Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T14:47:50.989Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Physical Appearance Anxiety Impedes the Therapeutic Effects of Video Feedback in High Socially Anxious Individuals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2013

Elizabeth M. J. Orr*
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
David A. Moscovitch
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
*
Reprint requests to Elizabeth Orr, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, CanadaN2L 3G1. E-mail: eorr@uwaterloo.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background: Video feedback (VF) interventions effectively reduce social anxiety symptoms and negative self-perception, particularly when they are preceded by cognitive preparation (CP) and followed by cognitive review. Aims: In the current study, we re-examined data from a study on the efficacy of a novel VF intervention for individuals high in social anxiety to test the hypothesis that physical appearance anxiety would moderate the effects of VF. Method: Data were analyzed from 68 socially anxious participants who performed an initial public speech, and were randomly assigned to an Elaborated VF condition (VF plus cognitive preparation and cognitive review), a Standard VF condition (VF plus cognitive preparation) or a No VF condition (exposure alone), and then performed a second speech. Results: As hypothesized, when appearance concerns were low, both participants who received Elaborated and Standard VF were significantly less anxious during speech 2 than those in the No VF condition. However, when levels of appearance concern were high, neither Elaborated nor Standard VF reduced anxiety levels during speech 2 beyond the No VF condition. Conclusions: Results from our analog sample suggest the importance of tailoring treatment protocols to accommodate the idiosyncratic concerns of socially anxious patients.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2013 

Introduction

A progression of empirical research over several decades has investigated the factors underlying social anxiety (SA) and social anxiety disorder (SAD). Central to these efforts have been varying conceptualizations of the role of social skills in the persistence of SA symptoms. In the late 1970s, proponents of the skills deficits model (Trower, Bryant and Argyle, Reference Trower, Bryant and Argyle1978) hypothesized that SA arises from deficits in one's repertoire of social behaviors and advocated for the use of social skills training (SST) in the treatment of SA (see Herbert et al., Reference Herbert, Gaudiano, Rheingold, Myers, Dalrymple and Nolan2005; Wlazlo, Schroeder-Hartwig, Hand, Kaiser and Munchau, Reference Wlazlo, Schroeder-Hartwig, Hand, Kaiser and Munchau1990). In support of this model, some studies have demonstrated that individuals high in SA show impairments in specific social skills relative to low SA individuals (e.g. Baker and Edelmann, Reference Baker and Edelmann2002; Fydric, Chambless, Perry, Buergener and Beazley, 1998); however, others have found that high SA individuals perform as adequately in social situations as non-anxious controls (Rapee and Lim, Reference Rapee and Lim1992; Strahan and Conger, Reference Strahan and Conger1998). Accordingly, proponents of cognitive models have argued that high SA individuals perceive their social abilities to be deficient and that it is these negatively distorted self-relevant beliefs that drive anxiety symptoms (Clark and Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2007; Rapee and Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997). In support of this viewpoint, individuals with high SA and SAD tend to underestimate their social skills relative to objective observers and rate themselves more negatively than do low SA controls despite being comparable on behavioral measures of actual social abilities (e.g. Rapee and Lim, Reference Rapee and Lim1992; Segrin and Kinney, Reference Segrin and Kinney1995). Even studies that have found high SA individuals to be objectively impaired on observer-rated measures of social performance have also demonstrated that such participants’ self-ratings significantly underestimate their actual performance (Norton and Hope, Reference Norton and Hope2001; Rapee and Abbott, Reference Rapee and Abbott2006).

In contrast to SST interventions that target actual skills deficits, video feedback (VF) has been examined in a number of studies as a potential remedy for perceived skills deficits in high SA individuals and those with a clinical diagnosis of SAD. In VF, individuals are provided with a video playback of their behavior following a social task and are directed to correct underestimations of perceived performance. When used with cognitive preparation (Harvey, Clark, Ehlers and Rapee, 2000), VF significantly reduces negative self-ratings of social performance amongst both non-treatment seeking high SA individuals (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Clark, Ehlers and Rapee2000; Kim, Lundh and Harvey, Reference Kim, Lundh and Harvey2002; Rodebaugh, Reference Rodebaugh2004) and treatment seeking patients with SAD (Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore, 2010). Some recent research suggests that significant reductions in subjective anxiety levels might require that VF is both preceded by cognitive preparation and followed by an elaborative cognitive review (Parr and Cartwright-Hatton, Reference Parr and Cartwright-Hatton2009; Orr and Moscovitch, Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010), while at least one study has reported such reductions in anticipatory anxiety with VF plus cognitive preparation alone (Rodebaugh et al., Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010).

Video feedback has been successfully integrated into cognitive behavioral treatment (CBT) protocols for patients with SAD (e.g. Clark et al., Reference Clark, Ehlers, McManus, Hackman, Fennell and Campbell2003; Mortberg, Clark, Sundin and Aberg Wistedt, Reference Mortberg, Clark, Sundin and Aberg Wistedt2007) and is thought to be a vital component of therapy. As such, it is now important to identify individual difference variables that may moderate the efficacy of VF on treatment outcomes. Identifying such moderators may help us understand how to tailor specific CBT interventions for particular patients based on a priori knowledge about who is likely to benefit from them and who is not. Ultimately, this process may lead to improved CBT outcomes and reductions in the proportion of socially anxious patients (currently about 25%-50%) who remain significantly symptomatic following CBT (e.g. Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Foa, Huppert, Keefe, Franklin and Compton2004; Otto et al., Reference Otto, Pollack, Gould, Worthington, McArdle and Rosenbaum2000).

Rodebaugh and his colleagues have reported that the discrepancy between self and observer ratings of performance during public speaking significantly moderates the relation between VF and changes in self-perception amongst both non-treatment seeking high SA individuals (Rodebaugh and Chambless, Reference Rodebaugh and Chambless2002; Rodebaugh and Rapee, Reference Rodebaugh and Rapee2006) and treatment seeking individuals with SAD (Rodebaugh et al., Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010). That is, individuals with higher underestimations of performance showed greater changes in self-perception following VF than those with lower underestimates. Further, Rodebaugh et al. (Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010) demonstrated that individuals with SAD who reported higher underestimations of performance showed greater reductions in ratings of state anxiety in anticipation of their speech relative to those with lower underestimations of performance. These results imply that VF is a particularly useful intervention for individuals with large baseline discrepancies between their perceived and actual level of performance – an important finding that may help clinicians tailor their interventions according to pretreatment differences between patients in negative self-perception. Apart from these studies, however, we know little about other individual difference variables that may moderate the effects of VF on treatment outcomes.

On the basis of clinical observation, Moscovitch (Reference Moscovitch2009) suggested that VF outcomes may be less efficacious for high SA individuals who are specifically worried about aspects of their appearance rather than their social skills per se. Moscovitch (Reference Moscovitch2009) proposed that socially anxious samples are heterogeneous with respect to each individual's primary focus of anxiety about negative self-portrayal, with individual differences in self-portrayal concerns potentially occurring across four non-orthogonal dimensions: (a) social skills; (b) personality; (c) signs of anxiety; and (d) physical appearance. Notably, Moscovitch's (2009) hypothesized link between SA and perceived flaws in physical appearance was based on research showing that individuals with SAD underestimate their level of physical attractiveness during social performances relative to ratings from objective observers, rate themselves more negatively than non-clinical controls on measures of physical attractiveness (Rapee and Abbott, Reference Rapee and Abbott2006), and are also more likely to endorse distorted body image (Izgiç, Akyüz, Dogan and Kugu, Reference Izgiç, Akyüz, Dogan and Kugu2004). Further, individuals high in SA are significantly more likely than individuals low in SA to report that their appearance is inherently flawed and socially unacceptable (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Flora, Palyo, Fresco, Holle and Heimberg2008). Moscovitch (Reference Moscovitch2009) proposed that perceived flaws in physical appearance combined with anxiety about revealing such flaws to others may drive symptoms of SA for a subset of individuals.

Subsequent empirical tests of Moscovitch's (2009) model supported a three-factor solution in which concerns about signs of anxiety and physical appearance each loaded onto their own respective factor, while concerns about social skills and personality loaded onto a single factor that was labeled “concerns about social competence” (Moscovitch and Huyder, Reference Moscovitch and Huyder2011). At present, standardized VF interventions are well-suited to address anxiety about social competence and signs of anxiety; indeed, socially anxious participants are asked during VF to pay attention to various observable indicators, including the quality of their voice and body language (social competence) as well as the extent to which they stutter, fidget, and sweat (signs of anxiety), and to consider the objective discrepancy between how they appear on video and how they thought or felt they appeared while they were interacting or performing (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Clark, Ehlers and Rapee2000; Rodebaugh, Reference Rodebaugh2004; Orr and Moscovitch, Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010). Through this process, VF promotes significant changes in self-perception as socially anxious individuals begin viewing themselves as being more socially competent and less visibly anxious than they initially thought or felt (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Clark, Ehlers and Rapee2000; Rodebaugh, Reference Rodebaugh2004; Orr and Moscovitch, Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010).

However, VF may be less than efficacious in targeting the third dimension of Moscovitch's (2009) model: perceived flaws in physical appearance. That is, clinical observation suggests that during VF, socially anxious patients who are particularly anxious about their physical appearance may become especially focused on their own perceived untoward physical characteristics and experience increased rather than decreased symptoms of anxiety upon exposure to their self-image. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that one's anxiety about physical appearance may moderate the efficacy of VF on treatment outcomes such that individuals with high appearance anxiety may benefit to a lesser extent from VF. This hypothesis has not yet been tested empirically in the context of SA. However, research on body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) has demonstrated that when individuals with BDD look in the mirror, they focus their attention on specific parts of their appearance (as opposed to the whole) or on an internal impression or feeling rather than the objective reality of their appearance (Mulkens and Jansen, Reference Mulkens and Jansen2009), and that mirror-gazing in such a manner is associated with increased distress and self-focus (Veale and Riley, Reference Veale and Riley2001). It is therefore possible that, like individuals with BDD who mirror-gaze, socially anxious individuals who are particularly high in appearance anxiety might respond to VF interventions with increased anxiety.Footnote 1

The present study sought to examine the extent to which physical appearance anxiety might moderate the relationship between VF and therapeutic outcomes amongst non-treatment seeking high SA individuals. In conjunction with this primary aim, we examined self-observer discrepancy as an additional moderator variable. To address our questions of moderation, we re-examined data that were collected as part of a prior experimental study on VF in which high SA participants completed two public speaking tasks in the laboratory (Orr and Moscovitch, Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010). The purpose of the initial study was to examine whether the standard VF protocol (which involved administering VF with cognitive preparation) could be optimized to facilitate reductions in self-reported SA by adding a post-VF cognitive review session (Elaborative VF). In Elaborative VF, participants were asked to compare discrepancies between their perception of their performance and their actual performance and answer elaborative questions about what they learned from VF. Results revealed that relative to the Standard and No VF conditions, only Elaborative VF was associated with reductions in performance anxiety from speech 1 to speech 2, and that Elaborative but not Standard VF led to significant improvements in self-perception and self-efficacy relative to exposure alone. Here we hypothesized that high appearance anxiety among participants who received VF (relative to exposure alone) would be associated with significantly less reduction in state performance anxiety from speech 1 to speech 2. We also hypothesized that greater self-observer discrepancies in participants’ initial speech performances would predict greater reductions in state performance anxiety from speech 1 to speech 2.

Method

Participants

As described in Orr and Moscovitch (Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010), 68 high SA individuals completed the study based on cut-off scores of 19 or higher on the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN; Connor et al., Reference Connor, Davidson, Churchill, Sherwood, Foa and Weisler2000), and 59 or higher on the Personal Report of Communication Apprehension (PRCA-24; McCroskey, Reference McCroskey1982). Consistent with Rodebaugh's (2004) approach, the PRCA-24 was used to ensure that participants were not only socially anxious but also specifically fearful of verbal communication (as per the experimental task). Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were randomly assigned to either Elaborated VF (n = 22; “CP + VF + CR” in Orr and Moscovitch, Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010); Standard VF (n = 21; “CP + VF” in the original paper) or No VF (n = 25; “Exposure Alone” in the original paper).

Measures

As reported above, all participants completed the SPIN and the PRCA-24 for pre-selection purposes. All participants also completed the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer and Brown, 1996), which allowed us to examine whether participants randomized across conditions may have unexpectedly differed in levels of depression. In the current study, these measures demonstrated strong internal consistency scores (all α's = .83 to .91). See Orr and Moscovitch (Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010) for a detailed description of these measures and their psychometric properties.

Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale: state version (PASTAS; Reed, Thompson, Brannick and Sacco, 1991)

The PASTAS is a 16-item measure that assesses anxiety about physical appearance. Respondents rate the extent to which they feel anxious, tense, or nervous “right now” about parts of their face and body on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 5 (exceptionally so). A principal components analysis of the scale performed by Reed et al. (Reference Reed, Thompson, Brannick and Sacco1991) revealed two components, which were labelled “weight” (e.g. thighs, buttocks) and “non-weight” attributes (e.g. ears, lips). The PASTAS has shown good test-retest reliability and internal consistency for each of the separate and combined subscales (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Thompson, Brannick and Sacco1991). In the current study, comparable results were found for analyses pertaining to each of the separate subscale scores and the total score; therefore only the full scale PASTAS score was included (α = .91).

Subjective Units of Distress Scale (SUDS)

After each of their two speeches, participants rated the anxiety they experienced during the speech at minutes one, two, and three (0–100). We computed the average of the three ratings for each speech, thus creating two composite SUDS scores (α = .83 and .89).

Perception of Speech Performance (PSP; Rapee and Lim, Reference Rapee and Lim1992)

The PSP (also referred to as the Speech Performance Questionnaire; Rapee and Abbott, Reference Rapee and Abbott2007) is a 17-item measure used to calculate self-observer discrepancies. Following their first speech, participants rated their performance across the items from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Two objective observers blind to the purpose and design of the study also later rated participants on the same indictors. Scores across the PSP items were summed separately for participants’ and observers’ ratings, with higher scores representing more negative perceptions. The interrater reliability for observer ratings (a two-way, mixed intraclass correlation coefficient for the consistency of the average measure) was .84, and total PSP scores of each observer were therefore averaged together to create one observer rating. Self-observer discrepancy was calculated in the manner described by Rodebaugh et al. (Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010), in which participants’ ratings of their first speech were regressed onto averaged observer ratings. The standardized residual of this analysis (i.e. the degree to which participants’ ratings could not be predicted by observers’ ratings) was used as a measure of self-observer discrepancy, with higher scores denoting greater self-observer discrepancies.

Procedure

Participants selected one of six controversial speech topics (e.g. the death penalty), prepared to give a 3-minute speech, and then delivered their speech in the laboratory in front of the experimenter and a video camera. They were informed that objective observers would later view and rate their speech performance. After their speech, participants estimated the level of anxiety they experienced at minutes one, two, and three. Participants randomly assigned to the Elaborated VF and the Standard VF conditions completed Cognitive Preparation (CP; Harvey et al., 2000) prior to VF, while those in the Elaborated VF condition also completed the post-VF Cognitive Review (CR). All participants then chose a different speech topic from the 6-item list and performed a second speech under the same conditions as the first. Following their second speech, they estimated their anxiety levels at minutes one, two, and three; see Orr and Moscovitch (Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010) for complete details.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Participants did not differ across conditions in trait social anxiety (SPIN), F, (2, 65) = 2.18, p = .12, partial η2 = .063, communication anxiety (PRCA-24), F, (2, 65) = 1.84, p = .17, partial η2 = .053, depressive symptoms (BDI-II), F (2, 66) = 1.06, p = .35, partial η2 = .054, or anxiety about physical appearance (PASTAS), F (2, 65) = 1.13, p = .33, partial η2 = .034. Anxiety about physical appearance (on the PASTAS full scale) was positively correlated with trait social anxiety (SPIN), r = .27, p = .024. Participants’ demographic information is available in Orr and Moscovitch (Reference Orr and Moscovitch2010).

Moderation: anxiety about physical appearance

A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted in SPSS 19.0 with average anxiety (SUDS) ratings during speech 2 regressed onto average SUDS ratings during speech 1 (as a covariate), the two dummy coded predictor variables (Elaborated VF and Standard VF, with No VF as the reference point), and anxiety about physical appearance (on the PASTAS full scale) as the moderator. The dummy coded interaction terms of Elaborated VF x anxiety about appearance and Standard VF x anxiety about appearance were entered in step 2.

As shown in Table 1, results revealed a significant omnibus ANOVA, F (6, 61) = 10.95, p < .001, R 2 = .52, with a main effect of condition. After controlling for average anxiety during speech 1, participants in the Elaborated VF condition reported significantly less anxiety during speech 2 (M = 43.23, SD = 25.44) than participants in the No VF condition (M = 57.31, SD = 19.15), B = −13.66, p = .012, R 2 = .053. Participants in the Standard VF condition also showed an advantage over No VF in reported anxiety during speech 2 (M = 45.33, SD = 26.30), but the effect only trended toward significance, B = −9.48, p = .076, R 2 = .026. There was no difference in anxiety during speech 2 between participants who received Elaborated VF and those who received Standard VF, B = 4.18, p = .45, R 2 = .005.

Table 1. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis predicting anxiety during speech 2 with anxiety about appearance as moderator (N = 68)

**p < .01; *p < .05.

As predicted, the effects of each of the VF manipulations relative to no VF on SA were, in fact, dependent on participants’ levels of concern about physical appearance (Elaborated VF x Anxiety about Appearance: B = .92, p = .036, R 2 = .036; Standard VF x Anxiety about Appearance: B = 1.01, p = .024, R 2 = .042). Results showed a significant change in R 2 following the addition of the two dummy coded interaction terms from Step 1 to Step 2 (ΔR 2 = .052, p = .044). The interaction terms were probed using the method proposed by Aiken and West (Reference Aiken and West1991) and described by Holmbeck (Reference Holmbeck2002). As shown in Figure 1, the Elaborated and the Standard VF showed a clear advantage over No VF in social anxiety reduction at low levels of concern about physical appearance (1 SD below the mean), (B = -25.18, p = .002, R 2 = .084 and B = -22.01, p = .003, R 2 = .077, respectively); moreover, there was no difference in anxiety reduction during speech 2 for participants who received Elaborated VF and those who received Standard VF (B = 3.17, p = .69, R 2 = .001). At high levels of concern about physical appearance (1 SD above the mean), VF (both Elaborated and Standard) showed no advantage over No VF in SA reduction; that is, there were no differences between any of the conditions in levels of reported anxiety during speech 2 at high levels of physical appearance concern (Elaborated VF vs. No VF: B = -2.14, p = .77, R 2 = .001; Standard VF vs. No VF: B = 3.05, p = .71, R 2 = .001; Elaborated VF vs. Standard VF: B = 5.19, p = .48, R 2 = .004). Differences in Figure 1 between the three conditions at low levels of concern about appearance appear to be due, in part, to an apparent heightening of anxiety from high to low appearance concerns amongst the No VF group. However, an examination of the within-condition simple slopes from high to low appearance concerns demonstrated that the slope for the No VF condition was non-significant, B = -.39, p = .22, R 2 = .012. The slope for the Standard VF condition from low to high appearance concerns was significant, B = .62, p = .047, R 2 = .033, while the slope for the Elaborated VF condition from low to high concerns about appearance trended toward significance, B = .54, p = .087, R 2 = .024.

Figure 1. The moderating effect of anxiety about physical appearance on self-reported anxiety (SUDS) ratings during speech 2 (controlling for speech 1 SUDS ratings).

Moderation: self-observer discrepancy

The hierarchical multiple regression analysis described above was repeated with self-observer discrepancy rather than appearance concerns entered as the moderator variable.Footnote 2 As displayed in Table 2, results revealed a significant omnibus ANOVA, F (6, 61) = 10.61, p < .001, R 2 = .46. However, the change in R 2 following the addition of the two dummy coded interaction terms from Step 1 to Step 2 was non-significant (ΔR 2 = .029, p = .17), as were the effects of the two interaction terms on self-reported anxiety, B Elaborated VF x self-observer discrepancy = 9.53, p = .080, R 2 = .025; B Standard VF x self-observer discrepancy = 8.15, p = .15, R 2 = .017. A post-hoc power analysis using the statistical program G-power demonstrated that there was relatively low power (.18) to detect the relatively small change in R2 following the addition of the two dummy coded interaction variables. It is therefore plausible that significant moderation with the self-observer discrepancy variable may have occurred with a larger sample size.Footnote 3

Table 2. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis predicting anxiety during speech 2 with self-observer discrepancy as moderator (N = 68)

**p < .01.

Discussion

The effect of VF on reductions in SA was dependent on the extent to which socially anxious participants were anxious about their physical appearance. When anxiety about physical appearance was low, participants who received either Elaborated or Standard VF reported significantly less anxiety following the intervention than those in the No VF condition. However, for those with high anxiety about physical appearance, neither Elaborated nor Standard VF led to significant reductions in SA above and beyond exposure alone without VF. Within-condition differences in state anxiety during speech 2 among participants who received VF demonstrated that those whose anxiety about appearance was low experienced greater anxiety reduction than those whose appearance anxiety was high.

These findings provide tentative support for recent claims that idiosyncratic self-attribute concerns may represent an important moderator of treatment outcomes in SA (Moscovitch, Reference Moscovitch2009). Indeed, VF protocols in their current format may not be efficacious for socially anxious patients high in appearance anxiety. Because current VF protocols for SA are designed to target concerns about social competence and signs of anxiety, they may serve as a distraction and therefore fail to help patients who possess unique core negative self-representations pertaining specifically to physical appearance concerns. Modifications to VF that help individuals pay attention to corrective information about specific threat stimuli are likely to facilitate changes in the content of existing self-schemas (Foa and McNally, Reference Foa, McNally and Rapee1996; Brewin, Reference Brewin2006). With supporting evidence from future studies, it might therefore be fruitful to consider employing VF interventions typically used in the treatment of BDD (Neziroglu, McKay, Todaro and Yaryura-Tobias, Reference Neziroglu, McKay, Todaro and Yaryura-Tobias1996; Phillips, Reference Phillips2005) to augment the treatment of individuals with SAD who are identified as having high levels of anxiety or concern about their physical appearance. This might include helping patients operationalize their specific appearance concerns prior to VF and then using VF to direct patients to pay specific attention to and correct discrepancies between their predictions about how their physical features will appear in the video and how they actually appear when viewed more objectively (Veale and Neziroglu, Reference Veale and Neziroglu2010).

Further, unlike Rodebaugh et al. (Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010) who found that individuals with SA with higher self-observer discrepancies demonstrated significantly greater reductions in anticipatory state SA than individuals with lower self-observer discrepancies, we found that self-observer discrepancy was not a significant moderator of the efficacy of VF on self-reported SA. However, the observed power for detecting the moderation effect was low – likely a result of both the small effect size of the interaction between VF and self-observer discrepancy and the limited sample size of the current study. Aside from low power, the null findings with respect to moderation may also have been due to differences between the current study and Rodebaugh et al.'s (2010) study in the time-points at which state anxiety was measured. That is, the current study assessed SA during participants’ speeches while Rodebaugh et al. (Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010) measured participants’ SA both in anticipation and after participants’ speeches. More research is needed to examine the moderating effects of self-observer discrepancy on self-reported SA in both clinical and analog samples and at various time points following administration of VF. In this manner, it is possible that participants’ concerns about physical appearance might also moderate the effect of VF on self-reported SA differently on the times at which SA is measured.

The present study was limited in a number of ways, including its use of post-hoc analyses, its relatively small sample size, and its focus on a convenient analog sample of non-treatment seeking participants. Further, although the PASTAS was useful for assessing state anxiety about physical appearance, it did not address the specific nature of participants’ concerns about their appearance. For example, for participants who endorsed exceptional anxiety about their face, we were unable to determine the specific reasons for this anxiety and the idiosyncratic nature of their concern. In this respect, it is possible that certain PASTAS items may have actually measured participants’ worries about revealing visible signs of anxiety rather than worries about their physical appearance per se. Indeed, Moscovitch (Reference Moscovitch2009) acknowledged that there is often substantial overlap between the dimensions of physical appearance and signs of anxiety in individuals with SAD. It was also unclear to what extent the PASTAS captured variance in actual concerns participants may have had about their own physical attributes (e.g. “I hate my legs”) versus anxiety about having such attributes evaluated negatively by others (“What if he/she criticizes my legs?”). Future research on VF could explicitly identify participants according to their primary self-related concerns (e.g. physical appearance, signs of anxiety, social competence, or a combination thereof) and in so doing, help to determine how to tailor VF protocols to target individuals’ symptoms most effectively. To this end, the Negative Self-Portrayal Scale (Moscovitch and Huyder, Reference Moscovitch and Huyder2011) may be used by researchers and clinicians to assess individuals’ explicit a priori concerns across these specific dimensions.

In conclusion, the present study is one of only a few to examine the moderating effects of individual differences on VF outcomes in SA, and the first to demonstrate that anxiety about physical appearance moderates the therapeutic benefits of VF. As clinical researchers continue to emphasize the need for an idiographic approach to CBT case formulation and treatment (e.g. Kuyken, Padesky and Dudley, Reference Kuyken, Padesky and Dudley2009), future experimental research on moderators of established CBT interventions will ultimately enable clinicians to tailor therapy to accommodate the specific presenting concerns and needs of individual patients.

Acknowledgements

This research was undertaken, in part, thanks to funding from the SSHRC J. Armand Bombardier Canada Graduate Doctoral Scholarship to the first author and funding from the Canada Research Chairs Program to the second author.

Footnotes

1 Though both individuals with SAD and those with BDD may perceive that problematic aspects of their appearance would have negative consequences for social evaluation, BDD and SAD are currently considered distinct disorders. In cases where a differential diagnosis is difficult to make, it may be useful to consider diagnosing BDD if the singularity and strength of focus on negative physical attributes are particularly high. Indeed, BDD tends to be associated with distorted, strongly-held beliefs that are specific to concerns about appearance (Veale, Kinderman, Riley and Lambrou, Reference Veale, Kinderman, Riley and Lambrou2003) whereas SAD is characterized by anxiety about perceived flaws in several interrelated areas, which may also include appearance (Moscovitch, Reference Moscovitch2009). Further, BDD tends to be primarily associated with self-directed feelings of disgust (Neziroglu, Hickey and McKay, Reference Neziroglu, Hickey and McKay2010) whereas SAD tends to be associated with excessive feelings of embarrassment and/or shame (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Lutwak and Ferrari, Reference Lutwak and Ferrari1997). Despite these differences, the two conditions are highly comorbid, share a similar age of onset and developmental trajectory, and show comparable cognitive biases for interpreting social information (see Fang and Hofmann, Reference Fang and Hofmann2010).

2 Combining the two moderators into one analysis led to a significant reduction in power which, in turn, reduced the relative strength of our effects, making them more difficult to interpret. Thus, for the sake of enhanced power and increased clarity, we opted to analyze the two hypothesized moderators in separate analyses.

3 Given that the moderating effect of self-observer discrepancy was non-significant, we were hesitant to probe the interaction terms further through post hoc tests. However, for the sake of comparing the current study's results with those from the literature (e.g. Rodebaugh et al., Reference Rodebaugh, Heimberg, Schultz and Blackmore2010), we have provided the following post hoc results: the Elaborated and the Standard VF conditions showed a clear advantage over No VF in social anxiety reduction for individuals with low self-observer discrepancies (B = -24.77, p = .002, R2 = .085 and B = -19.96, p = .010, R2 = .057, respectively); moreover, there was no difference in anxiety reduction during speech 2 for participants who received Elaborated VF and those who received Standard VF (B = 4.81, p = .57, R2 = .003). For participants with high self-observer discrepancies, VF (both Elaborated and Standard) showed no advantage over No VF in SA reduction; that is, there were no differences between any of the conditions in levels of reported anxiety during speech 2 for individuals with high self-observer discrepancies (Elaborated VF vs. No VF: B = -5.51, p = .48, R2 = .004; Standard VF vs. No VF: B = 3.65, p = .65, R2 = .002; Elaborated VF vs. Standard VF: B = 1.86, p = .79, R2 = .001).

References

Aiken, L. S. and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Baker, S. R. and Edelmann, R. J. (2002). Is social phobia related to lack of social skills? Duration of skill-related behaviours and ratings of behavioural adequacy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 243257. doi:10.1348/014466502760379118 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: Manual (2nd ed). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R. (2006). Understanding cognitive behaviour therapy: a retrieval competition account. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 765784. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., McManus, F., Hackman, A., Fennell, M., Campbell, H., et al. (2003). Cognitive therapy versus fluoxetine in generalized social phobia: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 10581067. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.71.6.1058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. M. and Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hope, D. A. and Schneier, F. R. (Eds.), Social Phobia: diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 6993). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Connor, K. M., Davidson, J. R. T., Churchill, L. E., Sherwood, A., Foa, E. and Weisler, R. H. (2000). Psychometric properties of the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN): new self-rating scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 379386. doi:10.1192/bjp.176.4.379 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, J. R. T., Foa, E. B, Huppert, J. D., Keefe, F. J., Franklin, M. E., Compton, J. S., et al. (2004). Fluoxetine, comprehensive cognitive behavioral therapy, and placebo in generalized social phobia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 10051013. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.10.1005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fang, A. and Hofmann, S. G. (2010). Relationship between social anxiety disorder and body dysmorphic disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 10401048. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B. and McNally, R. J. (1996). Mechanisms of change in exposure therapy. In Rapee, R. M. (Ed.), Current Controversies in the Anxiety Disorders (pp. 214227). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fydic, T., Chambless, D. L., Perry, K. J., Buergener, F. and Beazley, M. B. (1998). Behavioral assessment of social performance: a rating system for social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 9951010. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00069-2 Google Scholar
Hart, T. A., Flora, D. B., Palyo, S. A., Fresco, D. M., Holle, C. and Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Development and examination of the Social Appearance Anxiety Scale. Assessment, 15, 4859. doi:10.1177/1073191107306673 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G., Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A. and Rapee, R. (2000). Social anxiety and self-impression: cognitive preparation enhances the effects of video feedback following a stressful social task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 11831192. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00148-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbert, J. D., Gaudiano, B. A., Rheingold, A. A., Myers, V. H., Dalrymple, K. and Nolan, E. M. (2005). Social skills training augments the effectiveness of cognitive behavioral group therapy for social anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 36, 125138. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80061-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, S. G. (2007). Cognitive factors that maintain social anxiety disorder: a comprehensive model and its treatment implications. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36, 193209. doi:10.1080/16506070701421313 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and meditational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 8796. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.87 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izgiç, F., Akyüz, G., Dogan, O. and Kugu, N. (2004). Social phobia among university students and its relation to self-esteem and body image. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 630634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. Y., Lundh, L. G. and Harvey, A. G. (2002). The enhancement of video feedback by cognitive preparation in the treatment of social anxiety: a single session experiment. Journal of Behavior Therapy, 33, 1937. doi:10.1016/S0005-7916(02)00010-1 Google ScholarPubMed
Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A. and Dudley, R. (2009). Collaborative Case Conceptualization. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lutwak, N. and Ferrari, J. (1997). Shame-related social anxiety: replicating a link with various social interaction measures. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping: An International Journal, 10, 335340. doi: 10.1080/10615809708249307 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCroskey, J. C. (1982). An Introduction to Rhetorical Communication (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Mortberg, E., Clark, D. M., Sundin, O. and Aberg Wistedt, A. (2007). Intensive group cognitive treatment and individual cognitive therapy vs. treatment as usual in social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 115, 142154. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2006.00839.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moscovitch, D. A. (2009). What is the core fear in social phobia? A new model to facilitate individualized case conceptualization and treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, 123134. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.04.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, D. A. and Huyder, V. (2011). The negative self-portrayal scale: development, validation, and application to social anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 42, 183196. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulkens, S. and Jansen, A. (2009). Mirror gazing increases attractiveness in satisfied, but not in dissatisfied women: a model for body dysmorphic disorder? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 211218. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neziroglu, F., Hickey, M. and McKay, D. (2010). Psychophysiological and self-report components of disgust in body dysmorphic disorder: the effects of repeated exposure. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3, 4051. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2010.3.1.40 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neziroglu, F., McKay, D., Todaro, J. and Yaryura-Tobias, J. A. (1996). Effect of cognitive behavior therapy on persons with body dysmorphic disorder and comorbid axis II diagnoses. Behavior Therapy, 27, 6777. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(96)80036-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, P. J. and Hope, D. A. (2001). Kernels of truth or distorted perceptions: self and observer ratings of social anxiety and performance. Behavior Therapy, 32, 765786. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80020-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, E. M. and Moscovitch, D. A. (2010). Learning to re-appraise the self during video feedback for social anxiety: does depth of processing matter? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 728737. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Gould, R. A., Worthington, J. J. 3rd, McArdle, E. T. and Rosenbaum, J. F. (2000). A comparison of the efficacy of clonazepam and cognitive-behavioral group therapy for the treatment of social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 14, 345358. doi:10.1016/S0887-6185(00)00027-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, C. J. and Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2009). Social anxiety in adolescents: the effect of video feedback on anxiety and the self-evaluation of performance. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 16, 4654. doi:10.1002/cpp.599 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, K. (2005). The Broken Mirror: understanding and treating body dysmorphic disorder. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rapee, R. M. and Abbott, M. J. (2006). Mental representation of observable attributes in people with social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37, 113126. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2005.01.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, R. M. and Abbott, M. J. (2007). Modelling relationships between cognitive variables during and following public speaking in participants with social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 29772989. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, R. M. and Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741756. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00022-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, R. M. and Lim, L. (1992). Discrepancy between self- and observer ratings of performance in social phobics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 728731. doi:10.1037//0021-843X.101.4.728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, D. L., Thompson, J. K., Brannick, M. T. and Sacco, W. P. (1991). Development and validation of the Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale (PASTAS). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 323332. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(91)90032-O CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodebaugh, T. L. (2004). I might look OK, but I'm still doubtful, anxious, and avoidant: the mixed effects of enhanced video feedback on social anxiety symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 14351451. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodebaugh, T. L. and Chambless, D. L. (2002). The effects of video feedback on self-perception of performance: a replication and extension. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 629644. doi:10.1023/A:1020357210137 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodebaugh, T. L., Heimberg, R. G., Schultz, L. T. and Blackmore, M. (2010). The moderated effects of video feedback for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 663671. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.04.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodebaugh, T. L. and Rapee, R. M. (2006). Those who think they look worst respond best: self-observer discrepancy predicts response to video feedback following a speech task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 705715. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-9634-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segrin, C. and Kinney, T. (1995). Social skills deficits among the socially anxious: rejection from others and loneliness. Motivation and Emotion, 19, 124. doi:10.1007/BF02260670 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strahan, E. and Conger, A. J. (1998). Social anxiety and its effects on performance and perception. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 293305. doi:10.1016/S0887-6185(98)00016-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trower, P., Bryant, B. and Argyle, M. (1978). Social Skills and Mental Health. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Veale, D., Kinderman, P., Riley, S. and Lambrou, C. (2003). Self-discrepancy in body dysmorphic disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 157169. doi:10.1348/014466503321903571 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veale, D. and Neziroglu, F. (2010). Body Dysmorphic Disorder: a treatment manual. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veale, D. and Riley, S. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who is the ugliest of them all? The psychopathology of mirror gazing in body dysmorphic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 13811393. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00102-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wlazlo, Z., Schroeder-Hartwig, K., Hand, I., Kaiser, G. and Munchau, N. (1990). Exposure in-vivo vs. social skills training for social phobia: long-term outcome and differential effects. Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 28, 181193. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(90)90001-Y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis predicting anxiety during speech 2 with anxiety about appearance as moderator (N = 68)

Figure 1

Figure 1. The moderating effect of anxiety about physical appearance on self-reported anxiety (SUDS) ratings during speech 2 (controlling for speech 1 SUDS ratings).

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis predicting anxiety during speech 2 with self-observer discrepancy as moderator (N = 68)

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.