Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T08:14:25.512Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differences in negativity bias probably underlie variation in attitudes toward change generally, not political ideology specifically

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2014

Steven G. Ludeke
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344. lude0011@umn.educdeyoung@umn.edu
Colin G. DeYoung
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344. lude0011@umn.educdeyoung@umn.edu

Abstract

Many of the characteristics cited in Hibbing et al.'s account are ineffective predictors of economic conservatism. However, these same characteristics are often associated with differences not only in social conservatism but also in religiousness and authoritarianism. Hibbing et al. may have offered a useful explanation of traditionalism and attitudes toward change across domains rather than of general political attitudes.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Hibbing et al. argue that the association between political attitudes and a wide range of psychological and physiological characteristics reflects elevated levels of negativity bias among political conservatives. We suggest that their account is both too general and too specific. Too general because many of the variables invoked by Hibbing et al. show clearer associations with social than with economic conservatism, sometimes showing no association at all with the latter. And too specific because attitudes outside of the political domain show clear connections to many of the variables cited by Hibbing et al.: Attitudes concerning religion and the structure of family and society show highly comparable results to those observed for social conservatism. Accordingly, negativity bias may be best construed as a predictor not of political opinions specifically but of general attitudes toward change across a range of domains. This conclusion is buttressed by a recent behavioral genetic study which found that, in a sample of twins reared apart, attitudes in the political, social, and religious spheres (the Traditional Moral Values Triad; TMVT; Bouchard Reference Bouchard, Voland and Schiefenhövel2009) were best construed as superficially different representations of a single underlying trait, reflecting traditionalism (Ludeke et al. Reference Ludeke, Johnson and Bouchard2013). The TMVT hypothesis is supported to the extent that the development of attitudes in all three domains can be accounted for by the same mechanisms.

The importance of distinguishing between attitudes toward change (reflected in social conservatism) and attitudes toward equality (reflected in economic conservatism) has been demonstrated elsewhere (e.g., Jost et al. Reference Jost, Federico and Napier2009). Here we highlight how the predictors cited by Hibbing et al. show importantly different connections to these two dimensions, and how findings for the social dimension typically apply to all traits in the TMVT.

The Big Five personality model provides some of the most frequently used predictors of attitude constructs. Although general political conservatism is associated with both low Openness/Intellect and high Conscientiousness (Sibley et al. Reference Sibley, Osborne and Duckitt2012), studies that distinguish between social and economic dimensions report that Openness/Intellect and Conscientiousness are primarily associated with social conservatism, whereas economic conservatism is primarily associated with low Agreeableness (Carney et al. Reference Carney, Jost, Gosling and Potter2008; Hirsh et al. Reference Hirsh, DeYoung, Xu and Peterson2010; Van Hiel & Mervielde Reference Van Hiel and Mervielde2004). This pattern is particularly clear in studies that used social attitude measures related to social and economic conservatism: meta-analysis indicated that Openness/Intellect and Conscientiousness are more strongly associated with Altemeyer's (Reference Altemeyer1996) Right-Wing Authoritarianism (assessing attitudes toward change) than with Pratto et al.s' (Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994) Social Dominance Orientation (SDO, assessing attitudes toward equality); SDO was primarily correlated with Agreeableness and more modestly with Openness/Intellect (Sibley & Duckitt Reference Sibley and Duckitt2008). Consistent with the TMVT model, meta-analysis demonstrated correlations of religious fundamentalism with Conscientiousness and Openness/Intellect similar to those associated with social conservatism (Saroglou Reference Saroglou2010).

In their values, social conservatives diverge from economic conservatives but converge with authoritarians and religious individuals. Duriez et al. (Reference Duriez, Luyten, Snauwaert and Hutsebaut2002) reported that, although both social and economic conservatism were associated with a tendency to favor the self-enhancing cluster of Schwartz's Values Scale (Reference Schwartz and Zanna1992), only social conservatism was associated with a preference for conservation values; this preference for conservation values is, however, common to both authoritarians and religious individuals (Feather Reference Feather, Maehr and Karabenick2005; Saroglou et al. Reference Saroglou, Delpierre and Dernelle2004).

Similarly, disgust sensitivity appears to be more correlated with social conservatism than economic conservatism and is also correlated with religiousness and authoritarianism (e.g., Haidt et al. Reference Haidt, McCauley and Rozin1994; Hodson & Costello Reference Hodson and Costello2007; Inbar et al. Reference Inbar, Pizarro, Iyer and Haidt2012b; Terrizzi et al. Reference Terrizzi, Shook and Ventis2010). Because evolutionary accounts of disgust have posited that the emotion evolved to prevent infection, countries with elevated infection risk (higher parasite loads) might be expected to exhibit corresponding attitude differences; Hibbing et al. note precisely this effect for conservative religious and social beliefs (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2012), and a recent study (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Schaller and Suedfeld2013) observed a comparable association between parasite load and cross-national differences in authoritarian attitudes. Even humor preferences are informative: both conservatism and religiousness appear to be negatively correlated with enjoyment of “sick” humor (which includes jokes with morbid, gruesome, or sadistic content; Saroglou & Anciaux Reference Saroglou and Anciaux2004, Wilson & Patterson Reference Wilson and Patterson1969). In Hibbing et al.'s framework, this might indicate that elevated sensitivity among conservatives and religious individuals to the aversive imagery in such jokes interferes with their ability to find humor in them.

Variables related to cognitive function follow the same pattern. Cognitive style measures such as Need for Closure and Need for Cognition correlate moderately with measures of social conservatism, authoritarianism, and religiousness, though they appear to be only modestly or even not at all associated with economic conservatism and SDO (Crowson Reference Crowson2009; Hunsberger et al. Reference Hunsberger, Alisat, Pancer and Pratt1996; Saroglou Reference Saroglou2002; Van Hiel et al. Reference van Hiel, Pandelaere and Duriez2004). Intelligence and education are negatively correlated with political conservatism, authoritarianism, and conventional religiousness (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Ritchie and Bates2011; Van Hiel et al. Reference Van Hiel, Onraet and De Pauw2010), whereas the relation of education and intelligence to economic conservatism and SDO appears to be smaller than that observed for the TMVT traits, and possibly even inverted (Heaven et al. Reference Heaven, Ciarrochi and Leeson2011; Johnson & Tamney Reference Johnson and Tamney2001; Kemmelmeier Reference Kemmelmeier2008).

Studies of the physiological and neurological correlates of attitude differences represent a recent but expanding addition to this literature. Consistent with the TMVT account, Hibbing et al. note that the same pattern of neural activity exhibited by conservatives during a task requiring response inhibition was observed among highly religious individuals (Amodio et al. Reference Amodio, Jost, Master and Yee2007; Inzlicht et al. Reference Inzlicht, McGregor, Hirsh and Nash2009). (Significantly, the results of these studies seem to be in conflict with the “negativity bias” hypothesis, as the pattern of neural activity exhibited by conservatives and the highly religious is typically interpreted as an indication of reduced sensitivity to negative information; Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011.)

Although Hibbing et al. have provided a novel and compelling integration of a broad literature, we suggest their proposed mechanism is better suited to account for differences in the trait identified by the TMVT than for political conservatism broadly construed. Future empirical work exploring the origins of political attitude differences could employ a broader range of outcome measures (differentiating between social and economic conservatism, and assessing religiousness, authoritarianism, and traditionalism) to assess this possibility.

References

Altemeyer, B. (1996) The authoritarian specter. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L. & Yee, C. M. (2007) Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience 10(10):1246–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1979.Google Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2009) Authoritarianism, religiousness and conservatism: Is “obedience to authority” the explanation for their clustering, universality and evolution? In: The biological evolution of religious mind and behaviour, ed. Voland, E. & Schiefenhövel, W., pp. 165–80. Springer.Google Scholar
Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D. & Potter, J. (2008) The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology 29(6):807–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowson, H. M. (2009) Are all conservatives alike? A study of the psychological correlates of cultural and economic conservatism. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied 143(5):449–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duriez, B., Luyten, P., Snauwaert, B. & Hutsebaut, D. (2002) The importance of religiosity and values in predicting political attitudes: Evidence for the continuing importance of religiosity in Flanders (Belgium). Mental Health, Religion & Culture 5(1):3554.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (2005) Values, religion and motivation. In: Advances in motivation and achievement: Motivation and religion, ed. Maehr, M. L. & Karabenick, S. A., pp. 3573. JAI Press.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L. & Thornhill, R. (2012) Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35(2):6179.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., McCauley, C. & Rozin, P. (1994) Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences 16(5):701–13.Google Scholar
Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J. & Leeson, P. (2011) Cognitive ability, right-wing authoritarianism, and social dominance orientation: A five-year longitudinal study amongst adolescents. Intelligence 39(1):1521.Google Scholar
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X. & Peterson, J. B. (2010) Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(5):655–64.Google Scholar
Hodson, G. & Costello, K. (2007) Interpersonal disgust, ideological orientations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes. Psychological Science 18(8):691–8.Google Scholar
Hunsberger, B., Alisat, S., Pancer, S. & Pratt, M. (1996) Religious fundamentalism and religious doubts: Content, connections, and complexity of thinking. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 6(3):201–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Iyer, R. & Haidt, J. (2012b) Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(5):537–44.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., McGregor, I., Hirsh, J. B. & Nash, K. (2009) Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychological Science 20(3):385–92.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. & Tamney, J. (2001) Social traditionalism and economic conservatism: Two conservative political ideologies in the United States. The Journal of Social Psychology 141(2):233–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M. & Napier, J. L. (2009) Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology 60:307–37.Google Scholar
Kemmelmeier, M. (2008) Is there a relationship between political orientation and cognitive ability? A test of three hypotheses in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences 45(8):767–72.Google Scholar
Lewis, G. J., Ritchie, S. J. & Bates, T. C. (2011) The relationship between intelligence and multiple domains of religious belief: Evidence from a large adult US sample. Intelligence 39(6):468–72.Google Scholar
Ludeke, S., Johnson, W. & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2013) “Obedience to traditional authority:” A heritable factor underlying authoritarianism, conservatism and religiousness. Personality and Individual Differences 55(4):375–80.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M. & Suedfeld, P. (2013) Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PloS ONE 8(5):e62275.Google Scholar
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M. & Malle, B. F. (1994) Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67(4):741–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saroglou, V. (2002) Beyond dogmatism: The need for closure as related to religion. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 5(2)183–94.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V. (2010) Religiousness as a cultural adaptation of basic traits: A five-factor model perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14(1):108–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saroglou, V. & Anciaux, L. (2004) Liking sick humor: Coping styles and religion as predictors. Humor 17(3):257–77.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V., Delpierre, V. & Dernelle, R. (2004) Values and religiosity: A meta-analysis of studies using Schwartz's model. Personality and Individual Differences 37(4):721–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1992) Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 25, ed. Zanna, M. P., pp. 165. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V, Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J. & Davidson, R. J. (2011) The integration of negative affect, pain, and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12(3):154–67.Google Scholar
Sibley, C. G. & Duckitt, J. (2008) Personality and prejudice: A meta-analysis and theoretical review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 12(3):248–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, C. G., Osborne, D. & Duckitt, J. (2012) Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a Threat-Constraint Model. Journal of Research in Personality 46(6): 664–77.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J. & Ventis, W. L. (2010) Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences 49(6):587–92.Google Scholar
Van Hiel, A. & Mervielde, I. (2004) Openness to experience and boundaries in the mind: Relationships with cultural and economic conservative beliefs. Journal of Personality 72(4):659–86.Google Scholar
Van Hiel, A., Onraet, E. & De Pauw, S. (2010) The relationship between social-cultural attitudes and behavioral measures of cognitive style: A meta-analytic integration of studies. Journal of Personality 78(6):1765–99. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00669.x.Google Scholar
van Hiel, A., Pandelaere, M. & Duriez, B. (2004) The impact of need for closure on conservative beliefs and racism: Differential mediation by authoritarian submission and authoritarian dominance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30(7):824–37.Google Scholar
Wilson, G. D. & Patterson, J. R. (1969) Conservatism as a predictor of humor preferences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 33(3):271–74.Google Scholar