Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:20:03.281Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does neuroticism make you old? Prospective associations between neuroticism and leukocyte telomere length

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2013

S. L. van Ockenburg*
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Center for Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands
P. de Jonge
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Center for Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands
P. van der Harst
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands
J. Ormel
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Center for Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands
J. G. M. Rosmalen
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Center for Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands
*
* Address for correspondence: S. L. van Ockenburg, M.D., Interdisciplinary Center for Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, PO Box 30.001, 9700 RB Groningen, The Netherlands. (Email: s.l.van.ockenburg@umcg.nl)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Telomere attrition, causing accelerated aging, might be one of the mechanisms through which neuroticism leads to somatic disease and increased all-cause mortality. In the current study we investigated whether neuroticism is prospectively associated with shorter telomere length (TL), a biological marker of aging.

Method

Participants were 3432 adults (mean age 52.9 years, range 32–79). Data were collected at baseline (T1) and at two follow-up visits after 4 years (T2) and 6 years (T3). Neuroticism was assessed using the 12-item neuroticism scale of the Revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R) at T2 and T3. TL was measured by a monochrome multiplex quantitative polymerase chain reaction (PCR) assay at T1, T2 and T3. A linear mixed model was used to assess whether neuroticism could predict TL prospectively after adjusting for age, sex, body mass index (BMI), frequency of sports, smoking status, presence of chronic diseases and level of education.

Results

Neuroticism was a significant negative predictor of TL at follow-up (B = −0.004, p = 0.044) after adjusting for sex, age, baseline TL and various biological and lifestyle factors.

Conclusions

High neuroticism is significantly and prospectively associated with telomere attrition independent of lifestyle and other risk factors.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Introduction

Neuroticism is considered one of the personality traits most important to public health because of its association with, and its ability to predict, various mental (Ormel et al. Reference Ormel, Rosmalen and Farmer2004) and physical disorders (Lahey, Reference Lahey2009), including cardiovascular disease (Shipley et al. Reference Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor and Deary2007). Neuroticism measures refer to individual differences in the tendency to experience negative emotions, especially when confronted with threat, frustration or loss (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992a ). Operationally, neuroticism is defined by items referring to negative affect, such as anxiety, irritability, anger, worry, self-consciousness, frustration, reactivity, vulnerability, hostility, sensitivity to criticism of others, and accompanying behavioral and cognitive traits (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992b ). It has therefore been suggested that neuroticism is a measure for a person's set point of negative affect (Ormel et al. Reference Ormel, Rosmalen and Farmer2004). Moreover, neuroticism scores prospectively predict person-dependent stressful life events (i.e. adversities that a person might have brought upon themselves) and chronic adversity (Poulton & Andrews, Reference Poulton and Andrews1992).

Several studies have evaluated the potential role of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis in explaining the association of neuroticism with adverse health outcomes. Unfortunately, the results of investigations of the relationship between neuroticism and the HPA axis are inconsistent, with some studies reporting a positive relationship (Nater et al. Reference Nater, Hoppmann and Klumb2010; Madsen et al. Reference Madsen, Jernigan, Iversen, Frokjaer, Mortensen, Knudsen and Baare2012), some negative (Mangold et al. Reference Mangold, Mintz, Javors and Marino2012; Pineles et al. Reference Pineles, Rasmusson, Yehuda, Lasko, Macklin, Pitman and Orr2012) and others no relationship at all (Riese et al. Reference Riese, Rijsdijk, Rosmalen, Snieder and Ormel2009; Tabak & McCullough, Reference Tabak and McCullough2011; van Santen et al. Reference van Santen, Vreeburg, Van der Does, Spinhoven, Zitman and Penninx2011). Therefore, the mechanisms by which neuroticism may affect somatic health have yet to be elucidated. An interesting perspective is provided by findings that psychosocial stress (e.g. childhood adversities, caregivers stress) is associated with shorter telomere length (TL) both cross-sectionally (Epel et al. Reference Epel, Blackburn, Lin, Dhabhar, Adler, Morrow and Cawthon2004; Damjanovic et al. Reference Damjanovic, Yang, Glaser, Kiecolt-Glaser, Nguyen, Laskowski, Zou, Beversdorf and Weng2007; Kananen et al. Reference Kananen, Surakka, Pirkola, Suvisaari, Lonnqvist, Peltonen, Ripatti and Hovatta2010; Drury et al. Reference Drury, Theall, Gleason, Smyke, De Vivo, Wong, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2012) and prospectively (Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Moffitt, Sugden, Williams, Houts, Danese, Mill, Arseneault and Caspi2012), as shortening of telomeres is related to the process of cellular aging (Blasco, Reference Blasco2007). Moreover, a large cross-sectional study investigating the relationship between trait hostility and TL found telomeres to be significantly shorter in high-hostile men (Brydon et al. Reference Brydon, Lin, Butcher, Hamer, Erusalimsky, Blackburn and Steptoe2011). Telomeres are specialized chromatine structures that ‘cap’ the ends of chromosomes in eukaryotic cells. Telomeres prevent chromosome ends from being recognized as double-stranded DNA breaks, promoting chromosomal stability (Chan & Blackburn, Reference Chan and Blackburn2002). In addition, they play an important role in regulating the replicative lifespan of cells (Collado et al. Reference Collado, Blasco and Serrano2007) and in stem cell mobility (Flores et al. Reference Flores, Cayuela and Blasco2005). DNA polymerases cannot copy the end of chromosomes and a particular enzyme, telomerase, is needed to add telomere repeats during cell division. However, in somatic cells, only limited amounts of telomerase are present, thus telomeres shorten progressively with each cell division (Harley et al. Reference Harley, Futcher and Greider1990). Short telomeres are predictive of increased mortality rates (Cawthon et al. Reference Cawthon, Smith, O'Brien, Sivatchenko and Kerber2003; Honig et al. Reference Honig, Schupf, Lee, Tang and Mayeux2006) and increased incidence of various age-related diseases, such as cancer (Willeit et al. Reference Willeit, Willeit, Mayr, Weger, Oberhollenzer, Brandstatter, Kronenberg and Kiechl2010) and Alzheimer's disease (Honig et al. Reference Honig, Schupf, Lee, Tang and Mayeux2006).

As various forms of psychosocial stress are associated with shorter telomeres (Epel et al. Reference Epel, Blackburn, Lin, Dhabhar, Adler, Morrow and Cawthon2004; Damjanovic et al. Reference Damjanovic, Yang, Glaser, Kiecolt-Glaser, Nguyen, Laskowski, Zou, Beversdorf and Weng2007; Kananen et al. Reference Kananen, Surakka, Pirkola, Suvisaari, Lonnqvist, Peltonen, Ripatti and Hovatta2010; Tyrka et al. Reference Tyrka, Price, Kao, Porton, Marsella and Carpenter2010; Drury et al. Reference Drury, Theall, Gleason, Smyke, De Vivo, Wong, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2012) and neuroticism scores prospectively predict exposure to person-dependent stressful life events and chronic adversity (Poulton & Andrews, Reference Poulton and Andrews1992), accelerated telomere shortening might be one of the mechanisms through which neuroticism leads to somatic disease. A longitudinal study might be able to provide insight into the sequence of events and make more accurate statements about the direction of causality.

The aim of the present study was to prospectively test the effect of neuroticism on TL in a large population-based cohort. We hypothesized that higher scores on neuroticism would be associated with shorter TL as neuroticism is a predictor of a person's habitual level of distress, exposure to stressful life events and interpersonal difficulties. This hypothesis has not yet been tested. If it were true, it could mean an important step forward in our understanding of the relationship between neuroticism and somatic disease and longevity.

Method

Study population

Our study was performed in a cohort derived from the Prevention of REnal and Vascular ENd stage Disease (PREVEND) study, a population cohort study originally designed to investigate microalbuminuria as a risk factor for renal and cardiovascular disease. The recruitment of participants to the PREVEND study has been described extensively elsewhere (Pinto-Sietsma et al. Reference Pinto-Sietsma, Janssen, Hillege, Navis, de Zeeuw and de Jong2000). In brief, all inhabitants of the city of Groningen between the ages of 28 and 75 years (85 421 subjects) were asked to send in a morning urine sample and to fill out a short questionnaire on demographics and cardiovascular history. A total of 40 856 subjects (47.8%) responded. After exclusion of subjects with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus and pregnant women, all subjects with an elevated urinary albumin concentration (UAC) of ⩾10 mg/l (n = 7768), together with a randomly selected control group with a UAC of < 10 mg/l (n = 3395), were invited for further investigations (total n = 11 163). Finally, 8592 subjects completed the total screening program, providing the PREVEND study cohort. However, the PREVEND study is enriched for participants with higher albuminuria levels, a risk factor for developing renal disease, and we wanted to study a cohort that was a representative sample of the Groningen population and not at heightened risk for any specific disease. To that purpose we took all subjects with a UAC < 10 mg/l who had completed the first screening (n = 2592) and added a subset of the ‘oversampled’ subjects with a UAC > 10 mg/l by proportionally taking an SPSS-generated random subset (n = 840). This resulted in a group of 3432 subjects with a population representative ratio of albuminuria negative and positive subjects forming the basis for the current study. Three waves of data were available for this study: the baseline screening was completed in 1998 (T1), followed by two follow-up visits at 4.2 (T2) and 6.4 (T3) years from baseline. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee for Human Research of the University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG). All participants were aged ⩾18 years and provided written informed consent for participation in the study.

Neuroticism

Participants completed the Dutch translation of the 12-item neuroticism scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire – Revised Short Scale (EPQ-RSS-N; Sanderman et al. Reference Sanderman, Eysenck and Arrindell1991) at home prior to their visit to our research facilities at T2 and T3. The EPQ-RSS-N comprises 12 questions, representing nervousness, emotional lability, feelings of guilt and low self-esteem, in a ‘yes/no’ format. For each participant, a sum score was constructed by adding the questions answered in the affirmative. The sum score, therefore, represents the total number of neuroticism symptoms reported. Missing data were imputed according to the corrected item mean substitution (CIMS) method if at least half of the items were completed (Huisman, Reference Huisman2000). For the EPQ-RSS-N sum score, of the 135 participants who had at least one missing item, 12 were imputed, resulting in 2721 valid EPQ-RSSN sum scores (95.7% of the study sample at T2). The EPQ-RSS-N exceeded the criterion for acceptable instrument internal consistency reliability of ⩾0.70 (Kline, Reference Kline2000). The psychometric characteristics of the EPQ-RSS-N were as follows: Cronbach's α = 0.86; mean inter-item correlation, 0.35; range of item–rest correlations, 0.43–0.64. The test–retest coefficient for the EPQ-RSS-N sum score in this population was 0.73, the average test–retest interval 2.4 years.

Covariates

Covariates were selected for their known association with neuroticism or TL: body mass index (BMI) (Valdes et al. Reference Valdes, Andrew, Gardner, Kimura, Oelsner, Cherkas, Aviv and Spector2005), smoking (none, 1–5, 6–10, 11–15, 16–20, >20 cigarettes/day) (Valdes et al. Reference Valdes, Andrew, Gardner, Kimura, Oelsner, Cherkas, Aviv and Spector2005), frequency of sports (I don't exercise, once a week, at least twice a week) (Du et al. Reference Du, Prescott, Kraft, Han, Giovannucci, Hankinson and De Vivo2012), presence of a chronic disease [coronary heart disease (CHD), cerebrovascular accident (CVA), diabetes mellitus, chronic liver disease, chronic kidney disease, malignancy, rheumatoid arthritis, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) or asthma, severe skin disease, severe bowel disease lasting > 3 months] and level of education (none, low, middle, high) (Steptoe et al. Reference Steptoe, Hamer, Butcher, Lin, Brydon, Kivimaki, Marmot, Blackburn and Erusalimsky2011). The somatic diseases, except for diabetes, CHD and CVA, were self-reported diseases that were present in the previous year. Diabetes was defined as the use of antidiabetic treatment according to self-report or pharmacy data. CHD and CVA were defined as self-report of CHD/CVA upon inclusion in the study and/or confirmed occurrence of CHD/CVA between inclusion and date of visit to the research facilities at T2. Low educational level was defined as lower secondary education or less, middle educational level was defined as higher secondary education, and high educational level was defined as tertiary education.

TL

Fasting blood samples were collected from all participants by a nurse during a visit to the research facilities. In case of influenza or a febrile temperature, blood collection was postponed to a later time. TL in the PREVEND cohort was measured in leukocytes at T1, T2 and T3 by a monochrome multiplex quantitative polymerase chain reaction (PCR) method, whereby telomere specific amplification and the reference gene amplification take place in a single reaction well (Cawthon, Reference Cawthon2009). All samples were measured in triplicate and the average of the three runs was used to provide the mean relative measure of TL for each individual. The mean telomere repeat sequence copy number (T) was compared to a reference single copy gene copy number (S) in each sample. T/S = 1 when the unknown DNA is identical to the reference DNA in its ratio of telomere repeat sequence copy number to single copy gene copy number. The calibrator sample used was made up of a mixture of DNAs from young adult individuals (age around 25 years). The intra-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) were 2% (T), 1.9% (S) and 4.5% (T/S ratio). Reproducibility data were obtained for 216 subjects from PREVEND and good agreement between the T/S ratios was observed (R 2 = 0.99, p < 0.0001, inter-run CV 3.9%). There was a highly significant decline in the T/S ratio with age in PREVEND [−0.0047 (s.e. = 0.0004) decrease in the T/S ratio per year increase in age (p < 0.0001), confirming the internal validity of the assay. TL was available for 3209 participants at T1 and for 2298 at T3. Unfortunately, DNA and thus TL at T2 was only available for a subset of the population (n = 1236)].

Statistical analyses

We used a linear mixed model to account for the non-independence of observations (repeated measurements were nested within individuals). The model contained TL at T2 and T3 as the dependent variable and neuroticism (at T2 and T3) as a time-varying predictor. The model included as covariates gender, BMI, smoking, frequency of sports, presence of a chronic disease, level of education, TL at baseline (T1) and time in years between measurement occasions. A composite specification of the model is given by:

(1) $$\eqalign{{\rm TL}_{ij} = & {\rm \gamma} _{00} + {\rm \gamma} _{01} {\rm TL}_i + {\rm \gamma} _{02} {\rm Neuroticism}_{ij} + {\rm \gamma} _{03} {\rm Age}_i \cr & + {\rm \gamma} _{04} {\rm Gender}_i + {\rm \gamma} _{05} {\rm Presence \ of \ a \ chronic \ disease}_i \cr & + {\rm \gamma} _{06} {\rm Education}_i + {\rm \gamma} _{07} {\rm Smoking}_{ij} + {\rm \gamma} _{08} {\rm Sports}_{ij} \cr & + {\rm \gamma} _{09} {\rm BMI}_{ij} + {\rm \gamma} _{10} {\rm Time}_{ij} + (\varepsilon _{ij} + \zeta 0_i )} $$

In this notation TL ij denotes the length of the telomeres for individual i at measurement occasion j; γ 00 denotes the intercept; γ 02 denotes the average effect of the sum score of neuroticism symptoms at T2 and T3 on telomere length (T2, T3), adjusted for telomere length at T1 (γ 01). The fixed effects of the other covariates age, gender, presence of a chronic disease, education, smoking, sport, BMI and time are denoted as γ 03, γ 04, γ 05, γ 06, γ 07, γ 08, γ 09, γ 10, respectively. The residuals at the level of within-person observations are denoted by εij . ζ0 i denotes the random intercept variance. The maximum likelihood method was used for model estimation. The distribution of TL was checked for normality. As it had a slight positive skew, TL was naturally log transformed to meet the assumption. For each model we checked whether the associations were linear, quadratic or cubic. The results were considered statistically significant for a two-sided p value < 0.05. All models were analyzed using the nlme package (Pinheiro et al. Reference Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy and Sarkar2012) in R, version 2.15.2 (R Core Team, 2012).

Results

Study population

Descriptive statistics for our study population are provided in Table 1. During the PREVEND study at T2 and T3, 540 and 1028 participants respectively did not attend the follow-up visit. Some of the participants, however, who were not present at the follow-up at T2 did attend the follow-up visit at T3 and vice versa. A total of 572 (17%) did not attend any of the follow-up visits and had only baseline data available. It therefore becomes clear that, like any longitudinal study, PREVEND suffered from attrition. We therefore investigated the pattern of missingness. We assumed data were missing at random (MAR) as missingness depended on the observed variables (Graham, Reference Graham2009). Attrition was related to heavier smoking, drinking more alcohol, having a higher BMI, exercising less, being lower educated, being more neurotic, and having shorter telomeres.

Table 1. General characteristics of the study population at T2

T2, Follow-up visit after 4 years; BMI, Body mass index; IQR, interquartile range; s.d., standard deviation.

Previous longitudinal studies investigating TL have reported the possibility of both telomere attrition and telomere lengthening (Aviv et al. Reference Aviv, Chen, Gardner, Kimura, Brimacombe, Cao, Srinivasan and Berenson2009; Nordfjall et al. Reference Nordfjall, Svenson, Norrback, Adolfsson, Lenner and Roos2009; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Kimura, Kim, Cao, Srinivasan, Berenson, Kark and Aviv2011; Svenson et al. Reference Svenson, Nordfjall, Baird, Roger, Osterman, Hellenius and Roos2011). Most studies defined attrition as a decrease in TL > 15% and lengthening as an increase in TL > 15% between baseline and follow-up measures. We investigated the dynamics of TL in our cohort using these definitions. Over an average time of 6.5 years between baseline and follow-up, 65.2% showed a decrease in TL, 6.9% remained stable and 27.9% showed a lengthening of telomeres. This shows that TL is highly dynamic.

Neuroticism and TL

The results of the fully adjusted analysis are presented in Table 2. In a random intercept model adjusted only for gender, age, time and baseline TL, the sum score of neuroticism symptoms predicted a significant decrease in TL (coefficient = −0.005, s.e.  = 0.002, p = 0.008). In our second and final model, we added BMI, smoking, frequency of sports, the presence of chronic diseases and education. The sum score of neuroticism symptoms remained a significant predictor of telomere attrition (coefficient = −0.004, s.e.  = 0.002, p = 0.044). The coefficient of the sum score of neuroticism symptoms decreased only slightly, indicating that, although lifestyle factors explain a portion of the variance, neuroticism also still explains a unique portion of the variance in telomere attrition independent of lifestyle factors. Likewise, age was a significant predictor of telomere attrition. Furthermore, there was a non-significant trend of higher education being associated with telomere elongation. Surprisingly, smoking status, gender and the presence of a chronic disease did not predict changes in TL.

Table 2. Mixed model predicting telomere length (TL) at T2 and T3 by the sum score of neuroticism symptoms, adjusting for baseline TL (n = 2156)

s.e., standard error.

Intercept and random intercept not shown.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first prospective large population-based study showing that neuroticism is significantly associated with shorter TL over time, independent of BMI, frequency of sports, smoking, baseline TL, presence of a chronic disease, and level of education. Neuroticism can be viewed as a person's habitual level of distress and predicts exposure to psychosocial stress, in particular stressful life events and chronic difficulties (Ormel et al. Reference Ormel, Rosmalen and Farmer2004). Therefore, our results are in agreement with previous cross-sectional (Epel et al. Reference Epel, Blackburn, Lin, Dhabhar, Adler, Morrow and Cawthon2004; Damjanovic et al. Reference Damjanovic, Yang, Glaser, Kiecolt-Glaser, Nguyen, Laskowski, Zou, Beversdorf and Weng2007; Kananen et al. Reference Kananen, Surakka, Pirkola, Suvisaari, Lonnqvist, Peltonen, Ripatti and Hovatta2010; Tyrka et al. Reference Tyrka, Price, Kao, Porton, Marsella and Carpenter2010; Drury et al. Reference Drury, Theall, Gleason, Smyke, De Vivo, Wong, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2012; Wikgren et al. Reference Wikgren, Maripuu, Karlsson, Nordfjall, Bergdahl, Hultdin, Del-Favero, Roos, Nilsson, Adolfsson and Norrback2012) and prospective studies (Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Moffitt, Sugden, Williams, Houts, Danese, Mill, Arseneault and Caspi2012) demonstrating an association between psychosocial stress and decreased TL. Our results are also in concordance with two cross-sectional studies linking personality traits, hostility (Brydon et al. Reference Brydon, Lin, Butcher, Hamer, Erusalimsky, Blackburn and Steptoe2011) and pessimism (O'Donovan et al. Reference O'Donovan, Lin, Dhabhar, Wolkowitz, Tillie, Blackburn and Epel2009), to shorter TL. As mentioned earlier, both neuroticism (Shipley et al. Reference Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor and Deary2007; Mroczek et al. Reference Mroczek, Spiro and Turiano2009) and telomere attrition (Cawthon et al. Reference Cawthon, Smith, O'Brien, Sivatchenko and Kerber2003; Honig et al. Reference Honig, Schupf, Lee, Tang and Mayeux2006) are associated with increased all-cause mortality and various diseases of aging. Two mutually non-exclusive explanations for the entanglement of neuroticism, TL and somatic diseases and increased mortality can be offered. One possibility is that telomere shortening is part of the mechanism by which neuroticism leads to increased mortality rates through, for instance, chromosomal instability (Rudolph et al. Reference Rudolph, Millard, Bosenberg and Depinho2001) and cell senescence (Collado et al. Reference Collado, Blasco and Serrano2007). An alternative explanation, however, could be that, for a large part, decreased TL and increased mortality and neuroticism are the result of exposure to other shared risk factors, such as the amount of lifetime oxidative stress exposure and genetic vulnerability for psychosocial stress. Glucocorticoids, stress hormones released from the adrenal gland under conditions of psychosocial stress (Dickerson & Kemeny, Reference Dickerson and Kemeny2004), have been shown to increase damage by oxidative stress in neurons (McIntosh & Sapolsky, Reference McIntosh and Sapolsky1996; McIntosh et al. Reference McIntosh, Hong and Sapolsky1998). Because high neuroticism scores are prospectively associated with exposure to more psychosocial stress (Poulton & Andrews, Reference Poulton and Andrews1992), increased glucocorticoid exposure might be a mediating factor, explaining both telomere attrition and the increased morbidity and mortality that are associated with neuroticism. The results of a recently published study, showing that a hypocortisolemic state was associated with shorter TL in both patients with recurrent depression and healthy controls (Wikgren et al. Reference Wikgren, Maripuu, Karlsson, Nordfjall, Bergdahl, Hultdin, Del-Favero, Roos, Nilsson, Adolfsson and Norrback2012), contradicts the above stated hypothesis. We need to bear in mind, however, that this study was cross-sectional in nature, and therefore could not exclude the possibility that hypocortisolemia might be the end result of a repeatedly overstressed and finally exhausted HPA axis as suggested by Fries et al. (Reference Fries, Hesse, Hellhammer and Hellhammer2005).

There are several strengths and limitations of the current study that need to be taken into consideration when interpreting our results. The first major strength of this study is that it was conducted in a large population representative cohort, increasing the generalizability of our findings. It should be mentioned, however, that our population consisted mainly of white people and the results of our study cannot therefore be generalized to people with other racial or ethnic backgrounds. The second strength is the prospective nature of the design, allowing us to model telomere attrition over time. This is the first prospective study demonstrating a significant relationship between a personality trait and telomere attrition. All other studies to date have been cross-sectional, and thus have not provided insight into the sequence of events (Damjanovic et al. Reference Damjanovic, Yang, Glaser, Kiecolt-Glaser, Nguyen, Laskowski, Zou, Beversdorf and Weng2007; Brydon et al. Reference Brydon, Lin, Butcher, Hamer, Erusalimsky, Blackburn and Steptoe2011). One limitation of our study is that we measured TL by monochrome multiplex quantitative PCR. This makes it difficult to compare our findings to those of other cohorts as the results of the PCR are given in the form of a ratio and not in absolute kilobase pairs. This is, however, a commonly accepted and reliable method for measuring TL (Cawthon, Reference Cawthon2009) and our assay has good internal validity. A second limitation is that, like any longitudinal study, the PREVEND study suffered from attrition and no-show at some of the scheduled follow-up visits. The participants who dropped out of the study or missed a visit had significantly shorter TL, significantly higher scores of neuroticism at baseline, and a significantly unhealthier lifestyle than participants who had not dropped out. These attrition-associated differences will lead to either under- or overestimation of the true effect of neuroticism on TL, as the most extreme cases for most variables have ceased to participate in the study. Likelihood-based methods can, however, provide reliable estimates when the MAR assumption holds, as was the case in our study (Kenward & Molenberghs, Reference Kenward and Molenberghs1998). Finally, the observational design of our study does not permit us to draw any conclusions about causality.

In conclusion, this is the first study demonstrating that neuroticism is prospectively associated with telomere attrition. Future studies could investigate whether increased glucocorticoid levels caused by repeated stress exposure is one of the mechanisms through which neuroticism exerts its negative effects on TL and physical and mental health.

Acknowledgments

We thank all of those who participated in the PREVEND study. This study was supported by a grant from the Innovational Research Incentives Scheme Program of The Netherlands (NWO, VENI, P. van der Harst, grant no. 916.76.170).

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Aviv, A, Chen, W, Gardner, JP, Kimura, M, Brimacombe, M, Cao, X, Srinivasan, SR, Berenson, GS (2009). Leukocyte telomere dynamics: longitudinal findings among young adults in the Bogalusa Heart Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 169, 323329.Google Scholar
Blasco, MA (2007). Telomere length, stem cells and aging. Nature Chemical Biology 3, 640649.Google Scholar
Brydon, L, Lin, J, Butcher, L, Hamer, M, Erusalimsky, JD, Blackburn, EH, Steptoe, A (2011). Hostility and cellular aging in men from the Whitehall II cohort. Biological Psychiatry 71, 767773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cawthon, RM (2009). Telomere length measurement by a novel monochrome multiplex quantitative PCR method. Nucleic Acids Research 37, e21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cawthon, RM, Smith, KR, O'Brien, E, Sivatchenko, A, Kerber, RA (2003). Association between telomere length in blood and mortality in people aged 60 years or older. Lancet 361, 393395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, SW, Blackburn, EH (2002). New ways not to make ends meet: telomerase, DNA damage proteins and heterochromatin. Oncogene 21, 553563.Google Scholar
Chen, W, Kimura, M, Kim, S, Cao, X, Srinivasan, SR, Berenson, GS, Kark, JD, Aviv, A (2011). Longitudinal versus cross-sectional evaluations of leukocyte telomere length dynamics: age-dependent telomere shortening is the rule. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 66, 312319.Google Scholar
Collado, M, Blasco, MA, Serrano, M (2007). Cellular senescence in cancer and aging. Cell 130, 223233.Google Scholar
Costa, PT Jr., McCrae, RR (1992 a). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences 13, 653665.Google Scholar
Costa, PT, McCrae, RR (1992 b). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.: Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
Damjanovic, AK, Yang, Y, Glaser, R, Kiecolt-Glaser, JK, Nguyen, H, Laskowski, B, Zou, Y, Beversdorf, DQ, Weng, NP (2007). Accelerated telomere erosion is associated with a declining immune function of caregivers of Alzheimer's disease patients. Journal of Immunology 179, 42494254.Google Scholar
Dickerson, SS, Kemeny, ME (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin 130, 355391.Google Scholar
Drury, SS, Theall, K, Gleason, MM, Smyke, AT, De Vivo, I, Wong, JY, Fox, NA, Zeanah, CH, Nelson, CA (2012). Telomere length and early severe social deprivation: linking early adversity and cellular aging. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 719727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du, M, Prescott, J, Kraft, P, Han, J, Giovannucci, E, Hankinson, SE, De Vivo, I (2012). Physical activity, sedentary behavior, and leukocyte telomere length in women. American Journal of Epidemiology 175, 414422.Google Scholar
Epel, ES, Blackburn, EH, Lin, J, Dhabhar, FS, Adler, NE, Morrow, JD, Cawthon, RM (2004). Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 101, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flores, I, Cayuela, ML, Blasco, MA (2005). Effects of telomerase and telomere length on epidermal stem cell behavior. Science 309, 12531256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, E, Hesse, J, Hellhammer, J, Hellhammer, DH (2005). A new view on hypocortisolism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 10101016.Google Scholar
Graham, JW (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology 60, 549576.Google Scholar
Harley, CB, Futcher, AB, Greider, CW (1990). Telomeres shorten during ageing of human fibroblasts. Nature 345, 458460.Google Scholar
Honig, LS, Schupf, N, Lee, JH, Tang, MX, Mayeux, R (2006). Shorter telomeres are associated with mortality in those with APOE epsilon4 and dementia. Annals of Neurology 60, 181187.Google Scholar
Huisman, M (2000). Imputation of missing item responses: some simple techniques. Quality and Quantity 34, 331351.Google Scholar
Kananen, L, Surakka, I, Pirkola, S, Suvisaari, J, Lonnqvist, J, Peltonen, L, Ripatti, S, Hovatta, I (2010). Childhood adversities are associated with shorter telomere length at adult age both in individuals with an anxiety disorder and controls. PloS One 5, e10826.Google Scholar
Kenward, MG, Molenberghs, G (1998). Likelihood based frequentist inference when data are missing at random. Statistical Science 13, 236247.Google Scholar
Kline, P (2000). A Psychometrics Primer. Free Association Books: London.Google Scholar
Lahey, BB (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist 64, 241256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madsen, KS, Jernigan, TL, Iversen, P, Frokjaer, VG, Mortensen, EL, Knudsen, GM, Baare, WF (2012). Cortisol awakening response and negative emotionality linked to asymmetry in major limbic fibre bundle architecture. Psychiatry Research 201, 6372.Google Scholar
Mangold, D, Mintz, J, Javors, M, Marino, E (2012). Neuroticism, acculturation and the cortisol awakening response in Mexican American adults. Hormones and Behavior 61, 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, LJ, Hong, KE, Sapolsky, RM (1998). Glucocorticoids may alter antioxidant enzyme capacity in the brain: baseline studies. Brain Research 791, 209214.Google Scholar
McIntosh, LJ, Sapolsky, RM (1996). Glucocorticoids may enhance oxygen radical-mediated neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicology 17, 873882.Google Scholar
Mroczek, DK, Spiro, A, Turiano, N (2009). Do health behaviors explain the effect of neuroticism on mortality? Longitudinal findings from the VA Normative Aging Study. Journal of Research in Personality 43, 653659.Google Scholar
Nater, UM, Hoppmann, C, Klumb, PL (2010). Neuroticism and conscientiousness are associated with cortisol diurnal profiles in adults – role of positive and negative affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 15731577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordfjall, K, Svenson, U, Norrback, KF, Adolfsson, R, Lenner, P, Roos, G (2009). The individual blood cell telomere attrition rate is telomere length dependent. PLoS Genetics 5, e1000375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Donovan, A, Lin, J, Dhabhar, FS, Wolkowitz, O, Tillie, JM, Blackburn, E, Epel, E (2009). Pessimism correlates with leukocyte telomere shortness and elevated interleukin-6 in post-menopausal women. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 23, 446449.Google Scholar
Ormel, J, Rosmalen, J, Farmer, A (2004). Neuroticism: a non-informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 39, 906912.Google Scholar
Pineles, SL, Rasmusson, AM, Yehuda, R, Lasko, NB, Macklin, ML, Pitman, RK, Orr, SP (2012). Predicting emotional responses to potentially traumatic events from pre-exposure waking cortisol levels: a longitudinal study of police and firefighters. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping 26, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinheiro, J, Bates, D, DebRoy, S, Sarkar, D, and the R Development Core Team (2012). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1105.Google Scholar
Pinto-Sietsma, SJ, Janssen, WM, Hillege, HL, Navis, G, de Zeeuw, D, de Jong, PE (2000). Urinary albumin excretion is associated with renal functional abnormalities in a nondiabetic population. Journal of the American Society of Nephrology 11, 18821888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulton, RG, Andrews, G (1992). Personality as a cause of adverse life events. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 85, 3538.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2012). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria (http://www.R-project.org/).Google Scholar
Riese, H, Rijsdijk, FV, Rosmalen, JG, Snieder, H, Ormel, J (2009). Neuroticism and morning cortisol secretion: both heritable, but no shared genetic influences. Journal of Personality 77, 15611575.Google Scholar
Rudolph, KL, Millard, M, Bosenberg, MW, Depinho, RA (2001). Telomere dysfunction and evolution of intestinal carcinoma in mice and humans. Nature Genetics 28, 155159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanderman, R, Eysenck, SBG, Arrindell, WA (1991). Cross-cultural comparisons of personality – the Netherlands and England. Psychological Reports 69, 10911096.Google Scholar
Shalev, I, Moffitt, TE, Sugden, K, Williams, B, Houts, RM, Danese, A, Mill, J, Arseneault, L, Caspi, A (2012). Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: a longitudinal study. Molecular Psychiatry 18, 576581.Google Scholar
Shipley, BA, Weiss, A, Der, G, Taylor, MD, Deary, IJ (2007). Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: a 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine 69, 923931.Google Scholar
Steptoe, A, Hamer, M, Butcher, L, Lin, J, Brydon, L, Kivimaki, M, Marmot, M, Blackburn, E, Erusalimsky, JD (2011). Educational attainment but not measures of current socioeconomic circumstances are associated with leukocyte telomere length in healthy older men and women. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 25, 12921298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, U, Nordfjall, K, Baird, D, Roger, L, Osterman, P, Hellenius, ML, Roos, G (2011). Blood cell telomere length is a dynamic feature. PloS One 6, e21485.Google Scholar
Tabak, BA, McCullough, ME (2011). Perceived transgressor agreeableness decreases cortisol response and increases forgiveness following recent interpersonal transgressions. Biological Psychology 87, 386392.Google Scholar
Tyrka, AR, Price, LH, Kao, HT, Porton, B, Marsella, SA, Carpenter, LL (2010). Childhood maltreatment and telomere shortening: preliminary support for an effect of early stress on cellular aging. Biological Psychiatry 67, 531534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdes, AM, Andrew, T, Gardner, JP, Kimura, M, Oelsner, E, Cherkas, LF, Aviv, A, Spector, TD (2005). Obesity, cigarette smoking, and telomere length in women. Lancet 366, 662664.Google Scholar
van Santen, A, Vreeburg, SA, Van der Does, AJ, Spinhoven, P, Zitman, FG, Penninx, BW (2011). Psychological traits and the cortisol awakening response: results from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 240248.Google Scholar
Wikgren, M, Maripuu, M, Karlsson, T, Nordfjall, K, Bergdahl, J, Hultdin, J, Del-Favero, J, Roos, G, Nilsson, LG, Adolfsson, R, Norrback, KF (2012). Short telomeres in depression and the general population are associated with a hypocortisolemic state. Biological Psychiatry 71, 294300.Google Scholar
Willeit, P, Willeit, J, Mayr, A, Weger, S, Oberhollenzer, F, Brandstatter, A, Kronenberg, F, Kiechl, S (2010). Telomere length and risk of incident cancer and cancer mortality. Journal of the American Medical Association 304, 6975.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. General characteristics of the study population at T2

Figure 1

Table 2. Mixed model predicting telomere length (TL) at T2 and T3 by the sum score of neuroticism symptoms, adjusting for baseline TL (n = 2156)