Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T23:13:02.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relevance of uncertainty and goal conflict to mental disorders, their prevention and management: a unifying approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2016

Danielle A. Einstein*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, NSW, Australia
Warren Mansell
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr D. A. Einstein, Honorary Associate, Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, NSW 2109, Australia (email: danielle.einstein@mq.edu.au).
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Intolerance of Uncertainty (IU) has been shown to underlie a range of disorders. Technological advances have produced a decline in our development of an ability to wait in the face of uncertainty. The paper provides an update on empirical, theoretical and neural research in IU. Einstein's extended trandiagnostic model of IU is described. This model is based on control theory. The research update and IU model propose specific tools which can be implemented within transdiagnostic treatment approaches.

Type
Review
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2016 

Introduction

Over the past 25 years, burgeoning research on the impact of ‘intolerance of uncertainty’ (IU) on general health and on psychiatric disorders has begun to command attention in clinical arenas. IU has been defined as the dispositional tendency ‘to react negatively on an emotional, cognitive, and behavioural level to uncertain situations’ (Buhr & Dugas, Reference Buhr and Dugas2009, p. 216). Individuals vary in their willingness to experience uncertainty, and discomfort with uncertainty has been shown to increase with escalating comorbidity in Axis I diagnoses (Mahoney & McEvoy, Reference Mahoney and McEvoy2011; Carleton, Reference Carleton2012, Reference Carleton2016; Einstein, Reference Einstein2014). IU is argued to significantly contribute to personal and economic costs on the health system (Carleton, Reference Carleton2012).

Do existing psychological treatments target IU?

Estimates of certainty underlie cognitive models of anxiety. The traditional danger expectancy model (Reiss, Reference Reiss1991; Chan & Lovibond, Reference Chan and Lovibond1996; Beck & Clark, Reference Beck and Clark1997) argues that anxiety may arise from either an overestimation of the probability or cost of the feared event; or an underestimation of the individual's ability to cope with the feared event should it occur. For example, Beck & Clark (Reference Beck and Clark1997) describe danger expectancy as an underlying feature of anxiety. They propose that it is the ‘propensity of [the] information processing apparatus to inappropriately generate threat meaning assignments to innocuous stimuli that is the main problem that must be rectified in the treatment of anxiety disorders’ (p. 51). Nevertheless, Einstein et al. (Reference Einstein, Garradd, Boulton and Farkash2016) demonstrated that IU is not automatically altered through cognitive challenging, exposure and behavioural experiments in a high-school prevention programme. However, IU was altered for high-symptom students post-intervention when uncertainty was labelled and specifically targeted within the programme. Robichaud & Dugas (Reference Robichaud, Dugas, Davey and Wells2006) and Whittal & McLean (Reference Whittal, McLean, Frost and Steketee2002) have designed treatments which successfully alter IU within GAD and OCD populations.

Control theory and the extended transdiagnostic model of IU (Einstein, Reference Einstein2014)

Control theory can provide an explanation of how the individual's reaction to uncertainty leads to arousal and can be used to guide effective clinical treatment (Einstein, Reference Einstein2014). Control theory has its origins in control engineering and proposes that the brain operates in a way analogous to homeostatic control systems in biology – through negative feedback control (Carey et al. Reference Carey, Mansell and Tai2014). Specifically, we base our approach on an integration of independent work by Jeffrey Gray, and William T. Powers (Powers, Reference Powers1973; Gray, Reference Gray1982; McNaughton & Gray, Reference McNaughton and Gray2000; Einstein, Reference Einstein2014). According to control theory, psychological distress arises from the loss of control that results from unresolved conflict between competing goals (Powers, Reference Powers1973; Mansell, Reference Mansell2005; Higginson et al. Reference Higginson, Mansell and Wood2011; Einstein, Reference Einstein2014). Within this context, an uncertain event is one for which there is ambiguity regarding the most adaptive goal to pursue at that moment. This state is marked by persistent arousal because the physiological preparation for action is blocked. Gray and colleagues describe this anxious state as ‘defensive approach’. Figure 1 describes the extended transdiagnostic model of uncertainty.

Fig. 1. Model of Intolerance and Uncertainty. IU, Intolerance and Uncertainty; TE, threat estimate.

According to the transdiagnostic model of uncertainty (Einstein, Reference Einstein2014; Fig. 1), when faced with uncertainty, the individual first makes a threat estimate (TE). This estimate is an imagined aversive consequence. Gilbert & Wilson (Reference Gilbert and Wilson2009) describe the TE as a mental simulation or preview of the future. Multiple TEs may be generated. These estimates are drawn from cognitive biases in our memories of past experiences (see further Gilbert & Wilson, Reference Gilbert and Wilson2009). Figure 1 shows that TEs feed directly into the comparator system.

The comparator function is an important component of control theory. While Powers (Reference Powers1973) makes it clear that comparators are present throughout the nervous system to implement control, Gray and colleagues describe how a central comparator system may halt an operating program in an attempt to resolve the mismatch between the perceived threat and the individual's desired outcome during defensive approach. According to Powers (Reference Powers1973), a trial-and-error learning process known as reorganization allows the resolution of goal conflict to occur during this conflicted state. The reorganization requires sustained attention on the systems governing the goal conflict for it to eventually be resolved, and for adaptive actions to proceed (Carey, Reference Carey2011). Paradoxically, this state entails that people may experience short-term loss of control, in order to regain long-term control. Control of a wide range of variables is itself key to well-being. Examples include physiological variables such as body temperature and blood glucose levels, and psychological variables such as one's desired self-concept, one's social relationships and one's principles. However, because of the inevitability of conflict, control of one or more of these variables may be temporarily suspended to allow conflict to reduce and change to occur. For example, one might need to let go of one's attempt to be dominant in social relationships in order to allow a therapist to ask questions to help one explore one's priorities in relationships. In letting go of control at this time, changes in priorities may occur spontaneously or other ways to solve related problems may emerge. A key question therefore, is what kind of ‘stance’ towards one's experiences can facilitate change in this way (see also Carey, Reference Carey2011).

For the first time, we describe here how an Effort Intensity Continuum integrates a wide range of psychological models of engagement with internal states such as uncertainty (Fig. 2). This is demonstrated as follows. A successful shift in stance moves from effortful suppression (Richards & Gross, Reference Richards and Gross1999) and avoidance of the internal state (e.g. Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl1996) at one end, through intolerance (Buhr & Dugas, Reference Buhr and Dugas2009), towards a gradient of tolerance, to an ability to simply notice an uncertain state with a small amount of effort as often described within mindfulness meditation (Bishop et al. Reference Bishop, Lau, Shapiro, Carlson, Anderson, Carmody, Segal, Abbey, Speca, Velting and Devins2004) and metacognitive awareness (Wells, Reference Wells2002). From a control theory perspective, this shift allows awareness to be sustained on the most relevant personal goals, with sufficient time for reorganization to generate a successful resolution. ‘Insight’ may be also experienced as going ‘up levels’ to become aware of the superordinate goals that are driving conflict within a hierarchy. For example, a person who is uncertain as to whether to try to avoid meeting strangers or to approach them may realize that both goals are driven by the goal to be liked (and not rejected) by other people.

Fig. 2. The Effort Intensity Continuum.

There are two paths within the Transdiagnostic model. According to path 1 (Fig. 1), a TE is detected as conflicting with one's goals and is associated with emotional arousal described as a ‘premotion’ (Gilbert & Wilson, Reference Gilbert and Wilson2009). Within the model a separate component of arousal that follows in high Prospective IU individuals has been labelled ‘uncertainty arousal’. Uncertainty arousal may be subtle and is generally not accompanied by insight; the arousal is nonetheless experienced as unacceptable and is linked with a range of behaviours aimed at an immediate reduction of the arousal. Such behaviours include reassurance seeking, safety behaviours, and compulsions. They strive to manage the arousal without returning to sustain attention and reorganize goals. Path 2 within the model emphasizes the value of reflection. It suggests that reflection is indicated. This sometimes necessitates social support. Activation of the approach system occurs on this path. The model emphasizes that responses to uncertainty only trigger arousal when they occur in an area of importance to the individual (through activation of the central comparator system).

Neural support for the model

The Neural model explains the integration of emotion with uncertainty in the form of a distinct feeling state (Singer et al. Reference Singer, Critchley and Preuschoff2009). This interoceptive state is represented in the Uncertainty model (Fig. 1; Einstein, Reference Einstein2014) under the label of Uncertainty Arousal. Uncertainty Arousal possesses a motivational function, as it activates a behavioural prevention system. The anterior insula has been hypothesized to underlie this state by integrating somatic, affective and sensory responses to uncertainty. Evidence for this hypothesis is accumulating (Critchley et al. Reference Critchley, Mathias and Dolan2001; Simmons et al. Reference Simmons, Matthews, Paulus and Stein2008; Singer et al. Reference Singer, Critchley and Preuschoff2009). Shankman et al. (Reference Shankman, Gorka, Nelson, Fitzgerald, Phan and O'Daly2014) examined healthy controls. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analyses, they observed that the right anterior insula cortex was more strongly activated during the anticipation of unpredictable aversive images compared to predictable aversive images. Moreover, activation within this area was related to subjective reports of intolerance of uncertainty. Findings of increased activation in this area have also been noted in fMRI studies involving healthy volunteers (Rubio et al. Reference Rubio, Oudenhove, Pellissier, Giao, Dupont, Micheaux, Tack, Dantzer, Delon-Martin and Bonaz2015).

In processing uncertainty, hypervigilance is proposed to correspond to amygdala activation (Grupe & Nitschke, Reference Grupe and Nitschke2013). Krain et al. (Reference Krain, Gotimer, Hefton, Ernst, Castellanos, Pine and Milham2008) suggested that individuals with low IU exhibit amygdala deactivation when faced with uncertainty while individuals with high IU display the opposing neural response. Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Oler, Fox, McFarlin, Rogers, Jesson, Davidson, Pine and Kalin2015) examined fMRI results for children with anxiety disorders and matched controls. Children with anxiety disorders demonstrated greater amygdala activation that the matched controls when presented with unpleasant facial expressions.

Transdiagnostic approach to treatment

The question of how and why to provide a transdiagnostic psychological intervention as opposed to a disorder-specific intervention applies to interventions that are informed by any theoretical approach and is covered in detail elsewhere (e.g. Mansell et al. Reference Mansell, Harvey, Watkins and Shafran2009; McHugh et al. Reference McHugh, Murray and Barlow2009). Advantages of the transdiagnostic approach include the efficiencies involved in training and delivery when diagnosis is not required. With regards to more recent evidence on efficacy, there appears to be no clear advantage of either approach. For example, in a recent meta-analysis, transdiagnostic and disorder-specific CBT were seen to produce comparable effects with regard to clinical significance after acknowledging multiple sources of bias (Pearl & Norton, Reference Pearl and Norton2016). A second meta-analysis drew a preliminary conclusion that transdiagnostic approaches may be equivalent to disorder-specific approaches for the treatment of anxiety and have superior effects for the treatment of depression, thereby implicating a slight preference for use in presentations of comorbid anxiety and depression (Newby et al. Reference Newby, McKinnon, Kuyken, Gilbody and Dalgleish2015). However, this conclusion was based on four studies, reflecting a general paucity of research to firmly establish relative efficacy. In practice, the extent to which disorder-specific information is used often depends on the tradition within the service. Our model is therefore often applied in the context of psychoeducation about the meaning of symptoms of a disorder with which the patient has been diagnosed. For example, panic attacks in clear presentations of panic disorder may be explained and normalized, as might intrusive thoughts in people diagnosed with a primary obsessive compulsive disorder. Yet, it is clear that the symptoms of a disorder are rarely unique to that disorder (Mansell et al. Reference Mansell, Carey and Tai2015), and so even psychoeducation rarely proceeds on purely diagnostic lines.

According to the transdiagnostic model of uncertainty (Fig. 1), when exposed to an area of uncertainty, individuals choose to either avoid reflecting on the meaning of the threat (path 1) or to engage in structured reflection (path 2). Path 1 is associated with worry and symptoms of anxiety disorders, whereas path 2 leads to a healthier and more flexible response to the threat.

In order to assist patients manage their response to uncertainty, we carry out the following:

  1. (1) Identification of Prospective IU. This is based on the IUS-12 Scale. The IUS-12 is composed of two subscales: Anticipatory IU and Prospective IU (Carleton et al. Reference Carleton, Norton and Asmundson2007). Prospective IU emphasizes the individual's rigid need to be prepared to maximize control, e.g. ‘I always want to know what the future has in store for me’, ‘Unforeseen events upset me greatly’, ‘I can't stand being taken by surprise’ and ‘One should always look ahead to avoid surprises’. The rigid need for control assessed by the nature of prospective IU leads to path 1. Once labelled, the clinician is able to target Prospective IU as described below.

  2. (2) Management of physical, cognitive and behavioural responses to uncertainty. The transdiagnostic approach encourages the use of the following techniques that to some degree overlap with Cognitive Behavioural and Acceptance & Commitment approaches.

    1. (a) Client-driven exposure to the interoceptive experience of uncertainty, such that their stance shifts towards tolerating greater intensities of this state with less effort required. The Effort Intensity Continuum (Fig. 2) illustrates how stances towards internal experiences such as states of uncertainty lie on a continuum. The optimum way to effectively deal with uncertainty is through shifting the stance towards the experience of arousal during states of uncertainty. The continuum extends from complete avoidance, through tolerance, to the ability to face, accept or even embrace the benefits of an uncertain state.

    2. (b) Curious questioning regarding any catastrophic thinking that emerges when faced with ambiguity in areas perceived as important.

    3. (c) Developing insight into the behavioural response to uncertainty. Social media apps, iphones and search engines have lowered the threshold with which individuals need to sit with uncertainty in everyday life. Nominating a realistic time-frame within which an answer can be expected assists the individual to refrain from action. This may then be combined with mindfulness and emotion regulation and is best implemented in unison with point (3) below.

      The above techniques target conflict occurring at different levels of a clients’ goal hierarchy (Alsawy et al. Reference Alsawy, Mansell, Carey, McEvoy and Tai2014). Alsawy et al. (Reference Alsawy, Mansell, Carey, McEvoy and Tai2014) explain how techniques that address both higher level goals and lower level perceptual experiences should predominate. Take the example of a client with health anxiety who is struggling with the uncertainty of whether they have a life-threatening illness. The therapist would explore with them the life goals that span these levels: ‘When you notice that you might have the illness, what do you start to think about?’; ‘What would things be like for you with the illness, and without it?’ The therapist may use Method of Levels techniques (Carey, Reference Carey2006) to help catch background thoughts about the goals surrounding the uncertainty, and to explore current feelings, impulses and mental imagery (Alsawy et al. Reference Alsawy, Mansell, Carey, McEvoy and Tai2014). Reflection on goal conflict within specific spheres of life (e.g. work, school, peers, family, self) supports the development of healthy responses to uncertainty.

  3. (3) Building patience. Finally, many industries aim to exploit the incredulous person's desire to avoid uncertainty. People strive for certainty over matters such as child care, medical diagnosis and social approval. Commercial organizations may exploit this need by appearing to provide certainty for a high price. The ability to wait and tolerate uncertainty is therefore more widely advantageous. Tarot card readers, supernatural theorists and clairvoyants also take advantage of uncertainty and these examples may be shared with clients. For a detailed example of such an exercise, see Einstein & Menzies (Reference Einstein, Menzies and Einstein2007, pp. 23–24). Einstein et al. (Reference Einstein, Garradd, Boulton and Farkash2016) demonstrated a small but significant lowering of IU, using a one-session internet program (in comparison to a control internet program), when such an exercise was combined with mindfulness. This finding was based on participants drawn from a university and general adult population.

Summary

We have described a transdiagnostic approach to uncertainty that is guided by an established psychological theory of control. The therapy brings together a range of familiar techniques with a central focus on developing insight. By utilizing control theory we have developed the Effort Intensity Continuum that spans a range of psychological approaches in order to accompany exposure within states of uncertainty.

Acknowledgements

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of Interest

The authors have no conflict of interest with respect to this publication.

Learning objectives

  1. (1) To integrate a transdiagnostic treatment approach with recent theoretical and empirical developments in our understanding of Intolerance of Uncertainty (IU).

  2. (2) To understand the role of control theory in contributing to arousal when clients are confronted with uncertainty.

  3. (3) To consider a range of methods to intervene to reduce IU.

  4. (4) To provide an update on neural mechanisms which underlie a desire to avoid uncertainty.

References

Recommended follow-up reading

Carleton, RN (2016). Into the unknown: a review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 39, 3043.Google Scholar

References

Alsawy, S, Mansell, W, Carey, TA, McEvoy, P, Tai, SJ (2014). Science and practice of transdiagnostic CBT: a Perceptual Control Theory (PCT) approach. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy 7, 334359.Google Scholar
Beck, A, Clark, D (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy 35, 4958.Google Scholar
Bishop, SR, Lau, M, Shapiro, S, Carlson, L, Anderson, ND, Carmody, J, Segal, ZV, Abbey, S, Speca, M, Velting, D, Devins, G (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 11, 230241.Google Scholar
Buhr, K, Dugas, MJ (2009). The role of fear of anxiety and intolerance of uncertainty in worry: An experimental manipulation, Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 215223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, TA (2006). The Method of Levels: How to do Psychotherapy without Getting in the Way. Living Control Systems Publishing: Chicago.Google Scholar
Carey, TA (2011). Exposure and reorganization: the what and how of effective psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 236248.Google Scholar
Carey, TA, Mansell, W, Tai, SJ (2014). A biopsychosocial model based on negative feedback and control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, Chicago.Google Scholar
Carleton, RN (2012). The intolerance of uncertainty construct in the context of anxiety disorders: theoretical and practical perspectives. Expert review of Neurotherapeutics 12, 937947.Google Scholar
Carleton, RN (2016). Into the unknown: a review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 39, 3043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carleton, RN, Norton, PJ, Asmundson, GJG (2007). Fearing the unknown: a short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS-12). Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21, 105117.Google Scholar
Chan, KYC, Lovibond, PF (1996). Expectancy bias in trait anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 637647.Google Scholar
Critchley, HD, Mathias, CJ, Dolan, RJ (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron 29, 537545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einstein, DA (2014). Extension of the transdiagnostic model to focus on intolerance of uncertainty: a review of the literature and implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 21, 280300.Google ScholarPubMed
Einstein, DA, Garradd, A, Boulton, K, Farkash, L (2016). Is intolerance of uncertainty adequately addressed in intervention programs? Paper presented at the 8th World Congress of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies, Melbourne, June, 2016.Google Scholar
Einstein, DA, Menzies, RG (2007). The treatment of magical ideation. In: Innovations and Advances in Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (ed. Einstein, D. A.), pp. 1935. Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, DT, Wilson, TD (2009). Why the brain talks to itself: sources of error in emotional prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 364, 13351341.Google Scholar
Gray, JA (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry in to the Functions of the Septo-hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grupe, DW, Nitschke, JB (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience 14, 488501.Google Scholar
Hayes, SC, Wilson, KG, Gifford, EV, Follette, VM, Strosahl, K (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64, 11521168.Google Scholar
Higginson, S, Mansell, W, Wood, AM (2011). An integrative mechanistic account of psychological distress, therapeutic change and recovery: the Perceptual Control theory approach. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 249259.Google Scholar
Krain, AL, Gotimer, K, Hefton, S, Ernst, M, Castellanos, FX, Pine, DS, Milham, MP (2008). A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of uncertainty in adolescents with anxiety disorders. Biological Psychiatry 63, 563568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, AEJ, McEvoy, PM (2011). A transdiagnostic examination of intolerance of uncertainty across anxiety and depressive disorders. Cognitive Behavior Therapy. Published online: 28 October 2011. doi:10.1080/16506073.2011.622130 Google Scholar
Mansell, W (2005). Control theory and psychopathology: an integrative approach. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 78, 141178.Google Scholar
Mansell, W, Carey, TA, Tai, S (2015). Classification of psychopathology and unifying theory: the ingredients of a Darwinian paradigm shift in research methodology. Psychopathology Review 2, 129153.Google Scholar
Mansell, W, Harvey, A, Watkins, E, Shafran, R (2009). Conceptual foundations of the transdiagnostic approach to CBT. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 23, 619.Google Scholar
McHugh, RK, Murray, HW, Barlow, DH (2009). Balancing fidelity and adaptation in the dissemination of empirically-supported treatments: the promise of transdiagnostic interventions. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 946953.Google Scholar
McNaughton, N, Gray, JA (2000). Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders 61, 161176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newby, JM, McKinnon, A, Kuyken, W, Gilbody, S, Dalgleish, T (2015). Systematic review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and depressive disorders in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review 40, 91110.Google Scholar
Pearl, SB, Norton, PJ (2016). Transdiagnostic versus diagnosis specific cognitive behavioural therapies for anxiety: a meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. Published online: 16 July 2016. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.07.004.Google Scholar
Powers, WT (1973). Feedback: beyond behaviourism: stimulus-response laws are wholly predictable within a control-system model of behavioural organisation. Science 179, 351356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, S (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology Review 11, 41153.Google Scholar
Richards, JM, Gross, JJ (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of emotion suppression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25, 10331044.Google Scholar
Robichaud, M, Dugas, MJ (2006). A cognitive behavioral treatment targeting intolerance for uncertainty. In: Worry and its Psychological Disorders (ed. Davey, G. & Wells, A.), pp. 289304. Wiley: Chichester.Google Scholar
Rubio, A, Oudenhove, LV, Pellissier, S, Giao, H, Dupont, P, Micheaux, HL, Tack, J, Dantzer, C, Delon-Martin, C, Bonaz, B (2015). Uncertainty in anticipation of uncomfortable rectal distension is modulated by the autonomic nervous system – a fMRI study in healthy volunteers. NeuroImage 107, 1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankman, SA, Gorka, SM, Nelson, BD, Fitzgerald, DA, Phan, KL, O'Daly, O (2014). Anterior insula responds to temporally unpredictable aversiveness: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 25, 596600.Google Scholar
Simmons, A, Matthews, SC, Paulus, MP, Stein, MB (2008). Intolerance of uncertainty correlates with insula activation during affective ambiguity. Neuroscience Letters 430, 9297.Google Scholar
Singer, T, Critchley, HD, Preuschoff, K (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 334340.Google Scholar
Wells, A (2002). GAD, metacognition, and mindfulness: an information processing analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 9, 95100.Google Scholar
Whittal, ML, McLean, PD (2002). Group cognitive behavioural therapy for obsessive compulsive disorder. In Cognitive Approaches to Obsessions and Compulsions: Theory, Assessment and Treatment (ed. Frost, R. O. & Steketee, G.), pp. 417434. Amsterdam: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Williams, LE, Oler, JA, Fox, AS, McFarlin, DR, Rogers, GM, Jesson, MAL, Davidson, RJ, Pine, DS, Kalin, NH (2015). Fear of the unknown: uncertain anticipation reveals amygdala activation in childhood anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 14281435.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Model of Intolerance and Uncertainty. IU, Intolerance and Uncertainty; TE, threat estimate.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. The Effort Intensity Continuum.

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.