goes on: Music education, identity and lifelong learning. Music Education Research, 13, 369388. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2011.638505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LOSIER, G. & VALLERAND, R. (1994). The temporal relationship between perceived competence and self-determined motivation. The Journal of Social Psychology, 134, 793801. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1994.9923014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MARTIN, A. J. & JACKSON, S. A. (2008). Brief approaches to assessing task absorption and enhanced subjective experience: examining ‘short’ and ‘core’ flow in diverse performance domains. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 141157. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9094-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARTIN, A. J., TIPLER, D. V., MARSH, H. W., RICHARDS, G. E. & WILLIAMS, M. R. (2006). Assessing multidimensional physical activity motivation: a construct validity study of high school students. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 171192. doi: 10.1123/jsep.28.2.171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MOORE, G. (2012). ‘Tristan chords and random scores’: Exploring undergraduate pupils’ experiences of music in higher education through the lens of Bourdieu. Music Education Research, 14, 6378. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2012.657164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
OSBORNE, M. & KENNY, D. (2005). Development and validation of a music performance anxiety inventory for gifted adolescent musicians. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 725751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RHODES, R. & KATES, A. (2015). Can the affective response to exercise predict future motives and physical activity behavior? A systematic review of published evidence. Annals of Behavioural Medicine, 49, 715731. doi: 10.1007/s12160-015-9704-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RODRÍGUEZ-QUILES, J. A. (2017). Music teacher training: A precarious area within the Spanish University. British Journal of Music Education, 34, 8194. doi: 10.1017/S026505171600036XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
RUSINEK, G. (2004). Aprendizaje musical significativo. Revista Electrónica Complutense de Investigación en Educación Musical, 1, 116.Google Scholar
SCHÜLER, J., SHELDON, K. & FRÖHLICH, S. (2010). Implicit need for achievement moderates the relationship between competence need satisfaction and subsequent motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 112. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SWANN, C., PIGGOTT, D., SCHWEICKLE, M. & VELLA, S. A. (2018): A review of scientific progress in flow in sport and exercise: Normal science, crisis, and a progressive shift. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2018.1443525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TÖRNQVIST, G. (2004). Creativity in time and space. Geografiska Annaler Series B: Human Geography, 86, 227243. doi: 10.1111/j.0435-3684.2004.00165.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
VALENZUELA, R. & CODINA, N. (2014). Habitus and flow in primary school musical practice: Relations between family musical cultural. Music Education Research, 16, 505520. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2013.859660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VALENZUELA, R., CODINA, N. & PESTANA, J. V. (2018). Self-determination theory applied to flow in conservatoire music practice: The roles of perceived autonomy and competence, and autonomous and controlled motivation. Psychology of Music, 46, 3348. doi: 10.1177/0305735617694502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VAN DE VIJVER, F. & TANZER, N. K. (2004). Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: An overview. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliqué, 54, 119135. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2003.12.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WIGGINS, J. (2011). Vulnerability and agency in being and becoming a musician. Music Education Research, 13, 355367. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2011.632153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WORLD MEDICAL ASSOCIATION. (2013). World medical association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Journal of the American Medical Association, 310, 21912194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WRISTEN, B. (2013). Depression and anxiety in university music students. Applications of Research in Music Education, 31, 2027. doi: 10.1177/8755123312473613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Core Flow and Short Flow: Means, Standard Deviations and Differences According to Family Musical Practice