Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T20:52:05.278Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The cooperative breeding perspective helps in pinning down when uniquely human evolutionary processes are necessary

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2016

Judith Maria Burkart
Affiliation:
Anthropological Institute and Museum, University of Zurich.Judith.Burkart@aim.uzh.chvschaik@aim.uzh.chwww.aim.uzh.ch
Carel P. van Schaik
Affiliation:
Anthropological Institute and Museum, University of Zurich.Judith.Burkart@aim.uzh.chvschaik@aim.uzh.chwww.aim.uzh.ch

Abstract

The cultural group selection (CGS) approach provides a compelling explanation for recent changes in human societies, but has trouble explaining why our ancestors, rather than any other great ape, evolved into a hyper-cooperative niche. The cooperative breeding hypothesis can plug this gap and thus complement CGS, because recent comparative evidence suggests that it promoted proactive prosociality, social transmission, and communication in Pleistocene hominins.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Richerson et al. address two key changes in human evolution, the first being how cooperation could evolve in the small-scale Pleistocene societies of prehistoric times, and the second being how these small-scale societies successfully evolved into much larger and more complex societies during the Holocene. The authors' case for a role of cultural group selection (CGS) in the second transition is strong. However, we will argue that the adoption of cooperative breeding suffices to explain the origin of human hyper-cooperation in early forager societies, as it resulted in increased prosociality and social transmission and favored the emergence of language.

Richerson et al. mention cooperative breeding as a possible trigger of the process involving CGS, but argue that this alternative hypothesis is difficult to test independently. However, recent comparative work exploring the psychological and cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in nonhuman primates now increasingly allows us to identify general patterns that reliably emerge whenever a primate species adopts cooperative breeding. It is thus most parsimonious to assume that such psychological and cognitive consequences also arose when our hominin ancestors, but none of the other great apes, started to engage in cooperative breeding (Hrdy Reference Hrdy2009; Kramer Reference Kramer2010). Together, these consequences are likely to have paved the way for the emergence of CGS processes (van Schaik & Burkart Reference van Schaik, Burkart, Kappeler and Silk2010).

Proactive prosociality

The first and perhaps most important consequence of cooperative breeding is that it leads to higher levels of proactive prosociality at the group level. To test this, we used the group service paradigm, in which an individual can provide food to other group members without receiving any food itself, and applied it to 24 groups of 15 species of nonhuman primates (Burkart et al. Reference Burkart, Allon, Amici, Fichtel, Finkenwirth, Heschl, Huber, Isler, Kosonen, Martins, Meulman, Richiger, Rueth, Spillmann, Wiesendanger and van Schaik2014). The results (Fig. 1A) show that the extent of allomaternal care directed at immatures is the best predictor of experimentally assessed proactive prosociality within a group, far better in fact than any other factor proposed so far, including brain size, social tolerance, cooperative hunting, or the presence of strong social bonds. Most importantly, humans perfectly fit the nonhuman primate regression, suggesting that we do not need a special explanation for our proactive prosociality.

Figure 1. The relationship between allomaternal care and (A) proactive prosociality and (B) social tolerance. Humans fit the general primate trend and do not represent an outlier.

Social transmission

Cooperatively breeding primates, the callitrichid monkeys, consistently show enhanced performance in social learning tasks (Burkart & van Schaik Reference Burkart and van Schaik2010; Snowdon Reference Snowdon2001) compared to their independently breeding sister taxa, most likely due to their high social tolerance (Coussi-Korbel & Fragaszy Reference Coussi-Korbel and Fragaszy1995), which, like proactive prosociality, demonstrably increases with increasing levels of allomaternal care across primates (Fig. 1B). Indeed, callitrichid monkeys not only show high levels of social learning, but also the best evidence for teaching among nonhuman primates comes from callitrichids (Dell'Mour et al. Reference Dell'Mour, Range and Huber2009; Humle & Snowdon Reference Humle and Snowdon2008; Rapaport Reference Rapaport2011), and not from the intelligent, yet independently breeding great apes (Boesch Reference Boesch1991; Humle et al. Reference Humle, Snowdon and Matsuzawa2009; Lonsdorf Reference Lonsdorf2006). Among non-primates, teaching is similarly over-represented in cooperatively breeding species (Byrne & Rapaport Reference Byrne and Rapaport2011; Kline Reference Kline2015).

Communication and language

Callitrichids also show remarkably complex vocal communication (Rukstalis et al. Reference Rukstalis, Fite and French2003; Zuberbühler Reference Zuberbühler, Tallerman and Gibson2012), with large vocal repertoires and unusual levels of vocal learning, both as immatures (including babbling) and as adults, as well as vocal control (Roy et al. Reference Roy, Miller, Gottsch and Wang2011; Snowdon Reference Snowdon and Helekar2013). Intriguingly, and reflecting their prosocial attitude, cooperatively breeding monkeys regularly use food-offering calls (Vitale et al. Reference Vitale, Zanzoni, Queyras and Chiarotti2003) and cooperative vocal communication (Takahashi et al. Reference Takahashi, Narayanan and Ghazanfar2013), in that they take turns in extended sequences of call exchanges that are based on conversation rules strikingly similar to those used by humans (Stivers et al. Reference Stivers, Enfield, Brown, Englert, Hayashi, Heinemann, Hoymann, Rossario, de Ruiter, Yoon and Levinson2009). Notably, apes altogether lack communicative turn taking. Information donation (in the form of functionally referential food calls, teaching, or vocal cooperation; Takahashi et al. Reference Takahashi, Narayanan and Ghazanfar2013) is thus over-represented in cooperatively breeding primates compared to independently breeding ones. It is the basis for language, which explains why apes, who possess most basic cognitive requirements for language but largely lack this prosocial motivation (Tomasello Reference Tomasello2007), never evolved language.

These broad evolutionary trends can explain why human psychology became different from that of the other great apes: Alone among great apes, our ancestors started to rear their offspring cooperatively. Extensive allomaternal care is a better explanation than the other source of human cooperation, male bonding and the resulting cooperative hunting and indirect reciprocity in food sharing, because in other male-bonded or cooperatively hunting species, we do not see similar prosocial tendencies, unless they also are cooperative breeders (Burkart et al. Reference Burkart, Hrdy and Van Schaik2009; Burkart & van Schaik Reference Burkart and van Schaik2010).

In sum, comparative evidence supports the idea that cooperative breeding installed in our ancestors the psychological predispositions that functioned as the pre-adaptive substrate upon which CGS could eventually be built: the design for group function (language – see target article, sect. 6.1) and mechanisms maintaining intergroup variation (social learning – sect. 2; prosociality – sect. 6.3). This argument enhances the plausibility of CGS in the Holocene transition. First, without this foundation, CGS has a circularity problem. High-fidelity, low-cost signaling had to evolve first in order to allow gene-culture coevolutionary processes to install a more prosocial psychology, which in turn would have paved the way for high-fidelity, low-cost signaling and eventually language. Second, the adoption of cooperative breeding offers a plausible explanation for why our ancestors, but none of the other great apes, took this extraordinary trajectory. Thus, regardless of whether CGS is needed to explain the Holocene transition to large-scale societies, cooperative breeding is necessary to explain its operation in the first place.

References

Boesch, C. (1991) Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 41:530–32.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., Huber, J., Isler, K., Kosonen, Z. K., Martins, E., Meulman, E. J., Richiger, R., Rueth, K., Spillmann, B., Wiesendanger, S. & van Schaik, C. P. (2014) The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications 5, Article no. 4747. (Online article, published August 27, 2014).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B. & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 18:175–86.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (2010) Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates. Animal Cognition 13:119.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Rapaport, L. G. (2011) What are we learning from teaching? Animal Behaviour 82(5):1207–11. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.08.018.Google Scholar
Coussi-Korbel, S. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1995) On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour 50:1441–53.Google Scholar
Dell'Mour, V., Range, F. & Huber, L. (2009) Social learning and mother's behavior in manipulative tasks in infant marmosets. American Journal of Primatology 71(6):503509.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009) Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Humle, T. & Snowdon, C. T. (2008) Socially biased learning in the acquisition of a complex foraging task in juvenile cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Behaviour 27(1):267–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T. & Matsuzawa, T. (2009) Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Animal Cognition 12(1):3748.Google Scholar
Kline, M. A. (2015) How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38:e31. Available at: dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090 Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2010) Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. Annual Review of Anthropology 39:417–36.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. (2006) What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Animal Cognition 9(1):3646.Google Scholar
Rapaport, L. G. (2011) Progressive parenting behavior in wild golden lion tamarins. Behavioral Ecology 22(4):745–54.Google Scholar
Roy, S., Miller, C. T., Gottsch, D. & Wang, X. (2011) Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise. The Journal of Experimental Biology 214(21):3619–29.Google Scholar
Rukstalis, M., Fite, J. E. & French, J. A. (2003) Social change affects vocal structure in a callitrichid primate (Callithrix kuhlii). Ethology 109:327–40.Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. (2001) Social processes in communication and cognition in callitrichid monkeys: A review. Animal Cognition 4:247–57.Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. (2013) Language parallels in New World primates. In: Animal models of speech and language disorders, ed. Helekar, S. H., pp. 241–61. Springer.Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossario, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E. & Levinson, S. C. (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106(26):10587–92.Google Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Narayanan, D. Z. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Current Biology 23(21):2162–68.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2007) If they're so good at grammar, then why don't they talk? Hints from apes' and humans' use of gestures. Language Learning and Development 3(2):133–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. & Burkart, J. M. (2010) Mind the gap: Cooperative breeding and the evolution of our unique features. In: Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals, ed. Kappeler, P. M. & Silk, J., pp. 477–96. Springer.Google Scholar
Vitale, A., Zanzoni, M., Queyras, A. & Chiarotti, F. (2003) Degree of social contact affects the emission of food calls in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology 59:2128.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2012) Cooperative breeding and the evolution of vocal flexibility. In: The Oxford handbook of language evolution, ed. Tallerman, M. & Gibson, K., pp. 7181. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The relationship between allomaternal care and (A) proactive prosociality and (B) social tolerance. Humans fit the general primate trend and do not represent an outlier.