Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-09T15:28:30.900Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Visual Object Discrimination Impairment as an Early Predictor of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2019

Leslie S. Gaynor*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Rosie E. Curiel Cid
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Aging, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL, USA Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA
Ailyn Penate
Affiliation:
Wien Center for Alzheimer’s Disease and Memory Disorders, Miami Beach, FL, USA
Mónica Rosselli
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA
Sara N. Burke
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA McKnight Brain Institute and Department of Neuroscience, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Meredith Wicklund
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA Department of Neurology, University of Florida College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
David A. Loewenstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Aging, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL, USA Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA
Russell M. Bauer
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Gainesville, FL, USA
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Leslie S. Gaynor, University of Florida, College of Public Health and Health Professions, P.O. Box 100165, Gainesville, FL 32610, USA, Phone (352) 273-6014, Fax (352) 273-6156. E-mail: lesliesg2@ufl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective: Detection of cognitive impairment suggestive of risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) progression is crucial to the prevention of incipient dementia. This study was performed to determine if performance on a novel object discrimination task improved identification of earlier deficits in older adults at risk for AD. Method: In total, 135 participants from the 1Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center [cognitively normal (CN), Pre-mild cognitive impairment (PreMCI), amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), and dementia] completed a test of object discrimination and traditional memory measures in the context of a larger neuropsychological and clinical evaluation. Results: The Object Recognition and Discrimination Task (ORDT) revealed significant differences between the PreMCI, aMCI, and dementia groups versus CN individuals. Moreover, relative risk of being classified as PreMCI rather than CN increased as an inverse function of ORDT score. Discussion: Overall, the obtained results suggest that a novel object discrimination task improves the detection of very early AD-related cognitive impairment, increasing the window for therapeutic intervention. (JINS, 2019, 25, 688–698)

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2019. 

INTRODUCTION

Numerous clinical trials involving Alzheimer’s disease (AD) therapeutics have failed due to an inability to reverse clinical symptoms and gross cellular dysfunction once they are present (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Mangialasche, Andreasen, Feldman, Giacobini, Jones and Kivipelto2014). As a result, investigators have attempted to target groups of older adults earlier in the disease process, at an intermediate stage of cognitive decline between cognitively normal (CN) aging and clinical dementia. These older adults are at greater risk of progressing to AD-related dementia compared to CN individuals, and can be classified as “amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI)” or “pre-mild cognitive impairment (PreMCI).” Individuals who meet research criteria for PreMCI may have subjective cognitive deficits with normal-range performance on cognitive assessments and, therefore, do not meet formal diagnostic criteria for aMCI. Individuals with PreMCI may also be identified by having nonnormal performance on cognitive assessments without subjective cognitive decline, cognitive decline identified by knowledgeable informants, or clinically identified impairment. Crucially, individuals with PreMCI are 6 times more likely to progress to aMCI or dementia over time compared to their CN peers (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Greig, Schinka, Barker, Shen, Potter and Duara2012). Individuals with aMCI are 7 times more likely to progress to dementia compared to their CN counterparts (Duara et al., Reference Duara, Loewenstein, Greig, Potter, Barker, Raj and Potter2011). Identifying neurocognitive probes that are sensitive to decline at such early stages is a critical step in identifying patients who may benefit from therapeutics designed to prevent or delay AD onset (Snyder et al., Reference Snyder, Kahle-Wrobleski, Brannan, Miller, Schindler, DeSanti and Carrillo2014).

Memory impairment figures prominently into the diagnostic criteria of aMCI (Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Locke, Dueck, Knopman, Woodruff, Hoffman-Snyder and Reiman2014), since the site of the earliest appearance of tangle pathology in AD is the transentorhinal cortex, or Area 35 of the perirhinal cortex (PRC), which is a major source of input into the hippocampal memory system (Braak & Braak, Reference Braak and Braak1991; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Liu, Provenzano, Berman, Profaci, Sloan and Small2014). Notably, studies in patients diagnosed with prodromal aMCI revealed atrophy in the medial aspect of the PRC prior to the lateral PRC (Krumm et al., Reference Krumm, Kivisaari, Probst, Monsch, Reinhardt, Ulmer and Taylor2016). Additionally, regional tau deposition has been shown to correlate with atrophy of bilateral PRC in early AD (Sone et al., Reference Sone, Imabayashi, Maikusa, Okamura, Furumoto, Kudo and Matsuda2017), and has been shown to be relatively better than amyloid accumulation in predicting future cognitive decline (Huber et al., Reference Huber, Yee, May, Dhanala and Mitchell2017; Qiu et al., Reference Qiu, Yang, Zhang, Wang, Zhang, Huang and Ma2017). Therefore, investigating early sites of tau deposition like the PRC is critical to identifying early markers of AD-related cognitive decline.

The PRC is located on the ventral surface of the temporal lobe, lateral to the entorhinal cortex and anterior to the parahippocampal cortex. The PRC receives its primary inputs from unimodal sensory systems, such as the primary visual cortex and visual association cortices, which it then conveys to downstream brain regions as multimodal, three-dimensional object representations based on specific features of the perceived stimulus. The role the PRC plays in feature binding is important in object recognition and discrimination, as this computation leads to the identification of objects based on unique configurations of features (Barense et al., Reference Barense, Henson, Lee and Graham2010; Devlin & Price, Reference Devlin and Price2007). Results of primate and rodent experiments show that the PRC is more active when discriminating among objects that share common features (Buckley & Gaffan, Reference Buckley and Gaffan1997; Bussey et al., Reference Bussey, Saksida and Murray2003; Cowell et al., Reference Cowell, Bussey and Saksida2006). Interestingly, the medial PRC, which is more vulnerable to early AD disease processes, has been specifically shown to be associated with disambiguation of confusable object (Kivisaari et al., Reference Kivisaari, Tyler, Monsch and Taylor2012). The PRC has also been shown to support recognition of the same object presented from different viewpoints, allowing for the identification of a unique object that shares features with rotated copies of a nontarget object in an oddity discrimination task in human (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Barense and Graham2005) and animal studies (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Booth, Rolls and Gaffan2001). The location of the PRC at the anatomical nexus of cortical inputs to the medial temporal lobe memory system, as well as its involvement in processing 3D object representations, supports the involvement of the PRC in both mnemonic and visual–perceptual abilities (Bussey et al., Reference Bussey, Saksida and Murray2003; Murray & Bussey, Reference Murray and Bussey1999). As a result, tasks that tap such “pre-mnemonic” processes, like object recognition and discrimination, may have additional utility in the detection of incipient cognitive impairment. In fact, it has been argued that evaluating the structure and function of the ventral processing stream may reveal subtle deficits present in very early AD (Kurylo et al., Reference Kurylo, Corkin, Rizzo Iii and Growdon1996) and cognitively impaired aging (Fidalgo et al., Reference Fidalgo, Changoor, Page-Gould, Lee and Barense2016).

In human clinical studies, when lure items share a high degree of featural similarity with the familiar target items, individuals at risk for AD present with high rates of false recognition, tending to recognize novel objects as familiar (Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Ryan, Cowell and Barense2013). These studies suggest that PRC dysfunction may underlie recognition memory and perceptual deficits that are common in early AD. However, few researchers have implemented object discrimination tasks in humans to aid in identification of risk of AD progression (Mason et al., Reference Mason, Hussey, Molitor, Ko, Donahue and Ally2017; Newsome et al., Reference Newsome, Duarte and Barense2012), and no studies have investigated the sensitivity of these tasks to more subtle cognitive changes seen in PreMCI.

The Object Recognition and Discrimination Task (ORDT) used in this study is modeled after similar tasks used in rodent (Bartko et al., Reference Bartko, Winters, Cowell, Saksida and Bussey2007; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Turner, Santacroce, Carty, Shafiq, Bizon and Burke2017) and primate (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Wallace, Hartzell, Nematollahi, Plange and Barnes2011; Bussey et al., Reference Bussey, Saksida and Murray2003) literature and adapted from a visual discrimination (“oddity”) task first used in humans by Devlin and Price (Devlin & Price, Reference Devlin and Price2007). The purpose of the current investigation was to determine the utility of the ORDT in the identification of very early cognitive changes seen in PreMCI and aMCI participants. Our central hypothesis was that the ORDT would contribute to the prediction of current early cognitive change because it taps key functions of the PRC.

METHOD

In total, 143 participants from the Wien Center for Alzheimer’s Disease and Memory Disorders at Mount Sinai Medical Center in Miami Beach, Florida, were recruited as part of the National Institute on Aging (NIA)-sponsored 1Florida Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (1Florida ADRC). The sample was 40% male and 56% Hispanic/Latino. Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained at the Mount Sinai Medical Center, and it was allowed for sharing of de-identified patient information with collaborators at the University of Florida. Informed consent was obtained from all participants and study partners.

Participants

All participants had an informant age 18 or older who could be present at the participant’s first appointment and provide reliable collateral information. Participants were able to read at the 6th grade reading level [Wide Range Achievement Test, 4th edition (WRAT-IV; Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006) for English speakers and the Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey-Revised (WMLS-R; Woodcock et al., Reference Woodcock, Muñoz-Sandoval, Ruef and Alvarado2005) or Word Accentuation Test (WAT; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Lam and Wilson2006) for Spanish speakers]. If on medications, participants were required to be on steady dosages for 2 months or longer, and participants were not excluded if they were on memory-enhancing drugs. This included acetylcholinesterase inhibitors or other related medications that are prescribed by physicians for the treatment of MCI. Additional demographic and descriptive data are contained in Table 1.

Table 1. Participant demographic information.

Participant demographic information illustrated for all 135 participants included in the final analyses. CN = cognitively normal; PreMCI = pre-mild cognitive impairment; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment; E = English; S = Spanish; M = Male; F = Female; RT = reaction time. Note: Error terms included are standard deviation.

Exclusion criteria included the following: (1) sensory loss or motor impairments significant enough to interfere with the participant’s ability to complete a battery of neuropsychological tests; (2) previous stroke or other unrelated neurological disorder known to affect cognition (i.e. moderate-severe traumatic brain injury, seizure disorder, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, etc.); (3) current or past history of major psychiatric disturbance with hospitalization, active psychosis, bipolar disorder, current major depressive episode, or current or past history of alcohol or substance abuse within the 6 months prior to the first appointment; and (4) use of medications with anticholinergic properties (i.e. antipsychotics, sedatives, etc.).

Clinical Evaluation

As part of their participation in the Clinical Core at the 1Florida ADRC, each participant completed thorough clinical testing which included a physical and neurological examination performed by an experienced bilingual geriatric psychiatrist. Additionally, participants and their informants underwent a comprehensive interview designed to provide information sufficient for completing the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale (Rockwood et al., Reference Rockwood, Strang, MacKnight, Downer and Morris2000). Clinicians performing these evaluations were blinded to the participant’s neuropsychological test scores. Additional assessments, the results of which are not reported here, included the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center neuropsychological battery, and, whenever possible, neuroimaging studies including MRI and amyloid Positron Emission Tomography (PET) scans.

Neuropsychological Assessment

Participants were administered standardized memory measures, such as the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger and Brandt2010) and the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Delayed Paragraph Recall subtest (NACC-DR; Elwood, Reference Elwood1991). Additional tests included the Trail Making Test (Tombaugh, Reference Tombaugh2004), Category Fluency (animals, fruits, and vegetables; Loonstra et al., Reference Loonstra, Tarlow and Sellers2001), letter fluency (Lezak et al., Reference Lezak, Howieson, Loring and Fischer2004) as well as WAIS-R Block Design (Snow et al., Reference Snow, Tierney, Zorzitto, Fisher and Reid1989) and Digit Span of the NACC Uniform Data Set (UDS) (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Salmon, Mercaldo, Ferris, Graff-Radford, Chui and Morris2009). As discussed below, age- and education-corrected delayed recall scores for the memory measures were used in determining final diagnosis.

Spanish language testing with equivalent standardized neuropsychological tests was completed for primary Spanish speakers. Primary language was determined by ascertaining the language the participant uses most often, feels more comfortable speaking, and used in completing the majority of their education. Tests given for primary Spanish speakers had appropriate age, education, and cultural/language normative data for the translated version. Testing was performed by proficient Spanish/English psychometricians.

Diagnostic Criteria

Participant diagnosis utilized the following criteria as well as NACC UDS criteria, with final diagnostic decisions being made by consensus diagnosis (see Table 2).

Table 2. Algorithmic diagnostic criteria.

Participant diagnosis was determined following completion of thorough clinical testing which included a physical and neurological examination performed by an experienced bilingual geriatric psychiatrist. Additionally, participants and their informants underwent a comprehensive interview designed to provide information sufficient for completing the CDR Scale. Clinicians performing these evaluations were blinded to neuropsychological test scores. Additional assessments, the results of which are not reported here, included the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center neuropsychological battery.

Cognitively normal

(1) No subjective memory complaint evidenced from the participant or study partner; (2) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score > 26; (3) global CDR Scale of 0 [CDR Sum of Boxes (CDR-SB) = 0]; (4) scores less than 1 SD below expected levels on measures of delayed recall of the HVLT-R and NACC memory for passages based on appropriate age, educational, and cultural norms; (5) no reported functional impairment in independent activities of daily living by the participant or study partner; (6) does not meet Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) criteria for minor or major neurocognitive disorder; and (7) no scores less than −1 SD on any other neuropsychological test.

PreMCI-Clinical (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Greig, Schinka, Barker, Shen, Potter and Duara2012)

(1) Evidence of subjective memory complaint and cognitive decline verified by study partner; (2) MMSE > 24; (3) global CDR Scale of 0.5 (CDR-SB = 0.5–2.0); (4) no neuropsychological impairment on measures of delayed recall of the HVLT-R and NACC memory for passages as specified in the criteria for the CN group; (5) no significant functional impairment in independent activities of daily living by the participant or study partner; (6) does not meet DSM-V criteria for minor or major neurocognitive disorder; and (7) no scores < −1 SD on any other neuropsychological test.

PreMCI-Neuropsychological (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Greig, Schinka, Barker, Shen, Potter and Duara2012)

(1) No evidence of subjective memory or cognitive decline after an extensive interview with the participant and the collateral informant; (2) MMSE score > 24; (3) global CDR Scale of 0.0 (CDR-SB = 0); (4) scores −1.5 SD or below on measures of delayed recall of the HVLT-R and/or NACC memory for passages; (5) no significant functional impairment in independent activities of daily living; (6) does not meet DSM-V criteria for minor or major neurocognitive disorder.

In the current study, the two PreMCI groups were combined for analysis, based on previous results indicating that the two groups had equivalent risk of progression to impairment (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Greig, Schinka, Barker, Shen, Potter and Duara2012).

Amnestic MCI

(1) Evidence of subjective memory complaint verified by study partner; (2) MMSE score > 24; (3) global CDR Scale of 0.5 (0.5 < CDR-SB < 4); (4) scores −1.5 SD or below on measures of delayed recall of the HVLT-R and NACC memory for passages; (5) no significant functional impairment; (6) does not meet DSM-V criteria for major neurocognitive disorder.

Dementia

(1) Evidence of subjective memory complaint verified by study partner; (2) MMSE score > 18; (3) global CDR Scale of 1.0 (CDR-SB > 4); (4) scores 2 SD or more below expected levels on measures of delayed recall of the HVLT-R and NACC memory for passages; (5) significant functional impairment in activities of daily living; (6) meets DSM-V criteria for major neurocognitive disorder; and (7) other neuropsychological scores may be 1.5 SD or more below expected levels.

Object Recognition and Discrimination Task

The ORDT was adapted from a visual discrimination (“oddity”) task used by Devlin and Price (Devlin & Price, Reference Devlin and Price2007). Original stimuli were obtained with permission from the authors and were presented on a laptop or desktop computer using E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools).

Sample stimuli from the ORDT are presented in Figure 1. On each trial, participants were shown a horizontal array of four stimuli (Nonobjects, Shapes, Colors, and Animals). Nonobjects were photographs of models created from building blocks, resulting in entities that were not found in the real world. On each trial, three of the stimuli depicted the same object or entity, while the fourth stimulus was a different object/entity. The participant was asked to identify the different stimulus (the “target”) as quickly and as accurately as possible. Trial conditions included object type and difficulty level (Easy and Difficult). Trial difficulty and object type were varied throughout the test, such that the trial conditions were interspersed randomly within the stimulus train. In the “easy” condition, the three nontarget stimuli were all presented in the same orientation. In the “difficult” condition, the three nontarget stimuli are rotated individually (horizontally or vertically) in picture plane. In this way, success on the “difficult” condition requires the participant to recognize that the three nontarget stimuli are the same entity even though their appearance is changed by rotation. This study reports performance on the “Difficult Objects” trials only (Difficult Animals and Difficult Nonobjects), since it was this condition that was specifically associated with bilateral PRC activation in Devlin & Price’s original study (Devlin & Price, Reference Devlin and Price2007). The decision to combine difficult objects trials was further supported by nonsignificant findings described in the Results section. Responses, as well as latency and accuracy of response, were logged with E-Prime software.

Fig. 1. Representation of standard trials of the ORDT. On each trial, participants were shown a horizontal array of four stimuli (Nonobjects, Shapes, Colors, and Animals). Nonobjects were photographs of models created from building blocks, resulting in entities that were not found in the real world. Task conditions included object type and difficulty level (Easy and Difficult). In the “easy” condition, the three nontarget items were all presented in the same orientation. In the “difficult” condition, the three nontarget items are rotated individually (horizontally or vertically) in picture plane. In this way, success on the “difficult” condition requires the participant to recognize that the three nontarget items are the same entity even though their appearance is changed by rotation.

The Introduction and Practice phase consisted of visually presented instructions that were also spoken aloud by the test administrator. Then, the task began with 16 “easy” practice trials drawn from the stimulus sets described above. Participants had unlimited time to complete these trials.

The experimental task consisted of 40 trials in each test condition, presented for 6 s with a 1-s intertrial interval. Stimuli for each trial remained on the screen for 6 s or until a participant response was recorded.

Analysis

Parametric analyses of variance were used to determine if there were significant group differences in age and years of education. Pearson chi-square tests were employed to determine if there were significant group differences in primary language and gender of participants between diagnostic subgroups.

Prior to completing the proposed analyses involving both the difficult animal and difficult nonobject conditions in one score, a repeated- measures ANOVA was completed with Log-transformed, standardized versions (Z) of accuracy scores for each condition, separately. If no statistical difference was determined, then the conditions would be combined as was initially intended and shown in the original study (ORDT-DO; Devlin & Price, Reference Devlin and Price2007). Diagnostic subgroup was included as a between-subjects factor and interaction term.

Following these analyses and combination of the “difficult objects” trials, additional two-way ANOVAs were completed to assess the effect of primary language and gender on ORDT-DO performance, as well as interaction effects of either of these variables and diagnostic subgroup on ORDT-DO performance. An ANOVA was also completed to determine whether there were significant group mean differences in median reaction time on correct trials of the ORDT-DO across diagnostic subgroups.

We found unequal sample variance estimates across groups for the HVLT-R and ORDT-DO. Because of this, log-transformed standardized versions (Z) of the HVLT-R, NACC-DR, and ORDT-DO were used to determine if there were group differences in task performance related to clinical diagnosis. Following significant omnibus test findings, Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were performed in order to elucidate specific group mean differences. Nontransformed means (M) and standard deviations (SD) were reported and presented graphically for ease of interpretability. Independent samples t tests were also performed with these transformed Z scores to determine differences in task performance between participants in the CN and PreMCI groups. Additionally, because the average of the delayed recall score on one of the standard memory tests (HVLT-R) suggested floor performance of the dementia group, all analyses of variance and chi-square tests were completed again, excluding the dementia group, to ensure that previously obtained relationships were preserved. Further analyses of demographic and neuropsychological variables were also completed for the PreMCI group separated into its subgroups (PreMCI-Clinical, PreMCI-Neuropsychological) in order to determine if these subgroups evidenced significant group mean differences that would merit their separation in future analyses.

A multinomial logistic regression was performed to determine whether task performance contributed to prediction of diagnostic group membership. The dependent variable was diagnostic subgroup, and all groups except the dementia group were included due to the aforementioned floor effect of the HVLT-R. The CN group was the reference category. Independent variables for the first version of this analysis included the transformed, standardized version (Z) of the HVLT-R, NACC-DR, and the ORDT-DO. A version of this analysis was also completed that included age and years of education as covariates due to the known relationship between both variables and memory and cognitive task performance in older adults.

P values of less than .05 were considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

Of the original 143 participants who were screened for task participation, 8 participants were unable to complete the ORDT due to cognitive impairment (N = 4), visual impairment (N = 1), refusal to participate (N = 1), and computer error (N = 2). Overall, 135 participants were included in the final analyses (Table 1; CN: N = 23; PreMCI: N = 20; aMCI: N = 76; Dementia: N = 16). Age of participants ranged from 65 to 97 years. Participant education ranged from 4 years to 20 years. No significant group differences were found in average age, years of education, primary language, or gender (Age: F [3,129] = 1.80, p = .15; Education: F [3,133] = 2.04, p = .11; Primary Language: X 2[3] = 1.90, p = .59; Gender: X 2[3] = 1.16, p = .76). There were more English speakers than Spanish speakers in each group and there were variable ratios of males to females.

Additional analyses indicate that the average scores of each diagnostic subgroup on the HVLT-R and NACC-DR mirror age-matched decline across diagnostic subgroups with increasing amnestic impairment, suggesting that findings from the following analysis most likely reflect normative performance of these populations.

ORDT-specific Analyses

First, analyses were completed to determine whether there were significant differences between “difficult nonobjects” and “difficult animals” conditions on the ORDT. There was not a main effect of task condition (F [1,131] = 0.79, p = .37) or an interaction between task condition and diagnostic subgroup (F [3,131] = 0.37, p = .77). Therefore, a single “difficult objects” performance score (ORDT-DO) was used in subsequent analyses.

In terms of primary language, there was not a main effect of primary language or an interaction effect of primary language and diagnostic subgroup on ORDT-DO performance (F [1,120] = 0.06, p = .81; F [3,120] = 0.65, p = .56). In terms of participant gender, there was not a main effect of gender or an interaction effect of gender and diagnostic subgroup on ORDT-DO performance (F [1,127] = 0.07, p = .79; F [3,127] = 0.15, p = .93).

There was a significant difference in median reaction time for correct trials of the ORDT-DO across diagnostic subgroups (F [3,134] = 3.70, p = .01, η2p = .08). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed group mean differences between only the CN and dementia groups (CN: M = 2.81 s, SD = 0.62; Dementia: M = 3.53 s, SD = 0.83).

Group Differences

As would be expected due to the inclusion of traditional measures in defining diagnostic subgroups (incorporation bias; (Weissberger et al., Reference Weissberger, Strong, Stefanidis, Summers, Bondi and Stricker2017), there were statistically significant differences between diagnostic subgroups in performance on both traditional memory measures, as well as the ORDT-DO (Figure 2; HVLT-R: F [3,131] = 28.21, p < .001, η p2 = .40; NACC-DR: F [3,133] = 25.95, p < .001, η p2 = .37; ORDT-DO: F [3,134] = 8.74, p < .001, η p2 = .17). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed group mean differences between the CN and the aMCI and dementia groups [HVLT-R (CN: M = 7.35, SD = 3.06; aMCI: M = 1.96, SD = 2.81; Dementia: M = 0.31, SD = 1.25); NACC-DR (CN: M = 11.43, SD = 3.80; aMCI: M = 7.40, SD = 4.07; Dementia: M = 2.53, SD = 3.54); ORDT-DO (CN: M = 86.5, SD = 8.04; aMCI: M = 68.03, SD = 17.28; Dementia: M = 60.63, SD = 17.02)]. Importantly, when an independent samples t test was performed comparing the CN and PreMCI groups, only the ORDT-DO revealed significant group mean difference in test scores (t [23.93] = 2.18, p = .04, Cohen’s d = 0.61). The traditional neuropsychological measures failed to discriminate between these two groups, as would be expected based on diagnostic criteria for these subgroups (NACC-DR: t [41] = 0.43, p = .93; HVLT-R: t [28.29] = 1.33, p = .20).

Fig. 2. Graphical representation of performance of clinical diagnostic subgroups compared to the CN group on traditional memory measures and the ORDT. There were statistically significant differences between diagnostic subgroups in performance on both traditional memory measures, and also the ORDT-DO (HVLT-R: F [3,131] = 28.21, p < .001, η p2 = .40; NACC-DR: F [3,133] = 25.95, p < .001, η p2 = .37; ORDT-DO: F [3,134] = 8.74, p < .001, η p2 = .17). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed group mean differences between the CN and the aMCI and dementia groups [HVLT-R (CN: M = 7.35, SD = 3.06; aMCI: M = 1.96, SD = 2.81; Dementia: M = 0.31, SD = 1.25); NACC-DR (CN: M = 11.43, SD = 3.80; aMCI: M = 7.40, SD = 4.07; Dementia: M = 2.53, SD = 3.54); ORDT-DO (CN: M = 86.5, SD = 8.04; aMCI: M = 68.03, SD = 17.28; Dementia: M = 60.63, SD = 17.02)]. Importantly, when an independent samples t test was performed comparing the CN and PreMCI groups, only the ORDT-DO revealed significant group mean difference in test scores (t [23.93] = 2.18, p = .04, Cohen’s d = 0.61). The traditional neuropsychological measures failed to discriminate between these two groups, as would be expected based on diagnostic criteria (NACC-DR: t [41] = 0.43, p = .93; HVLT-R: t [28.29] = 1.33, p = .20). CN = cognitively normal; PreMCI = pre-mild cognitive impairment; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment; NACC Delayed Paragraph Recall (NACC-DR); Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R); Object Recognition and Discrimination Task Difficult Objects Trials (ORDT-DO). Note: Standard nontransformed scores compared to the CN group are presented, error bars equivalent to standard error of the mean.

When the dementia group was removed and previous analyses were rerun, all relationships were preserved.

Analyses of the PreMCI group revealed that 11 participants met criteria for PreMCI-Clinical (N = 11) and 9 participants met criteria for PreMCI-Neuropsychological (N = 9; Table 3). There were no significant differences between the PreMCI subgroups in terms of primary language or gender of participants (Primary Language: X 2[1] = 0.08, p = .78; Gender: X 2[1] = 0.02, p = .89). Additionally, there were no significant differences between the PreMCI subgroups in terms of age or years of education (Age: F [1,18] = 0.03, p = .86; Years of Education: F [1,18] = 0.001, p = .97). Comparisons of the PreMCI subgroups across test variables revealed no significant group mean differences (ORDT-DO: F [1,19] = 0.10, p = .75; ORDT-DO Reaction Time Correct Trials: F [1,19] = 0.08, p = .79; NACC-DR: F [1,19] = 0.33, p = .58), although the difference between these subgroups on the HVLT-R trended toward significant (HVLT-R: F [1,18] = 3.91, p = .06). Taken together, these findings justified combining the PreMCI subgroups in subsequent analyses.

Table 3. PreMCI subgroup specific demographic information.

PreMCI subgroup-specific demographic information illustrated for all PreMCI participants. As is illustrated by the p values included, there were no significant differences found between the PreMCI subgroups in terms of primary language, gender of participants, age, or years of education. Comparisons of the PreMCI subgroups across test variables also revealed no significant group mean differences, although the difference between these subgroups on the HVLT-R trended toward significant. PreMCI = pre-mild cognitive impairment; E = English; S = Spanish; M = Male; F = Female; RT = Reaction Time. Note: Error terms included are standard deviation.

Multinomial Regression

The first model included performance variables of the two traditional memory measures and the ORDT-DO. According to the model fitting information, the final model was statistically significant (X 2[6] = 69.69, p < .001). Neither Pearson nor Deviance Chi-Square tests were significant, which also supports the goodness of fit of the model (Pearson: X 2[214] = 218.67, p = .40; Deviance: X 2[214] = 140.02, p = 1.00). According to the tests of likelihood ratios, the traditional memory measures contributed to prediction of current diagnosis over the reduced model (HVLT-R: X 2[2] = 18.60, p < .001; NACC-DR: X 2[2] = 8.27, p = .02). The model, which includes all three measures, represented a significant increase in classification accuracy over accuracy achieved by chance alone (Classification Accuracy = 75.0%). The ORDT-DO was also a statistically significant predictor in the model (X 2[2] = 15.92, p < .001).

According to the parameter estimates, only the ORDT-DO contributed to prediction of current PreMCI group membership compared to the CN group (Wald[1] = 5.26, p = .02, Exp(B) = 0.60). This finding suggests that, given a one-unit increase in standardized ORDT-DO score, there is a 40% reduction in the odds of a participant being identified in the PreMCI group when other variables in the model are held constant.

When predicting current aMCI group membership over CN group membership, HVLT-R and ORDT-DO were both significant (HVLT-R Wald[1] = 10.87, p < .001, Exp(B) = 0.45; ORDT-DO: Wald[1] = 9.52, p = .002, Exp(B) = 0.50), while NACC-DR was not significant (NACC-DR: Wald[1] = 2.77, p = .10).

When including age and education, the final model was statistically significant (X 2[10] = 72.12, p < .001). According to the tests of likelihood ratios, HVLT-R, NACC-DR, and ORDT-DO remained statistically significant predictors in the model. However, age and years of education were not significant (Age: X 2[2] = 0.37, p = .83; Years of Education: X 2[2] = 1.32, p = .52), suggesting that these variables did not provide unique contributions to the model. Therefore, this model was not considered in final discussion.

DISCUSSION

Although traditional memory measures are sufficiently sensitive to general memory impairments found in later stages of AD (Bilgel et al., Reference Bilgel, An, Lang, Prince, Ferrucci, Jedynak and Resnick2014; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999), use of such “blunt” measures and diagnostic methods may lead to high rates of over- and underdiagnosis of MCI (Edmonds et al., Reference Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Clark, Jak, Nation, McDonald and Bondi2015, Reference Edmonds, Delano-Wood, Jak, Galasko, Salmon and Bondi2016). The current study suggests that a novel measure of object discrimination that assesses a primary function of the PRC increased the correct identification of individuals with subtle cognitive impairment associated with risk of AD progression. Moreover, using visual tasks like the ORDT that employ less culturally or educationally dependent paradigms and stimuli allows for the creation of linguistically and culturally valid normative scores, which is increasingly important as the older adult population in the US rapidly diversifies (Snyder et al., Reference Snyder, Kahle-Wrobleski, Brannan, Miller, Schindler, DeSanti and Carrillo2014). As the ORDT has in the current study, visual discrimination tasks have been investigated in international populations for the detection of early impairment related to MCI (Alegret et al., Reference Alegret, Boada-Rovira, Vinyes-Junque, Valero, Espinosa, Hernandez and Tarraga2009).

Notably, the PRC has also been shown to be vulnerable to age-related physiological changes (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Maurer, Nematollahi, Uprety, Wallace and Barnes2014, Reference Burke, Wallace, Hartzell, Nematollahi, Plange and Barnes2011; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Turner, Santacroce, Carty, Shafiq, Bizon and Burke2017; Maurer et al., Reference Maurer, Burke, Diba and Barnes2017; Moyer et al., Reference Moyer, Furtak, McGann and Brown2011). These changes have been shown to affect object discrimination performance in both animals (Gaynor et al., Reference Gaynor, Johnson, Mizell, Campos, Maurer, Bauer and Burke2018; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Sacks, Turner, Gaynor, Ormerod, Maurer and Burke2016, Reference Johnson, Turner, Santacroce, Carty, Shafiq, Bizon and Burke2017) and humans (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Cardoza, Barense, Kawa, Wallentin-Flores, Arnold and Alexander2012; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Ryan, Cowell and Barense2013). Because of this, it will be important to pursue further refinements of functionally specific tasks that are differentially sensitive to normal and pathological aging. The results of the current study provide an initial step in this regard, since the ORDT was able to differentiate the performance of CN older adults from those with PreMCI, aMCI, and dementia. These findings are similar to those found by Barense et al. (Reference Barense, Ngo, Hung and Peterson2012), who identified deficits in discrimination of high-ambiguity objects within high-interference stimulus trains in patients with medial temporal lobe damage including the PRC. Although our study only considered performance on the target condition, target trials were interspersed in the stimulus train with both low- and high-ambiguity stimuli, suggesting that the declining performance of our participant groups with greater predicted medial temporal lobe dysfunction on target trials is at least partially related to the presence of highly interfering stimuli throughout the test.

Notably, we did not identify significant differences in ORDT-DO performance between PreMCI group participants that met criteria for either PreMCI-Neuropsychological or PreMCI-Clinical, suggesting that ORDT-DO deficits are present in PreMCI with or without impaired performance on traditional memory measures. The even ratio of PreMCI-Neuropsychological and PreMCI-Clinical participants most likely impacted the calculated average performance of this group on traditional memory measures. However, we did not identify significant differences in the performance of these groups on traditional memory measures, although there was one trend toward significance that suggested the PreMCI-Neuropsychological group performed worse on the HVLT-R compared to the PreMCI-Clinical group. The current sample is not large enough to have power to investigate these diagnostic groups separately. In future studies, when we have accrued a larger group of participants meeting criteria for PreMCI-Neuropsychological and PreMCI-Clinical, we will investigate these subgroups and their relationship to neuroimaging measures and risk of progression. We are most interested in investigating the ORDT-DO in identification of the PreMCI-Clinical subgroup, as we would like to examine whether the ORDT can detect cognitive impairment prior to traditional memory measures.

The utility of the ORDT in detecting very early cognitive change may depend on the presence of “typical” pathological spread, starting in the transentorhinal cortex and affecting the hippocampus and its efferent targets (Kurylo et al., Reference Kurylo, Corkin, Rizzo Iii and Growdon1996). If true, this would suggest that the ORDT would show less early sensitivity to onset of non-AD pathology or AD pathology that begins atypically in other brain regions. Performance on the ORDT is currently being evaluated in our laboratory in the context of structural and functional neuroimaging data on these participants in order to determine if “typical” disease progression is predictive of ORDT task performance.

Unfortunately, measures such as structural MRI volumes or cortical thickness may show minimal changes in the PreMCI state (Duara et al., Reference Duara, Loewenstein, Shen, Barker, Potter, Varon and Buckley2013). Specifically, cognitive aging studies support the presence of functional neurophysiological change prior to pathological neurodegeneration in older adults exhibiting cognitive impairment (Burke & Barnes, Reference Burke and Barnes2006). This suggests the need for considering other measures of the integrity of the functional brain connectome such as fMRI, amyloid, or tau PET (Brooks & Loewenstein, Reference Brooks and Loewenstein2010). It will be particularly important to determine how connections of the PRC to other task-relevant structures affect ORDT performance. For example, because “difficult” discriminations are achieved by rotation of nontarget stimuli, it is possible that posterior parietal cortex, which has reciprocal connections with the PRC and medial temporal lobe, also supports task completion (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Egan, Sonkkila, Tochon-Danguy, Paxinos and Watson2018). If this is true, the ORDT-DO may be capturing the combined insult of both early pathological tau and amyloid plaque accumulation, supporting its impressive sensitivity to the earliest brain changes related to AD. However, as was mentioned previously, the PRC has been shown to support identification of same stimuli presented at different viewpoints, and may be primarily involved in ORDT-DO task performance as was suggested by Devlin and Price (Reference Devlin and Price2007) in their original study. Other structures that likely contribute to object recognition and discrimination abilities include the hippocampus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex.

A potential weakness of the study is that primary analyses involved assessing the ORDT-DO against traditional memory measures in identification of significant differences between diagnostic subgroups. These traditional memory measures were used for constructing diagnostic subgroups, which would be assumed to give them an advantage in predicting group differences. In fact, the ORDT was also able to identify differences between the CN group and both the dementia and aMCI groups, as well as the identification of PreMCI group membership compared to CN group membership, when controlling for all other measures. Thus, findings from the current study support the superiority of the ORDT in comparison to traditional memory measures in identifying individuals with subtle cognitive decline.

Another important limitation to consider in the current study is that it is cross-sectional, so the object discrimination deficits were evaluated across individuals at only one-time point. Since the data reported here are part of a longitudinal study, follow-up evaluation of these participants is ongoing and will allow us to determine whether ORDT performance predicts case-wise longitudinal decline in performance over time and, specifically, progression to clinical AD (verified by amyloid PET and, where available, autopsy).

Although the study supports the use of an object discrimination task as an independent measure of early cognitive impairment related to AD, performing well alongside traditional memory measures actually used in the diagnostic process, accurate diagnosis, or prediction of cognitive decline will likely not be provided by any single measure of performance, anatomy, or pathology. Creating a composite medial temporal lobe-functioning score, in which performance data are combined with premortem measures of AD structural changes and pathology, may emerge as the most useful approach to identifying incipient impairment (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Mangialasche, Andreasen, Feldman, Giacobini, Jones and Kivipelto2014). The success of such a composite depends on identifying the most sensitive and process-specific cognitive measures available. This is especially important in determining the true underlying pathology of individuals in the current study diagnosed as PreMCI or aMCI. Future studies will employ the ORDT as well as other cognitive, anatomical, and pathological measures of early impairment [i.e., recovery from proactive semantic interference (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Curiel, Greig, Bauer, Rosado, Bowers and Duara2016, Reference Loewenstein, Curiel, DeKosky, Bauer, Rosselli, Guinjoan and Duara2018), volumetric measures of temporal lobe anatomy, PRC thinning, and amyloid load]. Employing a composite score will refine our ability to provide early detection of those patients at risk for development of AD, thus expanding opportunities for targeted therapeutic intervention.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This research is supported by the efforts of Kevin Hanson, Luz Arazo, and other members of the 1Florida ADRC Project II research coordinator and psychometrician staff.

FUNDING

This work was supported by the 1Florida ADRC (National Institute on Aging (NIA)-sponsored grant: grant number 5 P50 AG047726602), the University of Florida Clinical and Translational Science Institute (grant number NCATS UL1TR001427), and by grant number 1 R01 AG047649-01A1 to D.A.L.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors have nothing to disclose.

References

REFERENCES

Alegret, M., Boada-Rovira, M., Vinyes-Junque, G., Valero, S., Espinosa, A., Hernandez, I., & Tarraga, L. (2009). Detection of visuoperceptual deficits in preclinical and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(7), 860867. doi: 10.1080/13803390802595568CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barense, M.D., Henson, R.N., Lee, A.C., & Graham, K.S. (2010). Medial temporal lobe activity during complex discrimination of faces, objects, and scenes: effects of viewpoint. Hippocampus, 20(3), 389401. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20641Google ScholarPubMed
Barense, M.D., Ngo, J.K., Hung, L.H., & Peterson, M.A. (2012). Interactions of memory and perception in amnesia: the figure-ground perspective. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 26802691. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr347CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartko, S.J., Winters, B.D., Cowell, R.A., Saksida, L.M., & Bussey, T.J. (2007). Perirhinal cortex resolves feature ambiguity in configural object recognition and perceptual oddity tasks. Learning & Memory, 14(12), 821832. doi: 10.1101/lm.749207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, R.H.B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., & Brandt, J. (2010). Hopkins verbal learning test—revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(1), 4355. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.1.43.1726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilgel, M., An, Y., Lang, A., Prince, J., Ferrucci, L., Jedynak, B., & Resnick, S.M. (2014). Trajectories of Alzheimer disease-related cognitive measures in a longitudinal sample. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 10(6), 735742.e734. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braak, H. & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82(4), 239259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, L.G. & Loewenstein, D.A. (2010). Assessing the progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: current trends and future directions. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy, 2(5), 28. doi: 10.1186/alzrt52Google ScholarPubMed
Buckley, M.J., Booth, M.C., Rolls, E.T., & Gaffan, D. (2001). Selective perceptual impairments after perirhinal cortex ablation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(24), 98249836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckley, M.J. & Gaffan, D. (1997). Impairment of visual object-discrimination learning after perirhinal cortex ablation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111(3), 467475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, S.N. & Barnes, C.A. (2006). Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 3040. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=16371948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, S.N., Maurer, A.P., Nematollahi, S., Uprety, A., Wallace, J.L., & Barnes, C.A. (2014). Advanced age dissociates dual functions of the perirhinal cortex. Journal of Neuoscience, 34(2), 467480.Google ScholarPubMed
Burke, S.N., Wallace, J.L., Hartzell, A.L., Nematollahi, S., Plange, K., & Barnes, C.A. (2011). Age-associated deficits in pattern separation functions of the perirhinal cortex: a cross-species consensus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125(6), 836847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bussey, T.J., Saksida, L.M., & Murray, E.A. (2003). Impairments in visual discrimination after perirhinal cortex lesions: testing “declarative” vs. “perceptual-mnemonic” views of perirhinal cortex function. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(3), 649660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, R.J., Locke, D.E., Dueck, A.C., Knopman, D.S., Woodruff, B.K., Hoffman-Snyder, C., & Reiman, E.M. (2014). The neuropsychology of normal aging and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(1), 8492. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowell, R.A., Bussey, T.J., & Saksida, L.M. (2006). Why does brain damage impair memory? A connectionist model of object recognition memory in perirhinal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(47), 1218612197. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2818-06.2006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devlin, J.T. & Price, C.J. (2007). Perirhinal contributions to human visual perception. Current Biology, 17(17), 14841488. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duara, R., Loewenstein, D.A., Greig, M.T., Potter, E., Barker, W., Raj, A., & Potter, H. (2011). Pre-MCI and MCI: neuropsychological, clinical, and imaging features and progression rates. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(11), 951960. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182107c69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duara, R., Loewenstein, D.A., Shen, Q., Barker, W., Potter, E., Varon, D., & Buckley, C. (2013). Amyloid positron emission tomography with (18)F-flutemetamol and structural magnetic resonance imaging in the classification of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 9(3), 295301. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edmonds, E.C., Delano-Wood, L., Clark, L.R., Jak, A.J., Nation, D.A., McDonald, C.R., & Bondi, M.W. (2015). Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false-positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11(4), 415424. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edmonds, E.C., Delano-Wood, L., Jak, A.J., Galasko, D.R., Salmon, D.P., & Bondi, M.W. (2016). “Missed” mild cognitive impairment: high false-negative error rate based on conventional diagnostic criteria. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 52(2), 685691. doi: 10.3233/jad-150986CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, R.W. (1991). The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised: psychometric characteristics and clinical application. Neuropsychology Review, 2(2), 179201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fidalgo, C.O., Changoor, A.T., Page-Gould, E., Lee, A.C., & Barense, M.D. (2016). Early cognitive decline in older adults better predicts object than scene recognition performance. Hippocampus, 26(12), 15791592. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaynor, L.S., Johnson, S.A., Mizell, J.M., Campos, K.T., Maurer, A.P., Bauer, R.M., & Burke, S.N. (2018). Impaired discrimination with intact crossmodal association in aged rats: a dissociation of perirhinal cortical-dependent behaviors. Behavioral Neuroscience, 132, 138151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, I.M., Egan, G.F., Sonkkila, C., Tochon-Danguy, H.J., Paxinos, G., & Watson, J.D.G. (2018). Selective right parietal lobe activation during mental rotationA parametric PET study. Brain , 123(1), 6573. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, C.M., Yee, C., May, T., Dhanala, A., & Mitchell, C.S. (2017). Cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: amyloid-beta versus tauopathy. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 61(1), 265281. doi: 10.3233/jad-170490CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S.A., Sacks, P.K., Turner, S.M., Gaynor, L.S., Ormerod, B.K., Maurer, A.P., & Burke, S.N. (2016). Discrimination performance in aging is vulnerable to interference and dissociable from spatial memory. Learning & Memory, 23, 339348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S.A., Turner, S.M., Santacroce, L.A., Carty, K.N., Shafiq, L., Bizon, J.L., & Burke, S.N. (2017). Rodent age-related impairments in discriminating perceptually similar objects parallel those observed in humans. Hippocampus, 27(7), 759776. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22729CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, U.A., Liu, L., Provenzano, F.A., Berman, D.E., Profaci, C.P., Sloan, R., & Small, S.A. (2014). Molecular drivers and cortical spread of lateral entorhinal cortex dysfunction in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Nature Neuroscience, 17(2), 304311. doi: 10.1038/nn.3606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kivisaari, S.L., Tyler, L.K., Monsch, A.U., & Taylor, K.I. (2012). Medial perirhinal cortex disambiguates confusable objects. Brain, 135(Pt 12), 37573769. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, K.R., Lam, C.S., & Wilson, R.S. (2006). The word accentuation test—Chicago. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(7), 12011207. doi: 10.1080/13803390500346603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krumm, S., Kivisaari, S.L., Probst, A., Monsch, A.U., Reinhardt, J., Ulmer, S., & Taylor, K.I. (2016). Cortical thinning of parahippocampal subregions in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 38, 188196. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurylo, D.D., Corkin, S., Rizzo Iii, J.F., & Growdon, J.H. (1996). Greater relative impairment of object recognition than of visuospatial abilities in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10(1), 7481. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.10.1.74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, A.C., Barense, M.D., & Graham, K.S. (2005). The contribution of the human medial temporal lobe to perception: bridging the gap between animal and human studies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58(3–4), 300325. doi: 10.1080/02724990444000168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., Loring, D.W., & Fischer, J.S. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, D.A., Curiel, R.E., DeKosky, S., Bauer, R.M., Rosselli, M., Guinjoan, S.M., & Duara, R. (2018). Utilizing semantic intrusions to identify amyloid positivity in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 91(10), e976e984. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, D.A., Curiel, R.E., Greig, M.T., Bauer, R.M., Rosado, M., Bowers, D., & Duara, R. (2016). A novel cognitive stress test for the detection of preclinical Alzheimer disease: discriminative properties and relation to amyloid load. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(10), 804813. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.02.056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, D.A., Greig, M.T., Schinka, J.A., Barker, W., Shen, Q., Potter, E., & Duara, R. (2012). An investigation of PreMCI: subtypes and longitudinal outcomes. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 8(3), 172179. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loonstra, A.S., Tarlow, A.R., & Sellers, A.H. (2001). COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Applied Neuropsychology, 8(3), 161166. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an0803_5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, E.J., Hussey, E.P., Molitor, R.J., Ko, P.C., Donahue, M.J., & Ally, B.A. (2017). Family history of Alzheimer’s disease is associated with impaired perceptual discrimination of novel objects. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 57(3), 735745. doi: 10.3233/jad-160772CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, A.P., Burke, S.N., Diba, K., & Barnes, C.A. (2017). Attenuated activity across multiple cell types and reduced monosynaptic connectivity in the aged perirhinal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(37), 89658974. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0531-17.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyer, J.R. Jr., Furtak, S.C., McGann, J.P., & Brown, T.H. (2011). Aging-related changes in calcium-binding proteins in rat perirhinal cortex. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(9), 16931706. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, E.A. & Bussey, T.J. (1999). Perceptual-mnemonic functions of the perirhinal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(4), 142151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newsome, R.N., Duarte, A., & Barense, M.D. (2012). Reducing perceptual interference improves visual discrimination in mild cognitive impairment: implications for a model of perirhinal cortex function. Hippocampus, 22(10), 19901999. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qiu, W.Y., Yang, Q., Zhang, W., Wang, N., Zhang, D., Huang, Y., & Ma, C. (2017). The correlations between postmortem brain pathologies and cognitive dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Current Alzheimer Research, 15(5), 462473. doi: 10.2174/1567205014666171106150915CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rockwood, K., Strang, D., MacKnight, C., Downer, R., & Morris, J.C. (2000). Interrater reliability of the Clinical Dementia Rating in a multicenter trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48(5), 558559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, L., Cardoza, J.A., Barense, M.D., Kawa, K.H., Wallentin-Flores, J., Arnold, W.T., & Alexander, G.E. (2012). Age-related impairment in a complex object discrimination task that engages perirhinal cortex. Hippocampus, 22(10), 19781989. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, L.S., Mangialasche, F., Andreasen, N., Feldman, H., Giacobini, E., Jones, R., & Kivipelto, M. (2014). Clinical trials and late-stage drug development for Alzheimer’s disease: an appraisal from 1984 to 2014. Journal of Internal Medicine, 275(3), 251283. doi: 10.1111/joim.12191CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, W.G., Tierney, M.C., Zorzitto, M.L., Fisher, R.H., & Reid, D.W. (1989). WAIS-R test-retest reliability in a normal elderly sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11(4), 423428. doi: 10.1080/01688638908400903CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, P.J., Kahle-Wrobleski, K., Brannan, S., Miller, D.S., Schindler, R.J., DeSanti, S., & Carrillo, M.C. (2014). Assessing cognition and function in Alzheimer’s disease clinical trials: do we have the right tools?: Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(6), 853860. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.07.158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sone, D., Imabayashi, E., Maikusa, N., Okamura, N., Furumoto, S., Kudo, Y., & Matsuda, H. (2017). Regional tau deposition and subregion atrophy of medial temporal structures in early Alzheimer’s disease: a combined positron emission tomography/magnetic resonance imaging study. Alzheimer’s & Dementia (Amst), 9, 3540. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.07.001Google ScholarPubMed
Tombaugh, T.N. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(2), 203214. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(03)00039-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weintraub, S., Salmon, D., Mercaldo, N., Ferris, S., Graff-Radford, N.R., Chui, H., & Morris, J.C. (2009). The Alzheimer’s Disease Centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS): the neuropsychological test battery. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 23(2), 91101. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7ddCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissberger, G.H., Strong, J.V., Stefanidis, K.B., Summers, M.J., Bondi, M.W., & Stricker, N.H. (2017). Diagnostic accuracy of memory measures in Alzheimer’s dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 27(4), 354388. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9360-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, G.S. & Robertson, G. (2006). Wide Range Achievement Test (4th ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R.W., Muñoz-Sandoval, A.F., Ruef, M.L., & Alvarado, C.G. (2005). Woodcock language proficiency battery—Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Yeung, L.K., Ryan, J.D., Cowell, R.A., & Barense, M.D. (2013). Recognition memory impairments caused by false recognition of novel objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 13841397. doi: 10.1037/a0034021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographic information.

Figure 1

Table 2. Algorithmic diagnostic criteria.

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Representation of standard trials of the ORDT. On each trial, participants were shown a horizontal array of four stimuli (Nonobjects, Shapes, Colors, and Animals). Nonobjects were photographs of models created from building blocks, resulting in entities that were not found in the real world. Task conditions included object type and difficulty level (Easy and Difficult). In the “easy” condition, the three nontarget items were all presented in the same orientation. In the “difficult” condition, the three nontarget items are rotated individually (horizontally or vertically) in picture plane. In this way, success on the “difficult” condition requires the participant to recognize that the three nontarget items are the same entity even though their appearance is changed by rotation.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Graphical representation of performance of clinical diagnostic subgroups compared to the CN group on traditional memory measures and the ORDT. There were statistically significant differences between diagnostic subgroups in performance on both traditional memory measures, and also the ORDT-DO (HVLT-R: F[3,131] = 28.21, p < .001, ηp2 = .40; NACC-DR: F[3,133] = 25.95, p < .001, ηp2 = .37; ORDT-DO: F[3,134] = 8.74, p < .001, ηp2 = .17). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed group mean differences between the CN and the aMCI and dementia groups [HVLT-R (CN: M = 7.35, SD = 3.06; aMCI: M = 1.96, SD = 2.81; Dementia: M = 0.31, SD = 1.25); NACC-DR (CN: M = 11.43, SD = 3.80; aMCI: M = 7.40, SD = 4.07; Dementia: M = 2.53, SD = 3.54); ORDT-DO (CN: M = 86.5, SD = 8.04; aMCI: M = 68.03, SD = 17.28; Dementia: M = 60.63, SD = 17.02)]. Importantly, when an independent samples t test was performed comparing the CN and PreMCI groups, only the ORDT-DO revealed significant group mean difference in test scores (t[23.93] = 2.18, p = .04, Cohen’s d = 0.61). The traditional neuropsychological measures failed to discriminate between these two groups, as would be expected based on diagnostic criteria (NACC-DR: t[41] = 0.43, p = .93; HVLT-R: t[28.29] = 1.33, p = .20). CN = cognitively normal; PreMCI = pre-mild cognitive impairment; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment; NACC Delayed Paragraph Recall (NACC-DR); Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R); Object Recognition and Discrimination Task Difficult Objects Trials (ORDT-DO). Note: Standard nontransformed scores compared to the CN group are presented, error bars equivalent to standard error of the mean.

Figure 4

Table 3. PreMCI subgroup specific demographic information.