Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-07T11:09:30.919Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The one and the many: effects of the cell adhesion molecule pathway on neuropsychological function in psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2013

A. Hargreaves
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
R. Anney
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
C. O'Dushlaine
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
K. K. Nicodemus
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
M. Gill
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
A. Corvin
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
D. Morris
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland
Gary Donohoe*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Republic of Ireland School of Psychology, National University of Ireland, Galway, Republic of Ireland
*
*Address for correspondence: Professor G. Donohoe, National University of Ireland, Galway, Republic of Ireland. (Email: donoghug@tcd.ie)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Genetic studies of single gene variants have been criticized as providing a simplistic characterization of the genetic basis of illness risk that ignores the effects of other variants within the same biological pathways. Of candidate biological pathways for schizophrenia (SZ), the cell adhesion molecule (CAM) pathway has repeatedly been linked to both psychosis and neurocognitive dysfunction. Here we tested, using risk allele scores derived from the Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study Consortium (PGC-SCZ), whether alleles within the CAM pathway were correlated with poorer neuropsychological function in patients.

Method

In total, 424 patients with psychosis were assessed in areas of cognitive ability typically found to be impaired in SZ: intelligence quotient, memory, working memory and attentional control. CAM pathway genes were identified using the Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) database. Alleles within these genes identified as significantly associated with SZ risk in the PGC-SCZ were then used to calculate a CAM pathway-based polygenic risk allele score for each patient and these scores were tested for association with cognitive ability.

Results

Increased CAM pathway polygenic risk scores were significantly associated with poorer performance on measures of memory and attention, explaining 1–3% of variation on these measures. Notably, the most strongly associated single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the CAM pathway (rs9272105 within HLA-DQA1) explained a similar amount of variance in attentional control, but not memory, as the polygenic risk analysis.

Conclusions

These data support a role for the CAM pathway in cognitive function, both at the level of individual SNPs and the wider pathway. In so doing these data highlight the value of pathway-based polygenic risk score studies as well as single gene studies for understanding SZ-associated deficits in cognition.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Introduction

The involvement of cell adhesion molecules (CAMs) in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (SZ) has long been hypothesized. Genes encoding CAMs play an important role in neurodevelopmental processes including axonal and dendritic growth and brain segmentation (e.g. cadherin-4, CDH4; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Zhang, Ma, Bucan, Glessner, Abrahams, Salyakina, Imielinski, Bradfield, Sleiman, Kim, Hou, Frackelton, Chiavacci, Takahashi, Sakurai, Rappaport, Lajonchere, Munson, Estes, Korvatska, Piven, Sonnenblick, Alvarez Retuerto, Herman, Dong, Hutman, Sigman, Ozonoff, Klin, Owley, Sweeney, Brune, Cantor, Bernier, Gilbert, Cuccaro, McMahon, Miller, State, Wassink, Coon, Levy, Schultz, Nurnberger, Haines, Sutcliffe, Cook, Minshew, Buxbaum, Dawson, Grant, Geschwind, Pericak-Vance, Schellenberg and Hakonarson2009), cell–cell binding (e.g. cadherin-7, CDH7; Soronen et al. Reference Soronen, Ollila, Antila, Silander, Palo, Kieseppa, Lonnqvist, Peltonen, Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and Paunio2010) and synapse formation (e.g. neurexin; Dean et al. Reference Dean, Scholl, Choih, DeMaria, Berger, Isacoff and Scheiffele2003). Disruption of several CAM genes has been reported in patients with psychosis, including de novo copy number variants in neurexin-1 (NRXN1) (Rujescu et al. Reference Rujescu, Ingason, Cichon, Pietilainen, Barnes, Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Vassos, Ettinger, Bramon, Murray, Ruggeri, Tosato, Bonetto, Steinberg, Sigurdsson, Sigmundsson, Petursson, Gylfason, Olason, Hardarsson, Jonsdottir, Gustafsson, Fossdal, Giegling, Moller, Hartmann, Hoffmann, Crombie, Fraser, Walker, Lonnqvist, Suvisaari, Tuulio-Henriksson, Djurovic, Melle, Andreassen, Hansen, Werge, Kiemeney, Franke, Veltman, Buizer-Voskamp, Sabatti, Ophoff, Rietschel, Nothen, Stefansson, Peltonen, St Clair, Stefansson and Collier2009; Kirov et al. Reference Kirov, Pocklington, Holmans, Ivanov, Ikeda, Ruderfer, Moran, Chambert, Toncheva, Georgieva, Grozeva, Fjodorova, Wollerton, Rees, Nikolov, van de Lagemaat, Bayes, Fernandez, Olason, Bottcher, Komiyama, Collins, Choudhary, Stefansson, Stefansson, Grant, Purcell, Sklar, O'Donovan and Owen2012), neuroligin-2 (Sun et al. Reference Sun, Cheng, Qin, Liao, Chen, Koong, Chen and Chen2011) and several others [including members of the DLG (Discs large) gene family; Kirov et al. Reference Kirov, Pocklington, Holmans, Ivanov, Ikeda, Ruderfer, Moran, Chambert, Toncheva, Georgieva, Grozeva, Fjodorova, Wollerton, Rees, Nikolov, van de Lagemaat, Bayes, Fernandez, Olason, Bottcher, Komiyama, Collins, Choudhary, Stefansson, Stefansson, Grant, Purcell, Sklar, O'Donovan and Owen2012], each of which has been associated with increased illness risk.

Genes encoding CAMs have also been shown to have an impact on cognitive function. A synthetic peptide derived from the neuronal CAM has been found to influence memory consolidation in an animal model at both behavioural and hippocampal neuron phenotypes (Cambon et al. Reference Cambon, Hansen, Venero, Herrero, Skibo, Berezin, Bock and Sandi2004). In patients, Soronen et al. (Reference Soronen, Ollila, Antila, Silander, Palo, Kieseppa, Lonnqvist, Peltonen, Tuulio-Henriksson, Partonen and Paunio2010) found that the gene CDH7 is associated with variation in performance on measures of working memory and visual attention in patients with bipolar disorder. Contactin-associated protein-like-2 (CNTNAP2), which encodes a member of the neurexin family, has been implicated in SZ and associated with epilepsy and intellectual disability (Friedman et al. Reference Friedman, Vrijenhoek, Markx, Janssen, van der Vliet, Faas, Knoers, Cahn, Kahn, Edelmann, Davis, Silverman, Brunner, van Kessel, Wijmenga, Ophoff and Veltman2008). The mechanism by which CAMs influence cognition is unknown, particularly whether this occurs via the same biological pathway as is associated with increased psychosis risk.

In addition to the study of single variants, researchers have recently begun to focus on methods to extract data from multiple variants. These include approaches that examine whether associated single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) are more likely to come from biologically related genes, termed pathway analysis. In a recent pathway analyses by our group of three independent Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) datasets, we identified using an enrichment of genetic association signals in psychosis for SNPs that were tagged to genes defined as members of the CAMs pathway [Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) Identifier: HSA04514; O'Dushlaine et al. Reference O'Dushlaine, Kenny, Heron, Donohoe, Gill, Morris and Corvin2011]. Evidence for involvement of CAM pathways has also emerged from two other recent studies taking different analytical approaches (Jia et al. Reference Jia, Wang, Fanous, Chen, Kendler and Zhao2012; Lips et al. Reference Lips, Cornelisse, Toonen, Min, Hultman, Holmans, O'Donovan, Purcell, Smit, Verhage, Sullivan, Visscher and Posthuma2012). A second approach, termed polygenic risk score analysis, has been designed to investigate whether illness-associated SNPs from one study (e.g. of SZ) can explain phenotypic variance in an independent sample using either the same illness phenotype (International Schizophrenia Consortium et al. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009) or a related phenotype (e.g. cognition; McIntosh et al. Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald, Harris, Corley, Hall, Starr, Porteous, Tenesa, Visscher and Deary2013). This analysis approach indicates that the composite effect of many small genetic variants contribute substantially to SZ variance (about 25%; International Schizophrenia Consortium et al. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009; Kirov et al. Reference Kirov, Pocklington, Holmans, Ivanov, Ikeda, Ruderfer, Moran, Chambert, Toncheva, Georgieva, Grozeva, Fjodorova, Wollerton, Rees, Nikolov, van de Lagemaat, Bayes, Fernandez, Olason, Bottcher, Komiyama, Collins, Choudhary, Stefansson, Stefansson, Grant, Purcell, Sklar, O'Donovan and Owen2012). Polygenic analysis has also provided further evidence of genetic overlap between related phenotypes; for example, McIntosh et al. (Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald, Harris, Corley, Hall, Starr, Porteous, Tenesa, Visscher and Deary2013) recently reported that polygenic SZ risk scores could be used to explain variance in longitudinal measures of age-related changes in cognitive function.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of common variants within the CAM pathway on neuropsychological function in patients with psychosis using a combination of pathway and polygenic analysis. We hypothesized that an additive effect of risk allele load from genetic variants located within the CAM pathway would account for a significant percentage of the variance in neuropsychological function in patients. To test this hypothesis we based our analysis on recent case-control analysis from the Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study Consortium (PGC-SCZ) (Ripke et al. Reference Ripke, Sanders, Kendler, Levinson, Sklar, Holmans, Lin, Duan, Ophoff, Andreassen, Scolnick, Cichon, St Clair, Corvin, Gurling, Werge, Rujescu, Blackwood, Pato, Malhotra, Purcell, Dudbridge, Neale, Rossin, Visscher, Posthuma, Ruderfer, Fanous, Stefansson, Steinberg, Mowry, Golimbet, De Hert, Jonsson, Bitter, Pietilainen, Collier, Tosato, Agartz, Albus, Alexander, Amdur, Amin, Bass, Bergen, Black, Borglum, Brown, Bruggeman, Buccola, Byerley, Cahn, Cantor, Carr, Catts, Choudhury, Cloninger, Cormican, Craddock, Danoy, Datta, de Haan, Demontis, Dikeos, Djurovic, Donnelly, Donohoe, Duong, Dwyer, Fink-Jensen, Freedman, Freimer, Friedl, Georgieva, Giegling, Gill, Glenthoj, Godard, Hamshere, Hansen, Hansen, Hartmann, Henskens, Hougaard, Hultman, Ingason, Jablensky, Jakobsen, Jay, Jurgens, Kahn, Keller, Kenis, Kenny, Kim, Kirov, Konnerth, Konte, Krabbendam and Krasucki2011). Selecting all SNP variants located within genes from the CAM pathway [defined in our previous work (KEGG Identifier: HSA04514)], we calculated a pathway-specific polygenic risk score based on the number of risk alleles they carried. We then determined the amount of variance in patients’ neuropsychological function explained by these scores. Finally, we compared the amount of variance explained from the full polygenic analysis to the amount of variance explained by individual variants from four CAM-related genes that were most strongly associated with SZ risk in the PGC case-control analysis: major histocompatibility complex, class II, DQ α−1 (HLA-DQA1), CDH4, NRXN1 and CNTNAP2. In doing so we sought to determine both whether polygenic risk scores from the CAM pathway were significantly associated with neuropsychological performance and, if so, how the amount of variance explained compared with that explained by individual risk variants within the pathway. Given the previous use of polygenic analysis in SZ and psychosis more broadly, a secondary question for our study was whether any significant associations observed were specific to SZ cases, or were shared across the broader psychosis phenotype.

Method

Neuropsychological sample characteristics

In total, 424 cases who had completed a full neuropsychological assessment battery and for whom full genome-wide SNP data were available were analysed (Irish Schizophrenia Genomics Consortium, 2012). Cases consisted of clinically stable patients with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) diagnosis of SZ, schizo-affective disorder (SZA), bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder with psychotic features, or psychosis not otherwise specified (see Table 1 for details) recruited from five sites across the Republic of Ireland. Inclusion criteria required that participants were clinically stable at the time of neuropsychological assessment, aged 18–65 years, had no history of co-morbid psychiatric disorder, no substance abuse in the preceding 6 months, no prior head injury with loss of consciousness and no history of seizures. Diagnosis was confirmed by trained psychiatrists using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Diagnoses (SCID; First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams2002). Due to the range of psychotic illness present in the sample and differences in cognitive deficits associated with these, we based our analysis on both (1) a narrow definition of SZ and SZA (n = 340); and (2) a broad definition of psychosis which encompassed all those meeting the criteria for psychosis (n = 424). Additional diagnostic details and clinical sample characteristics ascertained at the time of interview include medication dosage and symptom severity. This was calculated based on a factor analysis of Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychotic Illness (OPCRIT; McGuffin et al. Reference McGuffin, Farmer and Harvey1991), as previously described for this sample (Cummings et al. Reference Cummings, Donohoe, Hargreaves, Moore, Fahey, Dinan, McDonald, O'Callaghan, O'Neill, Waddington, Murphy, Morris, Gill and Corvin2013). All assessments were conducted in accordance with the relevant ethics committees’ approval from each participating site. All patients were of Irish ancestry (i.e. four grandparents born in Ireland) and all provided written informed consent.

Table 1. Patient demographic characteristics

Dx, Diagnosis; n.a., not applicable; SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms; SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, IQ, intelligence quotient.

Data are given as mean (standard deviation).

Cognitive assessment

All patients completed a full neuropsychological assessment battery designed to target the cognitive deficits typically reported in SZ – namely deficits in general cognitive function, memory function, working memory and attentional control. Where possible, both a verbal measure and a visuospatial measure of each construct were included.

Pre-morbid and current general cognitive functioning (intelligence quotient; IQ) was measured using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading and selected subtests (Vocabulary, Similarities, Block Design and Matrix Reasoning) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997a ). Verbal memory and visual episodic memory were assessed using the logical memory subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edition (WMS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997b ) and the Paired Associate Learning task from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB; Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, McInnes and Rabbitt1994), respectively. Working memory was assessed using the Spatial Working Memory Task from the CANTAB and Letter–Number Sequencing from the WMS-III. Attentional control was assessed using the Continuous Performance Task, identical pairs version (CPT-IP; Cornblatt et al. Reference Cornblatt, Risch, Faris, Friedman and Erlenmeyer-Kimling1988) and the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART; Robertson, Reference Robertson1994).

Calculating risk allele load

Polygenic scores for variants located within the CAM pathway were calculated in four steps. First, all available SNPs within 20 kb of genes in the CAM pathway were identified. CAM pathway genes were identified based on data from the KEGG database as previously described by us (O'Dushlaine et al. Reference O'Dushlaine, Kenny, Heron, Donohoe, Gill, Morris and Corvin2011). A total of 132 genes were identified (see online Supplementary Table S1); five of these could not be tagged with genotyped SNPs using the above criteria. Second, alleles within these SNPs were identified as risk or non-risk using data from the PGC-SCZ analysis according to three different thresholds: p < 10–5, p < 0.05 and p < 0.5. Using a variety of threshold cut-off points for determining risk is in line with procedures used in previous polygenic analysis (International Schizophrenia Consortium et al. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009). Regarding the three thresholds we selected, these are arbitrary and were pragmatically selected to reflect a distribution including strong (10 × 10–5), nominal (0.05) and non-significant baseline (0.5) associations. The SNPs were all coded so that the ‘target’ allele was the one positively associated with SZ; hence, the directionality (increased risk) is the same for all SNPs. Third, to account for differences between variants in the effect size of the association with illness, each risk allele was weighted as the log10 of the effect size described in the PGC dataset [WSNP = log10 (ORPGC)] (where W = weight and OR = odds ratio). Included variants were not linkage disequilibrium (LD)-pruned; while the strengths and weaknesses of LD-pruning are debatable, examining all available data including markers in partial LD with other SNPs in the study may incorporate additional signals that would be missed by pruning the data at an arbitrary correlation threshold. Inclusion of all variants has previously been recommended to capture all variation associated with risk (International Schizophrenia Consortium, Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009). Finally, a risk score for each individual was calculated based on the number of weighted risk alleles they carried at each of the three p value thresholds using the equation: score (p < threshold) = Σj(S SNP)/(j – m), where j = number of SNPs at p < threshold, m = number of SNPs with missing genotypes and S = SNP score = allele count * W, where W = weight for each risk allele = log(OR). A risk score for each of the CAM SNPs was calculated as (SSNP = WSNP × risk allele count). The number of missing genotypes was consistently low within each p value threshold [for p < 10–5 it was 6 (2.5%), for p < 0.05 it was 44 (0.85%), and for p < 0.5 it was 254 (0.49%)].

Single gene variant selection

As we hypothesized that polygenic risk scores would better explain variance in neuropsychological function than would be explained by single SNP analyses, we planned to follow up any significant neurocognitive findings from the CAM polygenic analysis by characterizing the effects of single SNPs within the CAM pathway on neurocognition. To do this, we selected the most strongly associated single SNPs from each of the four CAM genes most strongly associated with SZ risk in the PGC analysis. These were: HLA-DQA1 [rs9272105; OR 0.87, p = 9.97 × 10–9], CDH4 (rs2427104; OR 0.90, p = 0.00006), NRXN1 (rs1819972; OR 1.1, p = 0.0004) and CNTNAP2 (rs1548743; OR 0.92, p = 0.0005). Selecting these four variants for analysis was based on an arbitrary cut-off point for statistical association with SZ of p ⩽ 0.0005. Furthermore, while 11 other HLA genes exceeded this cut-off threshold we only included the most strongly associated given the high LD in this region.

Statistical analysis

Associations between CAM pathway polygenic risk allele scores and the phenotypes of IQ, episodic memory, working memory and attention were tested in a series of multiple regression analyses implemented in SPSS 17 (SPSS Inc., 2008). In each case, scores for each neuropsychological subtest were entered as dependent variables, and where appropriate age and gender were entered on the first step of the analysis as effects of no interest, followed by CAM pathway risk allele score on the second step. Exactly the same approach was taken in the analysis of the single variants, with the risk genotype score (0, 1, or 2 alleles) in each case replacing the polygenic score as the independent variable. The R 2 value, or variance explained, was calculated between these nested models; namely, a model containing the intercept plus gender and age (when appropriate) and the model containing these same terms plus the pathway score. We report this pathway score-specific R 2 plus the corresponding F test p value. Finally, effect sizes for all significant effects were calculated using Cohen's d in ClinTools software for Windows (version 4, 2005; http://www.clintools.com) to enable comparison between the polygenic scores analysis and analysis of the individual SNPs considered.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics for all patients appear in Table 1. No differences were observed between the narrow psychosis group (SZ and SZA only) and broad psychosis group (all patients with psychosis) in terms of age, gender, age at onset, or general cognitive ability as indexed by full-scale IQ. Based on factor scores previously calculated for symptom severity based on OPCRIT (Cummings et al. Reference Cummings, Donohoe, Hargreaves, Moore, Fahey, Dinan, McDonald, O'Callaghan, O'Neill, Waddington, Murphy, Morris, Gill and Corvin2013), no differences were observed between the two groups in severity of symptoms of depression, positive symptoms, negative symptoms or disorganization. Differences were, however, observed for the ‘mania’ factor, with the broad psychosis group scoring significantly higher on the manic scale than the narrow psychosis group. No between-group differences were observed in medication dosage as measured by chlorpromazine equivalents.

The effects of CAM pathway risk allele load on cognition

R 2 change and p values from the regression analyses for each of the three cognitive domains of IQ, memory and attention by CAM pathway risk allele load are presented in Fig. 1 and Table 2. Across both narrow-sense and broad-sense psychosis groups, higher polygenic risk scores within the CAM pathway were significantly associated with deficits in both memory function (as measured by the CANTAB paired associate learning test) and sustained attention (as measured by the SART). For the broad diagnoses groups, CAM pathway risk allele load was also associated with poorer verbal episodic memory function as measured by the WMS-III logical memory task. In the narrow psychosis group, this effect was observed at trend level only. By contrast, the narrow psychosis group showed a nominal association between preserved pre-morbid IQ and higher CAM pathway risk polygenic scores (but only for SNPs thresholded at p = 10–5), whereas this association was only observed at trend level in the broad psychosis group. The amount of variance in cognitive performance explained by CAM pathway polygenic risk scores ranged between 1 and 3% in regression models that were significant, with the highest percentage of variance explained on the SART attentional control task. Calculated effect sizes (Cohen's d) for this variance explained ranged from 0.23 to 0.37 with a mean effect size of 0.29.

Fig. 1. Plot of regression analyses of neuropsychological variables showing R 2 values and associated significance for each polygenic risk threshold. IQ, Intelligence quotient; SZ, schizophrenia; SZA, schizo-affective disorder; PAL, Paired Associate Learning; SART, Sustained Attention to Response Task; CPT, Continuous Performance Task.

Table 2. CAM pathway polygenic score regression analysis for each neuropsychological variable a

CAM, Cell adhesion molecule; SZ, schizophrenia; SZA, schizo-affective disorder; IQ, intelligence quotient; PAL, Paired Associate Learning; SART, Sustained Attention to Response Task; CPT, Continuous Performance Task.

a CAM risk alleles included were thresholded at p = 10–5, p = 0.05 and p = 0.5.

* Significant association.

Cognitive analysis of SZ-associated individual SNPs within the CAM pathway genes HLA-DQA1, CDH4, NRXN1 and CNTNAP2

We next tested whether variance in neurocognitive function explained by CAM pathway scores was comparable with that explained by individual SNPs within the CAM pathway. As described above, this was based on the analysis of SNPs within each of the four genes most strongly associated with SZ risk in the PGC analysis: rs9272105 within HLA-DQA1, rs2427104 within CDH4, rs1819972 within NRXN1 and rs1548743 within CNTNAP2 (see Table 3). Only the HLA-DQA1 SNP (the most strongly associated CAM pathway SNP from the PGC analysis and the only variant achieving genome-wide associated significance) was observed to be associated with variation in neurocognitive performance – specifically attentional control in both the narrow and broad diagnosis groups, and pre-morbid IQ in the narrow diagnosis group only. No association with memory function was observed. None of the other variants within CDH4, NRXN1 or CNTNAP2 was associated with variation on any of these three neurocognitive measures. Similarly, a combined regression analysis that included all four SNPs failed to explain a significant amount of variation on any of these three neurocognitive measures.

Table 3. Regression analysis for SNPs within HLA-DQA1, NRXN1, CNTNAP2 and CDH4

SNP, Single nucleotide polymorphism; HLA-DQA1, major histocompatibility complex, class II, DQ α−1; NRXN1, neurexin-1; CNTNAP2, contactin-associated protein-like-2; CDH4, cadherin-4; IQ, intelligence quotient; SART, Sustained Attention to Response Task; PAL, Paired Associate Learning; SZ, schizophrenia; SZA, schizo-affective disorder.

* p < 0.05.

Given the significant contribution of rs9272105 to explaining variation in neuropsychological functioning, we re-ran our CAM pathway polygenic regression analysis to exclude all variants at this gene locus (resulting in the exclusion of three SNPs). When we re-calculated CAM pathway polygenic risk allele load scores minus these three HLA-DQA1 variants, the association between the CAM polygenic score and memory largely remained significant (see Table 4). By comparison the association with attentional control was no longer significant. The nominal association with pre-morbid IQ seen in the narrow diagnosis group using the p = 10–5 threshold also became non-significant.

Table 4. Recomputation of the regression analyses for the CAM pathway polygenic risk scores without three SNPs mapped to the HLA-DQA1 gene

CAM, Cell adhesion molecule; SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; HLA-DQA1, major histocompatibility complex, class II, DQ α−1; SZ, schizophrenia; SZA, schizo-affective disorder; IQ, intelligence quotient; PAL, Paired Associate Learning; SART, Sustained Attention to Response Task.

* Significant association.

Discussion

This study used polygenic risk allele scores derived from the PGC-SCZAQ8 case-control analysis to investigate whether risk variants within the CAM pathway were associated with poorer neuropsychological function amongst 424 patients with either narrow-sense or broad-sense psychosis. We further compared the variation in neuropsychological performance explained by CAM pathway polygenic risk allele scores to the individual CAM pathway variants identified by the PGC as most strongly associated with SZ risk (CDH4, NRXN1, HLA-DQA1 and CNTNAP2). Based on these analyses we found that: (1) polygenic scores for the CAM pathway explained a statistically significant proportion of variance in neuropsychological function; (2) one risk SNP – HLA-DQA1 – was also individually associated with variation in neuropsychological function; and (3) after removal of this gene from the polygenic analysis, polygenic risk scores continued to explain variants in memory, but not attentional control. To our knowledge this is the first study to characterize the effects on neurocognition of risk variants within the CAM pathway – or any other pathway – in patients with SZ.

A specific criticism of the single variant approach in studying both illness phenotypes and intermediate phenotypes (including cognition) is that as illness risk is polygenically determined (International Schizophrenia Consortium et al. Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan, Sullivan and Sklar2009) the function of single variants cannot be understood in isolation from either other risk variants or from the biological pathways in which they function. In this context, we investigated both whether pathway-based risk estimates significantly account for variation in neuropsychological deficits and, if so, how the amount of variance explained compares with that explained by individual SNP genotypes. When compared with results from single SNP analysis from the four most strongly associated CAM pathway genes, CAM pathway polygenic scores explained a greater percentage of variance in memory function than the HLA-DQA1 SNP most strongly associated with risk in the PGC analysis, and this variance explained was undiminished after the HLA-DQA1 SNP was removed from the polygenic score analysis. For attentional control, however, variation on this variable was better explained by the SNP than the pathway.

The CAM pathway has previously been implicated in a variety of neurocognitive processes, including memory. Consistent with this, an association with variation on a neurocognitively associated phenotype has been reported for three of the four individual genes characterized: CDH4 has been associated with total brain volume (Seshadri et al. Reference Seshadri, DeStefano, Au, Massaro, Beiser, Kelly-Hayes, Kase, D'Agostino, Decarli, Atwood and Wolf2007), NRXN1 with white matter volume (Voineskos et al. Reference Voineskos, Lett, Lerch, Tiwari, Ameis, Rajji, Muller, Mulsant and Kennedy2011) and CNTNAP2 with language processing (Kos et al. Reference Kos, van den Brink, Snijders, Rijpkema, Franke, Fernandez and Hagoort2012). This is the first study to our knowledge that specifically implicates the HLA-DQA1 gene in neuropsychological performance in humans. Beyond these single variant analyses, this is the first study to our knowledge that demonstrates the relevance of CAM genes to cognition at the pathway level. Specifically, it highlights the role of previously identified SZ risk variants within the CAM pathway on cognition in a manner that goes beyond the effects of individual variants. While making this point, two caveats regarding the involvement of HLA-DQA1 are relevant. First, while the HLA-DQA1 gene is located within the KEGG cell adhesion pathway, its demonstrated roles in cell adhesion have been confined to the immune system. This is noteworthy because a role for immune-related major histocompatibility complex class 1 molecules in neuronal plasticity (which is strongly linked to learning and memory) has long been speculated (Shatz, Reference Shatz2009). It should, of course, also be noted that HLA-DQA1 resides in an area of strong LD encompassing many genes, many of which are not involved in cell adhesion. In summary, therefore, while this study highlights the role of CAM genes in cognition, the observed cognitive effects of HLA-DQA1 may occur independently of this pathway.

This finding that pathway-based polygenic risk scores for SZ explain variance in cognition is relevant to the recent debate regarding the genetic overlap between cognitive performance and SZ in particular and psychosis in general. While a high degree of overlap between cognitive deficits and SZ has previously been inferred from twin studies (Toulopoulou et al. Reference Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Hua-Hall, Ettinger, Sham and Murray2007), recent evidence by Fowler et al. (Reference Fowler, Zammit, Owen and Rasmussen2012) has suggested that this may not be quite as high when based on non-biased epidemiological samples. In a polygenic analysis of age-related changes in cognition in healthy adults over time, McIntosh et al. (Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald, Harris, Corley, Hall, Starr, Porteous, Tenesa, Visscher and Deary2013) found that polygenic scores (again based on the PGC-SCZ dataset) significantly predicted about 1% variance in cognitive change. In the present study we extend these findings by demonstrating that polygenic risk scores also predict variance in SZ-related cognitive deficits in patients (about 3%). Furthermore, by focusing on a previously implicated biological pathway our data highlight the relevance of polygenic risk in the CAM pathway, in particular to cognitive deficits in this group. The CAM pathway is unlikely to be unique in this regard – polygenic risk scores analysis based on other pathways is likely to also be informative about cognitive deficits in this group. For example, a recent study by Greenwood et al. (Reference Greenwood, Lazzeroni, Murray, Cadenhead, Calkins, Dobie, Green, Gur, Gur, Hardiman, Kelsoe, Leonard, Light, Nuechterlein, Olincy, Radant, Schork, Seidman, Siever, Silverman, Stone, Swerdlow, Tsuang, Tsuang, Turetsky, Freedman and Braff2011) of 94 candidate SZ genes suggests that glutamate pathway genes are also likely to be important. The approach taken in our study is equally applicable to the study of these other pathways, and may well be informative not just for explaining cognitive deficits in patients, but also for explaining the genetic architecture of cognition in the healthy population.

Conclusion

In conclusion this study, building on previous literature adopting either pathway-based or polygenic-based approaches to psychosis risk and cognitive dysfunction, investigated the role of pathway-specific risk variants in the cognitive deficits associated with psychosis. The data support a role for the CAM pathway in memory formation and attention-related reaction time. The data also provided evidence that a single SNP – in HLA-DQA1 – was also associated with variation in attentional control, and to a lesser extent pre-morbid IQ, in psychotic patients. In doing so, this study demonstrates the feasibility of pathway-specific polygenic risk analysis to studying an intermediate phenotype for psychosis, while at the same time suggesting that single gene analysis nonetheless continues to be informative about gene effects on cognition.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002663.

Acknowledgements

We thank all patients and staff who participated in the collection of patient data. We thank Professor Ted Dinan, Professor Kieran Murphy, Professor John Waddington, Professor Colm McDonald, Professor Eadbhard O'Callaghan, and Dr Anthony O'Neil for their participation in recruitment of samples. Recruitment of the participants was supported by funding awards; funding for this study was provided by the Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 project (no. 085475/B/08/Z and 085475/Z/08/Z), the Wellcome Trust (no. 072894/Z/03/Z, 090532/Z/09/Z and 075491/Z/04/B) and Science Foundation Ireland (SFI) to A.C. and M.G. and a Health Research Board (Ireland) grant to G.D. (HRA/2009/197). The work of K.K.N. is supported by Science Foundation Ireland and Marie Curie Actions (COFUND).

Members of Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 and Schizophrenia Psychiatric GWAS Consortium co-authors can be found in the online Supplementary material.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Bellenguez, C, Bevan, S, Gschwendtner, A, Spencer, CC, Burgess, AI, Pirinen, M, Jackson, CA, Traylor, M, Strange, A, Su, Z, Band, G, Syme, PD, Malik, R, Pera, J, Norrving, B, Lemmens, R, Freeman, C, Schanz, R, James, T, Poole, D, Murphy, L, Segal, H, Cortellini, L, Cheng, YC, Woo, D, Nalls, MA, Muller-Myhsok, B, Meisinger, C, Seedorf, U, Ross-Adams, H, Boonen, S, Wloch-Kopec, D, Valant, V, Slark, J, Furie, K, Delavaran, H, Langford, C, Deloukas, P, Edkins, S, Hunt, S, Gray, E, Dronov, S, Peltonen, L, Gretarsdottir, S, Thorleifsson, G, Thorsteinsdottir, U, Stefansson, K, Boncoraglio, GB, Parati, EA, Attia, J, Holliday, E, Levi, C, Franzosi, MG, Goel, A, Helgadottir, A, Blackwell, JM, Bramon, E, Brown, MA, Casas, JP, Corvin, A, Duncanson, A, Jankowski, J, Mathew, CG, Palmer, CN, Plomin, R, Rautanen, A, Sawcer, SJ, Trembath, RC, Viswanathan, AC, Wood, NW, Worrall, BB, Kittner, SJ, Mitchell, BD, Kissela, B, Meschia, JF, Thijs, V, Lindgren, A, Macleod, MJ, Slowik, A, Walters, M, Rosand, J, Sharma, P, Farrall, M, Sudlow, CL, Rothwell, PM, Dichgans, M, Donnelly, P, Markus, HS (2012). Genome-wide association study identifies a variant in HDAC9 associated with large vessel ischemic stroke. Nature Genetics 44, 328333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cambon, K, Hansen, SM, Venero, C, Herrero, AI, Skibo, G, Berezin, V, Bock, E, Sandi, C (2004). A synthetic neural cell adhesion molecule mimetic peptide promotes synaptogenesis, enhances presynaptic function, and facilitates memory consolidation. Journal of Neuroscience 24, 41974204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornblatt, BA, Risch, NJ, Faris, G, Friedman, D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L (1988). The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Research 26, 223238.Google Scholar
Cummings, E, Donohoe, G, Hargreaves, A, Moore, S, Fahey, C, Dinan, TG, McDonald, C, O'Callaghan, E, O'Neill, FA, Waddington, JL, Murphy, KC, Morris, DW, Gill, M, Corvin, A (2013). Mood congruent psychotic symptoms and specific cognitive deficits in carriers of the novel schizophrenia risk variant at MIR-137. Neuroscience Letters 532, 3338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, C, Scholl, FG, Choih, J, DeMaria, S, Berger, J, Isacoff, E, Scheiffele, P (2003). Neurexin mediates the assembly of presynaptic terminals. Nature Neuroscience 6, 708716.Google Scholar
First, M, Spitzer, R, Gibbon, M, Williams, J (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Fowler, T, Zammit, S, Owen, MJ, Rasmussen, F (2012). A population-based study of shared genetic variation between premorbid IQ and psychosis among male twin pairs and sibling pairs from Sweden. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 460466.Google Scholar
Friedman, JI, Vrijenhoek, T, Markx, S, Janssen, IM, van der Vliet, WA, Faas, BH, Knoers, NV, Cahn, W, Kahn, RS, Edelmann, L, Davis, KL, Silverman, JM, Brunner, HG, van Kessel, AG, Wijmenga, C, Ophoff, RA, Veltman, JA (2008). CNTNAP2 gene dosage variation is associated with schizophrenia and epilepsy. Molecular Psychiatry 13, 261266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwood, TA, Lazzeroni, LC, Murray, SS, Cadenhead, KS, Calkins, ME, Dobie, DJ, Green, MF, Gur, RE, Gur, RC, Hardiman, G, Kelsoe, JR, Leonard, S, Light, GA, Nuechterlein, KH, Olincy, A, Radant, AD, Schork, NJ, Seidman, LJ, Siever, LJ, Silverman, JM, Stone, WS, Swerdlow, NR, Tsuang, DW, Tsuang, MT, Turetsky, BI, Freedman, R, Braff, DL (2011). Analysis of 94 candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 930946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
International Schizophrenia Consortium; Purcell, SM, Wray, NR, Stone, JL, Visscher, PM, O'Donovan, MC, Sullivan, PF, Sklar, P (2009). Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460, 748752.Google ScholarPubMed
Irish Schizophrenia Genomics Consortium (2012). Genome-wide association study implicates HLA-C*01:02 as a risk factor at the major histocompatibility complex locus in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 72, 620628.Google Scholar
Jia, P, Wang, L, Fanous, AH, Chen, X, Kendler, KS, Zhao, Z (2012). A bias-reducing pathway enrichment analysis of genome-wide association data confirmed association of the MHC region with schizophrenia. Journal of Medical Genetics 49, 96103.Google Scholar
Kirov, G, Pocklington, AJ, Holmans, P, Ivanov, D, Ikeda, M, Ruderfer, D, Moran, J, Chambert, K, Toncheva, D, Georgieva, L, Grozeva, D, Fjodorova, M, Wollerton, R, Rees, E, Nikolov, I, van de Lagemaat, LN, Bayes, A, Fernandez, E, Olason, PI, Bottcher, Y, Komiyama, NH, Collins, MO, Choudhary, J, Stefansson, K, Stefansson, H, Grant, SG, Purcell, S, Sklar, P, O'Donovan, MC, Owen, MJ (2012). De novo CNV analysis implicates specific abnormalities of postsynaptic signalling complexes in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 142153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kos, M, van den Brink, D, Snijders, TM, Rijpkema, M, Franke, B, Fernandez, G, Hagoort, P (2012). CNTNAP2 and language processing in healthy individuals as measured with ERPs. PLOS ONE 7, e46995.Google Scholar
Lips, ES, Cornelisse, LN, Toonen, RF, Min, JL, Hultman, CM, Holmans, PA, O'Donovan, MC, Purcell, SM, Smit, AB, Verhage, M, Sullivan, PF, Visscher, PM, Posthuma, D (2012). Functional gene group analysis identifies synaptic gene groups as risk factor for schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 9961006.Google Scholar
McGuffin, P, Farmer, A, Harvey, I (1991). A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 764770.Google Scholar
McIntosh, AM, Gow, A, Luciano, M, Davies, G, Liewald, DC, Harris, SE, Corley, J, Hall, J, Starr, JM, Porteous, DJ, Tenesa, A, Visscher, PM, Deary, IJ (2013). Polygenic risk for schizophrenia is associated with cognitive change between childhood and old age. Biological Psychiatry 73, 938943.Google Scholar
O'Dushlaine, C, Kenny, E, Heron, E, Donohoe, G, Gill, M, Morris, D, Corvin, A (2011). Molecular pathways involved in neuronal cell adhesion and membrane scaffolding contribute to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder susceptibility. Molecular Psychiatry 16, 286292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ripke, S, Sanders, AR, Kendler, KS, Levinson, DF, Sklar, P, Holmans, PA, Lin, DY, Duan, J, Ophoff, RA, Andreassen, OA, Scolnick, E, Cichon, S, St Clair, D, Corvin, A, Gurling, H, Werge, T, Rujescu, D, Blackwood, DH, Pato, CN, Malhotra, AK, Purcell, S, Dudbridge, F, Neale, BM, Rossin, L, Visscher, PM, Posthuma, D, Ruderfer, DM, Fanous, A, Stefansson, H, Steinberg, S, Mowry, BJ, Golimbet, V, De Hert, M, Jonsson, EG, Bitter, I, Pietilainen, OP, Collier, DA, Tosato, S, Agartz, I, Albus, M, Alexander, M, Amdur, RL, Amin, F, Bass, N, Bergen, SE, Black, DW, Borglum, AD, Brown, MA, Bruggeman, R, Buccola, NG, Byerley, WF, Cahn, W, Cantor, RM, Carr, VJ, Catts, SV, Choudhury, K, Cloninger, CR, Cormican, P, Craddock, N, Danoy, PA, Datta, S, de Haan, L, Demontis, D, Dikeos, D, Djurovic, S, Donnelly, P, Donohoe, G, Duong, L, Dwyer, S, Fink-Jensen, A, Freedman, R, Freimer, NB, Friedl, M, Georgieva, L, Giegling, I, Gill, M, Glenthoj, B, Godard, S, Hamshere, M, Hansen, M, Hansen, T, Hartmann, AM, Henskens, FA, Hougaard, DM, Hultman, CM, Ingason, A, Jablensky, AV, Jakobsen, KD, Jay, M, Jurgens, G, Kahn, RS, Keller, MC, Kenis, G, Kenny, E, Kim, Y, Kirov, GK, Konnerth, H, Konte, B, Krabbendam, L, Krasucki, R, et al. (2011). Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nature Genetics 43, 969976.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, James, M, Owen, AM, Sahakian, BJ, McInnes, L, Rabbitt, P (1994). Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): a factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia 5, 266281.Google Scholar
Robertson, I (1994). Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Trinity College Dublin: Dublin.Google Scholar
Rujescu, D, Ingason, A, Cichon, S, Pietilainen, OP, Barnes, MR, Toulopoulou, T, Picchioni, M, Vassos, E, Ettinger, U, Bramon, E, Murray, R, Ruggeri, M, Tosato, S, Bonetto, C, Steinberg, S, Sigurdsson, E, Sigmundsson, T, Petursson, H, Gylfason, A, Olason, PI, Hardarsson, G, Jonsdottir, GA, Gustafsson, O, Fossdal, R, Giegling, I, Moller, HJ, Hartmann, AM, Hoffmann, P, Crombie, C, Fraser, G, Walker, N, Lonnqvist, J, Suvisaari, J, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Djurovic, S, Melle, I, Andreassen, OA, Hansen, T, Werge, T, Kiemeney, LA, Franke, B, Veltman, J, Buizer-Voskamp, JE, Sabatti, C, Ophoff, RA, Rietschel, M, Nothen, MM, Stefansson, K, Peltonen, L, St Clair, D, Stefansson, H, Collier, DA (2009). Disruption of the neurexin 1 gene is associated with schizophrenia. Human Molecular Genetics 18, 988996.Google Scholar
Seshadri, S, DeStefano, AL, Au, R, Massaro, JM, Beiser, AS, Kelly-Hayes, M, Kase, CS, D'Agostino, RB Sr, Decarli, C, Atwood, LD, Wolf, PA (2007). Genetic correlates of brain aging on MRI and cognitive test measures: a genome-wide association and linkage analysis in the Framingham Study. BMC Medical Genetics 8 (Suppl. 1), S15.Google Scholar
Shatz, CJ (2009). MHC class I: an unexpected role in neuronal plasticity. Neuron 64, 4045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soronen, P, Ollila, HM, Antila, M, Silander, K, Palo, OM, Kieseppa, T, Lonnqvist, J, Peltonen, L, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Partonen, T, Paunio, T (2010). Replication of GWAS of bipolar disorder: association of SNPs near CDH7 with bipolar disorder and visual processing. MolecularPsychiatry 15, 46.Google ScholarPubMed
SPSS (2008). SPSS 16.0 Command Syntax Reference. SPSS Inc.: Chicago.Google Scholar
Sun, C, Cheng, MC, Qin, R, Liao, DL, Chen, TT, Koong, FJ, Chen, G, Chen, CH (2011). Identification and functional characterization of rare mutations of the neuroligin-2 gene (NLGN2) associated with schizophrenia. Human Molecular Genetics 20, 30423051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T, Picchioni, M, Rijsdijk, F, Hua-Hall, M, Ettinger, U, Sham, P, Murray, R (2007). Substantial genetic overlap between neurocognition and schizophrenia: genetic modeling in twin samples. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 13481355.Google Scholar
Voineskos, AN, Lett, TA, Lerch, JP, Tiwari, AK, Ameis, SH, Rajji, TK, Muller, DJ, Mulsant, BH, Kennedy, JL (2011). Neurexin-1 and frontal lobe white matter: an overlapping intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One 6, e20982.Google Scholar
Wang, K, Zhang, H, Ma, D, Bucan, M, Glessner, JT, Abrahams, BS, Salyakina, D, Imielinski, M, Bradfield, JP, Sleiman, PM, Kim, CE, Hou, C, Frackelton, E, Chiavacci, R, Takahashi, N, Sakurai, T, Rappaport, E, Lajonchere, CM, Munson, J, Estes, A, Korvatska, O, Piven, J, Sonnenblick, LI, Alvarez Retuerto, AI, Herman, EI, Dong, H, Hutman, T, Sigman, M, Ozonoff, S, Klin, A, Owley, T, Sweeney, JA, Brune, CW, Cantor, RM, Bernier, R, Gilbert, JR, Cuccaro, ML, McMahon, WM, Miller, J, State, MW, Wassink, TH, Coon, H, Levy, SE, Schultz, RT, Nurnberger, JI, Haines, JL, Sutcliffe, JS, Cook, EH, Minshew, NJ, Buxbaum, JD, Dawson, G, Grant, SF, Geschwind, DH, Pericak-Vance, MA, Schellenberg, GD, Hakonarson, H (2009). Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders. Nature 459, 528533.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997 a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test, 3rd edn (WAIS-III). Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997 b). Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edn (WAIS-III). The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Patient demographic characteristics

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Plot of regression analyses of neuropsychological variables showing R2 values and associated significance for each polygenic risk threshold. IQ, Intelligence quotient; SZ, schizophrenia; SZA, schizo-affective disorder; PAL, Paired Associate Learning; SART, Sustained Attention to Response Task; CPT, Continuous Performance Task.

Figure 2

Table 2. CAM pathway polygenic score regression analysis for each neuropsychological variablea

Figure 3

Table 3. Regression analysis for SNPs within HLA-DQA1, NRXN1, CNTNAP2 and CDH4

Figure 4

Table 4. Recomputation of the regression analyses for the CAM pathway polygenic risk scores without three SNPs mapped to the HLA-DQA1 gene

Supplementary material: File

Hargreaves et al. Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Download Hargreaves et al. Supplementary Material(File)
File 68.6 KB