Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Suttora, Chiara
and
Salerni, Nicoletta
2011.
Maternal speech to preterm infants during the first 2 years of life: stability and change.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
Vol. 46,
Issue. 4,
p.
464.
Delafield-Butt, Jonathan T.
and
Gangopadhyay, Nivedita
2013.
Sensorimotor intentionality: The origins of intentionality in prospective agent action.
Developmental Review,
Vol. 33,
Issue. 4,
p.
399.
Caçola, Priscila M.
and
Pant, Mohan D.
2014.
Using a Generalized Linear Mixed Model Approach to Explore the Role of Age, Motor Proficiency, and Cognitive Styles in Children's Reach Estimation Accuracy.
Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Vol. 119,
Issue. 2,
p.
530.
Gonzalez, Claudia L. R.
Li, Fangfang
Mills, Kelly J.
Rosen, Nicole
and
Gibb, Robbin L.
2014.
Speech in action: degree of hand preference for grasping predicts speech articulation competence in children.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 5,
Issue. ,
Anzulewicz, Anna
Sobota, Krzysztof
and
Delafield-Butt, Jonathan T.
2016.
Toward the Autism Motor Signature: Gesture patterns during smart tablet gameplay identify children with autism.
Scientific Reports,
Vol. 6,
Issue. 1,
Kołakowska, Agata
Landowska, Agnieszka
Anzulewicz, Anna
and
Sobota, Krzysztof
2017.
Automatic recognition of therapy progress among children with autism.
Scientific Reports,
Vol. 7,
Issue. 1,
Gonzalez, Sandy L.
Alvarez, Veronica
and
Nelson, Eliza L.
2019.
Do Gross and Fine Motor Skills Differentially Contribute to Language Outcomes? A Systematic Review.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 10,
Issue. ,
Simeoli, Roberta
Arnucci, Miriana
Rega, Angelo
and
Marocco, Davide
2020.
Movement detection software to enhance autism assessment processes.
p.
1.
Möhring, Wenke
Klupp, Stephanie
Zumbrunnen, Rijana
Segerer, Robin
Schaefer, Sabine
and
Grob, Alexander
2021.
Age-related changes in children’s cognitive–motor dual tasking: Evidence from a large cross-sectional sample.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 206,
Issue. ,
p.
105103.
Mohamed, Malikka Begum Habib
and
O’Brien, Beth A.
2022.
Defining the relationship between fine motor visual-spatial integration and reading and spelling.
Reading and Writing,
Vol. 35,
Issue. 4,
p.
877.
McGowan, Timothy
and
Delafield-Butt, Jonathan
2023.
Neonatal Participation in Neonatal Imitation: Narrative in Newborn Dialogues.
Human Development,
Vol. 67,
Issue. 3,
p.
135.
Schiavon, Marta
Burton, Birgitte K
Hemager, Nicoline
Greve, Aja N
Spang, Katrine S
Ellersgaard, Ditte
Plessen, Kerstin Jessica
Jepsen, Jens Richardt M
Thorup, Anne A E
Werge, Thomas
Nordentoft, Merete
and
Nudel, Ron
2024.
Language, Motor Ability and Related Deficits in Children at Familial Risk of Schizophrenia or Bipolar Disorder.
Schizophrenia Bulletin,
Salazar, Ruby Moye
Rosello, Stacy Sue
and
O’Connell, Mary Ann
2025.
Understanding Autism.
p.
175.
As Iverson (this issue) points out, it is ironic that motor development has been relegated to peripheral ‘Cinderella’ status in developmental psychology. After all, for those researchers who view psychology as the study of behavior, motor development is the stuff of the science. All behaviors – walking, talking, looking, laughing, reaching, playing, sleeping, breathing – are motor behaviors. And for those researchers who view psychology as the study of mind, motor behaviors – looking times and reaction times, vocalizations and gestures, avoidance responses and proximity seeking – provide the basis for inferences about infants' percepts, thoughts and feelings.
Now, at long last, motor development, the neglected and wretched Cinderella of developmental psychology, has been rescued from the dustbin and invited to the language ball. As researchers in both motor (Adolph and Karasik) and language (Tamis-LeMonda) development, how do we feel about this? On the one hand, we embrace Iverson's proposal that developmental changes in infants' motor skills can affect developmental changes in linguistic, cognitive and social aspects of language acquisition. As one of many examples cited by Iverson, the transition from crawling to walking is associated with qualitative changes in the ways that infants share objects with their mothers, which in turn creates new opportunities for social interactions (Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda & Adolph, under review). We also find that infant motor development and expertise shape both the language and gestures mothers use to encourage and discourage infant actions in situations of potential risk (Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph & Dimitropoulou, Reference Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph and Dimitropoulou2008), and infant exploratory and play actions are key behaviors to which mothers verbally respond during everyday interactions (Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Hahn & Haynes, Reference Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Hahn and Haynes2008; Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, Reference Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein2002). Clearly, infants' motor actions and development are a driving force in their social development and interactions.
On the other hand, we feel that Iverson's proposal only captures a small piece of the story – one in which motor development is viewed as a ‘key participant’, as Iverson puts it, in the service of the true princesses of psychology – language, cognition, perception and social interaction. An alternative approach is to recast motor development in the leading role, with perception, cognition and social development playing supporting roles. Perception is a necessary condition for adaptive motor control; motor actions must be selected, modified, discovered and constructed to suit the constraints and demands of the current situation (Adolph & Berger, Reference Adolph, Berger, Kuhn and Siegler2006; Gibson & Pick, Reference Gibson and Pick2000). And, more germane to Iverson's arguments, language and social interaction can also be key participants.
Our own research illustrates the participatory role of social cognition in infant motor action. Inspired by Sorce, Emde, Campos & Klinnert's (Reference Sorce, Emde, Campos and Klinnert1985) classic study of social referencing on the visual cliff, we asked whether infants use social information from their mothers when deciding whether to descend safe and risky slopes (0°–50°). On some trials, mothers encouraged their infants to crawl or walk down the slopes, and on other trials, mothers discouraged descent (Karasik et al., Reference Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph and Dimitropoulou2008). If infants ignored the social information offered by their mothers, motor decisions would depend only on risk level. They did not. But neither did social information always win the day.
Use of social information depended on the infants' age (a stand-in for language knowledge and social cognition) and motor expertise. Experienced crawlers aged 1 ; 0 refused to descend risky slopes regardless of mothers' messages, but became slightly more cautious on safe slopes when their mothers discouraged them. Novice walkers aged 1 ; 0 marched blithely over the edge of both safe and risky slopes, but became slightly more cautious at the steepest 50° increment when their mothers discouraged them (Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Ishak, Karasik & Lobo, Reference Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Ishak, Karasik and Lobo2008). By age 1 ; 6, when infants are sophisticated consumers of social information and have accumulated six months of walking experience, they only deferred to mothers' advice when risk level was uncertain: on slopes at the limits of their abilities, infants walked when mothers said go and avoided when mothers said no. Otherwise, infants ignored their mothers' advice by avoiding steep slopes and crossing safe ones regardless of the social message (Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph, Lobo, Karasik & Dimitropoulou, Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Adolph, Lobo, Karasik and Dimitropoulou2008). However, when infants aged 1 ; 6 were outfitted with slippery Teflon-soled shoes that diminished their walking skill, they now relied on mothers' social messages to decide whether formerly safe slopes were safe or risky (Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Karasik & Lobo, in prep.). Clearly, when social information is available – as it is in the everyday lives of infants and caregivers – infants weigh and integrate social messages with the perceptual information generated by their own exploratory activities in making decisions about action.
Cross-cultural research makes an even stronger case for social influences on motor development. Childrearing practices – how infants are held, carried, bathed, dressed, exercised, toileted, and so on – can affect the onset of motor skills, their developmental trajectories and their ultimate form (Adolph, Karasik & Tamis-LeMonda, Reference Adolph, Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein2009). For example, rigorous Jamaican bathing routines accelerate the onset of sitting and walking (Hopkins & Westra, Reference Hopkins and Westra1988; Reference Hopkins and Westra1989), and restricted time in a prone position delays the onset of crawling (Davis, Moon, Sachs & Ottolini, Reference Davis, Moon, Sachs and Ottolini1998). Daily exercise of infants' upright stepping changes the developmental trajectory in alternating leg movements from the characteristic U-shape (in which stepping is seen in early infancy, disappears, and then ‘reappears’ when infants begin walking) to a monotonic increase (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo1983). Trajectories can even stop short: parents' lack of encouragement to walk resulted in a family of adult hand-and-foot crawlers (Humphrey, Skoyles & Keynes, Reference Humphrey, Skoyles and Keynes2005). Foot-binding of young Chinese girls (Fang & Yu, Reference Fang and Yu1960; Ping, Reference Ping2000), load carriage in East African girls (Heglung, Willems, Penta & Cavagna, Reference Heglund, Willems, Penta and Cavagna1995; Maloiy, Heglung, Prager, Cavagna & Taylor, Reference Maloiy, Heglung, Prager, Cavagna and Taylor1986) and endurance running in the Tarahumaran Indians (McDougall, Reference McDougall2009) alters the form of walking movements. The Chinese girls relearned how to walk with tottering steps on three-inch feet. The African load carriers alter the form of their walking movements to maximize the energetics of gait dynamics. And the Tarahumarans regularly run ultra-marathon distances by changing the positions of their heads, torsos and feet. Even the very skills we acquire – chopsticks versus spoons, body surfing versus bicycling – are affected by social context.
Finally, beyond both its supporting and leading roles vis-à-vis perception, cognition and social development, motor development is a model system for understanding fundamental developmental issues such as flexibility, prospectivity, agency, emergence of new forms, continuity and discontinuity, variability and variety of means, individual differences, age and experience, sampling intervals, and so on (Adolph & Berger, Reference Adolph, Berger, Kuhn and Siegler2006; Adolph & Joh, Reference Adolph, Joh, Slater and Lewis2007; Gibson, Reference Gibson, Dent-Read and Zukow-Goldring1997). For example, the specificity of learning between experienced crawlers who precisely perceive affordances for locomotion and novice walkers who haplessly do not, suggests that flexibility of behavior in the face of novel challenges (slopes, cliffs, etc.) requires something more than the acquisition of static rules (e.g. ‘steep slopes and big cliffs are dangerous’). Yesterday's cliff can become today's step as crawling skill improves; and yesterday's step can become today's cliff when infants transition from crawling to walking. Flexible and adaptive responding requires infants to gauge each potential obstacle relative to the current status of their bodies and skills.
Of course, coping with novel and variable circumstances is not limited to the motor domain. The ‘learning to learn’ evidenced by infants as they approach the brink of a slope or a cliff may prove to be a useful notion for understanding flexibility and generativity in language, cognition and social interaction. We suggest that a broader appreciation for general principles of developmental psychology may show poor Cinderella to be a true princess who needn't wait for rescue by a handsome prince or a make-over by a fairy godmother to be worthy of attending the ball.