Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-qdpjg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-23T08:25:06.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does Intentionality Matter? An Exploration of Discrimination With Ambiguous Intent

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2017

Danielle M. Gardner*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University
Ann Marie Ryan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Danielle M. Gardner, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, 316 Physics Road, Room 258, East Lansing, MI 48824. E-mail: gardn333@msu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Although intentionality may be a valuable spectrum on which to categorically distinguish instances of discrimination, we consider whether or not differences on this construct actually reveal differing impacts for targets. Specifically, we wonder whether intentionality is very relevant to the experiences of targets of discrimination or whether the negative consequences stemming from the discriminatory interactions occur regardless of the perpetrator's intent. Further, we explore the potential consequences related to a target attempting to confront discrimination of ambiguous intent. Finally, we discuss discrimination of ambiguous intent from the perspective of the perpetrator, outlining theories related to intentional versus unintentional subtle discrimination.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology 2017 

Although intentionality may be a valuable spectrum on which to categorically distinguish instances of discrimination, we consider whether or not differences on this construct actually reveal differing impacts for targets. Specifically, we wonder whether intentionality is very relevant to the experiences of targets of discrimination or whether the negative consequences stemming from the discriminatory interactions occur regardless of the perpetrator's intent. Further, we explore the potential consequences related to a target attempting to confront discrimination of ambiguous intent. Finally, we discuss discrimination of ambiguous intent from the perspective of the perpetrator, outlining theories related to intentional versus unintentional subtle discrimination.

Target Attributions of Discrimination of Ambiguous Intent

Whether or not discrimination is intentional is potentially irrelevant to a target. In situations in which discrimination is of ambiguous intent, it is often up to the target to interpret the perpetrator's intentions and mentally designate the cause of the negative interaction. For example, when an out-group member faces rejection, he/she may construe this rejection in a number of ways, including a sense that one's personal qualities were rejected, that one's group was rejected, or that the perpetrator was indeed biased (Mendes, Major, McCoy, & Blascovich, Reference Mendes, Major, McCoy and Blascovich2008). We contend that regardless of the true root of the negative interaction, a target's perception of that interaction is enough to generate negative consequences.

If a target attributes the cause of a negative interaction to discrimination, regardless of the intentions of the potential perpetrator, damaging consequences may follow. For example, research suggests that rejection in an interracial context can lead to more self-reported and nonverbal displays of anger than rejection from an in-group member, given the possible perception of bias and racism as the root of the rebuff (Mendes et al., Reference Mendes, Major, McCoy and Blascovich2008). Indeed, anger has been identified as one of the most common emotional states to follow racism perceptions (Bullock & Houston, Reference Bullock and Houston1987). As the Jones, Arena, Nittrouer, Alonso, and Lindsey (Reference Jones, Arena, Nittrouer, Alonso and Lindsey2017) focal article highlights, such outward expressions of anger may contribute to the cyclical nature of subtle discrimination; coworkers may respond to the target's negativity with attributions of blame and further instances of rejection and avoidance. Therefore, even though the initial interaction may not have been rooted in discriminatory intention, targets may still experience negative outcomes long beyond the interaction itself, given their construal of prejudice.

On the other hand, the theory of attributional ambiguity contends that attributions of discrimination in instances involving potential prejudice may have a buffering effect for self-esteem (Crocker, Voelkl, Testa, & Major, Reference Crocker, Voelkl, Testa and Major1991). Specifically, it is argued that attributing a negative encounter as discrimination deflects potential self-blame to blame of the other. Weiner's (Reference Weiner1985) attributional theory of emotion predicts similar outcomes, as he describes how attributing negative effects to causes external from the self (i.e., another person's prejudice) protects self-esteem, whereas attributions of internal causes, such as one's lack of worthiness, can damage self-esteem. As an extension, these theories would also suggest negative outcomes for those who experience discrimination and do not accurately mentally categorize it as such. Failing to attribute subtle discrimination as rooted in bias could cause targets to self-blame and consider internal factors as causal, such as one's personal deservingness. In line with this, individuals attributing ambiguous negative situations to causes other than discrimination have been shown to have lower self-esteem and greater depressed affect (Crocker & Major, Reference Crocker, Major, Zanna and Olson1994).

In attempting to reconcile these two assertions, we come to the conclusion of a lose–lose situation for targets of discrimination of ambiguous intent. On one hand, construing ambiguous discrimination as intentional may lead to anger (Mendes et al., Reference Mendes, Major, McCoy and Blascovich2008), which could trigger the cyclical nature of subtle discrimination. On the other hand, failing to identify a discriminatory experience as such may lead to damaged self-esteem and increased depressive affect (Crocker et al., Reference Crocker, Voelkl, Testa and Major1991). Therefore, we assert that intentionality of ambiguous discrimination is irrelevant to the outcomes of a target's experience; whether or not the discrimination is intended by the perpetrator, and whether or not the target attributes the experience to discrimination, the target may still experience negative outcomes as a result of the discriminatory interaction.

Costs of Confronting Ambiguous Discrimination

Beyond the consequences associated with experiencing and interpreting ambiguous discrimination, targets may face further negative outcomes when attempting to confront the discrimination in question. Although it is not the position of this commentary to entirely discourage targets from addressing the discrimination they face, as such confrontations have been associated with target feelings of empowerment and closure (Haslett & Lipman, Reference Haslett, Lipman and Benokraitis1997), we do wish to highlight the potentially increased costs associated with confronting discrimination of ambiguous intent compared with discrimination of clear intent.

Often, targets’ willingness to confront discrimination is related to perceptions of cost (Kaiser & Miller, Reference Kaiser and Miller2001). One potential cost involves coworker perceptions that the target is complaining. Such perceptions may result in dislike of the target, retaliation toward the target, and dismissal of the target's values (Kowalski, Reference Kowalski1996). These costs are particularly relevant in instances of discrimination with ambiguous intent, as coworkers may believe that the target is choosing to interpret the interaction negatively. Indeed, Kaiser and Miller (Reference Kaiser and Miller2001) found that stigmatized individuals citing discrimination were perceived as complainers and evaluated more negatively than individuals citing other attributions for a failure, regardless of the likelihood that discrimination had actually occurred.

Along with the potential social costs of confrontation, there exists a likelihood that targets may be ineffective in their attempts to address discrimination given a perpetrator's defensiveness. For instance, Czopp and Monteith (Reference Czopp and Monteith2003) found that confrontations from a target were associated with greater perpetrator feelings of irritation and antagonism compared with confrontations from a nontarget. Further, perpetrators were more likely to perceive a target's confrontation as an overreaction compared with the same confrontation from a nontarget. Such findings may stem from majority group members’ tendency to view themselves as nonprejudiced and to therefore react to accusations of discrimination from targets as threats to their sense of self (Kaiser & Miller, Reference Kaiser and Miller2003). Consequently, majority members may deal with this threat and anxiety by derogating and disliking the target (Shelton & Stewart, Reference Shelton and Stewart2004). This phenomenon may be especially pertinent to discrimination of ambiguous intent, in which the perpetrator can deflect a target's concern by blaming the target for misconstruing the situation.

Perpetrator Intentionality in Subtle and Ambiguous Discrimination

Although it is necessary to shed light on the negative effects experienced by stigmatized targets, an evaluation of discrimination of ambiguous intent would not be complete without consideration of the perpetrator. Specifically, we highlight the potential differences between those who indeed intend to discriminate compared with those who do so unintentionally. Although we argue that many negative effects experienced by the target remain regardless of perpetrator intention, it may be of value to characterize the differences stemming from varying intentionality to understand how to best address perpetrator behavior.

Perhaps obviously, a perpetrator may commit subtle discrimination because he/she intends to do so. Although instances of formal discrimination have become less common due to legislation (Hebl, Madera, & King, Reference Hebl, Madera, King and Thomas2008) and social pressures (Dovidio, Glick, & Rudman, Reference Dovidio, Glick and Rudman2005), that is not to say that prejudice is no longer expressed in the workplace. In fact, people are now more likely to express their prejudices in ways in which their motives can go undetected (Hebl et al., Reference Hebl, Madera, King and Thomas2008), such as in situations of subtle discrimination with ambiguous intent. Indeed, researchers have found that individuals will more likely reveal prejudice through interpersonal discrimination if they could appear to be acting on some other basis (Snyder, Kleck, Strenta, & Mentzer, Reference Snyder, Kleck, Strenta and Mentzer1979). Therefore, it is likely that a proportion of discrimination of this manner is indeed rooted in prejudice and intention.

However, it is also possible that perpetrators of subtle discrimination are committing these acts unintentionally. One possible framework to explain this phenomenon may lie in stereotype threat theory (Steele & Aronson, Reference Steele and Aronson1995). Specifically, researchers have found evidence suggesting that the fear of being labeled as prejudiced may cause majority members to distance themselves from minorities (Goff, Steele, & Davies, Reference Goff, Steele and Davies2008). Through this explanation, we come to the ironic conclusion that a perpetrator's desire to not discriminate may in fact lead to unintentional subtle discrimination. Other research has suggested that intergroup interactions may cause majority group members anxiety and that this anxiety may occur regardless of prejudice. Researchers have found that even low-prejudiced members of majority groups tend to behave in an anxious and distant manner during intergroup interactions (Devine, Evett, & Vasquez-Suson, Reference Devine, Evett, Vasquez-Suson, Sorrentio and Higgins1996). Consequently, we conclude that perpetrators may be committing subtle discrimination not only without intention but, in some cases, also without awareness of how they are acting. Remediation of such unintentional discrimination may be possible through adoption of diversity-focused organizational policies and informal efforts toward increased intergroup contact (Hebl et al., Reference Hebl, Madera, King and Thomas2008). Such efforts may influence the establishment of social norms toward the conscious and fair treatment of coworkers of all demographics (Hebl, Ruggs, Martinez, Trump-Steele, & Nittrouer, Reference Hebl, Ruggs, Martinez, Trump-Steele, Nittrouer and Nelson2015).

Conclusion

Although intentionality may be a useful continuum on which to measure varying instances of discrimination, it may not be as meaningful in terms of costs for the target. Specifically, we contend that intentionality is a perpetrator-focused construct that does not differentiate instances of target impact. However, that is not to say the continuum is without value, as categorizing instances of discrimination by levels of intentionality may help organizations decide how to most effectively confront the bias in question. Therefore, the intentionality continuum may provide indirect benefits to targets via improved organizational reactions to discrimination of varying intentionality.

Within this commentary, we explore the potential mechanisms through which subtle discrimination of ambiguous intent may impact targets, examine the costs associated with confronting discrimination of this nature, and consider varying intentionality from the perspective of the perpetrator. As Jones et al. note and this work highlights, discrimination is discrimination regardless of intent. Therefore, we contend that discrimination of ambiguous intent should be taken seriously from the perspective of researchers and practitioners, as discrimination of this type is becoming more common and continues to impact workers daily.

References

Bullock, S. C., & Houston, E. (1987). Perceptions of racism by Black medical students attending White medical schools. Journal of the National Medical Association, 79 (6), 601608. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.278 Google Scholar
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1994). Reactions to stigma: The moderating role of justifications. In Zanna, M. P. & Olson, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of prejudice: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 7, pp. 289314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Google Scholar
Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (2), 218228. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218 Google Scholar
Czopp, A. M., & Monteith, M. J. (2003). Confronting prejudice (literally): Reactions to confrontations of racial and gender bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (4), 532544. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202250923 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, P. G., Evett, S. R., & Vasquez-Suson, K. A. (1996). Exploring the interpersonal dynamics of intergroup contact. In Sorrentio, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context (Vol. 3, pp. 423464). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Glick, P. G., & Rudman, L. (Eds.). (2005). On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94 (1), 911079. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.91 Google Scholar
Haslett, B. B., & Lipman, S. (1997). Micro inequalities: Up close and personal. In Benokraitis, N. (Ed.), Subtle sexism: Current practice and prospects for change (pp. 3453). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hebl, M. R., Madera, J. M., & King, E. (2008). Exclusion, avoidance and social distancing. In Thomas, K. M. (Ed.), Diversity resistance in organizations (pp. 127150). New York, NY: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hebl, M., Ruggs, E. N., Martinez, L. R., Trump-Steele, R., & Nittrouer, C. (2015). Understanding and reducing interpersonal discrimination in the workplace. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (2nd ed., pp. 387407). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jones, K. P., Arena, D. F., Nittrouer, C. L., Alonso, N. M., & Lindsey, A. P. (2017). Subtle discrimination in the workplace: A vicious cycle. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 10 (1), 5176.Google Scholar
Kaiser, C. R., & Miller, C. T. (2001). Stop complaining! The social costs of making attributions to discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (2), 254263. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201272010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, C. R., & Miller, C. T. (2003). Derogating the victim: The interpersonal consequences of blaming events on discrimination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6 (3), 227237. https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302030063001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kowalski, R. M. (1996). Complaints and complaining: Functions, antecedents, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 119 (2), 179196. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendes, W. B., Major, B., McCoy, S., & Blascovich, J. (2008). How attributional ambiguity shapes physiological and emotional responses to social rejection and acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94 (2), 278291. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.278 Google Scholar
Shelton, N. J., & Stewart, R. E. (2004). Confronting perpetrators of prejudice: The inhibitory effects of social costs. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28 (3), 215223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00138.x Google Scholar
Snyder, M. L., Kleck, R. E., Strenta, A., & Mentzer, S. J. (1979). Avoidance of the handicapped: An attributional ambiguity analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (12), 22972306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.12.2297 Google Scholar
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (5), 797811. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92 (4), 548573. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 Google Scholar