Introduction
Striatal dopaminergic neurotransmission is overactive in drug-naïve psychotic subjects, both at rest and in response to stimulation, with no compensatory modulation.Reference Winton-Brown, Fusar-Poli, Ungless and Howes 1 The overactive dopaminergic neurotransmission may interfere with the attribution of salience, altering the potential motivational significance of stimuli: salience represents the ability of a stimulus to grab attention and drive behavior.Reference Kapur 2 , Reference Heinz and Schlagenhauf 3 Salience attribution is altered because the mesolimbic hyperdopaminergic state interferes with the basic mechanism of reward prediction error encoding,Reference Glimcher 4 , Reference Boehme, Deserno and Gleich 5 as experimentally demonstrated in studies reporting impaired learning in schizophrenic subjects.Reference Murray, Corlett and Clark 6 – Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire 8 In the present account, the unexpected “aberrant” assignment of salience to internal and external stimuli may initially induce a perplexing state marked by exaggerated importance of some percepts and thoughts and increased inner awareness. Such psychotic symptoms as delusions may arise from seemingly plausible top–down cognitive explanations that individuals come up with to understand the persistence of these experiences of internal and external stimuli with unexpected and aberrant salience.Reference Kapur 2 , Reference Howes and Nour 9
Striatal dopaminergic neurotransmission is also overactive in patients at the early stages of Parkinson’s disease “on” medication with levodopa or dopamine agonists.Reference Poewe, Antonini, Zijlmans, Burkhard and Vingerhoets 10 , Reference Bonuccelli, Del Dotto and Rascol 11 These drugs restore dopaminergic levels in the early-affected dorsolateral frontostriatal loop (linking the dorsal striatum and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), but they may overdose the preserved orbital frontostriatal loop (linking the ventral striatum with the medial prefrontal cortex).Reference Kish, Shannak and Hornykiewicz 12 , Reference Gotham, Brown and Marsden 13 The overdosing effect of dopaminergic drugs may be more severe in patients with younger PD onset, considering that dopaminergic function physiologically declines with aging.Reference Shingai, Tateno and Arakawa 14
Dopaminergic drugs interfere with phasic processing of rewards provided by ventral striatum dopamine neurons, strengthening dopaminergic peaks (associated with unexpected rewards), and preventing dopaminergic dips (associated with failures of expected rewards), as confirmed by impairments in reward learning induced by dopaminergic therapy in the early stages of PD;Reference MacDonald, Monchi, Seergobin, Ganjavi, Tamjeedi and MacDonald 15 , Reference Vaillancourt, Schonfeld, Kwak, Bohnen and Seidler 16 this effect is stronger for the tonic stimulation provided by dopamine agonists in comparison with the phasic stimulation provided by levodopa.Reference Grace 17 , Reference Poletti and Bonuccelli 18
The alteration of reward learning (strengthened dopaminergic peaks associated with rewards, prevented dopaminergic dips associated with failures of expected rewards) induced especially by dopamine agonists represents the pathophysiological basisReference Shiner, Symmonds, Guitart-Masip, Fleming, Friston and Dolan 19 , Reference Voon, Mehta and Hallett 20 of behavioral addictions in PD patients.Reference Ceravolo, Frosini, Rossi and Bonuccelli 21 These include dopamine dysregulation syndrome (DDS),Reference Giovannoni, O’Sullivan, Turner, Manson and Lees 22 which is an addictive pattern of medication intake, particularly of large doses of dopaminergic drugs in excess of that required to control motor symptoms, and such impulse control disorders (ICDs)Reference Dagher and Robbins 23 as pathological gambling, hypersexuality, compulsive eating, punding, hoarding, and compulsive shopping.
Hypothesis
Psychotic subjects and patients in the early stages of PD “on” dopaminergic drugs present with a hyperdopaminergic state that alters reward processing. The alterations described in these clinical populations are not overlapping but share an increased potential motivational significance of stimuli: psychotic subjects attribute salience to neutral stimuli, while medicated PD patients overvalue rewards (and “neglect” punishments).
On the basis of this shared feature in reward processing, it could be hypothesized that the hyperdopaminergic state induced by dopaminergic drugs in PD patients may also facilitate the attribution of salience to neutral stimuli. This hypothesis could be tested investigating salience attribution and clinical features related to its alteration (such as psychosis and psychosis-proneness) in PD patients in comparison with healthy controls.
PD patients with such behavioral addictions as ICDs and DDS present with more overactive striatal dopaminergic activityReference Voon, Rizos and Chakravarty 24 and more altered reward processingReference Piray, Zeighami, Bahrami, Eissa, Hewedi and Moustafa 25 in comparison with PD patients who do not present these neuropsychiatric manifestations; therefore, the prevalence and severity of salience features could be higher in PD patients with behavioral addictions compared to PD patients without them. These speculative hypotheses should not be primarily investigated in the advanced stages of PD, in which such psychotic features as delusions and hallucinationsReference Fénelon 26 are mainly due to the widespread cortical diffusion of Lewy-body neuropathology.Reference Braak, Del Tredici and Rub 27 , Reference Stahl 28
Saliency and related features in Parkinson’s disease
Two studies directly assessed salience features in PD patients. A behavioral studyReference Nagy, Levy-Gigi, Somlaiu, Takáts, Bereczki and Kéri 29 assessed newly diagnosed drug-naïve PD patients before and after 12 weeks of dopamine agonist treatment, with a speeded-up reaction time task and with the probe stimulus preceded by conditioned stimuli signaling monetary reward by color or shape. Dopamine agonists increased both adaptive and aberrant salience in PD patients, that is, formation of real and illusory associations between conditioned stimuli and reward, respectively. Unusual feelings and experiences, considered subclinical manifestations of psychotic-like symptoms, were specifically related to irrelevant and illusory stimulus–reward associations (aberrant salience). The authors concluded that dopamine agonists may rapidly increase psychotic-like experiences in PD patients, possibly by facilitating dopaminergic transmission in the ventral striatum, which results in aberrant associations between conditioned stimuli and reward.
Another studyReference Poletti, Frosini and Pagni 30 psychometrically investigated 50 medicated PD patients, 12 newly diagnosed drug-naïve PD patients, and 15 healthy controls with the Aberrant Salience Inventory.Reference Cicero, Kerns and McCarthy 31 Dopaminergic medication doses significantly correlated with some features of aberrant salience (increased significance, heightened emotionality, and heightened cognition), and some features of salience (impending understanding) were higher in comparison with drug-naïve PD patients.
One paradigm related to salience attribution is latent inhibition,Reference Schmidt and Roiser 32 a process that occurs when an organism is exposed to a stimulus that is not followed by a significant consequence: the stimulus subsequently becomes less effective, as compared to a novel stimulus, in the acquisition of a new association.Reference Lubow 33 Latent inhibition has been shown to be dysfunctional in psychotic patients,Reference Lubow 34 who allocate more attention to irrelevant stimuli in comparison with healthy controls. Dopaminergic drugs may decrease latent inhibition in de-novo PD patients,Reference Polner, Moustafa, Nagy, Takáts, Győrfi and Kéri 35 , Reference Gyorfi, Nagy, Bokor, Kelemen and Kéri 36 enhancing at the same time perceptual psychotic-like experiences (changes in subjective feelings in thinking, time perception, and mental “highness”). Psychosis proneness has also been reported in cognitively preserved PD patients, especially in the subgroup with ICDs.Reference Housden, O’Sullivan, Joyce, Lees and Roiser 37
Increased frequency of psychotic symptoms has been reported in PD patients with ICDsReference Pettorusso, Fasano and De Risio 38 – Reference Poletti, Logi and Lucetti 40 and DDSReference Cilia, Siri and Canesi 41 in comparison to PD patients without them, and one studyReference Poletti, Lucetti, Baldacci, Del Dotto and Bonuccelli 42 directly reported a case series of nondemented patients developing concomitantly both delusional jealousy and hypersexuality. Moreover, several studies reported isolated psychosis in cognitively preserved PD patients when exposed to dopamine agonists.Reference Cannas, Solla, Floris, Tacconi, Marrosu and Marrosu 43 – Reference Stefanis, Bozi and Christodoulou 46
Discussion
The empirical findings presented herein are preliminarily in agreement with the speculative hypothesis that dopaminergic treatments adopted to restore motor functions may interfere with the physiological process of salience attribution in patients in the early stages of PD, especially in those with younger disease onset. Moreover, they are also in agreement with the related speculative hypothesis that this interference effect is stronger in PD patients who present such behavioral addictions as ICDs and DDS. The tonic dopaminergic stimulation of dopamine agonists, rather than the phasic stimulation of levodopa, may cause an attribution of salience to neutral stimuli. The altered increased motivational significance of stimuli at the basis of behavioral addictions in medicated PD patients probably could include features related not only to overvalue of rewards but also related to aberrant salience attribution. Therefore, behavioral addictions may be considered a nonmotor side effect of dopamine agonists that may emerge in a subclinical “atmosphere” of altered salience, representing a condition of psychosis proneness and a risk factor for the development of comorbid psychotic features. The decreased latent inhibition induced by dopaminergic drugs may also contribute to alteration of this subclinical atmosphere, in which irrelevant stimuli continue to activate the attention of subjects.
This could explain why PD patients with such behavioral addictions as ICDs and DDS may present increased psychotic features compared to PD patients without behavioral addictions and why in some cases dopaminergic treatments, especially dopamine agonists, may induce the concomitant development of ICDs and psychosis.
This subclinical atmosphere of altered salience/psychosis proneness may represent the “dark” side of dopaminergic treatments in early-stage PD patients, especially those with younger onset. In this subgroup, the overdosing effect of dopamine agonists is stronger considering that dopaminergic function physiologically declines with aging.Reference Shingai, Tateno and Arakawa 14 Behavioral addictions, being associated with more overactive striatal dopaminergic activityReference Voon, Rizos and Chakravarty 24 and more altered reward processing,Reference Piray, Zeighami, Bahrami, Eissa, Hewedi and Moustafa 25 are probably also associated with more altered salience attribution, that is, psychosis proneness. From this perspective, disease-related characteristicsReference Stahl 28 and such individual characteristics as cognitive functioning, premorbid impulsivity, and previous history of addiction, as well as other psychiatric features, may play the role of moderating factors in the clinical expression of an increased motivational significance of stimuli.
Furthermore, the attribution of salience to neutral stimuli, increasing their motivational significance and inducing an atmosphere of altered salience, could be involved in the physiological bases of enhanced creativity detected in early PD patients on dopamine agonistsReference Faust-Socher, Kenett, Cohen, Hassin-Baer and Inzelberg 47 that is not simply due to an increase of impulsivity nor to behavioral addictions,Reference Canesi, Rusconi, Isaias and Pezzoli 48 and is associated with such signs of increased psychosis proneness as schizotypal traits.Reference Polner, Nagy, Takáts and Keri 49 This speculative hypothesis needs empirical investigation.
Conclusions
Reviewed empirical findings only preliminarily support the speculative hypothesis discussed in the present opinion piece. This hypothesis deserves further empirical investigation. Once supported by empirical data, the clinical implication could be that such behavioral addictions as ICDs and DDS should be considered a clinical indicator of a hyperdopaminergic state associated not only with overvalue of rewards (sustaining addictions) but also with altered salience attribution, increasing the risk of psychotic phenomena. Therefore, an accurate neuropsychiatric assessment covering both risks for addiction and psychosis proneness is needed before beginning any dopaminergic treatment, especially for young-onset PD patients.
Disclosures
Michele Poletti hereby declares that he does not have any conflicts of interest to disclose.