Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T17:42:28.505Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Post-Traumatic Symptomatology and Compulsions as Potential Mediators of the Relation Between Child Sexual Abuse and Auditory Verbal Hallucinations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 November 2017

Simon McCarthy-Jones*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Psychiatry, Trinity Centre for Health Sciences, St James's Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland. E-mail: s.mccarthyjones@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background: Whilst evidence is mounting that childhood sexual abuse (CSA) can be a cause of auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH), it is unclear what factors mediate this relation. Recent evidence suggests that post-traumatic symptomatology may mediate the CSA–AVH relation in clinical populations, although this hypothesis has not yet been tested in the general population. There is also reason to believe that obsessive ideation could mediate the CSA–AVH relation. Aims: To test for evidence to falsify the hypotheses that post-traumatic symptomatology, obsessions, compulsions, anxiety and depression mediate the relation between CSA and AVH in a general population sample. Method: Indirect effects of CSA on AVH via potential mediators were tested for, using a regression-based approach employing data from the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (n = 5788). Results: After controlling for demographics, IQ and child physical abuse, it was found that CSA, IQ, post-traumatic symptomatology and compulsions predicted lifetime experience of AVH. Mediation analyses found significant indirect effects of CSA on AVH via post-traumatic symptomatology [odds ratio (OR): 1.11; 95% confidence interval (CI):1.00–1.29] and compulsions (OR: 1.10, 95% CI: 1.01–1.28). Conclusions: These findings offer further support for the hypothesis that post-traumatic symptomatology is a mediator of the CSA–AVH relation. Although no evidence was found for obsessional thoughts as a mediating variable, a potential mediating role for compulsions is theoretically intriguing. This study's findings reiterate the need to ask about experiences of childhood adversity and post-traumatic symptomology in people with AVH, as well as the likely therapeutic importance of trauma-informed and trauma-based interventions for this population.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2017 

Introduction

For over a century there have been proposals that suffering child abuse is associated with an increased probability of later developing the experience of ‘hearing voices’, formally termed auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH; McCarthy-Jones, Reference McCarthy-Jones2012). Yet it has only been in the past two decades that large-scale empirical studies have confirmed an association of AVH with child abuse, and in particular child sexual abuse (CSA; Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012; Daalman et al., Reference Daalman, Diederen, Derks, van Lutterveld, Kahn and Sommer2012; Shevlin et al., Reference Shevlin, Dorahy and Adamson2007; Read et al., Reference Read, Agar, Argyle and Aderhold2003; Üçok and Bikmaz, Reference Üçok and Bıkmaz2007). Evidence is mounting that these associations are causal (Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012; Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Keeley, Corcoran, Ramsay, Wasserman and Carli2013; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Bulik, Silberg, Hettema, Myers and Prescott2000), although some concerns remain to be addressed (Susser and Widom, Reference Susser and Widom2012), including that this relation could stem from an evocative gene–environment correlation (e.g. Sideli et al., Reference Sideli, Mulè, La Barbera and Murray2012). As AVH can lead to distress, self-harm and suicide (Kjelby et al., Reference Kjelby, Sinkeviciute, Gjestad, Kroken, Løberg and Jørgensen2015), there is an urgent need to better understand their causes to better prevent such consequences.

Little empirical work has been done on what factors may mediate the specific relation between CSA and AVH, despite multiple studies calling for such investigations (e.g. Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012, Reference Bentall, de Sousa, Varese, Wickham, Sitko, Haarmans and Read2014). Initial studies that examined mediators of the relations between CSA and psychosis (Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, Jonas, Kuipers, King, Cooper and Brugha2011; Marwaha and Bebbington, Reference Marwaha and Bebbington2015), and between CSA and hallucinations in general (Sitko et al., Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014), found evidence consistent with depression and anxiety acting as mediators. However, a recent study of people with psychosis by Hardy et al. (Reference Hardy, Emsley, Freeman, Bebbington, Garety and Kuipers2016), which did specifically test for mediators between CSA and AVH, found no evidence that depression mediated the CSA–AVH relation. Instead, evidence was reported consistent with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology (avoidance, numbing and hyperarousal) mediating this relation.

Precise estimates of rates of PTSD in CSA survivors are hard to establish, due to methodological and sampling variation between studies and the impact of the nature of the CSA on the probability of PTSD developing. At the low end of estimates, Cutajar et al. (Reference Cutajar, Mullen, Ogloff, Thomas, Wells and Spataro2010) found that 4% of adult survivors of CSA had been formally diagnosed with PTSD, and Maikovich et al. (Reference Maikovich, Koenen and Jaffee2009) found that 14% of children who had experienced CSA had clinically significant levels of PTSD symptoms. Other studies have reported higher estimates. Deblinger et al. (Reference Deblinger, McLeer, Atkins, Ralphe and Foa1989) found that 21% of psychiatrically hospitalized children who had experienced CSA met diagnostic criteria for PTSD, Widom (Reference Widom1999) reported that 38% of adults who had suffered CSA met lifetime diagnostic criteria for PSTD, and McLeer et al. (Reference McLeer, Deblinger, Henry and Orvaschel1992) found that 10% of children sexually abused by strangers, 42% of children sexually abused by a trusted adult, and 54% of children sexually abused by their fathers met diagnostic criteria for PTSD. Whilst the prevalence of PTSD in CSA survivors remains unclear, what is not in debate is that CSA increases the risk of an individual developing PTSD symptomatology (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Murad, Paras, Colbenson, Sattler and Goranson2010; Fergusson et al., Reference Fergusson, McLeod and Horwood2013; Kendall-Tackett et al., Reference Kendall-Tackett, Williams and Finkelhor1993).

One of the hallmark symptoms of PTSD, highly prevalent in survivors of CSA, is intrusions (Kuyken and Brewin, Reference Kuyken and Brewin1994; Priebe et al., Reference Priebe, Kleindienst, Zimmer, Koudela, Ebner-Priemer and Bohus2013). The extent of these in CSA survivors was recently documented by Priebe et al. (Reference Priebe, Kleindienst, Zimmer, Koudela, Ebner-Priemer and Bohus2013), using an experience sampling methodology. They found that in a 12-hour period, from 08.00 h to 20.00 h, CSA survivors diagnosed with PTSD reported an average of 75 intrusions and 24 flashbacks. As intrusive thoughts are also associated with the presence of AVH (Morrison and Baker, Reference Morrison and Baker2000; Jones and Fernyhough, Reference Jones and Fernyhough2006) they are one potential mediator of the relation between CSA and AVH.

Intrusive thoughts are not a homogeneous category. Distinct types have been identified and separated off into named disorders. Intrusive thoughts about personal loss and failure (negative automatic thinking) are viewed as characteristic of depression (Beck, Reference Beck1967; Clark, Reference Clark1992). Intrusive thoughts about possible harm and danger are viewed as characteristic of anxiety (Clark, Reference Clark1992; Clark and Beck, Reference Clark, Beck, Kendall and Watson1989). A third category of intrusive thought that can be distinguished is those which are obsessive (Clark, Reference Clark1992; Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1985). Obsessive intrusive thoughts do not run parallel to the stream of consciousness, seem to involve socially unacceptable thoughts that indicate a loss of impulse control, and are associated with an exaggerated sense of personal responsibility (Clark, Reference Clark1992; Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1985).

There is reason to believe that intrusive obsessional thoughts, whose presence are a criterion for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; APA, 2013), may be a mediator of the relation between CSA and AVH. The idea that AVH specifically share something in common with obsessions goes back to (at least) 1895 and the French psychiatrist Jules Seglas, who coined the term hallucination obsédante (obsessional hallucinations) to refer to a hallucination accompanied by the characteristics of an obsession. Not only is there a marked phenomenological parallel between AVH and thoughts in OCD, with both being highly repetitive (McCarthy-Jones et al., Reference McCarthy-Jones, Trauer, Mackinnon, Sims, Thomas and Copolov2012), but there are also preliminary suggestions that drugs with anti-obsessional effects may be able to help AVH (Stephane et al., Reference Stephane, Polis and Barton2000). Furthermore, patients with OCD have been repeatedly shown to have deficits in intentional cognitive inhibition, the ability to effortfully, consciously suppress cognitions from consciousness (Badcock et al., Reference Badcock, Waters and Maybery2007; Bohne et al., Reference Bohne, Keuthen, Tuschen-Caffier and Wilhelm2005), as have individuals with AVH in both clinical (Waters et al., Reference Waters, Badcock, Maybery and Michie2003; Badcock et al., Reference Badcock, Waters, Maybery and Michie2005) and non-clinical (Paulik et al., Reference Paulik, Badcock and Maybery2007) populations. Indeed, Waters et al. (Reference Waters, Badcock, Maybery and Michie2003) have argued that a deficit in intentional cognitive inhibition is ‘critical’ to AVH (p. 276). To this may be added findings from diffusion tensor imaging studies, which have found that the presence of AVH in schizophrenia is associated with reduced structural integrity of the fronto-temporal segment of the inferior occipital-frontal fasciculus (Oestreich et al., Reference Oestreich, McCarthy-Jones and Whitford2015). Damage to this tract has been found in other populations to be associated with verbal perseverance (Khan et al., Reference Khan, Herbet, Moritz-Gasser and Duffau2014). Not only are AVH and schizophrenia associated with an increased risk of a diagnosis of OCD (Attademo et al., Reference Attademo, Bernardini, Paolini and Quartesan2015), but so is CSA (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Vishne, Sasson, Gross, Livne and Zohar2008). It is therefore plausible that CSA results in a reduction in inhibitory control, which leads to obsessional ideation, which may in some cases come to manifest as AVH.

It could be objected that obsessive ideation and AVH could be separate manifestations of a reduction in inhibitory control, with no causal relation between the two. However, clinical examples can be found that document the development of AVH from what began as obsessional-compulsive symptomatology. Consider the following example from Poyurovsky (Reference Poyurovsky2013):

Since the age of 18 Emma has heard voices which curse her and repeat phrases such as ‘yes, correct’. Her first psychiatric problem occurred when she was 12 when she became preoccupied with aggressive and sexual thoughts and fears of saying the wrong thing. As a result she started a range of compulsive checking, counting and repetitive behaviours for up to 2–3 hours a day. At 19 she developed ‘obsessive auditory hallucinations’ as well as delusions.

Less anecdotally, van Dael et al. (Reference van Dael, Van Os, De Graaf, Ten Have, Krabbendam and Myin-Germeys2011) have found that levels of obsessive-compulsive symptomatology prospectively predict the later development of psychotic symptoms. It is therefore plausible that obsessive ideation resulting from CSA may come to be experienced as AVH, and hence could act as a mediator between CSA and AVH. Of course, not all AVH have a repetitive nature (McCarthy-Jones et al., Reference McCarthy-Jones, Trauer, Mackinnon, Sims, Thomas and Copolov2012), and any such model of the AVH experience would probably only account for a subset of AVH (Jones, Reference Jones2010).

As a result of this, the first aim of this study was to use data collected by the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Meltzer, Brugha and Bebbington2009) to test the hypothesis that obsessional thoughts mediate the relation between CSA and AVH in the general population. The specificity of this effect to obsessional thoughts was to be tested for by also examining if there was any evidence that levels of compulsions, depression or anxiety could act as mediators between CSA. The specificity of this effect to CSA was to be tested for by controlling for levels of child physical abuse (CPA), which has previously been found to be associated with AVH in the APMS dataset (Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012). The second aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that PTSD symptomatology would mediate the relation between CSA and AVH in a general population sample. Although the cross-sectional APMS dataset cannot be used to prove the existence of a causal, mediating role for the candidate variables, the study nevertheless still had the ability to provide evidence to falsify the hypothesized mediating mechanisms.

Method

Participants

The study used data from the 2007 APMS (McManus et al., Reference McManus, Meltzer, Brugha and Bebbington2009). In this survey, 7403 individuals aged over 16 years living in private households completed interview and administered computer-assisted interviews and self-completion questionnaires on topics including demographics, physical health, mental health, social capital and child abuse.

Measures

Child sexual abuse (CSA)

Three types of CSA were assessed by the APMS: CSA involving intercourse (CSAint), CSA involving touch but not intercourse (CSAtouch), and CSA involving talk but not intercourse or touch (CSAtalk). Previous studies of the APMS dataset have found that of these three forms of sexual abuse, only CSAint was associated with AVH (Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012). This study therefore limited CSA to the binary variable CSAint, which was assessed by the question ‘Before the age of 16, did anyone have sexual intercourse with you without your consent?’. This was a binary variable (yes/no). In order to increase the power of the study, those who had experienced CSAtouch or CSAtalk, in the absence of CSAint, were removed from the study. This left two groups, a CSAint group and a control group who did not report any form of child sexual abuse.

Child physical abuse (CPA)

This was assessed using the item ‘Before the age of 16, were you ever severely beaten by a parent, step-parent, or carer?’. This was a binary variable (yes/no).

Auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH)

Experiences of complex AVHs over the past year were assessed using the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (Bebbington and Nayani, Reference Bebbington and Nayani1995) item ‘[Over the past year] Did you at any time hear voices saying quite a few words or sentences when there was no one around that might account for it?’. This was a binary variable (yes/no).

Anxiety

Following the approach of previous studies (Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, Jonas, Kuipers, King, Cooper and Brugha2011; Marwaha and Bebbington, Reference Marwaha and Bebbington2015), anxiety was defined as the sum of the anxiety and worry subscales of the Revised Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS-R; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn1992). CIS-R (worry) subscale scores (APMS variable name: DVI11) are calculated based on four questions, including ‘Been worrying about things other than physical health on four or more days out of past seven days?’ and ‘Have been worrying too much in view of your circumstances?’. Endorsement of an item is scored 1, and hence total scores on this subscale can range from 0 to 4. CIS-R (anxiety) subscale scores (APMS variable name: DVJ12) are also calculated based on responses to four questions. Endorsement of items such as ‘Felt generally anxious/nervous/tense on four or more of past seven days’ and ‘Anxiety/nervousness/tension has been very unpleasant in past week’ are scored one point. Total scores on this subscale can hence range from 0 to 4. Scores on the anxiety measure created by combining the CIS-R (worry) and CIS-R (anxiety) subscales could therefore range from 0 to 8, and this combined measure had satisfactory internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .80).

Depression

Following the approach of previous studies (Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, Jonas, Kuipers, King, Cooper and Brugha2011; Marwaha and Bebbington, Reference Marwaha and Bebbington2015), depression was defined as the sum of the CIS-R (depression) and CIS-R (depressive ideas) subscales. Responses to four items are used to calculate a depression subscale score (APMS variable name: DVG11). Endorsement of items such as ‘Unable to enjoy or take an interest in things as much as usual in past week’ and ‘Felt sad, miserable or depressed/unable to enjoy or take an interest in things on four days or more in past week’ are scored one point. Total scores on this subscale can hence range from 0 to 4. Responses to five items are used to calculate a depressive ideas subscale score (APMS variable name: DVH11). Endorsement of items such as ‘Felt guilty when things went wrong in past week’ are scored one point. Total scores on this subscale can hence range from 0 to 5. Scores on the depression measure created by combining the CIS-R (depression) and CIS-R (depressive ideas) subscales could therefore range from 0 to 9. This measure had satisfactory internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .80).

Obsessional thought

This was assessed using the four-item CIS-R (obsessions) subscale score (APMS variable name: DVN9). Example of items include ‘Unpleasant thoughts or ideas kept coming into your mind on four days or more in last week’ and ‘Tried to stop thinking any of these thoughts in past week’. Endorsement of an item is scored as one point, and total scores on this scale can hence range from 0 to 4. This scale had satisfactory internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .89).

Compulsions

This was assessed using the four-item CIS-R (compulsions) subscale score (APMS variable name: DVM9). Examples of these items include ‘Found yourself doing things over again (that you had already done) on four days or more in last week’ and ‘Have tried to stop repeating behaviour/doing these things over again during past week’. Endorsement of an item is scored as one point, and total scores on this scale can hence range from 0 to 4. This scale had satisfactory internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .85).

PTSD symptomatology

This was assessed using the total score (APMS variable name: PTSDcom) on the Trauma Screening Questionnaire (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Rose, Andrews, Green, Tata and McEvedy2002). This is a brief 10-item self-report measure that assesses the presence of re-experiencing and arousal symptoms using items derived from PTSD Symptom Scale-Self Report version (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Riggs, Dancu and Rothbaum1993). Examples of items include ‘Feeling upset about reminders of the event’ and ‘Being jumpy or being startled at something unexpected’. Endorsement of an item is scored as one point, and total scores on this scale can hence range from 0 to 10. This scale had satisfactory internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .82).

Demographic and other confounds

The following variables were also assessed to control for their potentially confounding effects: age, gender (self-reported male or female), ethnicity (white British, other), highest educational qualification (degree, non-degree higher qualification, A-level, GCSE, other, none), and IQ (assessed through the National Adult Reading Test; Nelson, Reference Nelson1991).

Statistical analyses

Multiple linear regression was used to test for total, direct and indirect relations between CSA and AVH using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, Reference Hayes2013) in SPSS version 22. PROCESS employs maximum likelihood logistic regression for dichotomous dependent variables, and was employed using bias corrected, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for indirect associations with 1000 bootstrap samples. In the first set of analyses, CSA was entered as the independent variable, AVH as the dependent variable, PTSD, anxiety, obsessions and compulsions as mediating variables (using PROCESS model 4 which uses a parallel mediation model), and age, gender, ethnicity (dummy coded), highest educational qualification (dummy coded), IQ, CPA and depression were added as covariates. Covariates were used in models of both the mediators and the dependent variables. As PROCESS model 4 can only test for four mediating variables at one time, the analyses were then re-run to test if there was any evidence of an indirect effect of depression on AVH. This analysis was performed in the same manner as above, with the only changes being that anxiety was removed as a mediator and included as a covariate, and depression was removed as a covariate and including as a mediator.

Results

A number of participants in the sample had missing data for some of the study variables. As PROCESS cannot work with missing data, these individual were removed from the analysis. In addition, as noted above, participants who had experienced either CSAtouch or CSAtalk, in the absence of CSAint, were also removed from the sample. This yielded a remaining sample size of 5788. Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for sample

The study variables were a significant predictor of the presence of AVH; χ2 (16) = 132.11, p < .001, –2 Log likelihood = 433.10, Nagelkerke R 2 = .24. Details of the final regression equation for predicting AVH are presented in Table 2; as this table shows, CSA, PTSD symptomatology, compulsions and IQ were significant predictors of AVH.

Table 2. Predictors of presence of auditory verbal hallucinations

Effect of gender is for men, relative to a female reference group. Effect of ethnicity is for being non-White British, relative to a White British reference group. Effect of education is relative to a reference group with no qualifications.

There was a significant total association between CSA and AVH, consisting of significant direct and indirect effects (Table 3). As Table 3 shows, when the significant indirect effect of CSA on AVH (at p < .05) was broken down into its component parts, evidence was only found of a significant indirect effect of CSA on AVH via PTSD symptomatology and compulsions.

Table 3. Direct and indirect effects of child sexual abuse on auditory verbal hallucinations

*p < .05.

It was next tested if there was an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via depression. The original analyses were repeated with the only difference being that depression was now entered as a mediator, not a covariate, and anxiety was entered as a covariate, not a mediator. This analysis found no evidence of an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via depression; odds ratio 1.10 (95% confidence interval: 0.93–1.32).

Discussion

This study aimed to test the hypotheses that obsessive thoughts and PTSD symptomatology mediate the relation between CSA and AVH in a general population sample. The hypothesis that obsessive thoughts mediated the relation between CSA and AVH was falsified. There was also no evidence of a main effect of obsessive thoughts on AVH. This was surprising, given the strong theoretical rationale for expecting a relation between obsessive thought and AVH. One possibility is that this was a Type II error, stemming from the measure of obsessive thought employed. The limited sensitivity of the measure, which was only scored on a five-point Likert scale, could have been the reason that no effect was found. However, this argument is mitigated against by the finding of an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via compulsions, a measure that employed the same scoring scale. A second possibility is that the CIS-R measure of obsessions only captures this construct at a high level. It could be helpful for future research to use more sensitive measures that capture specific aspects of obsessive thought. For example, research could examine if AVH are specifically associated with socially unacceptable obsessive thoughts involving an exaggerated sense of personal responsibility (Clark, Reference Clark1992), which are characteristic of OCD but yet were not specifically inquired about as part of the CIS-R measure of obsessions.

This study failed to falsify the hypothesis that PTSD symptomatology mediated the relation between CSA and AVH. A main effect of PTSD symptomatology on AVH was also found. These findings speak to the argument that AVH should be a named characteristic symptom of PTSD (McCarthy-Jones and Longden, Reference McCarthy-Jones and Longden2015). The evidence for an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via PTSD symptomatology in this general population sample mirrors the finding of such a relation in a clinical sample by Hardy et al. (Reference Hardy, Emsley, Freeman, Bebbington, Garety and Kuipers2016). The failure of this study to find evidence of an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via depression also mirrors the findings of Hardy et al. Further work is now needed, using longitudinal studies, to test if PTSD symptomatology does indeed mediate the relation between CSA and AVH. Such work should also aim to better understand the specific processes involved, such as whether it is intrusions from trauma memory resulting from CSA that form the basis of AVH (Hardy, Reference Hardy2017).

An unexpected finding was that there was an indirect effect of CSA on AVH via compulsions. It is possible that this represents a Type I error, and attempted replication of this finding is needed. Although further speculation on the meaning of this finding is premature, it is nonetheless of theoretical interest. Should the current pattern of findings be replicated, it would need to be explained why compulsions but not obsessions were associated with AVH. An interesting possibility is raised here by a consideration of trichotillomania. This condition involves the compulsive act of hairpulling, yet is not strongly associated with obsessive thoughts (Bohne et al., Reference Bohne, Keuthen, Tuschen-Caffier and Wilhelm2005). Trichotillomania has been found to be associated with abnormal motor inhibitory functioning (Bohne et al., Reference Bohne, Savage, Deckersbach, Keuthen and Wilhelm2008; van Velzen et al., Reference van Velzen, Vriend, de Wit and van den Heuvel2014), which is notable given arguments that AVH could be understood as based in inner speech, which can be conceived of as a motor action (Jones and Fernyhough, Reference Jones and Fernyhough2007). This raises the possibility that specifically abnormal motor inhibitory functioning could play an important role in AVH. This could be a profitable direction for future study. Of course, another possibility is that compulsions are a consequence, not a cause, of AVH. One way to understand such a relation would be to propose that what voices say drives people to undertake compulsive actions. Had the findings of this study come from a clinical population of voice-hearers, in which commands are common (McCarthy-Jones et al., Reference McCarthy-Jones, Trauer, Mackinnon, Sims, Thomas and Copolov2012; Nayani and David, Reference Nayani and David1996), this would be a plausible explanation. However, it appears unlikely that this was the case in this sample of voice-hearing individuals from the general population, as there is some suggestion that commands are less common in non-clinical voice-hearers (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Varese, Jackson and Linden2012).

This study had a number of limitations. First, it relied upon self-reported child abuse history, and such retrospective self-reports are likely to underestimate child abuse (Hardt and Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). Second, despite the parallels of a number of this study's findings to the recent results of Hardy et al. (Reference Hardy, Emsley, Freeman, Bebbington, Garety and Kuipers2016), it is unclear whether the finding relating to compulsions is likely to be generalizable to a clinical population. There is some evidence that compulsions are associated with AVH in people diagnosed with schizophrenia (Guillem et al., Reference Guillem, Satterthwaite, Pampoulova and Stip2009). This is also a population with high rates of CSA, with Read et al. (Reference Read, Os, Morrison and Ross2005) finding that 48% of women with a psychotic disorder, and 28% of men, reported CSA. However, the potential for compulsions to act as a mediator between CSA and AVH in clinical populations remains untested. Third, this study made the assumption that state measures (previous week) of anxiety, depression, obsessions and compulsions were a reflection of trait levels. This may not have been the case. Again this points to the need for prospective study.

Another limitation is that this study did not test for other key potential mediators. As the indirect effects of CSA on AVH through PTSD symptomatology and compulsions were small and could only represent partial mediation, this raises the question as to what other factors may act as mediators. The answer to this could be informed by a three-stage approach that asks: (1) what emotions did the CSA lead to?, (2) how did the individual attempt to regulate these emotions?, and (3) what is their impact on intrusions into consciousness? It has been argued that some post-trauma emotions are likely to be more hallucinogenic than others. In particular, I have argued that shame is likely to be hallucinogenic (McCarthy-Jones, Reference McCarthy-Jones2017). This is probably in large part due to the emotion regulation strategies that tend to be employed to deal with shame, such as dissociation (McCarthy-Jones, Reference McCarthy-Jones2017). Indeed, dissociation is one of the best evidenced mediators of the relationship between childhood trauma and hallucinations (Perona-Garcelán et al., Reference Perona-Garcelán, Carrascoso-López, García-Montes, Ductor-Recuerda, López Jiménez and Vallina-Fernández2012; Pearce et al., Reference Pearce, Simpson, Berry, Bucci, Moskowitz and Varese2017; Varese et al., Reference Varese, Barkus and Bentall2012). However, continuing with the example of shame, a number of other mediators are possible, such as suppression, rumination and hypervigilance (McCarthy-Jones, Reference McCarthy-Jones2017). There is hence the need for a large, prospective study that is able to test a complex model of the relation between CSA and AVH. This would assess key post-trauma emotions (e.g. shame, guilt, anger; Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Brewin, Rose and Kirk2000), a wide range of coping mechanisms employed to deal with these emotions (e.g. dissociation, suppression, hypervigilance), and the nature of intrusions (e.g. obsessive thoughts, trauma memories, etc.).

Any clinical implications of this study are necessarily tentative. However, as these findings add to the body of evidence that post-traumatic symptomatology may be a mediator of the CSA–AVH relation (Hardy et al., Reference Hardy, Emsley, Freeman, Bebbington, Garety and Kuipers2016), it reinforces a number of points recently made by Hardy (Reference Hardy2017). These are that the presence of trauma and post-traumatic symptomatology should be assessed in psychosis, and that there is a clinical need for trauma-based therapy for psychosis. Whilst there are multiple reasons why people hear voices, and we need multiple models to address this (Jones, Reference Jones2010), it is clear that trauma is a significant factor in many people's voice-hearing and will often be encountered and need to be addressed in practice.

In summary, this study found evidence consistent with the hypothesis that PTSD symptomatology and compulsions mediate the relation between CSA and AVH, but not depression, anxiety or obsessive thoughts. Further work is now needed to devise and test a comprehensive model of the potential mediating mechanisms between these experiences.

Acknowledgments

The author is grateful to Dr David Smailes for his comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Financial support: None.

Conflicts of interest: The author has no conflicts of interest with respect to this publication.

Ethics statement: All procedures included in this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and its most recent revision. This study received approval from the Trinity College Dublin School of Medicine Research Ethics Committee (reference 20170402).

References

American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Andrews, B., Brewin, C. R., Rose, S. and Kirk, M. (2000). Predicting PTSD symptoms in victims of violent crime: the role of shame, anger, and childhood abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 6973.Google Scholar
Attademo, L., Bernardini, F., Paolini, E. and Quartesan, R. (2015). History and conceptual problems of the relationship between obsessions and hallucinations. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 23, 1927. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000027Google Scholar
Badcock, J. C., Waters, F. A. and Maybery, M. (2007). On keeping (intrusive) thoughts to one's self: testing a cognitive model of auditory hallucinations. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12, 7889. doi: 10.1080/13546800600753120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badcock, J. C., Waters, F. A., Maybery, M. T. and Michie, P. T. (2005). Auditory hallucinations: failure to inhibit irrelevant memories. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 125136.Google ScholarPubMed
Bebbington, P., Jonas, S., Kuipers, E., King, M., Cooper, C., Brugha, T. et al. (2011). Childhood sexual abuse and psychosis: data from a cross-sectional national psychiatric survey in England. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 2937. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.083642Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. and Nayani, T. (1995). The Psychosis Screening Questionnaire. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 1119.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. (1967) Depression: Clinical, Experimenta, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper and RowGoogle Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Wickham, S., Shevlin, M. and Varese, F. (2012). Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 734740. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, R. P., de Sousa, P., Varese, F., Wickham, S., Sitko, K., Haarmans, M. and Read, J. (2014). From adversity to psychosis: pathways and mechanisms from specific adversities to specific symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 10111022. doi: 10.1007/s00127-014-0914-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohne, A., Keuthen, N. J., Tuschen-Caffier, B. and Wilhelm, S. (2005). Cognitive inhibition in trichotillomania and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 923942. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.014Google Scholar
Bohne, A., Savage, C. R., Deckersbach, T., Keuthen, N. J. and Wilhelm, S. (2008). Motor inhibition in trichotillomania and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 141150. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.11.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R., Rose, S., Andrews, B., Green, J., Tata, P., McEvedy, C. et al. (2002). Brief screening instrument for post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 158162. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.2.158Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Vishne, T., Sasson, Y., Gross, R., Livne, A. and Zohar, J. (2008). Relationship between childhood sexual abuse and obsessive-compulsive disorder: case control study. The Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 45, 177.Google Scholar
Chen, L. P., Murad, M. H., Paras, M. L., Colbenson, K. M., Sattler, A. L., Goranson, E. N. et al. (2010). Sexual abuse and lifetime diagnosis of psychiatric disorders: systematic review and meta-analysis. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 85, 618629. doi: 10.4065/mcp.2009.0583Google Scholar
Clark, D. A. (1992). Depressive, anxious and intrusive thoughts in psychiatric inpatients and outpatients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 93102.Google Scholar
Clark, D. A. and Beck, A. T. (1989). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression. In Kendall, P. C. and Watson, D. (eds), Anxiety and Depression: Distinctive and Overlapping Features. Academic Press: San Diego.Google Scholar
Cutajar, M. C., Mullen, P. E., Ogloff, J. R. P., Thomas, S. D., Wells, D. L. and Spataro, J. (2010). Psychopathology in a large cohort of sexually abused children followed up to 43 years. Child Abuse and Neglect, 34, 813822. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.04.004Google Scholar
Daalman, K., Diederen, K. M., Derks, E. M., van Lutterveld, R., Kahn, R. S. and Sommer, I. E. (2012). Childhood trauma and auditory verbal hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 42, 24752484. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000761Google Scholar
Deblinger, E., McLeer, S. V., Atkins, M. S., Ralphe, D. and Foa, E. (1989). Post-traumatic stress in sexually abused, physically abused, and nonabused children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 403408.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., McLeod, G. F. and Horwood, L. J. (2013). Childhood sexual abuse and adult developmental outcomes: findings from a 30-year longitudinal study in New Zealand. Child Abuse and Neglect, 37, 664674. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.013.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V. and Rothbaum, B. O. (1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 459473. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490060405Google Scholar
Guillem, F., Satterthwaite, J., Pampoulova, T. and Stip, E. (2009). Relationship between psychotic and obsessive compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 115, 358362. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.004Google Scholar
Hardt, J. and Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 260273.Google Scholar
Hardy, A. (2017). Pathways from trauma to psychotic experiences: a theoretically informed model of post-traumatic stress in psychosis. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 697. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00697Google Scholar
Hardy, A., Emsley, R., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P., Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E. E. et al. (2016). Psychological mechanisms mediating effects between trauma and psychotic symptoms: the role of affect regulation, intrusive trauma memory, beliefs, and depression. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42 (suppl 1), S34–43. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv175Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hill, K., Varese, F., Jackson, M. and Linden, D. E. (2012). The relationship between metacognitive beliefs, auditory hallucinations, and hallucination-related distress in clinical and non-clinical voice-hearers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 434447. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.2012.02039.xGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. R. (2010). Do we need multiple models of auditory verbal hallucinations? Examining the phenomenological fit of cognitive and neurological models. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 566575. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn129.Google Scholar
Jones, S. R. and Fernyhough, C. (2007). Thought as action: inner speech, self-monitoring, and auditory verbal hallucinations. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 391399. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.12.003Google Scholar
Jones, S. R. and Fernyhough, C. (2006). The roles of thought suppression and metacognitive beliefs in proneness to auditory verbal hallucinations in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 14211432. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.003Google Scholar
Kelleher, I., Keeley, H., Corcoran, P., Ramsay, H., Wasserman, C., Carli, V. et al. (2013). Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect and directionality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 734741. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091169Google Scholar
Kendall-Tackett, K., Williams, L. and Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual abuse on children: a review and synthesis of recent empirical studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164180.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Bulik, C. M., Silberg, J., Hettema, J. M., Myers, J. and Prescott, C. A. (2000). Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use disorders in women: an epidemiological and cotwin control analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 953959.Google Scholar
Khan, O. H., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S. and Duffau, H. (2014). The role of left inferior fronto-occipital fascicle in verbal perseveration: a brain electrostimulation mapping study. Brain Topography, 27, 403411. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0343-5Google Scholar
Kjelby, E., Sinkeviciute, I., Gjestad, R., Kroken, R. A., Løberg, E. M., Jørgensen, H. A. et al. (2015). Suicidality in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: the relationship to hallucinations and persecutory delusions. European Psychiatry, 30, 830836. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuyken, W. and Brewin, C. R. (1994). Intrusive memories of childhood abuse during depressive episodes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 525528.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., Pelosi, A.J., Araya, R. and Dunn, G. (1992). Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardised assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychological Medicine, 222, 465486.Google Scholar
Maikovich, A. K., Koenen, K. C. and Jaffee, S. R. (2009). Post-traumatic stress symptoms and trajectories in child sexual abuse victims: an analysis of sex differences using the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 727737. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9300-xGoogle Scholar
Marwaha, S. and Bebbington, P. (2015). Mood as a mediator of the link between child sexual abuse and psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50, 661663. doi: 10.1007/s00127-014-0966-1Google Scholar
McCarthy-Jones, S. (2017). Can't you Hear Them? The Science and Significance of Hearing Voices. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
McCarthy-Jones, S. (2012). Hearing Voices: The Histories, Causes and Meanings of Auditory Verbal Hallucinations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCarthy-Jones, S. and Longden, E. (2015). Auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia and post-traumatic stress disorder: common phenomenology, common cause, common interventions? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1071. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy-Jones, S., Trauer, T., Mackinnon, A., Sims, E., Thomas, N. and Copolov, D. L. (2012). A new phenomenological survey of auditory hallucinations: evidence for subtypes and implications for theory and practice. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40, 231235.Google Scholar
McLeer, S. V., Deblinger, E., Henry, D. and Orvaschel, H. (1992). Sexually abused children at high risk for PTSD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 875879.Google Scholar
McManus, S., Meltzer, H., Brugha, T. and Bebbington, P. (2009). Adult Psychiatric Morbidity in England, 2007: Results of a Household Survey. London: The NHS Information Centre.Google Scholar
Morrison, A. P. and Baker, C. A. (2000). Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: a comparative study of intrusions in psychosis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 10971106.Google Scholar
Nayani, T. H. and David, A. S. (1996). The auditory hallucination: a phenomenological survey. Psychological Medicine, 26, 177189.Google Scholar
Nelson, H. (1991). National Adult Reading Test (NART), 2nd edition. London: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Oestreich, L. K. L., McCarthy-Jones, S., Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank and Whitford, T. J. (2015). Decreased integrity of the fronto-temporal fibres of the left inferior occipito-frontal fasciculus associated with auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10, 445454. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9421-5Google Scholar
Paulik, G., Badcock, J. C. and Maybery, M. T. (2007). Poor intentional inhibition in individuals predisposed to hallucinations. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12, 457470. doi: 10.1080/13546800701394329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearce, J., Simpson, J., Berry, K., Bucci, S., Moskowitz, A. and Varese, F. (2017). Attachment and dissociation as mediators of the link between childhood trauma and psychotic experiences. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy (advanced online publication). doi: 10.1002/cpp.2100Google Scholar
Perona-Garcelán, S., Carrascoso-López, F., García-Montes, J. M., Ductor-Recuerda, M. J., López Jiménez, A. M., Vallina-Fernández, O. et al. (2012). Dissociative experiences as mediators between childhood trauma and auditory hallucinations. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25, 323329. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2016.1155193Google Scholar
Poyurovsky, M. (2013). Schizo-obsessive Disorder. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Priebe, K., Kleindienst, N., Zimmer, J., Koudela, S., Ebner-Priemer, U. and Bohus, M. (2013). Frequency of intrusions and flashbacks in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood sexual abuse: an electronic diary study. Psychological Assessment, 25, 13701376. doi: 10.1037/a0033816Google Scholar
Read, J., Agar, K., Argyle, N. and Aderhold, V. (2003). Sexual and physical abuse during childhood and adulthood as predictors of hallucinations, delusions and thought disorder. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 76, 122. doi: 10.1348/14760830260569210Google Scholar
Read, J., Os, J. V., Morrison, A. P. and Ross, C. A. (2005). Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: a literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112, 330350. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00634.xGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571583.Google Scholar
Shevlin, M., Dorahy, M. and Adamson, G. (2007). Childhood traumas and hallucinations: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 222228. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.03.004Google Scholar
Sideli, L., Mulè, A., La Barbera, D. and Murray, R. M. (2012). Do child abuse and maltreatment increase risk of schizophrenia? Psychiatry Investigation, 9, 8799. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.2.87Google Scholar
Sitko, K., Bentall, R. P., Shevlin, M., and Sellwood, W. (2014). Associations between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities are mediated by attachment styles: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychiatry Research, 217, 202209. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.019Google Scholar
Stephane, M., Polis, I. and Barton, S. N. (2000). A subtype of auditory verbal hallucinations responds to fluvoxamine. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 13, 425427. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.3.425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Susser, E. and Widom, C. S. (2012). Still searching for lost truths about the bitter sorrows of childhood. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 672675. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs074Google Scholar
Üçok, A. and Bıkmaz, S. (2007). The effects of childhood trauma in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116, 371377. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01079.xGoogle Scholar
van Dael, F., Van Os, J., De Graaf, R., Ten Have, M., Krabbendam, L. and Myin-Germeys, I. (2011). Can obsessions drive you mad? Longitudinal evidence that obsessive-compulsive symptoms worsen the outcome of early psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123, 136146. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01609.xGoogle Scholar
van Velzen, L. S., Vriend, C., de Wit, S. J. and van den Heuvel, O. A. (2014). Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419Google Scholar
Varese, F., Barkus, E. and Bentall, R. P. (2012). Dissociation mediates the relationship between childhood trauma and hallucination-proneness. Psychological Medicine, 42, 10251036. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001826Google Scholar
Waters, F. A., Badcock, J. C., Maybery, M. T. and Michie, P. T. (2003). Inhibition in schizophrenia: association with auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, 62, 275280.Google Scholar
Widom, C. S. (1999). Post-traumatic stress disorder in abused and neglected children grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 12231229.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Predictors of presence of auditory verbal hallucinations

Figure 2

Table 3. Direct and indirect effects of child sexual abuse on auditory verbal hallucinations

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.