Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:36:53.796Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

New insights into the correlation structure of DSM-IV depression symptoms in the general population v. subsamples of depressed individuals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2017

S. Foster*
Affiliation:
Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
M. Mohler-Kuo
Affiliation:
Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Address for correspondence: S. Foster, Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, University of Zurich, Hirschengraben 84, 8001 Zürich, Switzerland. (Email: simon.foster@uzh.ch)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims.

Previous research failed to uncover a replicable dimensional structure underlying the symptoms of depression. We aimed to examine two neglected methodological issues in this research: (a) adjusting symptom correlations for overall depression severity; and (b) analysing general population samples v. subsamples of currently depressed individuals.

Methods.

Using population-based cross-sectional and longitudinal data from two nations (Switzerland, 5883 young men; USA, 2174 young men and 2244 young women) we assessed the dimensions of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms in young adults. In each general-population sample and each subsample of currently depressed participants, we conducted a standardised process of three analytical steps, based on exploratory and confirmatory factor and bifactor analysis, to reveal any replicable dimensional structure underlying symptom correlations while controlling for overall depression severity.

Results.

We found no evidence of a replicable dimensional structure across samples when adjusting symptom correlations for overall depression severity. In the general-population samples, symptoms correlated strongly and a single dimension of depression severity was revealed. Among depressed participants, symptom correlations were surprisingly weak and no replicable dimensions were identified, regardless of severity-adjustment.

Conclusions.

First, caution is warranted when considering studies assessing dimensions of depression because general population-based studies and studies of depressed individuals generate different data that can lead to different conclusions. This problem likely generalises to other models based on the symptoms’ inter-relationships such as network models. Second, whereas the overall severity aligns individuals on a continuum of disorder intensity that allows non-affected individuals to be distinguished from affected individuals, the clinical evaluation and treatment of depressed individuals should focus directly on each individual's symptom profile.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Major depression, which is characterised by the core symptoms of depressed mood and anhedonia (World Health Organization, 2012; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), is a leading contributor to the global burden of disease (Murray & Lopez, Reference Murray and Lopez1996; Bromet et al. Reference Bromet, Andrade, Hwang, Sampson, Alonso, de Girolamo, de Graaf, Demyttenaere, Hu, Iwata, Karam, Kaur, Kostyuchenko, Lepine, Levinson, Matschinger, Mora, Browne, Posada-Villa, Viana, Williams and Kessler2011; Ferrari et al. Reference Ferrari, Charlson, Norman, Patten, Freedman, Murray, Vos and Whiteford2013; Kessler & Bromet, Reference Kessler and Bromet2013; Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Vogelzangs, Twisk, Kleiboer, Li and Penninx2014). For years, debate has raged over the clinical presentation of depression (Baumeister & Parker, Reference Baumeister and Parker2012; van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012). Specifically, studies have been conducted seeking to identify symptom-based dimensions and subtypes via statistical analysis of symptom co-occurrence using factor analysis, principal component analysis and latent class analysis (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Eaton, Gallo and Nestadt2000; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Neale and Kendler2005; Shafer, Reference Shafer2006; Carragher et al. Reference Carragher, Adamson, Bunting and McCann2009; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Kendler, Kubarych and Neale2011; Cole et al. Reference Cole, Cai, Martin, Findling, Youngstrom, Garber, Curry, Hyde, Essex, Compas, Goodyer, Rohde, Stark, Slattery and Forehand2011; Mezuk & Kendler, Reference Mezuk and Kendler2012; Hybels et al. Reference Hybels, Landerman and Blazer2013; Buhler et al. Reference Buhler, Seemuller and Lage2014; Li et al. Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendler2014a, Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendlerb; Rodgers et al. Reference Rodgers, Grosse Holtforth, Muller, Hengartner, Rossler and Ajdacic-Gross2014; Fried et al. Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016). Tightly correlated symptom sets are important as they might constitute dimensions or subtypes that imply different aetiologies and/or treatment responses. However, as summarised in a recent systematic review, these studies have failed to generate replicable results (van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012).

Two methodological issues have not yet been considered, however. First, previous studies failed to disentangle two distinct sources of correlation between any two symptoms. Such correlations may be: (1) due to differences in overall severity (i.e., individuals with more severe depression score higher for all symptoms than individuals with less severe depression; hence, symptoms A and B are correlated); or (2) due to a specific profile of symptom correlations (e.g., individuals scoring high for symptom A could typically score high for symptom B too, but not necessarily for symptom C, which is more closely linked to symptom D). Therefore, it may be more appropriate to study symptom correlations adjusted for overall depression severity. If a structure underlying the symptoms exists, it should be revealed more clearly in this way.

Second, most studies were conducted on samples of depressed individuals. Studies that examined the dimensions of depression in general population samples, however, consistently revealed one single dimension of depression severity (Muthén, Reference Muthén1989; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Neale and Kendler2005; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Kendler, Kubarych and Neale2011; Cole et al. Reference Cole, Cai, Martin, Findling, Youngstrom, Garber, Curry, Hyde, Essex, Compas, Goodyer, Rohde, Stark, Slattery and Forehand2011; Mezuk & Kendler, Reference Mezuk and Kendler2012; Familiar et al. Reference Familiar, Ortiz-Panozo, Hall, Vieitez, Romieu, Lopez-Ridaura and Lajous2015). Thus, in the general population, depression was found to be a uni-dimensional construct. Apparently, it makes a difference whether one studies the general population or depressed individuals only, but this issue remained unaddressed.

The present study's main objective was to examine the dimensional structure of the nine symptoms of depression listed in the DSM-IV (a) when adjusting symptom correlations for overall depression severity; and (b) in general-population samples v. subsamples of currently depressed individuals. We adopted a dimensional approach, since evidence increasingly suggests that many psychiatric syndromes, including depression, are continuous and hence dimensional rather than categorical (Slade & Andrews, Reference Slade and Andrews2005; Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Krueger, Andrews and Hobbs2009; Prisciandaro & Roberts, Reference Prisciandaro and Roberts2009; Markon et al. Reference Markon, Chmielewski and Miller2011; Haslam et al. Reference Haslam, Holland and Kuppens2012; Eaton et al. Reference Eaton, Krueger, Markon, Keyes, Skodol, Wall, Hasin and Grant2013).

Materials and methods

Study design

We used: (a) longitudinal data from the Cohort Study on Substance Use Risk Factors (C-SURF); and (b) cross-sectional data from the U.S. National Health and Nutrition Survey (NHANES). In total, we considered four samples: C-SURF baseline, C-SURF follow-up, NHANES men and NHANES women. Comparing C-SURF baseline and follow-up data permitted us to examine whether results were replicable across two time points in the same sample. Comparing the C-SURF and NHANES data allowed us to examine whether results were replicable in two different populations.

Each of the four samples was analysed twice: once in the full (general-population) sample, and once only in the subsample of participants within a current mild-to-severe depressive episode, generating eight analytical samples in total.

Participants

C-SURF is a large cohort study examining young men in Switzerland, for which details on sampling and non-response bias have been published elsewhere (Studer et al. Reference Studer, Baggio, Mohler-Kuo, Dermota, Gaume, Bertholet, Daeppen and Gmel2013a, Reference Studer, Mohler-Kuo, Dermota, Gaume, Bertholet, Eidenbenz, Daeppen and Gmelb). It was designed to be representative of young non-institutionalised Swiss men. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee for Clinical Research at Lausanne University Medical School (protocol number 15/07), and all subjects consented to participate.

5990 men completed the baseline survey between September 2010 and March 2012. Of these, 107 (1.8%) were excluded for missing data on the depression items. Of these, 5155 (87.6%) answered all necessary items of the follow-up survey performed between January 2012 and April 2013. The mean time elapsed between baseline and follow-up was 1.3 years (standard deviation 0.2).

NHANES is a continuous cross-sectional survey released in 2-year cycles (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS)). It was designed to be representative of the non-institutionalised U.S. civilian population. NHANES study protocols were approved by the National Center for Health Statistics Research Ethics Review Board, and all participants consented.

We included NHANES data from NHANES cycles 2005–2012 for men and women from 18 to 28 years old, an age range chosen to resemble the C-SURF cohort. Men and women were analysed separately. Of the total 2371 men and 2542 women, 197 (8.3%) and 298 (11.7%) were excluded for missing depression items data.

Measures

C-SURF: Self-reported depressive symptoms were assessed via the Major Depressive Inventory – WHO-MDI (Bech et al. Reference Bech, Rasmussen, Olsen, Noerholm and Abildgaard2001; Olsen et al. Reference Olsen, Jensen, Noerholm, Martiny and Bech2003). This validated measure covers DSM-IV and ICD-10 depression symptoms over the past 14 days, using 12 items with six-point answer scales ranging from ‘never’ (0) to ‘all the time’ (5). Items were aggregated into the nine DSM-IV symptoms, as proposed previously (Bech et al. Reference Bech, Rasmussen, Olsen, Noerholm and Abildgaard2001) (Table 1). Subjects were classified as having ‘no’, ‘mild’, ‘moderate’ or ‘severe’ depression based on the MDI summation score (Olsen et al. Reference Olsen, Jensen, Noerholm, Martiny and Bech2003). For correlation and factor analyses, symptoms were dichotomised into present/absent, as per ICD-10 definitions (Bech et al. Reference Bech, Rasmussen, Olsen, Noerholm and Abildgaard2001) (Table 1).

Table 1. Symptoms of depression in the ICD-10-based WHO-MDI and the DSM-IV-based PHQ-9

ICD-10, International Classification of Diseases 10th version; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition; WHO-MDI, World Health Organization Major Depression Inventory; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire.

a Translation rule to combine ICD-10 symptoms into DSM-IV symptoms. The rule is to take the highest value of the relevant ICD-10 symptoms to represent the corresponding DSM-IV symptom.

b Threshold for scoring the symptom as ‘present’.

NHANES 2005–2012: Self-reported depressive symptoms were assessed via the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) (Kroenke et al. Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001). This validated measure covers the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms over the past 14 days (Table 1). Four answer options are provided, ranging from ‘not at all’ (0) to ‘nearly every day’ (3). Participants were classified as having ‘no’, ‘mild’, ‘moderate’ or ‘severe’ depression based on the PHQ9 summation score (Kroenke et al. Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001). Note that the threshold score we used to denote ‘mild’ depression was termed ‘moderate’ depression by Kroenke et al. This threshold resembled most closely the threshold for ‘mild’ depression that we used in the C-SURF sample, in terms of the percentage summation score required for the diagnosis (40% in C-SURF, 37% in NHANES). For correlation and factor analyses, symptoms were dichotomised into present/absent, as per DSM-IV definitions (Kroenke et al. Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001) (Table 1).

Statistical analysis

First, we examined tetrachoric correlations of the depression symptoms. To compare the correlations of each general-population sample with those of the corresponding subsample of currently depressed subjects, we calculated the ratio of the squared correlations for each symptom pair and used Steiger's test to formally examine the hypothesis that the two correlation matrices differed (Steiger, Reference Steiger1980). Steiger's test sums the squared differences of the Fisher transformed correlations of the two matrices and tests this sum against the chi-square distribution. Second, we assessed the dimensionality of the depression symptoms in three steps, each step conducted separately for each of the eight samples to determine whether the results were replicable.

Step 1: We first performed one-factor confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory factor analysis (EFA). CFA consisted of the nine symptoms as indicators of an underlying depression factor, thereby modelling overall depression severity (model 1). With EFA, we tested one- to seven-factor models to determine which best fit the data. Both CFA and EFA were estimated using mean and variance-adjusted weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimations, which is the standard for categorical indicators (Barendse et al. Reference Barendse, Oort and Timmerman2014; Li et al. Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendler2014a). Model fit was evaluated via standard criteria for good model fit (Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Neale and Kendler2005; Li et al. Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendler2014a): root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ≤0.05; comparative fit index (CFI) ≥0.95; and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) ≥0.95. For EFA, the model with the lowest number of factors achieving these criteria was adopted (model 2).

Step 2: From model 1, we derived the modification indices for the residual symptom co-variances as indicators of symptom pairs correlated beyond the general factor (i.e., as indicators of substantial severity-adjusted symptom correlations). Modification indices estimate the degree of improvement in model fit if the corresponding parameter is included in the model (Brown & Moore, Reference Brown, Moore and Hoyle2012). Consequently, the modification index of a residual co-variance indicates whether the model would fit better if this co-variance was included in the CFA model.

We considered a modification index ≥3.84 statistically significant (Brown & Moore, Reference Brown, Moore and Hoyle2012). We then re-fitted the one-factor CFA, this time including the residual symptom co-variances revealed by the modification indices (model 3). The residual symptom correlations derived from this CFA model generated an estimate of symptom correlations corrected for overall depression severity. If there is a dimensional structure beyond overall depression severity, these correlations: (a) should be replicable across the samples and (b) form interpretable symptom clusters. Because adopting CFA models based on modification indices is associated with a high risk of overfitting (MacCallum et al. Reference MacCallum, Roznowski and Necowitz1992), we used the median of each modification index across 5000 case-based bootstrap samples.

Step 3: Finally, we estimated a series of bifactor models that, by definition, consist of one general factor and several group factors. Each indicator variable loads simultaneously on the general factor and one of the group factors (Reise et al. Reference Reise, Moore and Haviland2010). Thus, bifactor models allow for estimating group factors controlled for a general factor (Reise et al. Reference Reise, Moore and Haviland2010) and, hence, correspond directly to our notion of assessing depression dimensions (the group factors) controlled for overall depression severity (the general factor). If there is a replicable dimensional structure underlying the depression symptoms, at least one of the bifactor models should either converge with the residual correlations revealed in step 2, or provide an alternative model that is replicable across samples.

We used two approaches to identify the group factors:

  1. (a) We examined three theoretically-derived groupings (models 4.a1–3):

    1. (1) Three genetic factors revealed by (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Aggen and Neale2013).

    2. (2) The common distinction of a cognitive/affective factor v. a somatic factor, as defined in the systematic review by van Loo et al. (Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012);

    3. (3) The symptoms most consistently found on a single factor in the review (van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012) v. the remaining symptoms;

  2. (b) A non-rotated EFA that comprises several factors can be rotated into a bifactor structure (Jennrich & Bentler, Reference Jennrich and Bentler2011, Reference Jennrich and Bentler2012), resulting in an exploratory bifactor analysis (EBFA) that can then undergo confirmatory analysis. We derived the EBFA from the EFA calculated in step 1 and re-fitted it as a CFA model (model 4.b). However, only one EFA model revealed a sufficient number of factors to be rotated into a bifactor structure. We therefore used this bifactor model across all samples, rather than assessing a separate bifactor solution for each sample.

Bifactor models were estimated using WLSMV estimation and model fit was evaluated as in step 2.

Analyses were performed using R-software version 3.1.2 (R Core Team, 2014), particularly using the packages ‘psych’ (Revelle, Reference Revelle2013), ‘semTools’ (Pornprasertmanit et al. Reference Pornprasertmanit, Miller, Schoemann and Rosseel2013) and ‘lavaan’ (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). R-scripts are available at https://osf.io/a6tuw/.

Results

Participants’ baseline characteristics are summarised in Table 2. Prevalence rates for current depression of at least mild degree ranged from 4.9 to 7.6%.

Table 2. Baseline characteristics of study participants

M, mean; SD, standard deviation.

a Prevalence of current depression of at least mild-moderate degree.

b Average prevalence rate calculated across the NHANES cycles 2005–2012. The prevalence rates within each cycle were calculated using weighted data.

Symptom correlations

Substantial symptom correlations were revealed in the general-population samples (median correlations ranging from r = 0.55–0.74), while correlations in the depressed samples were surprisingly weak (median correlations from r = 0.04–0.24, Table 3). Correlations were greater in the general-population samples, and these differences were pronounced: in average correlations were higher by a factor ranging between 8.4 and 30.9 across samples (Table 4). Steiger tests confirmed that all general-population sample correlation matrices differed significantly from their counterparts in the depressed samples (Table 4). Only one correlation among women (‘life not worth living’ and ‘appetite changes’) was slightly higher in the depressed sample (ratio of squared correlation = 0.8, Table 3).

Table 3. Summary of tetrachoric correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms across general-population samples and subsamples of currently depressed subjects

M, median; IQR, inter-quartile range.

Table 4. Comparison of tetrachoric correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms in general-population samples v. subsamples of currently depressed subjects

M, median; IQR, inter-quartile range; df, degrees of freedom.

a Tests the hypothesis that two correlation matrices differ from each other.

b For each symptom pair, its squared correlation in the general-population sample was divided by its squared correlation in the corresponding sample of depressed. Ratios >1.0 indicate that the correlation was higher in the general population than among depressed.

Factor analyses

Step 1 revealed that the one-factor model fit the data very well in all general-population samples. This was revealed by both CFAs and EFAs (Table 5, models 1 and 2). In contrast, in depressed samples, no replicable dimensional structure was identified. Specifically, the one-factor CFAs failed to achieve good model fit in three of four samples and the EFAs revealed different numbers of factors across samples.

Table 5. Summary of exploratory and confirmatory factor and bifactor analyses of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms in general-population samples and subsamples of currently depressed subjects

CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; EFA, exploratory factor analysis; CFI, comparative fit index; TLI, Tucker–Lewis index; RMSEA, root-mean-square error of approximation.

a None of the admissible models reached the criteria for good model fit.

b Model inadmissible due to negative residual variance of at least one symptom.

Step 2 indicated that 50 of 288 (8 samples × 36 symptom pairs) possible residual co-variances (17.4%) were substantial. Including these residual co-variances in the CFAs improved the fit of all models and resulted in good-fitting models, except for the NHANES sample of depressed women (Table 5, model 3). Both positive and negative correlations were revealed, positive correlations ranging from 0.10 to 0.48 (median: r = 0.29) and negative correlations from  − 0.46 to −0.07 (median: r = −0.26, Table 6). However, the correlations failed to exhibit any replicable pattern across the samples, and 17 of the 50 correlations (34.0%) were not statistically significant (Table 6).

Table 6. Correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms adjusted for overall depression severity as estimated by confirmatory factor analysis (model 3)

Note. Correlations printed in bold are statistically significant with p < 0.05.

In Step 3, no bifactor model was replicable across the samples (Table 5, models 4.a1–4.b). The most stable model was model 4.a2, which achieved good model fit in three of four NHANES samples and one C-SURF sample. Note that all models were inadmissible in at least four of the eight samples, due to negative residual variances.

A closer look at the bifactor models revealed two issues (see Supplementary material available at https://osf.io/a6tuw/). First, 18 of 24 models that were inadmissible were inadmissible because at least one of the group factors consisted of one large factor loading, with all other loadings being virtually zero, thereby leading to a model that was empirically underidentified (Kline, Reference Kline2011). Furthermore, this pattern of one very large loading with otherwise negligible loadings is indicative of overfactoring (i.e., the inclusion of unnecessary factors) (Rindskopf, Reference Rindskopf1984). In the remaining six inadmissible models, the majority or all of the loadings were non-significant for at least one group factor. Second, among the admissible models, six had at least one group factor with only non-significant factor loadings, and only two models had significant factor loadings across both the general and group factors. Thus, bifactor analysis provided no evidence for any dimensional structure existing beyond the general severity factor.

Discussion

Main findings

We sought to examine the dimensions underlying the nine DSM-IV depressive symptoms in young adults while adjusting symptom correlations for overall depression severity, and while comparing general-population samples v. subsamples of currently depressed individuals. Analyses revealed three main results. First, adjusting symptom correlations for overall depression severity left little substantial correlation between the symptoms, and we failed to find any evidence to support a replicable dimensional structure when correcting symptom correlations for overall depression severity. Second, in the general-population samples, symptoms correlated substantially and were uni-dimensional. Third, among depressed individuals, symptom correlations were mostly weak and there was no evidence of any replicable dimensional structure, regardless of whether or not correlations were adjusted for overall severity.

Our finding that depressive symptoms were uni-dimensional in the general population is totally consistent with results from previous studies that analysed combined samples of healthy and affected individuals. These studies included general population samples from the USA and Mexico, and youths ages 5–18 in the USA and UK (Muthén, Reference Muthén1989; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Neale and Kendler2005; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Kendler, Kubarych and Neale2011; Cole et al. Reference Cole, Cai, Martin, Findling, Youngstrom, Garber, Curry, Hyde, Essex, Compas, Goodyer, Rohde, Stark, Slattery and Forehand2011; Mezuk & Kendler, Reference Mezuk and Kendler2012; Familiar et al. Reference Familiar, Ortiz-Panozo, Hall, Vieitez, Romieu, Lopez-Ridaura and Lajous2015). Our results replicate these results among young adults in the USA and extend them to young Swiss men. Furthermore, they resemble recent results reported by Fried et al. who found that, as samples of American and Dutch depression patients became more heterogeneous with respect to overall depression severity, average symptom correlations increased and the factor structures became simpler (Fried et al. Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016).

Our finding indicates that, within the general population, depression can be described by a single dimension of severity, the main reason being that depressed individuals form a comparably homogeneous group, relative to the large majority of individuals who are mostly or completely symptom-free (data not shown). The sizeable symptom correlations found in the general population samples mainly reflected this difference between depressed and non-depressed individuals. As such, the common set of ICD-10 and DSM-IV depression symptoms has diagnostic utility identifying individuals suffering from depression within the general population, and the listed symptoms seem to capture the basic scope and severity of the syndrome well.

Conversely, the uni-dimensionality of depression symptoms was not present among depressed individuals and we found no evidence of any replicable dimensional structure. Our failure to uncover such a structure is totally consistent with a recent systematic review that failed to identify any conclusive evidence that data-driven dimensions or subtypes of depression exist (van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012, see also Chen et al. Reference Chen, Eaton, Gallo and Nestadt2000; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Neale and Kendler2005; Shafer, Reference Shafer2006; Carragher et al. Reference Carragher, Adamson, Bunting and McCann2009; Aggen et al. Reference Aggen, Kendler, Kubarych and Neale2011; Cole et al. Reference Cole, Cai, Martin, Findling, Youngstrom, Garber, Curry, Hyde, Essex, Compas, Goodyer, Rohde, Stark, Slattery and Forehand2011 Mezuk & Kendler, Reference Mezuk and Kendler2012; Hybels et al. Reference Hybels, Landerman and Blazer2013; Buhler et al. Reference Buhler, Seemuller and Lage2014; Li et al. Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendler2014a, Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Tao, Zhang, Wang, Gao, Yang, Liu, Li, Shi, Wang, Liu, Zhang, Du, Jiang, Shen, Zhang, Liang, Sun, Hu, Liu, Miao, Meng, Hu, Huang, Li, Ha, Deng, Mei, Zhong, Gao, Sang, Zhang, Fang, Yu, Yang, Chen, Hong, Wu, Chen, Cai, Song, Pan, Dong, Pan, Zhang, Shen, Liu, Gu, Liu, Zhang, Flint and Kendlerb; Rodgers et al. Reference Rodgers, Grosse Holtforth, Muller, Hengartner, Rossler and Ajdacic-Gross2014; Fried et al. Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016). Furthermore, the factor structure of depression changes over time among depressed patients (Fried et al. Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016). It therefore seems unlikely that a dimensional structure underlies the symptoms of depression. Consequently, previous literature reporting and making use of depression dimensions should be considered cautiously.

The mostly-weak symptom correlations among depressed individuals were particularly surprising. Symptom correlations have seldom been reported in the literature and, hence, this phenomenon seems to have gone unnoticed. Nonetheless, Cramer et al. reported average symptom correlations among American adults with a ‘dysphoric episode’ (defined as an episode with at least two depressive symptoms), and Fried et al. reported average symptom correlations among American and Dutch depression patients. Consistent with our results, these authors reported average correlations ranging from r = 0.17 to 0.23 (Cramer et al. Reference Cramer, Borsboom, Aggen and Kendler2012) and from r = 0.12 to 0.39 (Fried et al. Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016). Additionally, previous studies failed to detect substantial stability of depression symptoms and subtypes between successive depressive episodes (Coryell et al. Reference Coryell, Winokur, Shea, Maser, Endicott and Akiskal1994; Lewinsohn et al. Reference Lewinsohn, Pettit, Joiner and Seeley2003; Melartin et al. Reference Melartin, Leskela, Rytsala, Sokero, Lestela-Mielonen and Isometsa2004; Oquendo et al. Reference Oquendo, Barrera, Ellis, Li, Burke, Grunebaum, Endicott and Mann2004). Thus, symptom correlations seem to be rather weak, both within and between depressive episodes.

That symptom correlations were so weak implies that, even if a replicable dimensional model existed, it would be based on an average correlation of r ≈ 0.20; the vast majority of symptom variance would remain unexplained, as correlation-based models cannot explain symptom variance beyond these correlations. This agrees with two recent studies that uncovered highly-diverse symptom profiles among depression patients (Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2015; Zimmerman et al. Reference Zimmerman, Ellison, Young, Chelminski and Dalrymple2015). For example, Fried and Nesse identified 1030 unique profiles of depression symptoms in a sample of 3703 depressed American outpatients, with the most frequent profile only occurring in 1.8% of patients (Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2015). One explanation of how such diverse profiles develop is that adverse life events and other risk factors exerted differential impacts on depressive symptoms (Keller & Nesse, Reference Keller and Nesse2006; Keller et al. Reference Keller, Neale and Kendler2007; Lux & Kendler, Reference Lux and Kendler2010; Fried et al. Reference Fried, Nesse, Zivin, Guille and Sen2014; Fried et al. Reference Fried, Nesse, Guille and Sen2015) and appeared to change the symptoms’ correlation patterns (Cramer et al. Reference Cramer, Borsboom, Aggen and Kendler2012). Thus, an individual's symptom profile depends at least partially on the aetiological factors that provoked the depressive episode. Furthermore, these different aetiologies are likely to imply differential responses to various treatment options. For example, evidence indicates that depression related to negative life events and trauma is more responsive to psychotherapy than to medication, whereas depressed individuals with maladaptive personality traits may respond better to selective serotonin-reuptake inhibitors (Simon & Perlis, Reference Simon and Perlis2010).

Two final issues concern the recent emergence of network models as an alternative account of mental disorders (Bringmann et al. Reference Bringmann, Vissers, Wichers, Geschwind, Kuppens, Peeters, Borsboom and Tuerlinckx2013; Goekoop & Goekoop, Reference Goekoop and Goekoop2014; van Borkulo et al. Reference van Borkulo, Borsboom, Epskamp, Blanken, Boschloo, Schoevers and Waldorp2014; Boschloo et al. Reference Boschloo, van Borkulo, Rhemtulla, Keyes, Borsboom and Schoevers2015; Bringmann et al. Reference Bringmann, Lemmens, Huibers, Borsboom and Tuerlinckx2015; van Borkulo et al. Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Borsboom, Penninx, Waldorp and Schoevers2015; Beard et al. Reference Beard, Millner, Forgeard, Fried, Hsu, Treadway, Leonard, Kertz and Bjorgvinsson2016). Network models are based on the premise that symptom inter-relationships reflect direct causal influences between symptoms, rather than underlying latent factors, as in the factor analysis framework. The exact relationship between factor and network models remains unclear, however (Molenaar, Reference Molenaar2010; Ross, Reference Ross2010), and various authors disagreed with the network proponents’ critique of the latent variable approach (Belzung et al. Reference Belzung, Billette de Villemeur, Lemoine and Camus2010; Danks et al. Reference Danks, Fancsali, Glymour and Scheines2010; Haig & Vertue, Reference Haig and Vertue2010; Humphry & McGrane, Reference Humphry and McGrane2010; Markus, Reference Markus2010). Most importantly, no empirical comparison of these two approaches has yet been reported (Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Deyoung and Markon2010). Thus, how and to what degree one would draw different conclusions when applying factor analysis v. network modelling to one and the same sample is unclear. Future research needs to address this issue.

Second, our results are likely of importance to network research, since they indicate that the choice of sample type can impact the strength of symptom relationships considerably. Since networks are also based on symptom relationships, this should be an issue in network research, too. Indeed, depression-related network studies have been based on all sorts of samples (Bringmann et al. Reference Bringmann, Vissers, Wichers, Geschwind, Kuppens, Peeters, Borsboom and Tuerlinckx2013; Goekoop & Goekoop, Reference Goekoop and Goekoop2014; van Borkulo et al. Reference van Borkulo, Borsboom, Epskamp, Blanken, Boschloo, Schoevers and Waldorp2014; Boschloo et al. Reference Boschloo, van Borkulo, Rhemtulla, Keyes, Borsboom and Schoevers2015; Bringmann et al. Reference Bringmann, Lemmens, Huibers, Borsboom and Tuerlinckx2015; van Borkulo et al. Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Borsboom, Penninx, Waldorp and Schoevers2015; Beard et al. Reference Beard, Millner, Forgeard, Fried, Hsu, Treadway, Leonard, Kertz and Bjorgvinsson2016). Even more intriguing, it was recently found that global network connectivity increased as disorder severity decreased over time (Beard et al. Reference Beard, Millner, Forgeard, Fried, Hsu, Treadway, Leonard, Kertz and Bjorgvinsson2016).

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. First, it was restricted to young adults, so the results’ generalisability must be re-examined in demographically-broader samples. Second, symptom lists that are more differentiated than the nine DSM-IV criteria might be required, especially considering the weak correlations we detected in our depressed samples. More differentiated symptoms might be needed to capture depression subtypes in patient samples. Note, however, that studies using more comprehensive symptom sets have thus far also failed to uncover replicable dimensions (van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012). Third, we used dichotomised symptom scores to facilitate comparisons against previous research. Doing so, some information might have been lost. Future studies should evaluate more finely-grained symptom scales. Fourth, step 2 of our analysis was exploratory and included multiple testing. Note, however, that we used a bootstrap procedure and replicated our analyses across different samples to safeguard against this. Finally, contrary to subtype research using latent class analysis, a dimensional approach could not detect subtypes that are based on only one or two symptoms (if a subtype is defined by several symptoms, however, these symptoms would be tightly correlated and, hence, emerge as a dimension). Thus, whereas our results rule out a dimensional structure of depression, there might still be subtypes of depression characterised by the presence of one or two specific symptoms. Note, however, that previous research focusing on statistically-derived subtypes has also failed to reveal replicable results (van Loo et al. Reference van Loo, de Jonge, Romeijn, Kessler and Schoevers2012).

Implications

Given prior research findings, our results have two implications. First, caution is warranted when considering studies assessing dimensions of depression because general population-based studies and studies of depressed individuals generate different data that can lead to different conclusions. This problem likely generalises to other models based on the symptoms’ inter-relationships (e.g., network models). Second, it appears that the two dominant aspects of depression are its overall severity and each individual's symptom profile. Whereas the overall severity aligns individuals on a continuum of disorder intensity that allows non-affected individuals to be distinguished from affected individuals, the clinical evaluation and treatment of depressed individuals should focus directly on each individual's symptom profile, since it seems to convey most clinically-relevant information.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial Support

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number FN33CS30_148493). The funder had no role in study design, data collection or analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 revised in 2008.

Availability of Data and Materials

The raw data of the C-SURF-cohort study and the NHANES study are available at http://www.c-surf.ch/en/30.html and at http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/nhanes_questionnaires.htm.

References

Aggen, SH, Neale, MC, Kendler, KS (2005). DSM criteria for major depression: evaluating symptom patterns using latent-trait item response models. Psychological Medicine 35, 475487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aggen, SH, Kendler, KS, Kubarych, TS, Neale, MC (2011). Differential age and sex effects in the assessment of major depression: a population-based twin item analysis of the DSM criteria. Twin Research and Human Genetics 14, 524538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. DSM-5. American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Barendse, MT, Oort, FJ, Timmerman, ME (2014). Using Exploratory Factor Analysis to Determine the Dimensionality of Discrete Responses. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 22, 87101.Google Scholar
Baumeister, H, Parker, G (2012). Meta-review of depressive subtyping models. Journal of Affective Disorders 139, 126140.Google Scholar
Beard, C, Millner, AJ, Forgeard, MJ, Fried, EI, Hsu, KJ, Treadway, MT, Leonard, CV, Kertz, SJ, Bjorgvinsson, T (2016). Network analysis of depression and anxiety symptom relationships in a psychiatric sample. Psychological Medicine Sep 14 [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Bech, P, Rasmussen, NA, Olsen, LR, Noerholm, V, Abildgaard, W (2001). The sensitivity and specificity of the Major Depression Inventory, using the Present State Examination as the index of diagnostic validity. Journal of Affective Disorders 66, 159164.Google Scholar
Belzung, C, Billette de Villemeur, E, Lemoine, M, Camus, V (2010). Latent variables and the network perspective. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 150151.Google Scholar
Boschloo, L, van Borkulo, CD, Rhemtulla, M, Keyes, KM, Borsboom, D, Schoevers, RA (2015). The network structure of symptoms of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. PLoS ONE 10, e0137621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bringmann, LF, Vissers, N, Wichers, M, Geschwind, N, Kuppens, P, Peeters, F, Borsboom, D, Tuerlinckx, F (2013). A network approach to psychopathology: new insights into clinical longitudinal data. PLoS ONE 8, e60188.Google Scholar
Bringmann, LF, Lemmens, LH, Huibers, MJ, Borsboom, D, Tuerlinckx, F (2015). Revealing the dynamic network structure of the beck depression inventory-II. Psychological Medicine 45, 747757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromet, E, Andrade, LH, Hwang, I, Sampson, NA, Alonso, J, de Girolamo, G, de Graaf, R, Demyttenaere, K, Hu, C, Iwata, N, Karam, AN, Kaur, J, Kostyuchenko, S, Lepine, JP, Levinson, D, Matschinger, H, Mora, ME, Browne, MO, Posada-Villa, J, Viana, MC, Williams, DR, Kessler, RC (2011). Cross-national epidemiology of DSM-IV major depressive episode. BMC Medicine 9, 90.Google Scholar
Brown, TA, Moore, MT (2012). Confirmatory factor analysis. In Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling (ed. Hoyle, RH), pp. 361379. The Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Buhler, J, Seemuller, F, Lage, D (2014). The predictive power of subgroups: an empirical approach to identify depressive symptom patterns that predict response to treatment. Journal of Affective Disorders 163, 8187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carragher, N, Adamson, G, Bunting, B, McCann, S (2009). Subtypes of depression in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Affective Disorders 113, 8899.Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS). National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Data. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Available at https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/index.htmGoogle Scholar
Chen, L, Eaton, WW, Gallo, JJ, Nestadt, G (2000). Understanding the heterogeneity of depression through the triad of symptoms, course and risk factors: a longitudinal, population-based study. Journal of Affective Disorders 59, 111.Google Scholar
Cole, DA, Cai, L, Martin, NC, Findling, RL, Youngstrom, EA, Garber, J, Curry, JF, Hyde, JS, Essex, MJ, Compas, BE, Goodyer, IM, Rohde, P, Stark, KD, Slattery, MJ, Forehand, R (2011). Structure and measurement of depression in youths: applying item response theory to clinical data. Psychological Assessment 23, 819833.Google Scholar
Coryell, W, Winokur, G, Shea, T, Maser, JD, Endicott, J, Akiskal, HS (1994). The long-term stability of depressive subtypes. The American Journal of Psychiatry 151, 199204.Google Scholar
Cramer, AO, Borsboom, D, Aggen, SH, Kendler, KS (2012). The pathoplasticity of dysphoric episodes: differential impact of stressful life events on the pattern of depressive symptom inter-correlations. Psychological Medicine 42, 957965.Google Scholar
Cuijpers, P, Vogelzangs, N, Twisk, J, Kleiboer, A, Li, J, Penninx, BW (2014). Comprehensive meta-analysis of excess mortality in depression in the general community v. patients with specific illnesses. The American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 453462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danks, D, Fancsali, S, Glymour, C, Scheines, R (2010). Comorbid science? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 153155.Google Scholar
Eaton, NR, Krueger, RF, Markon, KE, Keyes, KM, Skodol, AE, Wall, M, Hasin, DS, Grant, BF (2013). The structure and predictive validity of the internalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 8692.Google Scholar
Familiar, I, Ortiz-Panozo, E, Hall, B, Vieitez, I, Romieu, I, Lopez-Ridaura, R, Lajous, M (2015). Factor structure of the Spanish version of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 in Mexican women. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 24, 7482.Google Scholar
Ferrari, AJ, Charlson, FJ, Norman, RE, Patten, SB, Freedman, G, Murray, CJ, Vos, T, Whiteford, HA (2013). Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age, and year: findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. PLoS Medicine 10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, EI, Nesse, RM (2015). Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study. Journal of Affective Disorders 172, 96102.Google Scholar
Fried, EI, Nesse, RM, Guille, C, Sen, S (2015). The differential influence of life stress on individual symptoms of depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 131, 465471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, EI, Nesse, RM, Zivin, K, Guille, C, Sen, S (2014). Depression is more than the sum score of its parts: individual DSM symptoms have different risk factors. Psychological Medicine 44, 20672076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, EI, van Borkulo, CD, Epskamp, S, Schoevers, RA, Tuerlinckx, F, Borsboom, D (2016). Measuring depression over time…or not? lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychological Assessment Jan 28 [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
Goekoop, R, Goekoop, JG (2014). A network view on psychiatric disorders: network clusters of symptoms as elementary syndromes of psychopathology. PLoS ONE 9, e112734.Google Scholar
Goldberg, DP, Krueger, RF, Andrews, G, Hobbs, MJ (2009). Emotional disorders: cluster 4 of the proposed meta-structure for DSM-V and ICD-11. Psychological Medicine 39, 20432059.Google Scholar
Haig, BD, Vertue, FM (2010). Extending the network perspective on comorbidity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 158.Google Scholar
Haslam, N, Holland, E, Kuppens, P (2012). Categories v. dimensions in personality and psychopathology: a quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine 42, 903920.Google Scholar
Humphry, SM, McGrane, JA (2010). Is there a contradiction between the network and latent variable perspectives? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 160161.Google Scholar
Hybels, CF, Landerman, LR, Blazer, DG (2013). Latent subtypes of depression in a community sample of older adults: can depression clusters predict future depression trajectories? Journal of Psychiatric Research 47, 12881297.Google Scholar
Jennrich, RI, Bentler, PM (2011). Exploratory bi-factor analysis. Psychometrika 76, 537549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennrich, RI, Bentler, PM (2012). Exploratory bi-factor analysis: the oblique case. Psychometrika 77, 442454.Google Scholar
Keller, MC, Nesse, RM (2006). The evolutionary significance of depressive symptoms: different adverse situations lead to different depressive symptom patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91, 316330.Google Scholar
Keller, MC, Neale, MC, Kendler, KS (2007). Association of different adverse life events with distinct patterns of depressive symptoms. The American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 15211529.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Aggen, SH, Neale, MC (2013). Evidence for multiple genetic factors underlying DSM-IV criteria for major depression. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 599607.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Bromet, EJ (2013). The epidemiology of depression across cultures. Annual Review of Public Health 34, 119138.Google Scholar
Kline, RB (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modelling. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JB (2001). The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine 16, 606613.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF, Deyoung, CG, Markon, KE (2010). Toward scientifically useful quantitative models of psychopathology: the importance of a comparative approach. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 163164.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, PM, Pettit, JW, Joiner, TE Jr., Seeley, JR (2003). The symptomatic expression of major depressive disorder in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112, 244252.Google Scholar
Li, Y, Aggen, S, Shi, S, Gao, J, Tao, M, Zhang, K, Wang, X, Gao, C, Yang, L, Liu, Y, Li, K, Shi, J, Wang, G, Liu, L, Zhang, J, Du, B, Jiang, G, Shen, J, Zhang, Z, Liang, W, Sun, J, Hu, J, Liu, T, Miao, G, Meng, H, Hu, C, Huang, G, Li, G, Ha, B, Deng, H, Mei, Q, Zhong, H, Gao, S, Sang, H, Zhang, Y, Fang, X, Yu, F, Yang, D, Chen, Y, Hong, X, Wu, W, Chen, G, Cai, M, Song, Y, Pan, J, Dong, J, Pan, R, Zhang, W, Shen, Z, Liu, Z, Gu, D, Liu, X, Zhang, Q, Flint, J, Kendler, KS (2014 a). The structure of the symptoms of major depression: exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis in depressed Han Chinese women. Psychological Medicine 44, 13911401.Google Scholar
Li, Y, Aggen, S, Shi, S, Gao, J, Tao, M, Zhang, K, Wang, X, Gao, C, Yang, L, Liu, Y, Li, K, Shi, J, Wang, G, Liu, L, Zhang, J, Du, B, Jiang, G, Shen, J, Zhang, Z, Liang, W, Sun, J, Hu, J, Liu, T, Miao, G, Meng, H, Hu, C, Huang, G, Li, G, Ha, B, Deng, H, Mei, Q, Zhong, H, Gao, S, Sang, H, Zhang, Y, Fang, X, Yu, F, Yang, D, Chen, Y, Hong, X, Wu, W, Chen, G, Cai, M, Song, Y, Pan, J, Dong, J, Pan, R, Zhang, W, Shen, Z, Liu, Z, Gu, D, Liu, X, Zhang, Q, Flint, J, Kendler, KS (2014 b). Subtypes of major depression: latent class analysis in depressed Han Chinese women. Psychological Medicine 44, 32753288.Google Scholar
Lux, V, Kendler, KS (2010). Deconstructing major depression: a validation study of the DSM-IV symptomatic criteria. Psychological Medicine 40, 16791690.Google Scholar
MacCallum, RC, Roznowski, M, Necowitz, LB (1992). Model modifications in covariance structure analysis: the problem of capitalization on chance. Psychological Bulletin 111, 490504.Google Scholar
Markon, KE, Chmielewski, M, Miller, CJ (2011). The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology: a quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin 137, 856879.Google Scholar
Markus, KA (2010). Questions about networks, measurement, and causation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 164165.Google Scholar
Melartin, T, Leskela, U, Rytsala, H, Sokero, P, Lestela-Mielonen, P, Isometsa, E (2004). Co-morbidity and stability of melancholic features in DSM-IV major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine 34, 14431452.Google Scholar
Mezuk, B, Kendler, KS (2012). Examining variation in depressive symptoms over the life course: a latent class analysis. Psychological Medicine 42, 20372046.Google Scholar
Molenaar, PC (2010). Latent variable models are network models. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 166.Google Scholar
Murray, CJ, Lopez, AD (1996). Evidence-based health policy – lessons from the global burden of disease study. Science 274, 740743.Google Scholar
Muthén, BO (1989). Dichotomous factor analysis of symptom data. Sociological Methods and Research 18, 1965.Google Scholar
Olsen, LR, Jensen, DV, Noerholm, V, Martiny, K, Bech, P (2003). The internal and external validity of the major depression inventory in measuring severity of depressive states. Psychological Medicine 33, 351356.Google Scholar
Oquendo, MA, Barrera, A, Ellis, SP, Li, S, Burke, AK, Grunebaum, M, Endicott, J, Mann, JJ (2004). Instability of symptoms in recurrent major depression: a prospective study. The American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 255261.Google Scholar
Pornprasertmanit, S, Miller, P, Schoemann, K, Rosseel, Y (2013). sem-Tools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling (R package version 0.4-0). Available at http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semToolsGoogle Scholar
Prisciandaro, JJ, Roberts, JE (2009). A comparison of the predictive abilities of dimensional and categorical models of unipolar depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine 39, 10871096.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna.Google Scholar
Reise, SP, Moore, TM, Haviland, MG (2010). Bifactor models and rotations: exploring the extent to which multidimensional data yield univocal scale scores. Journal of Personality Assessment 92, 544559.Google Scholar
Revelle, W (2013). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Version 1.3.10. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA.Google Scholar
Rindskopf, D (1984). Structural equation models: empirical identification, Heywood cases, and related problems. Sociological Methods and Research 13, 109119.Google Scholar
Rodgers, S, Grosse Holtforth, M, Muller, M, Hengartner, MP, Rossler, W, Ajdacic-Gross, V (2014). Symptom-based subtypes of depression and their psychosocial correlates: a person-centered approach focusing on the influence of sex. Journal of Affective Disorders 156, 92103.Google Scholar
Ross, D (2010). Some mental disorders are based on networks, others on latent variables. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 166167.Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y (2012). lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 48, 136.Google Scholar
Shafer, AB (2006). Meta-analysis of the factor structures of four depression questionnaires: beck, CES-D, Hamilton, and Zung. Journal of Clinical Psychology 62, 123146.Google Scholar
Simon, GE, Perlis, RH (2010). Personalized medicine for depression: can we match patients with treatments? The American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 14451455.Google Scholar
Slade, T, Andrews, G (2005). Latent structure of depression in a community sample: a taxometric analysis. Psychological Medicine 35, 489497.Google Scholar
Steiger, JH (1980). Testing pattern hypotheses on correlation matrices: alternative statistics and some empirical results. Multivariate Behavioral Research 15, 335352.Google Scholar
Studer, J, Baggio, S, Mohler-Kuo, M, Dermota, P, Gaume, J, Bertholet, N, Daeppen, JB, Gmel, G (2013 a). Examining non-response bias in substance use research–are late respondents proxies for non-respondents? Drug and Alcohol Dependence 132, 316323.Google Scholar
Studer, J, Mohler-Kuo, M, Dermota, P, Gaume, J, Bertholet, N, Eidenbenz, C, Daeppen, JB, Gmel, G (2013 b). Need for informed consent in substance use studies – harm of bias? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 74, 931940.Google Scholar
van Borkulo, CD, Borsboom, D, Epskamp, S, Blanken, TF, Boschloo, L, Schoevers, RA, Waldorp, LJ (2014). A new method for constructing networks from binary data. Scientific Reports 4, 5918.Google Scholar
van Borkulo, C, Boschloo, L, Borsboom, D, Penninx, BWJH, Waldorp, LJ, Schoevers, RA (2015). Association of symptom network structure with the course of longitudinal depression. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 12191219.Google Scholar
van Loo, HM, de Jonge, P, Romeijn, JW, Kessler, RC, Schoevers, RA (2012). Data-driven subtypes of major depressive disorder: a systematic review. BMC Medicine 10, 156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2012). Taschenführer zur ICD-10 Klassifikation psychischer Störungen nach dem Pocket Guide von J.E. Cooper [Pocket guide to the ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders with glossary and diagnostic criteria for research: ICD-10 DCR-10]. Verlag Hans Huber: Bern.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, M, Ellison, W, Young, D, Chelminski, I, Dalrymple, K (2015). How many different ways do patients meet the diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder? Comprehensive Psychiatry 56, 2934.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Symptoms of depression in the ICD-10-based WHO-MDI and the DSM-IV-based PHQ-9

Figure 1

Table 2. Baseline characteristics of study participants

Figure 2

Table 3. Summary of tetrachoric correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms across general-population samples and subsamples of currently depressed subjects

Figure 3

Table 4. Comparison of tetrachoric correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms in general-population samples v. subsamples of currently depressed subjects

Figure 4

Table 5. Summary of exploratory and confirmatory factor and bifactor analyses of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms in general-population samples and subsamples of currently depressed subjects

Figure 5

Table 6. Correlations of the nine DSM-IV depression symptoms adjusted for overall depression severity as estimated by confirmatory factor analysis (model 3)