Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T02:11:17.002Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cinderella indeed – a commentary on Iverson's ‘Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development’*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2010

KAREN E. ADOLPH*
Affiliation:
New York University
CATHERINE S. TAMIS-LEMONDA
Affiliation:
New York University
LANA B. KARASIK
Affiliation:
New York University
*
Address for correspondence: Karen E. Adolph, Department of Psychology, New York University, 4 Washington Place, Room 410, New York, NY 10003. Email: karen.adolph@nyu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Review Article and Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

As Iverson (this issue) points out, it is ironic that motor development has been relegated to peripheral ‘Cinderella’ status in developmental psychology. After all, for those researchers who view psychology as the study of behavior, motor development is the stuff of the science. All behaviors – walking, talking, looking, laughing, reaching, playing, sleeping, breathing – are motor behaviors. And for those researchers who view psychology as the study of mind, motor behaviors – looking times and reaction times, vocalizations and gestures, avoidance responses and proximity seeking – provide the basis for inferences about infants' percepts, thoughts and feelings.

Now, at long last, motor development, the neglected and wretched Cinderella of developmental psychology, has been rescued from the dustbin and invited to the language ball. As researchers in both motor (Adolph and Karasik) and language (Tamis-LeMonda) development, how do we feel about this? On the one hand, we embrace Iverson's proposal that developmental changes in infants' motor skills can affect developmental changes in linguistic, cognitive and social aspects of language acquisition. As one of many examples cited by Iverson, the transition from crawling to walking is associated with qualitative changes in the ways that infants share objects with their mothers, which in turn creates new opportunities for social interactions (Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda & Adolph, under review). We also find that infant motor development and expertise shape both the language and gestures mothers use to encourage and discourage infant actions in situations of potential risk (Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph & Dimitropoulou, Reference Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph and Dimitropoulou2008), and infant exploratory and play actions are key behaviors to which mothers verbally respond during everyday interactions (Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Hahn & Haynes, Reference Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Hahn and Haynes2008; Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, Reference Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein2002). Clearly, infants' motor actions and development are a driving force in their social development and interactions.

On the other hand, we feel that Iverson's proposal only captures a small piece of the story – one in which motor development is viewed as a ‘key participant’, as Iverson puts it, in the service of the true princesses of psychology – language, cognition, perception and social interaction. An alternative approach is to recast motor development in the leading role, with perception, cognition and social development playing supporting roles. Perception is a necessary condition for adaptive motor control; motor actions must be selected, modified, discovered and constructed to suit the constraints and demands of the current situation (Adolph & Berger, Reference Adolph, Berger, Kuhn and Siegler2006; Gibson & Pick, Reference Gibson and Pick2000). And, more germane to Iverson's arguments, language and social interaction can also be key participants.

Our own research illustrates the participatory role of social cognition in infant motor action. Inspired by Sorce, Emde, Campos & Klinnert's (Reference Sorce, Emde, Campos and Klinnert1985) classic study of social referencing on the visual cliff, we asked whether infants use social information from their mothers when deciding whether to descend safe and risky slopes (0°–50°). On some trials, mothers encouraged their infants to crawl or walk down the slopes, and on other trials, mothers discouraged descent (Karasik et al., Reference Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph and Dimitropoulou2008). If infants ignored the social information offered by their mothers, motor decisions would depend only on risk level. They did not. But neither did social information always win the day.

Use of social information depended on the infants' age (a stand-in for language knowledge and social cognition) and motor expertise. Experienced crawlers aged 1 ; 0 refused to descend risky slopes regardless of mothers' messages, but became slightly more cautious on safe slopes when their mothers discouraged them. Novice walkers aged 1 ; 0 marched blithely over the edge of both safe and risky slopes, but became slightly more cautious at the steepest 50° increment when their mothers discouraged them (Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Ishak, Karasik & Lobo, Reference Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Ishak, Karasik and Lobo2008). By age 1 ; 6, when infants are sophisticated consumers of social information and have accumulated six months of walking experience, they only deferred to mothers' advice when risk level was uncertain: on slopes at the limits of their abilities, infants walked when mothers said go and avoided when mothers said no. Otherwise, infants ignored their mothers' advice by avoiding steep slopes and crossing safe ones regardless of the social message (Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph, Lobo, Karasik & Dimitropoulou, Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph, Lobo, Karasik and Dimitropoulou2008). However, when infants aged 1 ; 6 were outfitted with slippery Teflon-soled shoes that diminished their walking skill, they now relied on mothers' social messages to decide whether formerly safe slopes were safe or risky (Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Karasik & Lobo, in prep.). Clearly, when social information is available – as it is in the everyday lives of infants and caregivers – infants weigh and integrate social messages with the perceptual information generated by their own exploratory activities in making decisions about action.

Cross-cultural research makes an even stronger case for social influences on motor development. Childrearing practices – how infants are held, carried, bathed, dressed, exercised, toileted, and so on – can affect the onset of motor skills, their developmental trajectories and their ultimate form (Adolph, Karasik & Tamis-LeMonda, Reference Adolph, Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein2009). For example, rigorous Jamaican bathing routines accelerate the onset of sitting and walking (Hopkins & Westra, Reference Hopkins and Westra1988; Reference Hopkins and Westra1989), and restricted time in a prone position delays the onset of crawling (Davis, Moon, Sachs & Ottolini, Reference Davis, Moon, Sachs and Ottolini1998). Daily exercise of infants' upright stepping changes the developmental trajectory in alternating leg movements from the characteristic U-shape (in which stepping is seen in early infancy, disappears, and then ‘reappears’ when infants begin walking) to a monotonic increase (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo1983). Trajectories can even stop short: parents' lack of encouragement to walk resulted in a family of adult hand-and-foot crawlers (Humphrey, Skoyles & Keynes, Reference Humphrey, Skoyles and Keynes2005). Foot-binding of young Chinese girls (Fang & Yu, Reference Fang and Yu1960; Ping, Reference Ping2000), load carriage in East African girls (Heglung, Willems, Penta & Cavagna, Reference Heglund, Willems, Penta and Cavagna1995; Maloiy, Heglung, Prager, Cavagna & Taylor, Reference Maloiy, Heglung, Prager, Cavagna and Taylor1986) and endurance running in the Tarahumaran Indians (McDougall, Reference McDougall2009) alters the form of walking movements. The Chinese girls relearned how to walk with tottering steps on three-inch feet. The African load carriers alter the form of their walking movements to maximize the energetics of gait dynamics. And the Tarahumarans regularly run ultra-marathon distances by changing the positions of their heads, torsos and feet. Even the very skills we acquire – chopsticks versus spoons, body surfing versus bicycling – are affected by social context.

Finally, beyond both its supporting and leading roles vis-à-vis perception, cognition and social development, motor development is a model system for understanding fundamental developmental issues such as flexibility, prospectivity, agency, emergence of new forms, continuity and discontinuity, variability and variety of means, individual differences, age and experience, sampling intervals, and so on (Adolph & Berger, Reference Adolph, Berger, Kuhn and Siegler2006; Adolph & Joh, Reference Adolph, Joh, Slater and Lewis2007; Gibson, Reference Gibson, Dent-Read and Zukow-Goldring1997). For example, the specificity of learning between experienced crawlers who precisely perceive affordances for locomotion and novice walkers who haplessly do not, suggests that flexibility of behavior in the face of novel challenges (slopes, cliffs, etc.) requires something more than the acquisition of static rules (e.g. ‘steep slopes and big cliffs are dangerous’). Yesterday's cliff can become today's step as crawling skill improves; and yesterday's step can become today's cliff when infants transition from crawling to walking. Flexible and adaptive responding requires infants to gauge each potential obstacle relative to the current status of their bodies and skills.

Of course, coping with novel and variable circumstances is not limited to the motor domain. The ‘learning to learn’ evidenced by infants as they approach the brink of a slope or a cliff may prove to be a useful notion for understanding flexibility and generativity in language, cognition and social interaction. We suggest that a broader appreciation for general principles of developmental psychology may show poor Cinderella to be a true princess who needn't wait for rescue by a handsome prince or a make-over by a fairy godmother to be worthy of attending the ball.

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by NICHD grants R37 33486 to Karen E. Adolph and R01 42607 to Karen E. Adolph & Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda.

References

REFERENCES

Adolph, K. E. & Berger, S. E. (2006). Motor development. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (eds), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 6th edn, 161213. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Adolph, K. E. & Joh, A. S. (2007). Motor development: How infants get into the act. In Slater, A. & Lewis, M. (eds), Introduction to infant development, 2nd edn. 6380. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adolph, K. E., Karasik, L. B. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2009). Motor skill. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science, 6188. New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ishak, S., Karasik, L. B. & Lobo, S. A. (2008). Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Developmental Psychology 44, 1705–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Karasik, L. B. & Lobo, S. A. (in prep.). Multiple sources of information for guiding locomotion over slippery slopes. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C. & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology 44, 867–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, B. E., Moon, R. Y., Sachs, H. C. & Ottolini, M. C. (1998). Effects of sleep position on infant motor development. Pediatrics 102, 1135–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fang, H. S. Y. & Yu, F. Y. K. (1960). Foot binding in Chinese women. Canadian Journal of Surgery 293, 195202.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J. (1997). An ecological psychologist's prolegomena for perceptual development: A functional approach. In Dent-Read, C. & Zukow-Goldring, P. (eds), Evolving explanations of development: Ecological approaches to organism–environment systems, 2345. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. J. & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heglund, N. C., Willems, P. A., Penta, M. & Cavagna, G. A. (1995). Energy-saving gait mechanics with head-supported loads. Nature 375, 5254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, B. & Westra, T. (1988). Maternal handling and motor development: An intracultural study. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs 114, 379408.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, B. & Westra, T. (1989). Maternal expectations of their infants' development: Some cultural differences. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 31, 384–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphrey, N., Skoyles, J. R. and Keynes, R. (2005). Human hand-walkers: Five siblings who never stood up. Discussion paper, Centre for Philosophy of Natural and Social Science, London, United Kingdom.Google Scholar
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Adolph, K. E. (under review). The transition from crawling to walking affects infants' social actions with objects.Google Scholar
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Adolph, K. E. & Dimitropoulou, K. A. (2008). How mothers encourage and discourage infants' motor actions. Infancy 13, 366–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maloiy, G. M. O., Heglung, N. C., Prager, L. M., Cavagna, G. A. & Taylor, C. R. (1986). Energetic costs of carrying loads: Have African women discovered an economic way? Nature 319, 668–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, C. (2009). Born to run: A hidden tribe, superathletes, and the greatest race the world has never seen. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Ping, W. (2000). Aching for beauty: Footbinding in China. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J. & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effects on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology 21, 195200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Adolph, K. E., Lobo, S. A., Karasik, L. B. & Dimitropoulou, K. A. (2008). When infants take mothers' advice: 18-month-olds integrate perceptual and social information to guide motor action. Developmental Psychology 44, 734–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Maternal responsiveness and early language acquisition. Advances in Child Development and Behavior 29, 89–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walking: New findings and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior 2, 99–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar