Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T22:47:22.711Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain correlates of recognition of communicative interactions from biological motion in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 November 2017

Ł. Okruszek*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
M. Wordecha
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Lab, Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
M. Jarkiewicz
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology, Warsaw, Poland
B. Kossowski
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
J. Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, USA
A. Marchewka
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
*
Author of correspondence: Ł. Okruszek, Email: lukasz.okruszek@psych.pan.pl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Recognition of communicative interactions is a complex social cognitive ability which is associated with a specific neural activity in healthy individuals. However, neural correlates of communicative interaction processing from whole-body motion have not been known in patients with schizophrenia (SCZ). Therefore, the current study aims to examine the neural activity associated with recognition of communicative interactions in SCZ by using displays of the dyadic interactions downgraded to minimalistic point-light presentations.

Methods

Twenty-six healthy controls (HC) and 25 SCZ were asked to judge whether two agents presented only by point-light displays were communicating or acting independently. Task-related activity and functional connectivity of brain structures were examined with General Linear Model and Generalized Psychophysiological Interaction approach, respectively.

Results

HC were significantly more efficient in recognizing each type of action than SCZ. At the neural level, the activity of the right posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) was observed to be higher in HC compared with SCZ for communicative v. individual action processing. Importantly, increased connectivity of the right pSTS with structures associated with mentalizing (left pSTS) and mirroring networks (left frontal areas) was observed in HC, but not in SCZ, during the presentation of social interactions.

Conclusion

Under-recruitment of the right pSTS, a structure known to have a pivotal role in social processing, may also be of importance for higher-order social cognitive deficits in SCZ. Furthermore, decreased task-related connectivity of the right pSTS may result in reduced use of additional sources of information (for instance motor resonance signals) during social cognitive processing in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Recognition and appropriate interpretation of communicative intentions is one of the essential abilities that enable one to navigate social interactions. In healthy individuals, both recognition of another person's communicative intentions (Ciaramidaro et al. Reference Ciaramidaro, Becchio, Colle, Bara and Walter2014) and observation of dyadic interactions of other agents (Georgescu et al. Reference Georgescu, Kuzmanovic, Santos, Tepest, Bente, Tittgemeyer and Vogeley2014; Eskenazi et al. Reference Eskenazi, Rueschemeyer, de Lange, Knoblich and Sebanz2015; Quadflieg et al. Reference Quadflieg, Gentile and Rossion2015) were found to elicit increased activity in specific networks of brain structures. These structures constitute mentalizing network (bilateral posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)) and action observation (mirroring) network (bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), premotor cortex (PMC), supplementary motor area (SMA), intraparietal sulcus (IaPS)) (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011).

Furthermore, it was proposed that even in the absence of other cues, information that is conveyed by the motion of the whole body is sufficient for healthy individuals to effectively detect communicative interactions (Manera et al. Reference Manera, Ianì, Bourgeois, Haman, Okruszek, Rivera, Robert, Schilbach, Sievers, Verfaillie, Vogeley, von der Lühe, Willems and Becchio2015). Point-light motion displays (PLD), a methodology introduced in the 1970s by the Swedish psychologist Johansson (Reference Johansson1973), is the most common way to examine the processing of social information while limiting and precisely controlling the amount of visual input. Additionally, use of point-light displays allows one to examine the perception of social interactions while reducing the impact of other social perceptive abilities (for example eye-gaze processing, facial emotion recognition) or other possible confounds which may be linked to sympathy toward the agents or culturally-specific factors (Pica et al. Reference Pica, Jackson, Blake and Troje2011). Healthy individuals are able to easily differentiate situations in which two agents, who are displayed only by the point-lights attached to main joints of their bodies, are interacting with the ones where agents are acting independently (Manera et al. Reference Manera, Ianì, Bourgeois, Haman, Okruszek, Rivera, Robert, Schilbach, Sievers, Verfaillie, Vogeley, von der Lühe, Willems and Becchio2015). Moreover, studies investigating the ‘social predictive brain’ hypothesis revealed that actions of an agent are detected more effectively under conditions of visual noise masking if they are semantically related to the actions of the other person (Manera et al. Reference Manera, Becchio, Schouten, Bara and Verfaillie2011). Also, presentation of communicative interactions between PLDs receives priority in conscious perception over the independent actions of two agents (Su et al. Reference Su, van Boxtel and Lu2016). In line with these behavioral findings, recognition of social interactions v. individual actions is linked with neural activity in both action observation and mentalizing networks in healthy individuals (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011).

Schizophrenia is a severe mental disorder, which usually starts in early adulthood and profoundly impacts patients’ functioning. Social cognitive deficits are considered as one of the key areas of impairment in the functioning of patients with schizophrenia. Moderate to large deficits are found in most social cognitive domains in patients with schizophrenia (Savla et al. Reference Savla, Vella, Armstrong, Penn and Twamley2012), and patients have been repeatedly found to present deficits in lower-level social perceptive abilities which must be drawn upon to successfully interpret complex social interactions (see Kohler et al. (Reference Kohler, Walker, Martin, Healey and Moberg2009) and Hoekert et al. (Reference Hoekert, Kahn, Pijnenborg and Aleman2007) for meta-analyses of studies on facial affect and verbal prosody processing, respectively, in people with schizophrenia). Thus, previous behavioral findings, suggesting reduced ability to comprehend social cues conveyed by the means of non-verbally (White et al. Reference White, Borgan, Ralley and Shergill2016) or interpret the actions and intentions of participants of naturalistic social situations (Montag et al. Reference Montag, Dziobek, Richter, Neuhaus, Lehmann, Sylla, Heekeren, Heinz and Gallinat2011; Scherzer et al. Reference Scherzer, Leveillé, Achim, Boisseau and Stip2012; Rocca et al. Reference Rocca, Galderisi, Rossi, Bertolino, Rucci, Gibertoni, Montemagni, Sigaudo, Mucci, Bucci, Acciavatti, Aguglia, Amore, Bellomo, De Ronchi, Dell'Osso, Di Fabio, Girardi, Goracci, Marchesi, Monteleone, Niolu, Pinna, Roncone, Sacchetti, Santonastaso, Zeppegno and Maj2016) in patients with schizophrenia, may stem from numerous factors associated with both lower- and higher-level processing of social information. Similarly, abnormal activity of mentalizing network, which was found in patients during processing of cooperative v. individual actions of two agents (Backasch et al. Reference Backasch, Straube, Pyka, Klöhn-Saghatolislam, Müller, Kircher and Leube2013) or social v. non-social intentions (Walter et al. Reference Walter, Ciaramidaro, Adenzato, Vasic, Ardito, Erk and Bara2009), may be linked to both lower- and higher-level social cognitive deficits which diminish patients’ ability to interpret communicative interactions between real-life actors. In addition, we have previously demonstrated that patients demonstrate reduced ability to interpret the dyadic actions of two agents, even when the complexity of the stimuli has been limited by using the point-light displays of two agents (Okruszek et al. Reference Okruszek, Haman, Kalinowski, Talarowska, Becchio and Manera2015). However, to the best of our knowledge, no previous neuroimaging study has examined processes associated with recognition of social interactions from point-light motion in schizophrenia.

Therefore, the current study seeks out behavioral and neural correlates of inferring communicative interactions in patients with schizophrenia by presenting actions of agents that have been visually degraded to PLDs. We hypothesize that in healthy controls (HC) recognition of communicative interactions from point-light motion will be linked to activation in mentalizing and action observation networks, indicating the engagement of both reflective and, more automatic, reflexive social cognitive processes during communicative interactions inference (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011). A recent comprehensive review of the mechanisms of social cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia has proposed that while there is a considerable evidence for impairment of reflective social cognitive processes (e.g. mentalizing), reflexive processes (e.g. motor resonance) may be possibly intact in patients with schizophrenia (Green et al. Reference Green, Horan and Lee2015). In line with this suggestion and previous findings in this area, we predict that activity of mentalizing network during the processing of communicative interactions will differentiate patients from controls (Walter et al. Reference Walter, Ciaramidaro, Adenzato, Vasic, Ardito, Erk and Bara2009; Backasch et al. Reference Backasch, Straube, Pyka, Klöhn-Saghatolislam, Müller, Kircher and Leube2013). However, no discrepancies will be observed between the groups in terms of action observation network activity (Horan et al. Reference Horan, Iacoboni, Cross, Korb, Lee, Nori and Green2014a). Additionally, we aim to explore the task-related functional connectivity of structures that will be differentially activated by the processing of communicative interactions in both groups.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-five right-handed patients (age: 35.7 ± 6.9 yrs; sex: 13M/12F) diagnosed with schizophrenia according to the ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) criteria, who did not participate in our previous study (Okruszek et al. Reference Okruszek, Haman, Kalinowski, Talarowska, Becchio and Manera2015), were recruited through leaflets in outpatient clinics in the Warsaw area. Only patients with an established diagnosis of schizophrenia, verified by the available documentation and confirmed by the clinical interview done by the qualified psychiatrist (MJ), were included in the study. Exclusion criteria included any change of pharmacotherapy during the 2 weeks prior to the study, history of comorbid head trauma, drug abuse, intellectual disability. Additionally, each patient has undergone a clinical assessment with Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al. Reference Kay, Flszbein and Opfer1987) and the Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS) (Morosini et al. Reference Morosini, Magliano, Brambilla, Ugolini and Pioli2000) upon arrival at the testing facility. All but one medication-free patient were treated with atypical neuroleptics at the time of the study.

Twenty-six right-handed HC (age: 35.3 ± 7.1 yrs; sex: 14M/12F) with no history of psychiatric or neurological treatment or relatives diagnosed with schizophrenia were recruited through online advertisements from the same community sample.

All of the subjects (HC and SCZ) had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision and gave written consent prior to participating in the study. The procedure of the study was approved by the University of Warsaw Ethics Committee. Each participant was reimbursed (50 PLN) for participation in the study. Information on demographic and clinical variables for the participants is shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of the samples

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; SCZ, patients with schizophrenia; HC, healthy controls; PANSS, positive and negative syndrome scale; SOFAS, social and occupational functioning assessment scale; CPZ, chlorpromazine.

Experimental task

Stimuli

The stimuli and the task for the current study have been adapted from Centelles et al. (Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011). For the details of the stimuli production please refer to the original study. Stimuli consisted of 112 animations depicting actions of dyads of agents presented as point-light walkers (PLW). Each PLW consisted of 20 white point-lights, which had been attached to head, limbs and major joints of the body. Actions of the agents were presented against the black background. Each animation lasted 3 s. Half of the stimuli used for the current study (n = 56) presented social interactions (communicative actions; COM) between agents. COM animations included the conventional use of communicative gestures (e.g. agent asks other agent to sit down, another agent sits down; n = 23), emotional situations (e.g. agents start to jump for joy; n = 20) or synchronous activity of the agents during the games/dancing (n = 13). Actions were presented either from the forward or the sideways point of view. Furthermore, during the COM condition half of the animations was presented with an actor on the left initiating the interaction, while the other half of the animations was presented with an actor on the right initiating the interaction. During the individual condition (IND; n = 56) actors performed physical movements (e.g. jumps, squats down) without impacting one another.

Paradigm

During the experiment, both types of actions of two agents were displayed. The task was presented in two runs of 56 trials presented in an event-related design with jittered ITI of 3–5 s. After the presentation of the PLDs, the response screen with two geometric figures (green rectangle and red triangle) was shown for another 3 s. Participants were instructed to respond to the question ‘Are the two persons acting together or separately?’ by pressing the button of the response pad corresponding to the placement of one of the figures on the screen (green rectangle for communicative interactions, red triangle for individual actions). The order of the animations and response screens was pseudorandomized, to allow half of the animations of each type (COM, IND) to be paired with each type of the response screen. The scheme of the experimental task is shown in Fig. 1. The paradigm was created with NBS Presentation software (https://www.neurobs.com/) and displayed on a 21-inch screen which was presented to the participant with a mirror system. The scanning procedure was preceded by the training, which took place outside the scanner. During the training, a set of 24 unique animations, which have not been used during the main procedure, was presented while participants learned how to use the response pads.

Fig. 1. Scheme of the experimental task.

fMRI data acquisitions

MRI data acquisition took place at the Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Neurobiology Center, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, with a 3-Tesla MR scanner (Siemens Magnetom Trio TIM, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with 32-channel phased array head coil. Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient echo planar imaging (EPI) (parameters: TR = 2500 ms, TE = 28 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix 64 × 64 mm, FOV = 216 mm, and 41 axial slices, slice thickness = 3 mm). There were two functional sessions (each of 234 volumes). Field mapping was done based on Jezzard & Balban's (Reference Jezzard and Balaban1995) method by using double echo FLASH (TE1 = 4.92, TE2 = 7.38, time repetition = 600, same spatial parameters as functional scans). Anatomical data were acquired using a T1-weighted (T1w) sequence (parameters: TR = 2530 ms, TE = 3.32 ms, flip angle = 7°, 176 slices with an in-plane resolution of 1 mm3, FOV = 256 mm, slice thickness = 1 mm). To ensure that group differences were not associated with motion artifacts, we inspected all the data for motion artifacts and removed the participant who showed head movements greater than 3 mm during the scanning procedure. Then, we investigated if groups differed in head motion by calculating a sum of the length of between-slices translation vectors on the basis of SPM head motion regressors. No between-group differences were found for either the first (t(47) = 1.3 n.s.) or second (t(47) = 1.4 n.s.) functional block. Finally, six head motion regressors were included in the GLM first level model. Furthermore, data from one patient were discarded from further analyses, due to the lack of behavioral response to the task (n = 1).

Behavioral analysis

Repeated-measures ANOVA with Type (COM, IND) as a within-subject factor and Group (HC, SCZ) as a between-subject factor was used to examine the factors impacting behavioral accuracy during the task. All of the reported results were Greenhouse-Geisser corrected. The same statistical approach was adapted to analyze the reaction time data. Spearman's rho was applied to examine the relationship between overall task accuracy and RTs in patients and clinical variables (age of onset, number of hospitalizations, PANSS Positive and Negative subscales, SOFAS score). To account for the multiple comparisons, the threshold for significance was corrected to p = 0.01 for correlational analyses.

Neuroimaging data

General linear model (GLM) analysis

The Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, UK) toolbox was used for data preprocessing and the statistical analyses. In order to minimize geometrical distortions in EPI images caused by field inhomogeneities we used additional B0 field map scans. The FieldMap toolbox was used to calculate voxel maps displacement. Functional images were motion-corrected and unwrapped from susceptibility artifacts. Structural images (T1w) from single subjects were co-registered to the mean functional image. Then, T1w scans were classified into grey and white matter and also cerebrospinal fluid using the ‘New Segmentation’ tool (based on Gaussian models and also tissue probability maps). The functional images were normalized to a 2 mm isotropic voxel size and smoothed with a 5 mm isotropic Gaussian kernel.

The design for the GLM included five conditions: presentation of correctly classified COM animations, presentation of incorrectly classified COM animations, presentation of correctly classified IND animations, presentation of incorrectly classified IND animations and presentation of response (RESP) screen and six head motion regressors.

At the first-level analysis, a statistical image for the contrast between correctly classified animations containing social interactions (COM) and correctly classified animations presenting two non-interacting agents (IND) was created for each participant. At the second-level analysis, all of the COM v. IND contrast images obtained at the 1st level were included in the two-sample t test model. To minimize type I error, baseline p was set at 0.0001 and cluster size was FWE corrected (k ⩾ 26) to obtain p = 0.05.

Functional connectivity analysis

To investigate the task-related connectivity of structures that are differentially activated by the processing of communicative interactions in both groups, functional connectivity analyses were computed based on GLM findings. A mask of the right pSTS cluster which had been based on the contrast of activation of HC > schizophrenia patients for the COM v. IND contrast in a GLM analysis was used as a seed region. The CONN functional connectivity toolbox (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon2012) was employed. Preprocessed data were high-pass 0.008 Hz filtered. Six motion parameters, scrubbing parameters, as well as main condition effects and five variables associated with the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid, were added as potential confounders for the denoising step. Generalized psychophysiological interaction (gPPI) was used to investigate the right pSTS cluster connectivity change for the COM v. IND contrast. Separate PPI maps were extracted for HC, SCZ and between-groups contrasts with a threshold p < 0.001 FWE corrected (k ⩾ 48) to obtain p = 0.05.

Results

Behavioral results

A main effect of Group was found (F(1,47) = 8.2; p < 0.01, η p2 = 0.15) with an overall lower accuracy in patients than in controls (HC: 99% ± 1% v. SCZ: 96% ± 4%). An effect of Type was also observed, with lower accuracies for the COM compared with IND condition (F(1,47) = 5.9, p < 0.05, η p2 = 0.11 COM: 97% ± 4% v. IND: 98% ± 3%). No interaction between Group and Type was observed (F(1,47) = 2.9 n.s., η p2 = 0.06). Patients’ social functioning, as measured with SOFAS, was linked to their overall behavioral accuracy during the task (ρ = 0.61, p = 0.003); no other clinical variables were associated with the behavioral performance in patients. In an additional step of the analysis, we have calculated a correlation coefficients between clinical factors and number of errors which have been linked specifically to a/ overinterpretation of individual actions as an communicative interaction (overmentalizing) and b/ classifying the interaction as an individual actions of two agents (undermentalizing), as both types of errors have been suggested to be differentially linked to symptoms of schizophrenia (Frith, Reference Frith2004). The results of the analysis revealed the correlation between the number of individual actions misclassified as communicative ones and SOFAS scores (ρ = −0.54, p = 0.008) and, surprisingly, PANSS negative symptoms (ρ = 0.56, p = 0.006).

For the RTs a main effect of Group was found (F(1,47) = 5.9, p < 0.05, η p2 = 0.11) with slower responses in patients than in HC(730 ± 173 ms v. SCZ: 864 ± 214 ms). No effect of Type (F(1,47) < 0.1 n.s., η p2 = 0.001) or interaction between Group and Type (F(1,47) = 0.2 n.s., η p2 = 0.03) were observed. In patients, RTs for COM (ρ = −0.67, p < 0.001) were negatively correlated with SOFAS; no other clinical variables were linked to RTs.

Neuroimaging results

Results for the COM v. IND contrast for patients, HC and between-group comparisons are described in Table 2 and visualized in Figs. 2 and 3.

Fig. 2. Clusters of activation found at the FWE corrected p = 0.05 for COM v. IND contrasts for healthy controls (left) and patients with schizophrenia (right).

Fig. 3. A Cluster of activation which has shown higher activity for COM v. IND contrasts in healthy controls compared with patients with schizophrenia at the FWE corrected p = 0.05.

Table 2. Brain areas showing increased BOLD response during presentation of correctly classified social interactions in comparison with correctly classified individual actions of the agents

Results are FWE p = 0.05 corrected at the cluster level (k ⩾ 26). R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere.

Healthy controls

Increased engagement of numerous brain structures was found in HC for communicative interactions compared with individual action processing. A robust pattern of activity was found in the temporal cortices with clusters found in the right STS, extending from the right anterior temporal pole (TP) to the posterior STS/TPJ, and in left posterior and, to the less extent, anterior STS. Furthermore, posterior activations were observed in bilateral fusiform gyrus (FG) and bilateral middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The increased BOLD response was also found in parietal cortices: bilaterally in the IaPS and in the left supramarginal gyrus (SMG). Frontal activations were found bilaterally in the IFG and in the left mPFC. Subcortical activity was also observed in the left thalamus (THAL). Additionally, increased engagement of motor (left PMC, right SMA) and somatosensory (right somatosensory cortex; SC) areas was observed.

Patients with schizophrenia

The pattern of structures activated by communicative v. individual actions processing in patients with schizophrenia corresponded with the one observed in HC. However, increased BOLD response during recognition of social interactions was found in a limited set of regions, mostly restricted to the temporal and frontal cortices in patients. Processing of communicative interaction was associated with higher activity in the left pSTS and right aSTS compared with individual actions of two agents. Two clusters of activity corresponding to the left pars triangularis were also found in the left IFG. Furthermore, increased engagement of the left IaPS and left PMC was also observed in patients during interaction recognition.

Between-group comparisons

Increased activity of the right posterior STS for the COM v. IND contrast was found in HC compared with SCZ. No significant clusters were found to be more activated in SCZ compared with HC. To ensure that this effect reflects the decreased activation for COM v. IND in patients, we extracted the mean percent signal change from the cluster identified in the GLM analysis separately for COM and IND in each group (Brett et al. Reference Brett, Anton, Valabregue and Poline2002). The results of this exploratory region-of-interest analysis have revealed a significant Group by Condition interaction (F(1,47) = 30.4, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.39), with increased right pSTS activity during COM, as compared with IND in HC (COM: 2.8 ± 2.0 v. IND: 1.9 ± 1.7, t(25) = 6.9, p < 0.001), but not in SCZ (COM: 1.4 ± 2.3 v. IND: 1.4 ± 2.2, t(22) = 1.4 n.s.). Furthermore, between-group differences with a higher signal change in HC, as compared with SCZ were found during the COM (t(47) = 2.3, p < 0.05), but not IND (t(47) = 1.0 n.s.) condition.

Functional connectivity analyses

Results of the seed-based analysis are shown in Table 3. In HC increased connectivity between right pSTS and left IFG and Middle Frontal Gyrus (MFG), as well as left pSTS, was found for the COM v. IND contrast. No clusters of increased connectivity were found in SCZ.

Table 3. Seed-based connection table for right pSTS for the presentation of social interactions in comparison with individual actions of the agents FWE corrected at the cluster level to p = 0.05 (k ⩾ 48)

R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere.

Increased connectivity between the right pSTS and right cerebellar Crus II was observed in HC compared with SCZ. No clusters were found for the opposite contrast (SCZ v. HC).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine behavioral and neural correlates of processing of the communicative interactions presented only with a point-light motion in patients with schizophrenia compared with HC. The behavioral results of the study revealed that, albeit both groups were highly effective in discriminating communicative actions from individual ones, HC outperformed patients with schizophrenia. Furthermore, reduced activity and connectivity of the right posterior STS was observed in patients with schizophrenia compared with controls for communicative v. individual actions processing.

Behavioral results of this study are in line with previous reports of reduced capacity to extract affective (Kern et al. Reference Kern, Penn, Lee, Horan, Reise, Ochsner, Marder and Green2013; Vaskinn et al. Reference Vaskinn, Sundet, Østefjells, Nymo, Melle and Ueland2016) and social (Okruszek et al. Reference Okruszek, Haman, Kalinowski, Talarowska, Becchio and Manera2015) information from point-light motion in schizophrenia. Two well-powered studies have revealed that irrespective of the type of presented emotion, medium to large deficits can be seen in patients for the recognition of affective states conveyed by PLDs (Kern et al. Reference Kern, Penn, Lee, Horan, Reise, Ochsner, Marder and Green2013; Vaskinn et al. Reference Vaskinn, Sundet, Østefjells, Nymo, Melle and Ueland2016). The ability to infer communicative interactions from point-light motion in patients with schizophrenia was also examined in our previous study (Okruszek et al. Reference Okruszek, Haman, Kalinowski, Talarowska, Becchio and Manera2015). Results of both studies are convergent in showing that patients display reduced accuracy in recognition of communicative interactions from point-light motion. Moreover, patients’ real-life social functioning was linked to accuracy during the task. In line with previous reports of associations between ‘Emotion in Biological Motion’ task performance and functional capacity in patients with schizophrenia (Olbert et al. Reference Olbert, Penn, Kern, Lee, Horan, Reise, Ochsner, Marder and Green2013), we argue that the link between the ability to extract social information from whole-body motion and real-life functioning of patients with schizophrenia should be further investigated.

In line with a previous neuroimaging study that used the same paradigm (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011), recognition of communicative interactions engaged a broad network of structures, which are critical nodes of the mentalizing network (bilateral STS and TPJ, mPFC) and action observation network (bilateral IFG, bilateral IaPS, left PMC, right SMA and SC) in healthy individuals. In patients, activations were found in structures linked with both mentalizing (left pSTS/TPJ, right anterior STS) and action observation (left IFG, left PM, left IaPS) networks. However, the pattern of activations elicited by observing the communicative interactions in comparison with the individual actions of two PLW agents in individuals with schizophrenia was not as robust as in HC. Patients’ recognition of the communicative interactions elicited activity only in anterior temporal and temporo-parietal nodes of the mentalizing network, while in HC involvement of mPFC was also found. Importantly, reduced activity of mPFC during various social cognitive tasks was one of the main findings of Sugranyes et al.’s (Reference Sugranyes, Kyriakopoulos, Corrigall, Taylor and Frangou2011) meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on social cognition in schizophrenia.

However, the main finding of this study is an under-recruitment of the right pSTS during communicative interactions recognition in a group of patients with schizophrenia. Hypoactivation of certain brain regions have been repeatedly observed in patients with schizophrenia during cognitive and affective processing (e.g. ACC: Adams & David (Reference Adams and David2007); amygdala network: Li et al. (Reference Li, Chan, McAlonan and Gong2010); frontoparietal network: Minzenberg et al. (Reference Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen, Carter and Glahn2009)). The pSTS is believed to play a pivotal role in the perception of the dynamic aspects of human faces (Haxby & Gobbini, Reference Haxby, Gobbini, Calder, Rhodes, Johnson, Johnson and Haxby2011), or the biological motion in general (Grosbras et al. Reference Grosbras, Beaton and Eickhoff2012). While reduced pSTS sensitivity to the biological motion has been previously reported in patients with schizophrenia (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Park and Blake2011), we argue that the results of this study cannot be fully accounted for by the decreased recognition of biological motion in schizophrenia. Increased pSTS activity during communicative interactions processing was previously attributed to the detection of contingencies between the agents, rather than to processing of biological motion per se (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011). Furthermore, vignettes presenting communicative or individual actions, which have been used in this study, were identical in terms of complexity of presented biological motion, thus between-group differences in right pSTS activity cannot be explained by the processes associated with biological motion recognition.

A large body of research on the role of the pSTS in social cognitive processes provides a rationale for an alternative interpretation of the differences in the pSTS activity observed between the groups. The right pSTS is the only structure that has been repeatedly found within various methodological approaches applied by Schurz et al. (Reference Schurz, Radua, Aichhorn, Richlan and Perner2014) to examine the core brain network for mentalizing in a meta-analytic review of 73 fMRI studies with healthy individuals. Furthermore, the right pSTS was found to be a region that supports a number of processes that are required to appropriately process complex social situations, namely detection of animacy (Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Friston, O'Doherty, Wolpert and Frith2005) and intentionality of actions (Saxe et al. Reference Saxe, Xiao, Kovacs, Perrett and Kanwisher2004) as well as the integration of multimodal information about social stimuli (Kreifelts et al. Reference Kreifelts, Ethofer, Shiozawa, Grodd and Wildgruber2009). Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Rosenblau, Keifer and Pelphrey2015) recently reviewed the available data regarding the involvement of pSTS in social perception, action observation, and theory of mind (ToM) processes and proposed an integrative model of the role of pSTS for social cognition. The authors emphasized that due to its extensive functional connectivity, the pSTS may be treated as an intersection of three networks and that ToM computations may be performed only after lower-level social information from social perception and action observation networks is successfully integrated in the pSTS (Yang et al. Reference Yang, Rosenblau, Keifer and Pelphrey2015). Finally, a recent review of the neural processes engaged in the processing of the third-party encounters (TPEs), concluded that ‘the prominent role of the pSTS during the observation of TPEs does not come as a surprise. The region (…) not only implements the visual analysis of conspecifics but also contributes to interpreting their actions and internal mental states’ (Quadflieg & Koldewyn, Reference Quadflieg and Koldewyn2017, p. 7). At the same time, the authors pointed out that while there is strong support for the direct contribution of the pSTS to the analysis of scenarios with multiple agents, the extent to which specific relations between the agents modulate the pSTS activity should be further investigated.

In line with previous reports suggesting pSTS involvement in higher-order social cognitive processes, differences in pSTS activity between patients with schizophrenia and HC were observed not only during the basic social perception tasks (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Park and Blake2011), but also during emotion recognition (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Kang, Brege, Tso, Hosanagar and Johnson2012) and in a wide range of studies which examined ToM abilities with comic strip tasks (Vistoli et al. Reference Vistoli, Brunet-Gouet, Lemoalle, Hardy-Baylé and Passerieux2011; Ciaramidaro et al. Reference Ciaramidaro, Bölte, Schlitt, Hainz, Poustka, Weber and Walter2015) or social animations (Das et al. Reference Das, Lagopoulos, Coulston, Henderson and Malhi2012) as well as during the perception of (Shin et al. Reference Shin, Choi, Lee, Shin, Jang and Kim2015) or participation in (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Ku, Kim, Jang, Yoon, Kim and Kim2014) social interactions. Similarly, in a magnetoencephalographic study, an increased activation of the right pSTS during the early stages (200–600 ms) of attribution of intentions to others (compared with the processing of physical causality with human characters) was found in healthy individuals, but not in patients with schizophrenia (Vistoli et al. Reference Vistoli, Brunet-Gouet, Lemoalle, Hardy-Baylé and Passerieux2011). These observations provide compelling evidence that aberrant pSTS activity in this study may be linked to the ineffective processing of social information and may be one of the mechanisms which underlie higher-order social cognitive deficits in patients with schizophrenia.

Furthermore, the role of the right pSTS connectivity in all levels of social information processing was emphasized by the results of a recent study which employed graph theory methods to study brain connectivity during the processing of different types of social stimuli (biological motion, face perception, social animations) (Dasgupta et al. Reference Dasgupta, Tyler, Wicks, Srinivasan and Grossman2016). The right pSTS was observed to be the most highly connected region, regardless of task type. The authors suggested that the pSTS may be perceived as ‘a hub of the social brain’ (Dasgupta et al. Reference Dasgupta, Tyler, Wicks, Srinivasan and Grossman2016), thus it may be hypothesized that it may be crucial for integration of information from various brain networks (e.g. person perception network, mirroring network, mentalizing network; Quadflieg & Koldewyn, Reference Quadflieg and Koldewyn2017) which is necessary to correctly process communicative interactions between other persons. This notion is supported by our exploratory analyses of the right pSTS's functional connectivity during communicative v. individual actions processing. In HC increased connectivity was observed for the right pSTS seed both within the mentalizing network (left hemispheric pSTS regions) and with structures from action observation network (left IFG). No such effects were observed in patients with schizophrenia. Furthermore, between-group contrast revealed decreased connectivity between the right pSTS and right cerebellar Crus 2 region in patients with schizophrenia. This finding may be interpreted in terms of reduced connectivity between pSTS and mentalizing networks in patients. A rationale for such interpretation stems from Buckner et al.’s (Reference Buckner, Krienen, Castellanos, Diaz and Yeo2011) study, which provided a complete map of the cerebellar organization in a relationship with major cerebral functional networks on the basis of the resting-state functional connectivity data from 1000 healthy participants. Van Overwalle et al. (Reference Van Overwalle, Baetens, Mariën and Vandekerckhove2015a) observed a large overlap between regions commonly activated in the cerebellum by social cognitive tasks and default and somatomotor networks from Buckner et al.’s (Reference Buckner, Krienen, Castellanos, Diaz and Yeo2011) parcellation. Furthermore, evidence for a domain-specific role of cerebro-cerebellar connectivity during social cognitive processes was provided by meta-analytic connectivity modelling based on the results of 133 neuroimaging studies (Van Overwalle et al. Reference Van Overwalle, D'aes and Mariën2015b), which has found a specific coactivation patterns between cerebellar default/mentalizing regions and cerebral mentalizing network (mPFC, TPJ, pCC) and between cerebellar somatomotor regions and cerebral action observation network (pSTS, IFG, precentral/postcentral areas). To examine the significance of the decreased pSTS-cerebellar connectivity during the communicative v. individual actions processing in patients with schizophrenia, we analyzed the results of our PPI analysis in relation to seven major cerebellar networks described by Buckner et al. (Reference Buckner, Krienen, Castellanos, Diaz and Yeo2011; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/CerebellumParcellation_Buckner2011). As may be observed in the Supplementary Fig. 1, all of the clusters of decreased pSTS-cerebellar connectivity in patients group are confined within the boundaries of cerebellar default/mentalizing network. It has been suggested, that connectivity between right posterior cerebellar ‘mentalizing’ areas and cerebral ToM network may be crucial for matching external information with internal predictions generated in the cerebellum (Van Overwalle & Mariën, Reference Van Overwalle and Mariën2016). Decreased connectivity between right pSTS and cerebellar ‘mentalizing’ areas may thus impact the patients’ ability to use internal predictions while sequencing the actions of agents during the communicative interactions. Furthermore, this result may be seen as an adjunct to previous findings, which have documented reductions in ToM task-related connectivity in patients with schizophrenia for right pSTS connections with cerebral mentalizing network nodes, including contralateral pSTS (Mier et al. Reference Mier, Eisenacher, Rausch, Englisch, Gerchen, Zamoscik, Meyer-Lindenberg, Zink and Kirsch2016) and medial PFC (Ciaramidaro et al. Reference Ciaramidaro, Bölte, Schlitt, Hainz, Poustka, Weber and Walter2015). Additionally, the increased coupling between right pSTS and action observation network was found during communicative v. individual action processing only in HC. This finding may suggest that even despite motor resonance information is successfully generated in patients during social interactions processing (Horan et al. Reference Horan, Iacoboni, Cross, Korb, Lee, Nori and Green2014a, Reference Horan, Pineda, Wynn, Iacoboni and Greenb), it is not effectively incorporated for the communicative interactions inference and mentalizing purposes. These findings add to the previous literature that suggests that the role of the pSTS in social cognitive processes in humans extends far beyond the basic social perceptive processes. Furthermore, our results add to the mounting evidence pointing to the crucial role of the right pSTS hypoactivity and hypoconnectivity for the processes of social cognition in schizophrenia. Clinical significance of the current study is further established by the relationship which has been observed between patients’ social functioning and behavioral results of the task. This finding suggests that misinterpretation of third-party encounters may be directly linked to the real-life problems observed in a social domain in schizophrenia. Another clinical implication of the current study is that right pSTS may be a suitable target for noninvasive brain stimulation therapies aimed at improving social cognition in patients with schizophrenia. The possibility of improving social cognition by using transcranial electric stimulation has been investigated so far mostly with regard to the stimulation of prefrontal areas (Sellaro et al. Reference Sellaro, Nitsche and Colzato2016), and just a few studies focused on the impact of the tDCS stimulation of the right TPJ/STS areas on social cognitive processes (Santiesteban et al. Reference Santiesteban, Banissy, Catmur and Bird2012; Mai et al. Reference Mai, Zhang, Hu, Zhen, Xu, Zhang and Liu2016). Given the importance of the right pSTS for multiple levels of social cognition, future studies should examine the effectiveness of such interventions in schizophrenia.

While the findings of this study are robust, some of the study's limitations should be pointed out. We examined only clinically-stable outpatients with no acute psychotic symptoms at the time of the examination (see Table 1 for patients’ PANSS scores). As revealed by previous studies, pattern of social cognitive deficits may be differentially related to the various clinical profiles observed in patients with schizophrenia (Russell et al. Reference Russell, Reynaud, Herba, Morris and Corcoran2006; Montag et al. Reference Montag, Dziobek, Richter, Neuhaus, Lehmann, Sylla, Heekeren, Heinz and Gallinat2011; Rocca et al. Reference Rocca, Galderisi, Rossi, Bertolino, Rucci, Gibertoni, Montemagni, Sigaudo, Mucci, Bucci, Acciavatti, Aguglia, Amore, Bellomo, De Ronchi, Dell'Osso, Di Fabio, Girardi, Goracci, Marchesi, Monteleone, Niolu, Pinna, Roncone, Sacchetti, Santonastaso, Zeppegno and Maj2016). Thus, our results should be replicated in patients with more pronounced clinical symptoms. Additionally, we did not use any additional localizer task to identify individual coordinates for the pSTS anatomical location, and no basic biological motion recognition task was applied; thus, it is impossible to estimate the extent to which between-groups differences in abilities associated with biological motion processing affected the pattern of behavioral and neural results observed during social interaction recognition. Furthermore, to avoid motor preparation processes, which could confound our findings on action observation network activity, we randomized a response screen between trials, so participants could not predict which hand would be used to provide a response (Centelles et al. Reference Centelles, Assaiante, Nazarian, Anton and Schmitz2011). However, it cannot be ruled out that this manipulation may have affected the behavioral and neural findings, as patients with schizophrenia have well-documented problems with task switching (Reichenberg & Harvey, Reference Reichenberg and Harvey2007). Additionally, one may suggest that, as the main effect of the group was observed for the behavioral performance, the under-recruitment of the pSTS may reflect patients inability to perform the task. However, our exploratory ROI analysis confirmed that the pSTS activity showed strong modulation by the condition in HC, but not in patients and between-group differences in the pSTS activity were found to be limited to the communicative condition of the task. Finally, the stimuli used in the current study combined different types of interactions (conventional communicative gestures, affective situations, scenes from games) for the COM condition. The extent to which pSTS activity is modulated by the specific types of interactions between agents is largely unexplored (Quadflieg & Koldewyn, Reference Quadflieg and Koldewyn2017), thus future studies should examine neural response to each of the specific TPEs types.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003385

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Christina Schmitz, PhD, for the stimuli used for the study. This work was funded by University of Warsaw DSM 2014 and 2015 funds. ŁO was supported by the National Science Centre, Poland (UMO-2015/16/T/HS6/00336 and UMO-2016/23/D/HS6/02947) and Foundation for Polish Science Start programme. The project was realized with the aid of CePT research infrastructure purchased with funds from the European Regional Development Fund as part of the Innovative Economy Operational Programme, 2007–2013.

References

Adams, R and David, AS (2007) Patterns of anterior cingulate activation in schizophrenia: a selective review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 3, 87101.Google Scholar
Backasch, B, Straube, B, Pyka, M, Klöhn-Saghatolislam, F, Müller, MJ, Kircher, TT and Leube, DT (2013) Hyperintentionality during automatic perception of naturalistic cooperative behavior in patients with schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience 8, 489504.Google Scholar
Brett, M, Anton, J-L, Valabregue, R and Poline, J-B (2002) Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99. NeuroImage 16, S497.Google Scholar
Buckner, R, Krienen, F, Castellanos, A, Diaz, JC and Yeo, BT (2011) The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology 106, 23222345.Google Scholar
Centelles, L, Assaiante, C, Nazarian, B, Anton, JL and Schmitz, C (2011) Recruitment of both the mirror and the mentalizing networks when observing social interactions depicted by point-lights: a neuroimaging study. PloS ONE 6, e15749.Google Scholar
Ciaramidaro, A, Becchio, C, Colle, L, Bara, BG and Walter, H (2014) Do you mean me? Communicative intentions recruit the mirror and the mentalizing system. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9, 909916.Google Scholar
Ciaramidaro, A, Bölte, S, Schlitt, S, Hainz, D, Poustka, F, Weber, B and Walter, H (2015) Schizophrenia and autism as contrasting minds: neural evidence for the hypo-hyper-intentionality hypothesis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 171179.Google Scholar
Das, P, Lagopoulos, J, Coulston, CM, Henderson, AF and Malhi, GS (2012) Mentalizing impairment in schizophrenia: a functional MRI study. Schizophrenia Research 134, 158164.Google Scholar
Dasgupta, S, Tyler, SC, Wicks, J, Srinivasan, R and Grossman, ED (2016) Network connectivity of the right STS in three social perception localizers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29, 221234.Google Scholar
Eskenazi, T, Rueschemeyer, SA, de Lange, FP, Knoblich, G and Sebanz, N (2015) Neural correlates of observing joint actions with shared intentions. Cortex 70, 90100.Google Scholar
Frith, CD (2004) Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine 34, 385389.Google Scholar
Georgescu, AL, Kuzmanovic, B, Santos, NS, Tepest, R, Bente, G, Tittgemeyer, M and Vogeley, K (2014) Perceiving nonverbal behavior: neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions. Human Brain Mapping 35, 13621378.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Horan, WP and Lee, J (2015) Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16, 620631.Google Scholar
Grosbras, MH, Beaton, S and Eickhoff, SB (2012) Brain regions involved in human movement perception: a quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping 33, 431454.Google Scholar
Haxby, JV and Gobbini, MI (2011) Distributed neural systems for face perception. In Calder, A, Rhodes, G, Johnson, M, Johnson, M and Haxby, J (eds). Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 93110.Google Scholar
Hoekert, M, Kahn, RS, Pijnenborg, M and Aleman, A (2007) Impaired recognition and expression of emotional prosody in schizophrenia: review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 96, 135145.Google Scholar
Horan, WP, Iacoboni, M, Cross, KA, Korb, A, Lee, J, Nori, P and Green, MF (2014 a) Self-reported empathy and neural activity during action imitation and observation in schizophrenia. NeuroImage: Clinical 5, 100108.Google Scholar
Horan, WP, Pineda, JA, Wynn, JK, Iacoboni, M and Green, MF (2014 b) Some markers of mirroring appear intact in schizophrenia: evidence from mu suppression. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 14, 10491060.Google Scholar
Jezzard, P and Balaban, RS (1995) Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 34, 6573.Google Scholar
Johansson, G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics 14, 201211.Google Scholar
Kay, SR, Flszbein, A and Opfer, LA (1987) The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261.Google Scholar
Kern, RS, Penn, DL, Lee, J, Horan, WP, Reise, SP, Ochsner, KN, Marder, SR and Green, MF (2013) Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 2: trolling the depths of psychometric properties. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 12011210.Google Scholar
Kim, J, Park, S and Blake, R (2011) Perception of biological motion in schizophrenia and healthy individuals: a behavioral and FMRI study. PLoS ONE 6, e19971.Google Scholar
Kohler, CG, Walker, JB, Martin, EA, Healey, KM and Moberg, PJ (2009) Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 10091019.Google Scholar
Kreifelts, B, Ethofer, T, Shiozawa, T, Grodd, W and Wildgruber, D (2009) Cerebral representation of non-verbal emotional perception: fMRI reveals audiovisual integration area between voice-and face-sensitive regions in the superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia, 47, 30593066.Google Scholar
Lee, H, Ku, J, Kim, J, Jang, DP, Yoon, KJ, Kim, SI. and Kim, JJ (2014) Aberrant neural responses to social rejection in patients with schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience 9, 412423.Google Scholar
Li, H, Chan, RC, McAlonan, GM and Gong, QY (2010) Facial emotion processing in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 10291039.Google Scholar
Mai, X, Zhang, W, Hu, X, Zhen, Z, Xu, Z, Zhang, J and Liu, C (2016) Using tDCS to explore the role of the right TPJ in theory of mind and cognitive empathy. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 380.Google Scholar
Manera, V, Becchio, C, Schouten, B, Bara, BG and Verfaillie, K (2011) Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS ONE 6, e14594.Google Scholar
Manera, V, Ianì, F, Bourgeois, J, Haman, M, Okruszek, ŁP, Rivera, SM, Robert, P, Schilbach, L, Sievers, E, Verfaillie, K, Vogeley, K, von der Lühe, T, Willems, S and Becchio, C (2015) The multilingual CID-5: a new tool to study the perception of communicative interactions in different languages. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 1724.Google Scholar
Mier, D, Eisenacher, S, Rausch, F, Englisch, S, Gerchen, MF, Zamoscik, V, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Zink, M and Kirsch, P (2016 Aberrant activity and connectivity of the posterior superior temporal sulcus during social cognition in schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 267, 597610.Google Scholar
Minzenberg, MJ, Laird, AR, Thelen, S, Carter, CS and Glahn, DC (2009) Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 66, 811822.Google Scholar
Montag, C, Dziobek, I, Richter, IS, Neuhaus, K, Lehmann, A, Sylla, R, Heekeren, HR, Heinz, A and Gallinat, J (2011) Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Research 186, 203209.Google Scholar
Morosini, PL, Magliano, L, Brambilla, L, Ugolini, S and Pioli, R (2000) Development, reliability and acceptability of a new version of the DSM-IV Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS) to assess routine social funtioning. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 101, 323329.Google Scholar
Okruszek, Ł, Haman, M, Kalinowski, K, Talarowska, M, Becchio, C and Manera, V (2015) Impaired recognition of communicative interactions from biological motion in schizophrenia. PloS ONE 10, e0116793.Google Scholar
Olbert, CM, Penn, DL, Kern, RS., Lee, J., Horan, WP., Reise, SP., Ochsner, K.N., Marder, S.R. and Green, M.F. (2013) Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 3: fathoming external validity. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 12111218.Google Scholar
Quadflieg, S, Gentile, F and Rossion, B (2015) The neural basis of perceiving person interactions. Cortex 70, 520.Google Scholar
Quadflieg, S and Koldewyn, K (2017) The neuroscience of people watching: how the human brain makes sense of other people's encounters. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1396, 166182.Google Scholar
Pica, P, Jackson, S, Blake, R and Troje, NF (2011) Comparing biological motion perception in two distinct human societies. PloS ONE 6, e28391.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A and Harvey, PD (2007) Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin 133, 833.Google Scholar
Rocca, P, Galderisi, S, Rossi, A, Bertolino, A, Rucci, P, Gibertoni, D, Montemagni, C, Sigaudo, M, Mucci, A, Bucci, P, Acciavatti, T, Aguglia, E, Amore, M, Bellomo, A, De Ronchi, D, Dell'Osso, L, Di Fabio, F, Girardi, P, Goracci, A, Marchesi, C, Monteleone, P, Niolu, C, Pinna, F, Roncone, R, Sacchetti, E, Santonastaso, P, Zeppegno, P, Maj, M; Italian Network for Research on Psychoses (2016) Social cognition in people with schizophrenia: a cluster-analytic approach. Psychological Medicine 46, 2717.Google Scholar
Russell, TA, Reynaud, E, Herba, C, Morris, R and Corcoran, R (2006) Do you see what I see? Interpretations of intentional movement in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 81, 101111.Google Scholar
Santiesteban, I, Banissy, MJ, Catmur, C and Bird, G (2012) Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Current Biology 22, 22742277.Google Scholar
Savla, GN, Vella, L, Armstrong, CC, Penn, DL and Twamley, EW (2012) Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 979992.Google Scholar
Saxe, R, Xiao, DK, Kovacs, G, Perrett, DI and Kanwisher, N (2004) A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42, 14351446.Google Scholar
Scherzer, PB, Leveillé, E, Achim, A, Boisseau, E and Stip, E (2012) A study of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: a theory or many theories?. Frontiers in Psychology 14, 432.Google Scholar
Schultz, J, Friston, KJ, O'Doherty, J, Wolpert, DM and Frith, CD (2005) Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron 45, 625635.Google Scholar
Schurz, M, Radua, J, Aichhorn, M, Richlan, F and Perner, J (2014) Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 42, 934.Google Scholar
Sellaro, R, Nitsche, MA and Colzato, LS (2016) The stimulated social brain: effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1369, 218239.Google Scholar
Shin, JE, Choi, SH, Lee, H, Shin, YS, Jang, DP and Kim, JJ (2015) Involvement of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus in impaired social perception in schizophrenia. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 58, 8188.Google Scholar
Su, J, van Boxtel, JJ and Lu, H (2016) Social interactions receive priority to conscious perception. PloS ONE 10, e0160468.Google Scholar
Sugranyes, G, Kyriakopoulos, M, Corrigall, R, Taylor, E and Frangou, S (2011) Autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia: meta-analysis of the neural correlates of social cognition. PloS ONE 6, e25322.Google Scholar
Taylor, SF, Kang, J, Brege, IS, Tso, IF, Hosanagar, A and Johnson, TD (2012) Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of emotion perception and experience in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 71, 136145.Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F, Baetens, K, Mariën, P and Vandekerckhove, M (2015 a) Cerebellar areas dedicated to social cognition? A comparison of meta-analytic and connectivity results. Social Neuroscience 10, 337344.Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F, D'aes, T and Mariën, P (2015 b) Social cognition and the cerebellum: a meta-analytic connectivity analysis. Human Brain Mapping 36, 51375154.Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F and Mariën, P (2016) Functional connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum in social cognition: a multi-study analysis. NeuroImage 124, 248255.Google Scholar
Vaskinn, A, Sundet, K, Østefjells, T, Nymo, K, Melle, I and Ueland, T (2016) Reading emotions from body movement: a generalized impairment in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2058.Google Scholar
Vistoli, D, Brunet-Gouet, E, Lemoalle, A, Hardy-Baylé, MC and Passerieux, C (2011) Abnormal temporal and parietal magnetic activations during the early stages of theory of mind in schizophrenic patients. Social Neuroscience 6, 316326.Google Scholar
Walter, H, Ciaramidaro, A, Adenzato, M, Vasic, N, Ardito, RB, Erk, S and Bara, BG (2009) Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: an fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4, 166176.Google Scholar
White, TP, Borgan, F, Ralley, O and Shergill, SS (2016) You looking at me?: Interpreting social cues in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 46, 149160.Google Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S and Nieto-Castanon, A (2012) CONN: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity 2, 125141.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Yang, DY, Rosenblau, G, Keifer, C and Pelphrey, KA (2015) An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 51, 263275.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of the samples

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Scheme of the experimental task.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Clusters of activation found at the FWE corrected p = 0.05 for COM v. IND contrasts for healthy controls (left) and patients with schizophrenia (right).

Figure 3

Fig. 3. A Cluster of activation which has shown higher activity for COM v. IND contrasts in healthy controls compared with patients with schizophrenia at the FWE corrected p = 0.05.

Figure 4

Table 2. Brain areas showing increased BOLD response during presentation of correctly classified social interactions in comparison with correctly classified individual actions of the agents

Figure 5

Table 3. Seed-based connection table for right pSTS for the presentation of social interactions in comparison with individual actions of the agents FWE corrected at the cluster level to p = 0.05 (k ⩾ 48)

Supplementary material: File

Okruszek et al supplementary material

Okruszek et al supplementary material 1

Download Okruszek et al supplementary material(File)
File 14.1 KB
Supplementary material: Image

Okruszek et al supplementary material

Okruszek et al supplementary material 2

Download Okruszek et al supplementary material(Image)
Image 2.8 MB