Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T18:00:36.215Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Advancing Employee Resilience Research: Additional Thoughts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2016

Natalie E. Wolfson*
Affiliation:
TRACOM Group, Centennial, Colorado
Casey Mulqueen
Affiliation:
TRACOM Group, Centennial, Colorado
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Natalie E. Wolfson, TRACOM Group, 6675 South Kenton Street, Suite 118, Centennial, CO 80111. E-mail: nwolfson@tracom.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Britt, Shen, Sinclair, Grossman, and Klieger (2016) draw attention to issues in the psychological literature regarding how we define, assess, select for, and build employee resilience. We offer a handful of recommendations for complementing and expanding on these important issues. Specifically, we propose that research should include more common forms of workplace adversity, versus extreme and rare types of adversity; resilience should be assessed via objective multirater methodology rather than subjective self-report; because context is important when studying resilience, researchers should delineate the purposes of the research; resilience should be treated as a malleable rather than a fixed characteristic; and finally, the field would benefit from qualitative research in addition to quantitative research.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology 2016 

Britt, Shen, Sinclair, Grossman, and Klieger (Reference Britt, Shen, Sinclair, Grossman and Klieger2016) draw attention to issues in the psychological literature regarding how we define, assess, select for, and build employee resilience. We offer a handful of recommendations for complementing and expanding on these important issues. Specifically, we propose that research should include more common forms of workplace adversity, versus extreme and rare types of adversity; resilience should be assessed via objective multirater methodology rather than subjective self-report; because context is important when studying resilience, researchers should delineate the purposes of the research; resilience should be treated as a malleable rather than a fixed characteristic; and finally, the field would benefit from qualitative research in addition to quantitative research.

Throughout their article, the authors insist that to advance resilience research, researchers need to clarify what is meant by “significant adversity” and document its occurrence in the workplace. Much in line with the authors, we define “significant adversity” as any challenge that is present at a high intensity or duration. Certainly, traumatic events such as abusive supervision and disasters count, but more common workplace stressors also represent significant adversity. In our work with organizations, we have witnessed many examples of workers who face significant adversity as defined by Britt et al. but who are not in the military, in the police, or involved in disaster relief: for example, workers faced with chronic heavy workloads and deadlines that contribute to significant mental and physical health problems and medical staff members who face multiple demands for their attention and time, at times affecting their performance to a degree that could diminish patient safety. Recognizing these more common forms of adversity opens up more avenues for fruitful research on resilience.

The authors spend a considerable amount of time reviewing methods used to assess resilience, with an emphasis on distinguishing capacity for resilience from demonstration of resilience. Specifically, they describe various personality-based assessments of resilience as well as research on resilient trajectories following adversity. The rest of our recommendations focus on these aspects of their review.

The authors note that the capacity for resilience has primarily been assessed via personality-based models. These models assume the following: The capacity for resilience is an individual-level characteristic, resilience is appropriately assessed through self-reports, resilience varies along a single continuum from high resilience to vulnerability, and individuals demonstrate a relatively consistent level of resilience across multiple contexts. The models differ in their assumptions about the extent to which one's current level of resilience is malleable.

The authors critically evaluate this personality literature but fail to make some important points. First, the fact that most resilience assessments are based on self-perception is highly problematic. Research across many domains of psychology converges on the notion that people tend to be flawed in their self-assessment (Dunning, Heath, & Suls, Reference Dunning, Heath and Suls2004). For example, self-ratings of skill hold only moderate to meager relationships with measures of actual performance (Mabe & West, Reference Mabe and West1982; Harris & Schaubroeck, Reference Harris and Schaubroeck1988). Additionally, people are often out of touch with their own emotions and stress levels (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2006; Dutton & Aaron, Reference Dutton and Aaron1974). Moreover, people overestimate themselves—they see themselves as more skilled than they actually are. These erroneous self-perceptions arise for a variety of reasons (e.g., people seek to see themselves in a flattering light, and they ignore relevant information such as past experiences and background circumstances; Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Heath and Suls2004). Research shows that acquaintances see others at a more abstract level and often assess others’ performance and abilities more accurately than they themselves can (Bass & Yammarino, Reference Bass and Yammarino1991; Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Heath and Suls2004; Eyal & Epley, Reference Eyal and Epley2010; Risucci, Torolani, & Ward, Reference Risucci, Torolani and Ward1989). It is imperative that we incorporate a multirater perspective into our studies of employee resilience; otherwise, we risk introducing systematic errors into our conclusions about resiliency.

Britt et al. point out that researchers regard resilience as a uniform personality trait that remains stable across circumstances. Certainly, this is worth addressing in future research. There is evidence to suggest that people are more tolerant of certain forms of stress compared with others. For example, Idris (Reference Idris2011) found that academics had a higher level of tolerance for role conflict compared with role ambiguity or role overload. Among a sample of nurses, role conflict more strongly predicted occupational stress compared with role overload or role ambiguity (Karimi, Omar, Alipour, & Karimi, Reference Karimi, Omar, Alipour and Karimi2014). Although the authors argue that the stress literature contributes “virtually nothing” to our understanding of resilience, we would argue that these are not distinct concepts and that how people respond to low-level stressors is predictive of their responses to high-level stressors. It is worthwhile, therefore, to make finer distinctions within the resilience literature regarding “resilience for what?” Resilience for extreme work overload? Resilience for abusive leadership? Resilience for disaster? By delineating these differences in the research, we will gain a more fine-grained understanding of individuals’ capacity for resilience and demonstration of resilience over time.

Next, resilience models vary in the extent to which they treat resilience as fixed. We argue that resilience researchers should move away from conceptualizing resilience as a personality trait to studying it as a malleable quality. Britt et al. cite research showing resilience training programs are effective and produce noticeable changes in people, even if these programs are evaluated in the same way as stress-management interventions. Moreover, organizational factors like learning culture and empowering leadership have been linked to increased employee resilience (Hodliff, Reference Hodliff2014). What is more, interventions like meditation and cognitive behavioral therapy have been shown to induce neurological changes associated with greater resilience (Davidson & Begley, Reference Davidson and Begley2012). Although studying individuals using functional magnetic resonance imaging and other advanced technologies is inherently challenging, working with neuroscientists on this line of research could be vastly informative. Studying resilience as a developable and malleable characteristic is more productive, as it encourages both organizational leaders and employees to take ownership for their individual and organizations’ resilience, rather than “blame the victim,” as Britt et al. suggest may occur in some circumstances.

Next, the authors review research on the demonstration of resilience. The authors insist that time is an integral component of the study of resilience and recommend that researchers primarily use longitudinal designs to assess individuals’ adaptation. Specifically, they advocate that researchers first assess employees on relevant individual differences and contextual factors, then document significant adversity, and finally, assess workers on multiple measures of positive adaptation over the course of time. Certainly, this approach is worth pursuing. However, as noted previously, cases of extreme adversity occur abruptly and infrequently, making longitudinal research very difficult to implement.

As a complement to, or in lieu of, this type of longitudinal research, researchers should consider incorporating more qualitative methods. Qualitative methods allow researchers to engage more deeply with participants and undertake a detailed process of inquiry that can uncover patterns and themes in humans’ responses to adverse events. Through interview, field notes, observations, and so on, researchers gain a sense of the totality of the human adaptation process, including its link to the social context.

Ungar (Reference Ungar2003) argues that qualitative research compensates for weaknesses in quantitative resilience research. For example, the selection of outcome variables is often arbitrary. If quantitative researchers do not test for particular outcome variables, they may erroneously conclude that someone was vulnerable and not resilient. This is an issue that was also acknowledged by Britt et al. Qualitative research, however, takes a bottom-up approach, allowing the relevant dimensions to emerge during data collection. In addition, quantitative resilience research studies behavior under controlled conditions, failing to contextualize the adaptation process. Qualitative research, on the other hand, acknowledges the context that informs the importance of various factors within an individual case. Overall, qualitative methods provide researchers intimate access to the processes by which individuals make sense of and respond to their own reality and may give rise to novel research questions and theory. Considering the personal and sensitive nature of the topic, this methodology is worthwhile.

Britt et al. provide a comprehensive review of the state of resilience research, shedding light on a troubling amount of discrepancy and uncertainty regarding our conceptualization of resilience. They offer valid critiques and recommendations for further research. The purpose of this article was to build on Britt et al.’s argument and suggest ways we can better assess and study resilience in the workplace.

References

Bass, B. M., & Yammarino, F. J. (1991). Congruence of self and others’ leadership ratings of Naval officers for understanding successful performance. Applied Psychology, 40, 437454.Google Scholar
Britt, T. W., Shen, W., Sinclair, R. R., Grossman, M. R., & Klieger, D. M. (2016). How much do we really know about employee resilience? Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 9 (2), 378404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J., & Begley, S. (2012). The emotional life of your brain: How its unique patterns affect the way you think, feel, and live–and how you can change them. New York, NY: Penguin Group.Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 69106.Google Scholar
Dutton, D. G, & Aaron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 510517.Google Scholar
Eyal, T., & Epley, N. (2010). How to seem telepathic: Enabling mind reading by matching construal. Psychological Science, 21, 700705.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Harris, M. M., & Schaubroeck, J. (1988). A meta-analysis of self-supervisor, self-peer, and peer-supervisor ratings. Personnel Psychology, 41, 4362.Google Scholar
Hodliff, M. (2014). The development and validation of the employee resilience scale (EmpRes): The conceptualization of a new model (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.Google Scholar
Idris, M. K. (2011). Over time effects of role stress on psychological strain among Malaysian public university academics. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 2, 154161.Google Scholar
Karimi, R., Omar, Z. B., Alipour, F., & Karimi, Z. (2014). The influence of role overload, role conflict, and role ambiguity on occupational stress among nurses in selected Iranian hospitals. International Journal of Asian Social Science, 4 (1), 3440.Google Scholar
MabeP. A., III P. A., III, & West, S. G. (1982).Validity of self-evaluation of ability: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 280286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Risucci, D. A., Torolani, A. J., & Ward, R. J. (1989). Ratings of surgical residents by self, supervisors and peers. Surgical Gynecology and Obstetrics, 169, 519526.Google Scholar
Ungar, M. (2003). Qualitative contributions to resilience research. Qualitative Social Work, 2, 85102.Google Scholar