Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T15:41:47.331Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship of Contextual Cueing and Hippocampal Volume in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment Patients and Cognitively Normal Older Adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2015

Selam Negash*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania Penn Memory Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania
Daria Kliot
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania
Darlene V. Howard
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington, DC
James H. Howard Jr
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington, DC The Catholic University of America, Washington, DC
Sandhistu R. Das
Affiliation:
Penn Image Computing and Science Laboratory, Department of Radiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Paul A. Yushkevich
Affiliation:
Penn Image Computing and Science Laboratory, Department of Radiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
John B. Pluta
Affiliation:
Penn Image Computing and Science Laboratory, Department of Radiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Steven E. Arnold
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania Penn Memory Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania
David A. Wolk
Affiliation:
Penn Memory Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia Pennsylvania
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Selam Negash, Penn Memory Center, University of Pennsylvania, 3615 Chestnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104. E-mail: selamawit.negash@uphs.upenn.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

There is currently some debate as to whether hippocampus mediates contextual cueing. In the present study, we examined contextual cueing in patients diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy older adults, with the main goal of investigating the role of hippocampus in this form of learning. Amnestic MCI (aMCI) patients and healthy controls completed the contextual cueing task, in which they were asked to search for a target (a horizontal T) in an array of distractors (rotated L’s). Unbeknownst to them, the spatial arrangement of elements on some displays was repeated thus making the configuration a contextual cue to the location of the target. In contrast, the configuration for novel displays was generated randomly on each trial. The difference in response times between repeated and novel configurations served as a measure of contextual learning. aMCI patients, as a group, were able to learn spatial contextual cues as well as healthy older adults. However, better learning on this task was associated with higher hippocampal volume, particularly in right hemisphere. Furthermore, contextual cueing performance was significantly associated with hippocampal volume, even after controlling for age and MCI status. These findings support the role of the hippocampus in learning of spatial contexts, and also suggest that the contextual cueing paradigm can be useful in detecting neuropathological changes associated with the hippocampus. (JINS, 2015, 21, 285–296)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Introduction

With the increased focus on early and accurate detection of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), there is burgeoning interest in cognitive neuroscience of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and in studies that use analytic experimental, well-designed paradigms to disentangle the cognitive deficits in MCI. Such paradigms have advantages over more global assessments of cognition because they are aimed at specific aspects of cognition whose underlying brain networks are well understood. Implicit learning is a less studied, yet potentially important system that can offer insights into the neural effects of MCI.

Implicit learning generally refers to a situation where a person learns about the structure of a stimulus environment without conscious effort to learn and without ability to describe what has been learned (Reber, Reference Reber1993). It is said to occur when participants demonstrate knowledge of the regularity by the speed and/or accuracy of their responses, but reveal little awareness of what they have learned. Implicit learning has multiple forms, which call upon distinct neural systems and are differentially affected by neurodegenerative diseases, such as Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and Parkinson’s disease (Eldridge, Masterman, & Knowlton, Reference Eldridge, Masterman and Knowlton2002; Ferraro, Balota, & Connor, Reference Ferraro, Balota and Connor1993; Fleischman, Reference Fleischman2007; Gabrieli, Stebbins, Singh, Willingham, & Goetz, Reference Gabrieli, Stebbins, Singh, Willingham and Goetz1997; Heindel, Salmon, Shults, Walicke, & Butters, Reference Heindel, Salmon, Shults, Walicke and Butters1989; Machado et al., Reference Machado, Cunha, Minc, Portella, Velasques, Basile and Ribeiro2009). The nature of implicit learning in the construct of MCI has been less studied, with mixed results. Our earlier study, the first to examine implicit sequence learning and contextual cueing in MCI, has shown context learning deficits, while sequence learning remained relatively intact in MCI (Negash et al., Reference Negash, Petersen, Geda, Knopman, Boeve, Smith and Petersen2007). Studies that used a more complex sequence structure, however, have shown sequence learning deficits in MCI (Nemeth et al., Reference Nemeth, Janacsek, Kiraly, Londe, Nemeth, Fazekas and Csanyi2013). Results from priming studies have also been mixed (Gong et al., Reference Gong, Tian, Cheng, Chen, Yin, Meng and Wang2010; LaVoie & Faulkner, Reference LaVoie and Faulkner2008; Perri, Carlesimo, Serra, & Caltagirone, Reference Perri, Carlesimo, Serra and Caltagirone2005). Moreover, the neural mechanisms underlying implicit learning systems in MCI are even less understood (for reviews see Fleischman & Gabrieli, Reference Fleischman and Gabrieli1998; Howard & Howard, Reference Howard and Howard2012).

The present study examined contextual cueing in the amnestic subtype of MCI, and the role of hippocampus in this form of learning. The contextual cueing paradigm is a visual search task developed by Chun and colleagues to examine learning of visuospatial contexts (Chun & Jiang, Reference Chun and Jiang1998). In this task, people are asked to search for a target (e.g., a horizontal T) in an array of distractors (rotated L’s). Unbeknownst to participants, some displays contain repeated configurations that provide a contextual cue to the location of the target, while novel displays are generated randomly. Contextual learning is measured by the difference in performance between repeated and novel configurations. Furthermore, such learning has been shown to occur implicitly in that people do not develop explicit knowledge of the relationship between the spatial context and the target location (Chun & Phelps, Reference Chun and Phelps1999; Howard, Howard, Dennis, Yankovich, & Vaidya, Reference Howard, Howard, Dennis, Yankovich and Vaidya2004).

The contextual cueing paradigm is of particular interest in MCI and AD because, unlike other implicit learning forms, it has been argued to be sensitive to medial temporal lobe (MTL) function. For example, contextual cueing is impaired in amnesic patients with damage to the MTL system (Chun & Phelps, Reference Chun and Phelps1999; Manns & Squire, Reference Manns and Squire2001; Preston & Gabrieli, Reference Preston and Gabrieli2008). MTL atrophy, particularly in the hippocampus, is established as one of the earliest changes accompanying amnestic MCI (Duara et al., Reference Duara, Loewenstein, Potter, Appel, Greig, Urs and Potter2008; Korf, Wahlund, Visser, & Scheltens, Reference Korf, Wahlund, Visser and Scheltens2004; Whitwell et al., Reference Whitwell, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve, Petersen and Jack2007). While this implicates the potential role of contextual cueing in detecting hippocampal change in aMCI, there have been only few studies that examined this learning system in MCI (Kessels, Meulenbroek, Fernandez, & Olde Rikkert, Reference Kessels, Meulenbroek, Fernandez and Olde Rikkert2010; Negash et al., Reference Negash, Petersen, Geda, Knopman, Boeve, Smith and Petersen2007), and none that have related learning directly to hippocampal volume. Establishing a relationship between hippocampal integrity and contextual cueing effect can provide important information in our understanding of the neural basis of contextual cueing. To this end, the present study examined the extent to which contextual cueing effect is modulated by hippocampal volume in patients diagnosed with amnestic MCI and healthy older adults.

Method

Participants

Participants were identified from the ongoing cohort in the University of Pennsylvania Alzheimer’s Disease Center/Penn Memory Center (ADC/PMC). Informed consent was obtained from all participants, which was approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board. As part of their participation in the ADC/PMC, each patient undergoes an extensive evaluation, including medical history and physical examination, neurological history and examination, and psychometric testing, often including all elements of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center’s (NACC) Uniform Data Set (Beekly et al., Reference Beekly, Ramos, Lee, Deitrich, Jacka, Wu and Kukull2007; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Weintraub, Chui, Cummings, Decarli, Ferris and Kukull2006; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Salmon, Mercaldo, Ferris, Graff-Radford, Chui and Morris2009). For the purposes of this study, each subject completed at least the following psychometric battery within 3 months of the experimental paradigm: Mini-Mental Status Exam [MMSE; (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975)]; Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1987); Category fluency (animals) (Spreen & Strauss, Reference Spreen and Strauss1998); Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) Word List Memory (WLM) test (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Heyman, Mohs, Hughes, van Belle, Fillenbaum and Clark1989); Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B (Reitan, Reference Reitan1958); and a 15- or 30-item version of the Boston Naming Test [BNT; (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, Reference Kaplan, Goodglass and Weintraub1983)]. Clinical diagnosis was determined by review of the above data, in addition to relevant blood work and brain imaging, at a consensus conference attended by neurologists, neuropsychologists and/or psychiatrists.

The diagnosis of amnestic MCI was made in accordance with the criteria of Petersen et al. (Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999), and included both single and multiple domain MCI (Petersen & Negash, Reference Petersen and Negash2008). Cognitively normal (CN) was defined as an absence of significant cognitive complaints, normal performance on age-adjusted cognitive measures, and consensus conference designation as “normal.” Participants were excluded if they had a history of another significant neurological condition, such as clinical stroke, alcohol or drug abuse/dependence within 2 years of enrollment, or any significant medical/psychiatric condition that would impact compliance with the study protocol.

MRI Acquisition

Participants underwent structural MRI as part of their participation in the study. MRI scans were acquired on a 3 Tesla (T) Siemens Trio scanner at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania. An 8-channel array coil was used in all but one subject, who was scanned using a 32-channel head coil. A standard T1-weighted (MPRAGE) whole brain scan was acquired with the following parameters [8-channel: repetition time/echo time/inversion time (TR/TE/TI)=1600/3.87/950 ms, 15° flip angle, 1.0×1.0×1.0 mm3 resolution, acquisition time 5:13; 32-channel: TR/TE/TI=1900/2.89/900 ms, 9° flip angle, 1.0×1.0×1.0 mm3 resolution, acquisition time 4:26 min]. In addition, a “dedicated” T2-weighted (turbo spin echo) scan with partial field of view and an oblique coronal slice orientation perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus, adapted from (Mueller et al., Reference Mueller, Stables, Du, Schuff, Truran, Cashdollar and Weiner2007), was acquired in all but one subject. The parameters for the T2-weighted MRI were: 8-channel: TR/TE: 5310/68 ms, echo train length 15, 18.3 ms echo spacing, 150° flip angle, 0% phase oversampling, 0.4×0.4 mm2 in plane resolution, 2 mm slice thickness, 30 interleaved slices with 0.6 mm gap, acquisition time 7:12 min; 32-channel: TR/TE: 7200/76 ms, echo train length 15, 15.2 ms echo spacing, 150° flip angle, 75% phase oversampling, 0.4×0.4 mm2 in plane resolution, 2 mm slice thickness, 30 interleaved slices with no gap, acquisition time 6:29 min.

Image Processing

Participants’ hippocampi were segmented from T1-weighted MRI using the open-source, publicly available AHEAD software package (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/ahead/). The method uses a multi-atlas label fusion strategy, first described in (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Das, Suh, Altinay, Pluta, Craige and Yushkevich2011) and further refined in (Wang & Yushkevich, Reference Wang and Yushkevich2013). It begins by computing one-to-one spatial correspondences of a target image with several manually labeled atlas images using deformable registration. Segmentation labels from the atlas images are mapped into the target image space and combined into a single consensus segmentation using label fusion (Artaechevarria, Munoz-Barrutia, & Ortiz-de-Solorzano, Reference Artaechevarria, Munoz-Barrutia and Ortiz-de-Solorzano2009). This segmentation is then refined using a learning-based error correction technique that further improves labeling accuracy. This method has been evaluated in the ADNI dataset, yielding a Dice overlap of ~0.9 for hippocampus.

Hippocampal subfields and MTL cortical regions were segmented in T2-weighted MRI using the recently developed automatic segmentation method called ASHS (Yushkevich et al., Reference Yushkevich, Pluta, Wang, Xie, Ding, Gertje and Wolk2014). This method uses a similar multi-atlas label fusion technique as AHEAD, describe above. An open source implementation is available at http://www.nitrc.org/projects/ashs/.

Given the potential role of caudate in implicit learning, the caudate nucleus was labeled in each participant using Hammers atlas (Hammers et al., Reference Hammers, Allom, Koepp, Free, Myers, Lemieux and Duncan2003). To do so, each participant’s T1-MRI was registered to a template brain containing these labels using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) (Avants, Epstein, Grossman, & Gee, Reference Avants, Epstein, Grossman and Gee2008), and the caudate labels were transferred to the native MRI to compute the volumes.

Contextual Cueing Task

Upon completion of informed consent, participants were asked to perform the contextual cueing task. In this task, participants were instructed to locate and identify a target item among 11 distractors. Items were presented on a computer screen in white on a gray background, as shown in Figure 1. The target was a horizontal T with the tail pointing either left or right by 90°, and the distractors were Ls randomly rotated by 0°, 90°, 180°, or 270°. Each element subtended approximately 1.1° of visual angle at a viewing distance of 56 cm. Arrays were generated by randomly placing the 12 items into cells of an invisible grid (6 rows×8 columns) grid. Every element was randomly repositioned by ±2 pixels along each axis to avoid colinearity. Targets never appeared in the four center cells or at the extreme corners of the display grid.

Figure 1 Schematic for the contextual cueing task. Participants locate the rotated target T (indicated by arrow) amongst L distractors, and indicate its orientation (in this case, rotated to the left).

On each trial, a white fixation dot appeared for 1 s, followed by an array that remained on the screen for up to 10 s until a response was made. Participants were instructed to locate the “T” and respond to its orientation as quickly and accurately as possible using keys on a keyboard (“z” for targets facing left and “/” for right facing right). Auditory feedback of a high-pitch tone indicated a correct response, and a low-pitch tone indicated incorrect response or no response within the 10-s time-out period.

Following 24 practice trials, participants completed 20 blocks of 24 trials each. Twelve of these trials contained unique configurations of distractors (Novel) that were generated randomly. The remaining 12 contained configurations of distractors that repeated across the blocks, appearing once in each block (Repeated). In repeated arrays, the locations of the distractors predicted the location of the target, but not its orientation (left vs. right). Therefore, this regularity could be used to predict the target location but not the correct response. Remaining arrays for each block also had a fixed set of target locations, but with novel configurations of distractors across the experiment. The order of repeated and novel arrays was randomized within each block. Each participant received a different set of repeated and novel arrays. Participants were encouraged to take short breaks between blocks. Several previous studies, including our own, have shown this task to be implicit in that participants are not able to describe what they had learned in verbal reports, or distinguish between novel and repeated trials on the recognition test (Chun & Phelps, Reference Chun and Phelps1999; Greene, Gross, Elsinger, & Rao, Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2007; Negash et al., Reference Negash, Petersen, Geda, Knopman, Boeve, Smith and Petersen2007). The present study, however, is limited in addressing this issue directly, as we did not assess explicit awareness.

Statistical Analysis

The mean reaction time (RT) for correct trials was calculated separately for each configuration type (novel or repeated) for each block for each participant. The 20 blocks were grouped into five epochs, each containing four blocks. For each participant, a mean response time (RT) was determined separately for correct responses to new and repeated configurations on each block. The mean RTs were then averaged across blocks to obtain a single RT for each individual and configuration type (new or repeated) on each epoch. A similar data reduction was performed on accuracy. As is typical with these tasks, accuracy levels were high, particularly for the control group [mean (SD): Control=0.96 (0.19); MCI: 0.89 (0.30)], and comparisons involving accuracy did not reach significance (p-values >.1), likely due to very high level of accuracy and ceiling effects (data not shown).

To examine the role of hippocampal volume in contextual learning, linear regression was conducted, in which a measure of contextual learning was entered as the dependent variable and hippocampal volume as the independent variable. The model also adjusted for intracranial volume. All analyses were performed using SPSS 20.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY), with two tailed tests and a type I error of 0.05.

Results

There were 22 MCI patients and 37 healthy controls. Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1. The groups did not differ in age or years of education. Psychometric testing revealed a pattern characteristic of amnestic MCI, where the MCI group performed significantly worse than controls on measures of global cognition (MMSE), memory (CERAD Word List Learning, immediate and delayed recall), language (Boston Naming, Category Fluency), and attention (Trail Making Tests, Digit Span).

Table 1. Participant characteristics

a APOE data were not available for all participants.

Contextual Learning

Figure 2 shows the mean response times of novel and repeated configurations across the five epochs for MCI and control groups. As the figure indicates, both aMCI and Control groups exhibited non-specific skill learning in that response times decreased across epochs; the main effect of Epoch was significant [F(4,228)=63.05; p<.0001]. The figure also indicates that aMCI patients were slower overall compared to controls, main effect of Group was significant [F(1,57)=7.79; p=.007], consistent with several studies showing processing speed deficits in MCI patients (Facal, Juncos-Rabadan, Pereiro, & Lojo-Seoane, Reference Facal, Juncos-Rabadan, Pereiro and Lojo-Seoane2014; Lopez et al., Reference Lopez, Becker, Jagust, Fitzpatrick, Carlson, DeKosky and Kuller2006; Wenger, Negash, Petersen, & Petersen, Reference Wenger, Negash, Petersen and Petersen2010). The Group×Epoch interaction did not reach significance (p>.10), indicating no group differences in overall skill learning.

Figure 2 Mean reaction times (RTs) across epochs for Novel (solid lines) and Repeated trials (dotted lines) for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy control groups. Error bars represent one standard error.

Importantly, both groups showed similar amounts of contextual learning; the Group×Configuration interaction was not significant [F(1,57)=0.541; p=0.46]. Separate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on each group showed a main effect of Configuration for both MCI, [ F (1,21)=62.08; p<.0001] and control groups, [F(1,36)=99.57; p<.0001], suggesting that each group showed contextual cueing.

In addition, given that this task is susceptible to noise, especially at the beginning of training, we carried out further analysis in which we grouped the 20 blocks into two (first 10 blocks and last 10 blocks). We then examined learning on the last 10 blocks, where both groups clearly displayed contextual cueing effects, as can be observed in Figure 2 (i.e., learning is present by the third epoch). Both groups showed significant contextual cueing, and group differences were not observed; the Group×Configuration (Novel vs. Repeated) ANOVA on the last 10 blocks revealed a main effect of Configuration [F(1,57)=31.42; p<.0001] while the Group×Configuration interaction was not significant [F(1,57)=.616; p=.44]. Subsequent separate ANOVAs on each group showed main effects of Configuration for both aMCI [F(1,21)=8.42; p=.009] and Control [F(1,36)=29.09; p<.0001] groups, indicating that both groups showed contextual cueing.

We also calculated a difference score on the last 10 blocks, that is, mean RT on novel configurations minus mean RT on repeated configurations. Thus, a positive difference score indicated contextual learning. Figure 3 shows the mean difference score for aMCI and Control groups. As the figure indicates, the absolute difference scores were relatively larger for controls (mean: 98.76, SD: 111.4) compared to the MCI (mean: 74.51, SD: 120.5), but the group comparison did not reach significance, t(57)=0.78, p=.436.

Figure 3 Mean difference scores for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy control groups. Error bars represent one standard error.

Hippocampal Volume

Hippocampal data were available for 46 participants (27 controls, 19 aMCI). As expected, mean hippocampal volumes (average of left and right) were significantly smaller in aMCI patients compared to controls (MCI=1764.5 mm3 [SD=71.3], Controls, 2090.2 mm3 [SD=57.5]; t(44)=3.6; p=.001). Of particular interest, we examined whether performance on contextual cueing was associated with hippocampal volume, since that would provide evidence for its dependence on hippocampal function and perhaps AD-related change. In a linear regression model that adjusted for intracranial volume (ICV), contextual cueing (difference score on last 10 blocks) significantly correlated with mean hippocampal volume (estimate=0.14, SE=0.056; p=.0172), in that higher hippocampal volume was associated with greater learning. Based on literature suggesting right-lateralized involvement of hippocampus in visuospatial tasks (Burgess, Maguire, & O’Keefe, Reference Burgess, Maguire and O’Keefe2002; Manelis & Reder, Reference Manelis and Reder2012; Smith & Milner, Reference Smith and Milner1981), we also repeated this analysis for left and right hippocampus separately. Figures 4a and b show the scatterplots for contextual cueing versus raw left and right hippocampal volumes, respectively. In regression models that adjusted for ICV, we observed a significant linear relationship between contextual cueing level and right hippocampal volume (estimate=0.191, SE=0.056; p=0.0015), but not left, (estimate=0.078, SE=0.049; p=0.12).

Figure 4 Scatterplots of mean difference scores versus left hippocampal (a) and right hippocampal (b) volumes, with regression line superimposed, for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (solid circle) and control (open circle) participants.

Even though the initial ANOVA reported earlier had not revealed significant group differences in contextual cueing, MCI patients had displayed numerically (but not significantly) smaller learning effects than the controls, as well as the expected significantly smaller hippocampi. Therefore, to reduce the possibility that the contextual cueing by hippocampal volume relationship described above is driven by group effects, we also added MCI status as a covariate. The right hippocampus retained significance even after adding MCI diagnosis into the model, (estimate=0.188, SE=0.062; p=0.0045), whereas MCI status was not significant (estimate=1.703, SE=17.54; p=0.92). Consistent with a lack of group effect, a univariate model with MCI status was not significant, (estimate=12.126, SE=15.45; p=0.44).

We also examined the extent to which contextual cueing within the MCI group alone is related to their hippocampal volume. In an analysis where MCI patients were dichotomized into large and small hippocampal volumes based on median split, individuals with larger hippocampi (mean: 149.72 ms; SD: 58.2; n=9) showed significantly higher contextual cueing compared to those with smaller hippocampi (mean: −22.72 ms; SD: 103.6; n=10), t(17)=4.39; p=.0004. These findings, then, provide further support for the notion that contextual cueing deficit (or lack thereof) in MCI patients is linked to their hippocampal volumes.

Similarly, we examined the potential predicting role of age in contextual cueing in this cohort (Age range: 61–84 years). The univariate model with age as covariate was not significant, (estimate=−1.787; SE=2.352; p=.45), indicating that contextual cueing is not affected by age in this sample. Indeed, the relationship of right hippocampal volume with contextual cueing remained significant after adding age as a covariate to the model with ICV, (estimate=0.176, SE=0.059; p=.0051). Furthermore, in a model that included all the above covariates (age, ICV, and MCI status), right hippocampal volume continued to be a significant predictor of contextual cueing, (estimate=0.169, SE=0.067; p=0.015). The right hippocampus remained significant even after a Bonferroni correction of p=0.025, for multiple comparisons involving left and right hippocampi. Thus, these findings suggest that it is hippocampal volume, and not MCI diagnosis or age, which best predicts contextual cueing effect.

Hippocampal Subfields and other MTL Regions

While the hippocampus is often presented as a single structure, it is in fact a complex, heterogeneous region composed of several histologically distinct regions, which include the hippocampus proper (consisting of cornu ammonis subfields, CA1, CA2, CA3), dentate gyrus (DG), and subiculum (SUB) (Amaral & Lavenex, Reference Amaral and Lavenex2007). These subfields likely make unique contributions to memory processes and appear to be differentially vulnerable to AD pathology; for instance, the neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) pathology in AD tends to most prominently involve the CA1 subfield of the hippocampus proper (Bobinski et al., Reference Bobinski, DeLeon, Tarnawski, Wegiel, Reisberg, Miller and Wisniewski1998; West, Coleman, Flood, & Troncoso, Reference West, Coleman, Flood and Troncoso1994). Thus, we further examined the differential role of hippocampal subfields in contextual cueing using T2-weighted images. More importantly, there is currently some debate over the involvement of hippocampus in contextual cueing, with some studies suggesting that it is the extrahippocampal MTL region, rather than hippocampus per se, that is critical to contextual cueing (Manns & Squire, Reference Manns and Squire2001; Preston & Gabrieli, Reference Preston and Gabrieli2008). As anterior extrahippocampal MTL regions are also significantly affected in prodromal AD, particularly the entorhinal cortex (ERC) and perirhinal cortex (PRC, which includes Brodmann areas 35 and 36: BA35/BA36), we wanted to examine the relationship of contextual cuing to these regions. This is a particularly important issue to assess given that hippocampal atrophy may serve, to some extent, as a surrogate for the atrophy in these extrahippocampal regions.

In a linear regression model, each of the regions (CA1, CA2, CA3, DG, SUB, ERC, BA35, and BA36) was entered as an independent variable, adjusted for ICV, and contextual cueing (difference score on last 10 blocks) was entered as a dependent variable. As Table 2 indicates, there were significant associations between contextual cueing and hippocampal subfields in the right hemisphere, particularly, CA1, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Parahippocampal regions (ERC/PRC), on the other hand, did not show significance. These findings provide further support for the distinct role of hippocampus in contextual cueing; the involvement of CA1 and dentate gyrus is also consistent with literature from animal model studies postulating that these regions are involved in pattern separation and higher-order representation of stimuli embedded in contexts and places (Chen, Olsen, Preston, Glover, & Wagner, Reference Chen, Olsen, Preston, Glover and Wagner2011; Lee & Solivan, Reference Lee and Solivan2010; Tsien, Huerta, & Tonegawa, Reference Tsien, Huerta and Tonegawa1996)

Table 2. Associations between Contextual Cueing and MTL Subregions

Note. All the variables represent regions on the right hemisphere.

a Linear regression models adjusted for intracranial volume (ICV).

b The estimates are unstandardized beta weights.

CA=cornu ammonis; DG=dentate gyrus; SUB=subiculum; ERC=entorhinal cortex; BA=Brodmann area.

Finally, we examined the potential role of the caudate nucleus in contextual cueing as this region has been implicated in several implicit learning forms (Knowlton, Mangels, & Squire, Reference Knowlton, Mangels and Squire1996; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2000; Stillman et al., Reference Stillman, Gordon, Simon, Vaidya, Howard and Howard2013). In an analysis that controlled for ICV, we observed no significant relationship between contextual cueing and the caudate nucleus, either for the left, (estimate=0.027, SE=0.021; p=0.19) or right (estimate=.025, SE=0.016; p=0.13), hemisphere. These findings, then, provide further support for the notion that contextual cueing performance is dependent on hippocampal, but not caudate, integrity.

Discussion

The present study investigated contextual cueing in aMCI, with the main goal of examining the potential role of hippocampal volume in this form of learning. Several findings emerged. First, behavioral results did not reveal a statistically significant difference in contextual cueing between aMCI and healthy control groups, with both groups showing the cueing effect. Second, and more importantly, the contextual cueing effect was found to have a significant and linear relationship with right hippocampal volume, but not extrahippocampal MTL structures or caudate nucleus. Third, we confirmed the presence of a contextual cuing effect in an older adult population and a lack of modulation with age. Indeed, the current results specifically link the integrity of contextual cuing to hippocampal volume beyond the influence of both group status and age effects. We will discuss these findings in turn.

The lack of a statistically significant difference in contextual cuing between the groups is in contrast to our previous study in which we reported contextual cueing deficits in MCI compared to cognitively normal older adults (Negash et al., Reference Negash, Petersen, Geda, Knopman, Boeve, Smith and Petersen2007). Several factors may have contributed to the conflicting findings, including heterogeneity of MCI and the individual variability exhibited by the contextual cueing task.

As discussed in more detail below, MCI is a heterogeneous diagnostic construct and only a subset of such individuals truly have underlying AD pathology (Albert et al., Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox and Phelps2011). Likewise, cognitively normal adults are also heterogeneous in that a significant fraction of patients harbor “preclinical AD” related brain changes (Sperling et al., Reference Sperling, Aisen, Beckett, Bennett, Craft, Fagan and Phelps2011). As such, difference in results between cohorts may reflect the relative enrichment of preclinical and prodromal AD in these populations. It is certainly possible that participants in the current cohort were in a milder stage of impairment and potentially more heterogeneous with regard to etiology than our prior study. In light of this etiologic uncertainty, to the extent that experimental paradigms can be specific to the underlying pathology, such as hippocampal atrophy, they can prove useful in early detection of AD-related changes.

The observed cross-experiment differences may also have been due to variability in the contextual cueing task itself. Several studies now report wide variations in contextual cueing performance, where participants within a reasonably homogenous group show different levels of cueing effects (Bennett, Barnes, Howard, & Howard, Reference Bennett, Barnes, Howard and Howard2009; Lleras & Von Muhlenen, Reference Lleras and Von Muhlenen2004; Vaidya, Huger, Howard, & Howard, Reference Vaidya, Huger, Howard and Howard2007). As Figure 4 shows, we have also observed individual differences in the present study, where some participants within a given group (MCI or control) displayed learning effects while others did not. Furthermore, there were also participants who showed a negative or ‘reverse’ effect such that they responded faster to novel versus repeated configurations. The phenomenon of ‘reverse learning’ is puzzling and has also been observed by other studies (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Barnes, Howard and Howard2009; Lleras & Von Muhlenen, Reference Lleras and Von Muhlenen2004). It is unclear exactly what participants are learning about the regularity in this context, and more work is needed to understand the nature of learning in contextual cueing. In the present study, truncating negative scores to zero did not substantially impact the results. Nonetheless, relatively large variability of contextual cuing does reflect a limitation of the experimental paradigm.

Despite the lack of clear group difference in contextual cueing, we observed that contextual cueing was significantly associated with hippocampal volume, with lower hippocampal volume being associated with less learning. Furthermore, consistent with the lateralization of hippocampal involvement in visuospatial learning, we observed a significant association of contextual cueing for the right, but not left, hippocampus. We also examined the contributions of age and MCI status to the contextual cueing effect, and observed that right hippocampal volume was significantly associated with contextual cueing even after adjusting for these covariates. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study highlighting the potential role of hippocampal volume, even more than MCI status or age, in predicting contextual cueing performance in MCI patients and healthy controls. Finally, we explored potential unique contributions of hippocampal subfields and extrahippocampal regions, and observed a distinct role for the hippocampus proper, with CA1, dentate gyrus, and subiculum, but not extrahippocampal MTL regions, displaying significant associations with contextual cueing. It is important to note, however, that because hippocampal subfield measures are highly correlated with each other, it is difficult to disentangle the role of specific subfields in this contextual cueing performance (e.g., CA1 vs. DG).

Whether hippocampus is involved in contextual cueing has been investigated in several patient and neuroimaging studies, with mixed results. Chun and Phelps conducted the first study using the contextual cueing paradigm in amnesic patients with damage to the medial temporal lobe system, including the hippocampus, and showed cueing deficits in patients compared to age- and education-matched controls (Chun & Phelps, Reference Chun and Phelps1999). This pattern was also replicated in a study using midazolam, a drug that induces temporary anterograde amnesia (Park, Quinlan, Thornton, & Reder, Reference Park, Quinlan, Thornton and Reder2004). Manns and Squire, on the other hand, have compared amnesic patients with damage specific to hippocampal region and those with extensive damage to the MTL region, including extrahippocampal cortical regions, and found that patients with damage confined to the hippocampal region displayed contextual cueing, whereas those with more extensive MTL damage did not, and have, thus, argued that contextual cueing is not dependent on hippocampus (Manns & Squire, Reference Manns and Squire2001). Results from functional neuroimaging studies have also been mixed. Some studies report that facilitated performance to repeated configurations is associated with increased hippocampal activation (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2007), with some evidence pointing to increased activation in the right hippocampus for repetition facilitation (Manelis & Reder, Reference Manelis and Reder2012). Others argue that hippocampal activation occurs only when there is explicit recognition of repeated contexts (Preston & Gabrieli, Reference Preston and Gabrieli2008; Westerberg, Miller, Reber, Cohen, & Paller, Reference Westerberg, Miller, Reber, Cohen and Paller2011). The present study provides support for the notion that the integrity of the hippocampus proper modulates contextual cueing performance; in patients diagnosed with MCI and healthy controls, lower hippocampal volume correlated with reduced cueing effect, suggesting that the observed effects in MCI may be mediated by associated hippocampal disease.

It is important to note the apparent paradoxical result in that we observed no group differences in contextual cueing despite group differences in hippocampal volume and associations between hippocampal volume and contextual cueing. We should first note that the absolute difference between groups was larger when the analysis was restricted to participants who underwent MRI scanning; 111.69 (SD=110.4) and 58.96 (SD=121.2), for control and MCI groups, respectively, which is more consonant with the hippocampal findings. Nonetheless, this group difference in contextual cueing still did not reach significance (p=.13).

Furthermore, it is possible that the higher variance of the experimental task might limit the power to observe a group difference relative to average hippocampal volume measurements despite the correlation of these measures. As stated above, there is relatively wide variation in contextual cueing performance within reasonably homogenous groups, a limitation of the method. Finally, the lack of group effect for contextual cueing despite the relationship with hippocampal volume may have been due, in part, to the heterogeneity of the MCI patients, and even controls. While a significant proportion of MCI patients likely have an underlying prodromal AD, many do not and either have other neurodegenerative (e.g., FTD, DLB) or non-neurodegenerative (vascular disease, more robust age-associated impairment) causes of their mild memory difficulties. Indeed, as the scatterplot in Figure 4 indicates, there is considerable overlap between the hippocampal volume of MCI and controls, consistent with the notion that several the MCI group likely do not have underlying AD given the much lower risk of progression in those with more normal hippocampal volumes, which has been extensively reported in the literature (e.g., Heister, Brewer, Magda, Blennow, & McEvoy, Reference Heister, Brewer, Magda, Blennow and McEvoy2011; Jack et al., Reference Jack, Petersen, Xu, O’Brien, Smith, Ivnik and Kokmen1999). Furthermore, as several studies have shown, a substantial proportion of neuronal loss (30–50%) is required before volumetric changes are evident (Price et al., Reference Price, Ko, Wade, Tsou, McKeel and Morris2001; Rossler, Zarski, Bohl, & Ohm, Reference Rossler, Zarski, Bohl and Ohm2002); as such, it is likely that some individuals harbor neuronal loss even when volumetric loss is not apparent. Additionally, recent work has suggested that approximately one-fourth or more of controls in this age range have “preclinical AD” (Sperling et al., Reference Sperling, Aisen, Beckett, Bennett, Craft, Fagan and Phelps2011) and it is possible that this militates against the group difference on task performance due to some of these individuals beginning to develop hippocampal atrophy and/or dysfunction; nonetheless, these individuals would still support the overall relationship between hippocampal volume and the contextual cuing effect. These are important issues in the field currently, and warrant longitudinal studies.

As discussed above, most controversy around the neural bases of contextual cueing has focused on the hippocampus and related structures. However, our findings also speak to recent claims that the striatum might also be involved in contextual cueing. These claims are based on the finding that early stage Parkinson’s disease patients (van Asselen et al., Reference van Asselen, Almeida, Andre, Januario, Goncalves and Castelo-Branco2009) and prodromal and early stage Huntington’s disease patients, conditions with primary basal ganglia pathology, fail to show contextual cueing (van Asselen et al., Reference van Asselen, Almeida, Julio, Januario, Campos, Simoes and Castelo-Branco2012). The lack of correlation between caudate nucleus volume and contextual cueing in our study does not support involvement of this region, at least in the population investigated here. More research is needed to clarify this issue.

Another finding from the present study was that, more generally, the data support the notion that older adults may display evidence of intact contextual cueing effects, as the term for age in the regression model was not significant and was not related to performance. The lack of relationship with age, however, should be considered in the context of a relatively restricted range in this sample (61–84 years old). There is currently some debate as to whether contextual cueing is affected in healthy aging. Howard and colleagues compared young and healthy older adults on the standard contextual cueing task using 30 learning blocks, and showed that contextual cueing is spared in healthy aging (Howard et al., Reference Howard, Howard, Dennis, Yankovich and Vaidya2004). Recently, Smyth and Shanks sought to replicate this finding in a contextual cueing task that used only 16 blocks, and found impaired cueing deficits in older adults (Smyth & Shanks, Reference Smyth and Shanks2011). Thus, the observed age deficits in their study may have been due to insufficient training for older adults. Furthermore, it is important to note that despite this lack of effect of age per se, the relationship of age with hippocampal atrophy may be what drives any age deficits in contextual cueing reported in the literature. The present study provides support for this by showing that contextual cueing was dependent on hippocampal volume rather than age.

Finally, the significant, linear relationship between hippocampal volume and contextual cueing effect adds to the accumulating evidence that hippocampus is important for cognitive processes that require learning of spatial contexts. This linear relationship may also support the potential of this cognitive measure for detecting pathology within the hippocampus and for monitoring progression, as well as have implications for the nature of the computations performed by this structure, or specific subfields. However, while we expect the hippocampal correlations with contextual cueing are largely driven by AD-related hippocampal changes, it is possible that this relationship is non-specific; in some participants, it could be due to other factors that may be associated with hippocampal volume, such as non-AD related aging effects, variations in hippocampal development, and vascular risk factors. The relationship of contextual cueing with specific causes of hippocampal pathology is worthy of further study.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH/NIA grants P30-AG010124, K23-AG028018, and R01AG036863. The authors report no conflicting interests.

References

Albert, M.S., DeKosky, S.T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H.H., Fox, N.C., & Phelps, C.H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. [Consensus Development Conference, NIH Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Alzheimers & Dementia, 7(3), 270279. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 Google Scholar
Amaral, D., & Lavenex, P. (2007). Hippocampal neuroanatomy. In P. Andersen (Ed.), The hippocampus book (pp. 37129). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Artaechevarria, X., Munoz-Barrutia, A., & Ortiz-de-Solorzano, C. (2009). Combination strategies in multi-atlas image segmentation: Application to brain MR data. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 28(8), 12661277. doi:10.1109/TMI.2009.2014372 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avants, B.B., Epstein, C.L., Grossman, M., & Gee, J.C. (2008). Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: Evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Medical Image Analysis, 12(1), 2641. doi:10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beekly, D.L., Ramos, E.M., Lee, W.W., Deitrich, W.D., Jacka, M.E., Wu, J., & Kukull, W.A. (2007). The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) database: The Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 21(3), 249258. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e318142774e 00002093-200707000-00009 [pii].Google Scholar
Bennett, I.J., Barnes, K.A., Howard, J.H. Jr., & Howard, D.V. (2009). An abbreviated implicit spatial context learning task that yields greater learning. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 391395. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.2.391 Google Scholar
Bobinski, M., DeLeon, M.J., Tarnawski, M., Wegiel, J., Reisberg, B., Miller, D.C., & Wisniewski, H.M. (1998). Neuronal and volume loss in CA1 of the hippocampal formation uniquely predicts duration and severity of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Research, 805, 267269.Google Scholar
Burgess, N., Maguire, E.A., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]. Neuron, 35(4), 625641.Google Scholar
Chen, J., Olsen, R.K., Preston, A.R., Glover, G.H., & Wagner, A.D. (2011). Associative retrieval processes in the human medial temporal lobe: Hippocampal retrieval success and CA1 mismatch detection. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Learning & Memory, 18(8), 523528. doi:10.1101/lm.2135211 Google Scholar
Chun, M.M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 2871.Google Scholar
Chun, M.M., & Phelps, E.A. (1999). Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nature Neuroscience, 2(9), 844847.Google Scholar
Duara, R., Loewenstein, D.A., Potter, E., Appel, J., Greig, M.T., Urs, R., & Potter, H. (2008). Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI scans and the diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neurology, 71(24), 19861992. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000336925.79704.9f Google Scholar
Eldridge, L.L., Masterman, D., & Knowlton, B.J. (2002). Intact implicit habit learning in Alzheimer’s disease. [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116(4), 722726.Google Scholar
Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadan, O., Pereiro, A.X., & Lojo-Seoane, C. (2014). Working memory span in mild cognitive impairment. Influence of processing speed and cognitive reserve. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. International Psychogeriatrics, 26(4), 615625. doi:10.1017/S1041610213002391 Google Scholar
Ferraro, F.R., Balota, D.A., & Connor, L.T. (1993). Implicit memory and the formation of new associations in nondemented Parkinson’s disease individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: A serial reaction time (SRT) investigation. Brain and Cognition, 21(2), 163180.Google Scholar
Fleischman, D.A. (2007). Repetition priming in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: An integrative review and future directions. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Review]. Cortex, 43(7), 889897.Google Scholar
Fleischman, D.A., & Gabrieli, J.D. (1998). Repetition priming in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease: A review of findings and theories. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 88119.Google Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Stebbins, G.T., Singh, J., Willingham, D.B., & Goetz, C.G. (1997). Intact mirror-tracing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill learning in patients with Huntington’s disease: Evidence for dissociable memory systems in skill learning. Neuropsychology, 11(2), 272281.Google Scholar
Gong, L., Tian, Y., Cheng, H., Chen, Z., Yin, C., Meng, Y., & Wang, K. (2010). Conceptual implicit memory impaired in amnestic mild cognitive impairment patient. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuroscience Letters, 484(2), 153156. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.08.041 Google Scholar
Greene, A.J., Gross, W.L., Elsinger, C.L., & Rao, S.M. (2007). Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: An fMRI analysis of the contextual cueing task. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Learning & Memory, 14(8), 548553. doi:10.1101/lm.609807 Google Scholar
Hammers, A., Allom, R., Koepp, M.J., Free, S.L., Myers, R., Lemieux, L., & Duncan, J.S. (2003). Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Human Brain Mapping, 19(4), 224247. doi:10.1002/hbm.10123 Google Scholar
Heindel, W.C., Salmon, D.P., Shults, C.W., Walicke, P.A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients. [Comparative Study Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S. Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Journal of Neuroscience, 9(2), 582587.Google Scholar
Heister, D., Brewer, J.B., Magda, S., Blennow, K., & McEvoy, L.K. (2011). Predicting MCI outcome with clinically available MRI and CSF biomarkers. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Neurology, 77(17), 16191628. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182343314 Google Scholar
Howard, D.V., & Howard, J.H.J. (2012). Dissociable forms of implicit learning in aging. In M..Naveh-Benjamin & N. Ohta (Eds.), Memory and aging: Current issues and future directions (pp. 125151). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Howard, J.H. Jr., Howard, D.V., Dennis, N.A., Yankovich, H., & Vaidya, C.J. (2004). Implicit spatial contextual learning in healthy aging. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 124134.Google Scholar
Jack, C.R. Jr., Petersen, R.C., Xu, Y.C., O’Brien, P.C., Smith, G.E., Ivnik, R.J., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Neurology, 52(7), 13971403.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea and Feibiger.Google Scholar
Kessels, R.P., Meulenbroek, O., Fernandez, G., & Olde Rikkert, M.G. (2010). Spatial working memory in aging and mild cognitive impairment: Effects of task load and contextual cueing. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17(5), 556574. doi:10.1080/13825585.2010.481354 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B.J., Mangels, J.A., & Squire, L.R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. [Comparative Study Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S. Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Science, 273(5280), 13991402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korf, E.S., Wahlund, L.O., Visser, P.J., & Scheltens, P. (2004). Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI predicts dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neurology, 63(1), 94100.Google Scholar
LaVoie, D.J., & Faulkner, K.M. (2008). Production and identification repetition priming in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 15(4), 523544. doi:10.1080/13825580802051497 Google Scholar
Lee, I., & Solivan, F. (2010). Dentate gyrus is necessary for disambiguating similar object-place representations. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Learning & Memory, 17(5), 252258. doi:10.1101/lm.1678210 Google Scholar
Lieberman, M.D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. [Review]. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 109137.Google Scholar
Lleras, A., & Von Muhlenen, A. (2004). Spatial context and top-down strategies in visual search. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Spatial Vision, 17(4-5), 465482.Google Scholar
Lopez, O.L., Becker, J.T., Jagust, W.J., Fitzpatrick, A., Carlson, M.C., DeKosky, S.T., & Kuller, L.H. (2006). Neuropsychological characteristics of mild cognitive impairment subgroups. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77(2), 159165. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2004.045567 Google Scholar
Machado, S., Cunha, M., Minc, D., Portella, C.E., Velasques, B., Basile, L.F., & Ribeiro, P. (2009). Alzheimer’s disease and implicit memory. [Review]. Arquivos de Neuro-psiquiatria, 67(2A), 334342.Google Scholar
Manelis, A., & Reder, L.M. (2012). Procedural learning and associative memory mechanisms contribute to contextual cueing: Evidence from fMRI and eye-tracking. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Learning & Memory, 19(11), 527534. doi:10.1101/lm.025973.112 Google Scholar
Manns, J.R., & Squire, L.R. (2001). Perceptual learning, awareness and the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 11, 776782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J.C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R.C., Hughes, J.P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., & Clark, C. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39(9), 11591165.Google Scholar
Morris, J.C., Weintraub, S., Chui, H.C., Cummings, J., Decarli, C., Ferris, S., & Kukull, W.A. (2006). The Uniform Data Set (UDS): Clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from Alzheimer Disease Centers. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 20(4), 210216. doi:10.1097/01.wad.0000213865.09806.92 00002093-200610000-00007 [pii].Google Scholar
Mueller, S.G., Stables, L., Du, A.T., Schuff, N., Truran, D., Cashdollar, N., & Weiner, M.W. (2007). Measurement of hippocampal subfields and age-related changes with high resolution MRI at 4T. [Comparative Study Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(5), 719726. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.007 Google Scholar
Negash, S., Petersen, L.E., Geda, Y.E., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Smith, G.E., & Petersen, R.C. (2007). Effects of ApoE genotype and mild cognitive impairment on implicit learning. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(6), 885893.Google Scholar
Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Kiraly, K., Londe, Z., Nemeth, K., Fazekas, K., & Csanyi, A. (2013). Probabilistic sequence learning in mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 318. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00318 Google Scholar
Park, H., Quinlan, J., Thornton, E., & Reder, L.M. (2004). The effect of midazolam on visual search: Implications for understanding amnesia. [Clinical Trial Controlled Clinical Trial Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Proceedings of the Nationall Academy of Science of the United States of America, 101(51), 1787917883. doi:10.1073/pnas.0408075101 Google Scholar
Perri, R., Carlesimo, G.A., Serra, L., & Caltagirone, C. (2005). Characterization of memory profile in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 10331055. doi:10.1080/13803390490919317 Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C., & Negash, S. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment: An overview. [Review]. CNS Spectrums, 13(1), 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303308.Google Scholar
Preston, A.R., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2008). Dissociation between explicit memory and configural memory in the human medial temporal lobe. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Cerebral Cortex, 18(9), 21922207. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm245 Google Scholar
Price, J.L., Ko, A.I., Wade, M.J., Tsou, S.K., McKeel, D.W., & Morris, J.C. (2001). Neuron number in the entorhinal cortex and CA1 in preclinical Alzheimer disease. [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Archives of Neurology, 58(9), 13951402.Google Scholar
Reber, A.S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reitan, R. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain disease. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossler, M., Zarski, R., Bohl, J., & Ohm, T.G. (2002). Stage-dependent and sector-specific neuronal loss in hippocampus during Alzheimer’s disease. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Acta Neuropathologica, 103(4), 363369. doi:10.1007/s00401-001-0475-7 Google Scholar
Smith, M.L., & Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuropsychologia, 19(6), 781793.Google Scholar
Smyth, A.C., & Shanks, D.R. (2011). Aging and implicit learning: Explorations in contextual cuing. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 127132. doi:10.1037/a0022014 Google Scholar
Sperling, R.A., Aisen, P.S., Beckett, L.A., Bennett, D.A., Craft, S., Fagan, A.M., & Phelps, C.H. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers & Dementia, 7(3), 280292. doi:S1552-5260(11)00099-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003 Google Scholar
Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentatory (2nd ed.)., New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stillman, C.M., Gordon, E.M., Simon, J.R., Vaidya, C.J., Howard, D.V., & Howard, J.H. Jr. (2013). Caudate resting connectivity predicts implicit probabilistic sequence learning. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Brain Connectivity, 3(6), 601610. doi:10.1089/brain.2013.0169 Google Scholar
Tsien, J.Z., Huerta, P.T., & Tonegawa, S. (1996). The essential role of hippocampal CA1 NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in spatial memory. Cell, 87(7), 13271338.Google Scholar
Vaidya, C.J., Huger, M., Howard, D.V., & Howard, J.H. Jr. (2007). Developmental differences in implicit learning of spatial context. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Neuropsychology, 21(4), 497506. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.497 Google Scholar
van Asselen, M., Almeida, I., Andre, R., Januario, C., Goncalves, A.F., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in implicit contextual learning: A study of Parkinson’s disease. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuropsychologia, 47(5), 12691273. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Asselen, M., Almeida, I., Julio, F., Januario, C., Campos, E.B., Simoes, M., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2012). Implicit contextual learning in prodromal and early stage Huntington’s disease patients. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(4), 689696. doi:10.1017/S1355617712000288 Google Scholar
Wang, H., Das, S.R., Suh, J.W., Altinay, M., Pluta, J., Craige, C., & Yushkevich, P.A. (2011). A learning-based wrapper method to correct systematic errors in automatic image segmentation: Consistently improved performance in hippocampus, cortex and brain segmentation. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Neuroimage, 55(3), 968985. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.006 Google Scholar
Wang, H., & Yushkevich, P.A. (2013). Multi-atlas segmentation with joint label fusion and corrective learning-an open source implementation. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 7, 27. doi:10.3389/fninf.2013.00027 Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). WMS-R Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised Manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.Google Scholar
Weintraub, S., Salmon, D., Mercaldo, N., Ferris, S., Graff-Radford, N.R., Chui, H., & Morris, J.C. (2009). The Alzheimer’s Disease Centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS): The neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 23(2), 91101. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7dd 00002093-200904000-00001 [pii].Google Scholar
Wenger, M.K., Negash, S., Petersen, R.C., & Petersen, L. (2010). Modeling and estimating recall processing capacity: Sensitivity and diagnostic utility in application to mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Mathmatical Psychology, 54(1), 7389. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2009.04.012 Google Scholar
West, M.J., Coleman, P.D., Flood, D.G., & Troncoso, J.C. (1994). Differences in the pattern of hippocampal neuronal loss in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Lancet, 344(8925), 769772.Google Scholar
Westerberg, C.E., Miller, B.B., Reber, P.J., Cohen, N.J., & Paller, K.A. (2011). Neural correlates of contextual cueing are modulated by explicit learning. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 34393447. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.019 Google Scholar
Whitwell, J.L., Przybelski, S.A., Weigand, S.D., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Petersen, R.C., & Jack, C.R. Jr. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Brain, 130(Pt 7), 17771786. doi:10.1093/brain/awm112 Google Scholar
Yushkevich, P.A., Pluta, J.B., Wang, H., Xie, L., Ding, S.L., Gertje, E.C., & Wolk, D.A. (2014). Automated volumetry and regional thickness analysis of hippocampal subfields and medial temporal cortical structures in mild cognitive impairment. Human Brain Mapping, doi:10.1002/hbm.22627 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Schematic for the contextual cueing task. Participants locate the rotated target T (indicated by arrow) amongst L distractors, and indicate its orientation (in this case, rotated to the left).

Figure 1

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Figure 2

Figure 2 Mean reaction times (RTs) across epochs for Novel (solid lines) and Repeated trials (dotted lines) for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy control groups. Error bars represent one standard error.

Figure 3

Figure 3 Mean difference scores for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy control groups. Error bars represent one standard error.

Figure 4

Figure 4 Scatterplots of mean difference scores versus left hippocampal (a) and right hippocampal (b) volumes, with regression line superimposed, for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (solid circle) and control (open circle) participants.

Figure 5

Table 2. Associations between Contextual Cueing and MTL Subregions