Introduction
Robust changes in grey matter are observed using structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in patients with schizophrenia both before and after the onset of psychosis, and are linked to the clinical features of the illness (Glahn et al. Reference Glahn, Laird, Ellison-Wright, Thelen, Robinson, Lancaster, Bullmore and Fox2008; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Fornito, Radua, Walterfang, Seal, Wood, Yücel, Velakoulis and Pantelis2011; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011). Rather controversially, these neuroanatomical changes have been argued to be progressive in nature, indicating a deteriorating pathophysiological process (Weinberger & McClure, Reference Weinberger and McClure2002; DeLisi, Reference DeLisi2008; Hulshoff Pol & Kahn, Reference Hulshoff Pol and Kahn2008; Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Nopoulos, Magnotta, Pierson, Ziebell and Ho2011; Olabi et al. Reference Olabi, Ellison-Wright, McIntosh, Wood, Bullmore and Lawrie2011; Vita et al. Reference Vita, Peri, Deste and Sacchetti2012). Longitudinal MRI studies report loss of grey matter at a rate that is often implausible (e.g. ~0.43% of frontal grey-matter volume/year in the first 12–15 years of illness; Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Nopoulos, Magnotta, Pierson, Ziebell and Ho2011) and if maintained, would result in little brain tissue being left after 2–3 decades of illness in an individual (Weinberger & McClure, Reference Weinberger and McClure2002). In addition, the putative neuroprogressive changes do not seem to reflect the severity of clinical deterioration (McGlashan, Reference McGlashan2006) even in the samples showing such significant tissue loss (Zipparo et al. Reference Zipparo, Whitford, Redoblado Hodge, Lucas, Farrow, Brennan, Gomes, Williams and Harris2008), although conflicting data exist in the literature (Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Cahn, Brans, Carati, Rais and Kahn2008; Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Nopoulos, Magnotta, Pierson, Ziebell and Ho2011). Some studies with notable sample sizes have failed to observe neuroprogressive changes (Schaufelberger et al. Reference Schaufelberger, Lappin, Duran, Rosa, Uchida, Santos, Murray, McGuire, Scazufca, Menezes and Busatto2011), and instead report a significant increase in the volume of some brain regions (Roiz-Santiáñez et al. Reference Roiz-Santiáñez, Ayesa-Arriola, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, Ortiz-García de la Foz, Pérez-Iglesias, Pazos, Sánchez and Crespo-Facorro2014). In light of this evidence, neuroprogression, if present, is likely to be limited not only in time (as highlighted by Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Cahn, Brans, Carati, Rais and Kahn2008) but also in spatial distribution (Rosa et al. Reference Rosa, Zanetti, Duran, Santos, Menezes, Scazufca, Murray, Busatto and Schaufelberger2015), and occur alongside compensatory changes in the opposite direction.
As pointed out recently (Zipursky et al. Reference Zipursky, Reilly and Murray2013), the idea of ‘neuroprogression’ or ‘structural degeneration’ is somewhat inconsistent with the improvement and/or stabilization in clinical and functional domains in a large number of patients (Arndt et al. Reference Arndt, Andreasen, Flaum, Miller and Nopoulos1995; Levine et al. Reference Levine, Lurie, Kohn and Levav2011). Diffuse patterns of neuroanatomical changes reflect clinical features of schizophrenia, for example, grey-matter reduction in left-perisylvian regions is associated with positive symptoms, extended frontolimbic reductions with negative symptoms and temporal, insular and medial prefrontal changes with disorganization (Koutsouleris et al. Reference Koutsouleris, Gaser, Jäger, Bottlender, Frodl, Holzinger, Schmitt, Zetzsche, Burgermeister, Scheuerecker, Born, Reiser, Möller and Meisenzahl2008). Either a reversal of these changes, or the appearance of compensatory changes could be expected to occur in those with long-standing illness. McGlashan (Reference McGlashan2006) argued that the pathological brain changes in schizophrenia are a result of both destructive and ameliorative processes and suggested that chronic illness may lead to concomitant ‘disuse atrophy in some circuits and overuse hypertrophy in other circuits’. This prospect has not been examined in detail to date. Functional MRI studies note that increase in functional activation in certain regions compensates for the reduced activation of the primary task-relevant brain regions (Quintana et al. Reference Quintana, Wong, Ortiz-Portillo, Kovalik, Davidson, Marder and Mazziotta2003; Tan et al. Reference Tan, Sust, Buckholtz, Mattay, Meyer-Lindenberg, Egan, Weinberger and Callicott2006), thus producing a rescuing effect in patients with a longer duration of illness (Faget-Agius et al. Reference Faget-Agius, Boyer, Lançon, Richieri, Fassio, Soulier, Chanoine, Auquier, Ranjeva and Guye2013). In terms of brain structure, if tissue reduction occurs as a primary change, then concomitant (covarying) increase in tissue in other brain regions can be expected to compensate for the deficits. Grey-matter increases are shown to be associated with clinical improvement in some patients with schizophrenia (Ansell et al. Reference Ansell, Dwyer, Wood, Bora, Brewer, Proffitt, Velakoulis, McGorry and Pantelis2015), suggesting that such increases are likely to be ‘ameliorative’ or compensatory processes. But structural studies have mostly focused on localizing the regions with most consistent morphological change (Glahn et al. Reference Glahn, Laird, Ellison-Wright, Thelen, Robinson, Lancaster, Bullmore and Fox2008; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Fornito, Radua, Walterfang, Seal, Wood, Yücel, Velakoulis and Pantelis2011; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011), and have not paid close attention to the nature of relationship (covariance) among brain regions that can uncover the subtle changes in opposite direction that occur concomitantly.
Several distinct observations in a cross-sectional sample of patients with varying illness duration could provide indirect evidence for ameliorative or compensatory neuroanatomical changes in schizophrenia. First, volumetric studies in schizophrenia suggest that while some brain regions with reduced grey matter in early stages show more pronounced deficits in chronic schizophrenia, several other regions with early reduction do not appear to have the same degree or more pronounced deficits in later stages (Ellison-Wright et al. Reference Ellison-Wright, Glahn, Laird, Thelen and Bullmore2008; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011). This latter observation has not been adequately highlighted in the past but is readily evident in an extensive meta-analysis of stage-specific volumetric changes reported by Chan et al. (Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011) (see Fig. 3 and Table 4). Thus, in the presence of an ameliorative process, brain regions showing pronounced changes in the early stages of illness will be expected to show less pronounced deviation from healthy controls in those with more chronic illness (hypothesis 1). Second, in the absence of a pathophysiological process affecting distributed brain regions (i.e. in healthy controls), widespread age-related decline occurs in cortical thickness, with few regional exceptions (e.g. primary visual cortex, medial temporal lobe) (Brans et al. Reference Brans, Kahn, Schnack, Baal, Van Posthuma, Haren, van Lepage, Lerch, Collins, Evans, Boomsma and Pol2010; Thambisetty et al. Reference Thambisetty, Wan, Carass, An, Prince and Resnick2010; Storsve et al. Reference Storsve, Fjell, Tamnes, Westlye, Overbye, Aasland and Walhovd2014). Thus, within the population of healthy controls, the structural covariance between any two brain regions is more likely to show a positive than a negative relationship brought about by regionally non-specific determinants of tissue volume (i.e. both regions show concomitant increase or decrease in thickness across individuals). In contrast, provided active regional remodelling/compensation accompanies tissue reduction, a large number of negative covarying pairs of regions can be expected in patients (i.e. as one region shows thinning, another may show thickening across individuals) (hypothesis 2). Third, meta-analytical anatomical likelihood estimates of morphometric changes consistently demonstrate regions with relative tissue reduction but not tissue increase in different stages of schizophrenia (Chan et al. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011; Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Radua, McGuire and Stefan2011). This suggests that compensatory increases in grey matter, if present, are likely to be highly variable across individuals. As a result of the higher inter-individual variability of putative compensatory changes, we can expect a reduction in the discriminatory ability when using neuroanatomical patterns to differentiate controls from patients with longer illness duration (hypothesis 3).
We tested these hypotheses in a sample of 98 clinically stable patients receiving treatment for schizophrenia and 83 healthy controls using a pattern classification approach and covariance analysis of cortical thickness. We chose MRI-derived cortical thickness as several studies have previously established that altered cortical thickness is robust feature of schizophrenia (Kuperberg et al. Reference Kuperberg, Broome, McGuire, David, Eddy, Ozawa, Goff, West, Williams, Van der Kouwe, Salat, Dale and Fischl2003; Narr et al. Reference Narr, Bilder, Toga, Woods, Rex, Szeszko, Robinson, Sevy, Gunduz-Bruce, Wang, DeLuca and Thompson2005; Nesvåg et al. Reference Nesvåg, Lawyer, Varnäs, Fjell, Walhovd, Frigessi, Jönsson and Agartz2008; Venkatasubramanian et al. Reference Venkatasubramanian, Jayakumar, Gangadhar and Keshavan2008; Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Koch, Wagner, Roebel, Schachtzabel, Gaser, Nenadic, Reichenbach, Sauer and Schlösser2010; Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, Van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins, Evans, Pol and Kahn2011). Further thickness is more likely to reflect illness-related factors than surface area (Sprooten et al. Reference Sprooten, Papmeyer, Smyth, Vincenz, Honold, Conlon, Moorhead, Job, Whalley, Hall, McIntosh, Owens, Johnstone and Lawrie2013; Haukvik et al. Reference Haukvik, Rimol, Roddey, Hartberg, Lange, Vaskinn, Melle, Andreassen, Dale and Agartz2014), making it a suitable index when studying the malleability of the cortex.
Method and materials
Participants
Ninety-eight patients at various stages of schizophrenia (80 males, 18 females) were recruited for this study. The mean illness duration assessed using case notes, patient interview and referral information as the time from the point of first contact with psychiatric services with psychotic symptoms to the date of scan acquisition for these patients was 5.5 years (median = 3 years, range 6 months–29 years). The diagnosis of schizophrenia (DSM-IV criteria) was made in accordance with the procedure of Leckman et al. (Reference Leckman, Sholomskas, Thompson, Belanger and Weissman1982) using data from all available sources [case notes, a standardized clinical interview based on the Signs and Symptoms of Psychotic Illness scale (Liddle et al. Reference Liddle, Ngan, Duffield, Kho and Warren2002) and reports from other informants when required]. Subjects with neurological disorders, current substance dependence, IQ <70 using the Quick Test (Ammons & Ammons, Reference Ammons and Ammons1962), and diagnosis of any other Axis I disorder were excluded. Seventy-eight out of 98 patients were receiving psychotropic medications (average dose in chlorpromazine equivalents 490 mg, range 25–3000 mg). Healthy controls were recruited from the local community via advertisements and comprised 83 subjects free of any psychiatric or neurological disorder matched groupwise in age (±3 years) and socio-economic status (measured using National Statistics – Socio Economic Classification; Rose & Pevalin, Reference Rose and Pevalin2003) to the patient group. Controls had similar exclusion criteria to patients; in addition subjects with history of psychotic illness in first-degree relatives were excluded. Regional Ethics Committees (Nottinghamshire and Derbyshire) approved the study and all participants provided written informed consent. More details are given in Table 1.
SSPI, Signs and symptoms in psychotic illness.
Image acquisition and morphometry
For details on image acquisition see Appendix 1 in the Supplementary material. Image processing for morphometry was carried out by a single researcher (L.P.) applying previously used criteria to reduce Freesurfer-related processing artefacts (Appendix 1, Supplementary material) (Palaniyappan & Liddle, Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle2012). The cortical thickness was measured by calculating the Euclidean distance between linked vertices on the inner and outer cortical surfaces (Fischl & Dale, Reference Fischl and Dale2000), as shown in Fig. 1 a, and in accordance with standard descriptions (Dale et al. Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999). Anatomical parcellations were obtained using the Destrieux sulcogyral atlas (Destrieux et al. Reference Destrieux, Fischl, Dale and Halgren2010), which follows the anatomical conventions of Duvernoy (Duvernoy & Bourgouin, Reference Duvernoy and Bourgouin1999). The details of the 148 resulting parcellations are presented in Supplementary Fig. S1 and Table S1. Regional thickness values were obtained from the mean thickness of all tessellations that were confined within the boundaries of the parcellated sulcogyral regions. Compared to the 148 values obtained per subject for regional thickness measures, vertexwise data provide more than 1 50 000 values per subject for pattern classification. Nevertheless, regional measures provide anatomically intuitive data that enables a meaningful interpretation of individual data points. Increasing the number of data dimensions in the presence of a limited sample size will affect the trade-off between complexity and error margin, resulting in overfitting (Song et al. Reference Song, Zhan, Long, Zhang and Yao2011). Further, covariance between two anatomical regions is more readily interpretable than the covariance between two vertices in the cortical surface. For these reasons, we used parcellation-based regional thickness for pattern classification and covariance analysis as detailed below.
Pattern classification and covariance analysis
For pattern classification analysis, we used Support Vector Machine (SVM), a learning machine for two-class problems. SVM toolkit libsvm written by Lin Chih-Jen from Taiwan University (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm/) was used with a radial basis function (RBF) as kernel function. In a different dataset published elsewhere (Guo et al. Reference Guo, Palaniyappan, Yang, Liu, Xue and Feng2014) we tested optimal parameters for employing RBF-based SVM, and chose the values of t = 2 and parameter C = 10 to achieve a robust trade-off between model complexity and training error for our classification problem. Other SVM parameters were kept as default values, in line with our previous work (Guo et al. Reference Guo, Palaniyappan, Yang, Liu, Xue and Feng2014). RBF has been shown to perform optimally when region-based neuroimaging data is used in SVM classification (Song et al. Reference Song, Zhan, Long, Zhang and Yao2011).The trade-off parameter C = 10 is the best one to get the highest accuracy rate between parameter range (1–15). To measure the test performance and to validate the classifier, a leave-one-subject-out cross-validation approach was employed, where the classifier is trained on all subjects except one, which is used as test data. Balanced accuracy, specificity, sensitivity, and predictive values for each classifier were obtained and statistical significance of these measures was determined by way of permutation testing (n = 1000 permutations with random assignment of patient/control labels to the training data). Mean discrimination accuracy, sensitivity and specificity was obtained for the entire sample. To study the effect of illness duration, we categorized patients into multiple bins on the basis of their illness duration as shown in Table 2 and Supplementary Table S6. The SVM classification was carried out for each bin independently, estimating the classification accuracy against the same set of controls using regional thickness measures after removing the linear effect of age and gender.
D, Duration of illness.
Effects of covariates (age and gender) removed from this analysis using linear regression. See also Supplementary Tables S7 and S8.
For covariance analysis structural cortical networks were constructed from the regional cortical thickness measurements of all 148 regions after removing the effects of age and gender, in line with previous studies (He et al. Reference He, Chen and Evans2007; Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Lin, Lin, Zhou, Chou, Lo, Su and Jiang2012). More details of the mathematical computation are provided in the Supplementary material.
Demographic and clinical characteristics were compared using either two-sample t test or χ2 test. Correlation analyses among normally distributed variables or residuals were conducted using Pearson's test.
Results
Global measures of thickness
Mean global thickness measures compared using two-sample t test revealed reduced thickness in patients compared to controls in both hemispheres (left: p = 9.8 × 10−7; right: p = 6.9 × 10−9), as shown in Fig. 1 b. Group difference in mean global thickness was seen in males (p < 0.001) but not in females (p = 0.50), as shown in Fig. 1 c. There was a significant negative relationship between age and global thickness in both groups (controls: r = −0.55, p = 7.07 × 10−8; schizophrenia: r = −0.49, p = 3.7 × 10−7), as shown in Fig. 1 d. After regressing out the influence of age and gender, global thickness had no significant association with illness duration, although the correlation was numerically positive for both hemispheres (left: r = 0.06, p = 0.57; right: r = 0.08, p = 0.41), as shown in Fig. 1 e. There was no significant difference of global thickness between two different scanners for all samples (p = 0.7720), for controls (p = 0.3880) and for patients (p = 0.9566) after controlling for duration, age and gender.
Regional measures of thickness
When all patients were compared to controls, 45 regions showed significantly reduced thickness (Bonferroni corrected). Most significant reduction was seen in parahippocampal gyrus (p < 10−20 for left and right), supramarginal gyrus (p < 10−20 for left and right), precentral gyrus (p < 10−20 for left and right), temporal pole (left: p < 10−20; right: p = 10−16) and right superior insula (p = 10−15). Eleven regions including inferior occipital gyrus (left: p < 10−9; right: p < 10−11), lateral fissures (left: p < 10−9; right: p < 10−8 ), occipital pole (left: p < 10−8; right: p < 10−8 ) and calcarine sulcus (left: p < 10−6; right: p < 10−9 ) showed significantly greater thickness in patients. The results are shown in Fig. 2.
Within the patient group, several regions showed significant correlation (uncorrected p < 0.05) with illness duration after regressing out the influence of age and gender (listed in Supplementary Table S2). Rather strikingly, the regions that showed significant reduction in thickness in the patients v. controls comparison, showed a positive association with illness duration, while those that had increased thickness showed a negative association. In other words, with increasing illness duration, the group differences in thickness had a tendency to ‘normalize’.
To study if this ‘normalization’ across long illness duration occurs in regions that show notable differences in the early stage of illness (<2 years), we plotted the group difference of the mean residual of regional thickness for three regions with maximum group difference in the early stage (left and right parahippocampal, and left supramarginal gyrus), against the illness duration. As shown in Fig. 3 a, a pattern of reducing deviation is seen with increasing illness duration across the group.
SVM classification
When the entire sample of patients and controls were compared on the basis of age- and gender-adjusted regional thickness, a discrimination accuracy of 81.77% with specificity 86.75% and sensitivity 77.55% was obtained. When patients were categorized to different illness duration bins, the discrimination accuracy reduced steadily when patients with longer durations were included in the classification matrix. We further studied this effect by studying four distinct subgroups of illness duration (<2, 2–4, 4–10, > 10 years). Very high levels of accuracy (96.3%), specificity (98.8%) and sensitivity (88%) were noted for those with <2 years of illness. This was not affected by scanner or sample size differences (Supplementary Tables S7 and S8). The discrimination accuracy is shown in Table 2 and the best predictors in the different bins are shown in Supplementary Table S3.
Structural covariance
Pearson correlation matrices constructed for patients and controls are displayed in Fig. 3 b. A large number of divergent relationships (negative covariance) were noted in patients compared to controls. When the strongest 100 relationships (irrespective of the spatial location) were considered in each group, 36 divergent (negative) relationships were seen in patients while only one relationship was divergent in controls, indicating that in patients, a number of regions increase in (‘gain’) thickness while some regions decrease (‘loss’). The 36 regional pairs identified in this analysis are listed in Supplementary Table S4. When the within-group, inter-individual variation in the covariance scores were plotted for these 100 regional pairs, as shown in Fig. 3 d, patients with schizophrenia had greater variances than controls (mean standard deviation in patients = 2.1814, controls = 1.0801; t test = −13.1107, p < 0.001), suggesting that the reciprocal (putatively compensatory) relationships do not always involve the same pairs of brain regions in patients. The correlation matrices were thresholded into a set of binarized matrices that describe the topological organization of the structural cortical networks for two groups. We examined different correlation thresholds (0–1 in steps of 0.05) to examine the proportion of divergent links (negative covariance) for all possible regional pairs at the different thresholds values. As shown in Fig. 3 c, we noted that patients had more negative links for all thresholds, with the gap between the two groups widening notably when stronger pairwise relationships are considered. This pattern was not related to the dose of antipsychotics used by patients (Supplementary Fig. S3). These covariance results indicate that the patient group shows a larger than expected number of reciprocal changes in regional thickness.
A hub-and-spokes plot showing the covariance patterns for two brain regions with maximal group difference in each direction (left parahippocampal and supramarginal regions with reduced thickness and bilateral inferior occipital region with increased thickness in patients) is shown in Fig. 4. In these regions, patients had a number of divergent relationships with a distributed set of regions. More details are presented in Supplementary Table S5.
Discussion
In a large sample of patients recruited at a single centre (Nottingham), we investigated the effect of illness duration on cortical thickness and report four key observations. In accordance with our prediction, a pattern of concomitant group differences in both directions (i.e. relative increase and decrease) resulting in an overall reduction in anatomical deviation from controls was noted in later stages of schizophrenia. The ability to discriminate patients from controls on the basis of cortical thickness is greatest during the early phase of the illness, and reduces with longer illness duration. The features that discriminate a patient from controls vary across the different stages of the illness.
At the outset, it is important to note that our results neither imply an overall increase in thickness nor refute the appearance of more extensively distributed tissue reduction in later stages of schizophrenia. In fact, the most prominent group difference in the entire sample, irrespective of illness duration, is a reduction in thickness seen in patients. We noted that in each duration bin, the best predictors in each classifier were regions showing reduced thickness in patients (Supplementary Table S3). This is consistent with meta-analyses of volumetric studies that show pronounced grey-matter reduction in the later stages of schizophrenia (Bora et al. Reference Bora, Fornito, Radua, Walterfang, Seal, Wood, Yücel, Velakoulis and Pantelis2011; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Di, McAlonan and Gong2011; Haijma et al. Reference Haijma, Haren, Cahn, Koolschijn, Pol and Kahn2013). We add two important cross-sectional observations to this literature. (1) Across subjects with schizophrenia, concomitant increases in thickness accompany reductions. (2) Regions showing most pronounced changes (increase or decrease) among patients in early stage of illness are not necessarily ‘worse’ among the patients in later stages of illness. Taken together, these observations are suggestive of a compensatory/remodelling process contributing to the cortical thickness variations in schizophrenia.
Using a structural covariance analysis we noted a higher number of reciprocal changes in thickness within the patient group than in controls. Most previous studies of cortical thickness in schizophrenia have sought the brain regions showing the largest magnitude of group differences, and predominantly report reduced thickness in patients (Kuperberg et al. Reference Kuperberg, Broome, McGuire, David, Eddy, Ozawa, Goff, West, Williams, Van der Kouwe, Salat, Dale and Fischl2003; Narr et al. Reference Narr, Bilder, Toga, Woods, Rex, Szeszko, Robinson, Sevy, Gunduz-Bruce, Wang, DeLuca and Thompson2005; Venkatasubramanian et al. Reference Venkatasubramanian, Jayakumar, Gangadhar and Keshavan2008; Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Koch, Wagner, Roebel, Schachtzabel, Gaser, Nenadic, Reichenbach, Sauer and Schlösser2010). To the best of our knowledge no study has investigated the concomitant changes in the brain that relate to localized changes in thickness. We note that regional thickness reduction is frequently accompanied by thickness increase in other brain regions in patients, with a high degree of between-subjects variation in the spatial distribution of the covariance patterns in patients. Further, the brain regions with the most significant thickness reduction that best discriminate patients with <2 years of illness from controls, do not show the same magnitude of group difference in those with longer duration of illness. In line with this obliteration, these regions do not retain their discriminatory ability when patients with longer duration of illness are compared with controls. Due to the cross-sectional nature of our sample, we cannot infer whether this obliteration is taking place in the same set of individuals over the illness course. But these observations lead us to speculate that a secondary remodelling process that influences brain structure may be present in schizophrenia. In a large sample of patients with differing illness duration (range 6 months–29 years) followed up for 5 years, the rate of change of cortical thickness (reduction) was notably pronounced in patients, irrespective of their age (Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, Van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins, Evans, Pol and Kahn2011). This higher rate of thinning over the course of 5 years indicates a dynamic morphometric process, but not necessarily ‘degeneration’ or a progressive deviation from normality. Interestingly, in line with our results, in that study several regions that showed higher baseline thickness revealed increased thinning over the course of illness, resulting in ‘normalization’ though this was not explicitly examined (Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, Van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins, Evans, Pol and Kahn2011). Taken together, these observations contradict a ubiquitous degenerative process, and highlight the dynamic/plastic nature of the morphological changes that index a reorganization process whereby during the course of illness, the difference between patients and controls may become less pronounced.
In our sample, the most significant group differences in thickness involved the bilateral parahippocampal, supramarginal and temporal pole regions, and the precentral and superior anterior insula regions, while increase in thickness was mostly limited to regions in the occipital cortex. Thinning of parahippocampal region is also a well-documented feature of schizophrenia (Kuperberg et al. Reference Kuperberg, Broome, McGuire, David, Eddy, Ozawa, Goff, West, Williams, Van der Kouwe, Salat, Dale and Fischl2003; Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, Van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins, Evans, Pol and Kahn2011). Parahippocampal thinning may be related to the risk of psychosis, with many studies reporting parahippocampal thinning/volume reduction in ultra-high-risk subjects (Jung et al. Reference Jung, Kim, Jang, Choi, Jung, Park, Han, Choi, Kang, Chung and Kwon2011; Mechelli et al. Reference Mechelli, Riecher-Rössler, Meisenzahl, Tognin, Wood, Borgwardt, Koutsouleris, Yung, Stone, Phillips, McGorry, Valli, Velakoulis, Woolley, Pantelis and McGuire2011; Tognin et al. Reference Tognin, Riecher-Rossler, Meisenzahl, Wood, Hutton, Borgwardt, Koutsouleris, Yung, Allen, Phillips, McGorry, Valli, Velakoulis, Nelson, Woolley, Pantelis, McGuire and Mechelli2014) and in those with the genetic risk (Goghari et al. Reference Goghari, Rehm, Carter and MacDonald2007; Palaniyappan et al. Reference Palaniyappan, Balain and Liddle2012). Both occipital thickening (Szeszko et al. Reference Szeszko, Narr, Phillips, McCormack, Sevy, Gunduz-Bruce, Kane, Bilder and Robinson2012; Yoon et al. Reference Yoon, Lee, Im, Shin, Cho, Kim, Kwon and Kim2007; Mané et al. Reference Mané, Falcon, Mateos, Fernandez-Egea, Horga, Lomeña, Bargalló, Prats-Galino, Bernardo and Parellada2009; Van Haren et al. Reference Van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, Van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins, Evans, Pol and Kahn2011; Xiao et al. Reference Xiao, Lui, Deng, Yao, Zhang, Li, Wu, Xie, He, Huang, Hu, Bi, Li and Gong2015) and lack of thinning (Nesvåg et al. Reference Nesvåg, Lawyer, Varnäs, Fjell, Walhovd, Frigessi, Jönsson and Agartz2008) in schizophrenia has been noted in previous studies. In particular (Szeszko et al. Reference Szeszko, Narr, Phillips, McCormack, Sevy, Gunduz-Bruce, Kane, Bilder and Robinson2012) found that patients responding to antipsychotic treatment have a higher occipital thickness than non-responders, suggesting that the occipital thickening is of prognostic importance. In line with this, Mitelman & Buchsbaum (Reference Mitelman and Buchsbaum2007) suggested that the loss of grey matter in the posterior aspect of the brain (including occipital cortex) is a feature of poor outcome in schizophrenia. Occipital thickening appears to arise early and is seen in conjunction with thinning in patients with untreated first episode schizophrenia (Xiao et al. Reference Xiao, Lui, Deng, Yao, Zhang, Li, Wu, Xie, He, Huang, Hu, Bi, Li and Gong2015).While the plasticity of multimodal, representational cortex may be reduced in schizophrenia (Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Loewy, Hardy, Schlosser and Vinogradov2013) the increase in grey matter restricted to the visual cortex suggest that putative compensatory responses in schizophrenia may be more localized to the primary sensory cortices than in other regions.
We achieved a very high degree of accuracy (96.3% for the first 2 years) when discriminating patients from controls, after removing effect of age and gender. This high accuracy deteriorated steadily when all patients irrespective of their stage of illness were included in the discrimination model. We consider this finding as important in the context of increasing relevance of machine learning approaches in diagnosis and outcome prediction in schizophrenia (Lawrie et al. Reference Lawrie, Olabi, Hall, McIntosh, Sm, BO and Am2011; Borgwardt & Fusar-Poli, Reference Borgwardt and Fusar-Poli2012; Koutsouleris et al. Reference Koutsouleris, Davatzikos, Borgwardt, Gaser, Bottlender, Frodl, Falkai, Riecher-Rössler, Möller, Reiser, Pantelis and Meisenzahl2014). A number of studies, using varied neuroimaging features, report an accuracy ranging between 65% and 90% in discriminating patients from controls (Orrù et al. Reference Orrù, Pettersson-Yeo, Marquand, Sartori and Mechelli2012; Veronese et al. Reference Veronese, Castellani, Peruzzo, Bellani and Brambilla2013; Zarogianni et al. Reference Zarogianni, Moorhead and Lawrie2013). Models that provide >90% accuracy can substantially reduce the proportion of uncertainty inherent to classification (Kasparek et al. Reference Kasparek, Thomaz, Sato, Schwarz, Janousova, Marecek, Prikryl, Vanicek, Fujita and Ceskova2011; Iwabuchi et al. Reference Iwabuchi, Liddle and Palaniyappan2013). For the first time, we have examined the effect of illness duration on the utility of machine learning in schizophrenia, and note that the features that differentiate patients from controls are likely to be specific to the stage of illness. A classifier to separate a patient with schizophrenia from healthy control is redundant for clinical use even if is >90% accurate, as such an accurate separation can be readily made by clinicians. But it is worth noting that a great deal of investment is being made on prognostic prediction using neuroanatomical features. Our results suggest that acknowledging the process of obliteration of pathophysiological changes and incorporating stage-specific predictors could improve the sensitivity of classification approaches across various imaging modalities, and move us closer to the goal of using neuroanatomical features for prospective patient selection in future clinical trials in schizophrenia.
In clinical samples, patients in early stage of illness are likely to have varied outcome trajectories and thus more diverse pathophysiological signatures compared to patients with more established chronic illness. With chronicity the degree of clinical heterogeneity reduces to some extent as only those who require continued support remain with services. Despite this, we note that the neuroanatomical features in chronic sample are less informative than the pattern seen in earlier stages, suggesting that the degree of putative anatomical reorganization in more representative samples may indeed be greater than that captured by this study.
Our study has several strengths. The sample was recruited from a single site, with identical acquisition parameters used in the two scanners and identical image processing. Further, we demonstrate (Supplementary Table S7) that the scanner variation does not influence the observed results. We studied the entire cortical surface without restricting our analysis to a priori regions. Studies estimating lobar volumes are likely to miss the reciprocal regional changes; in contrast, we examined anatomically defined sulcogyral subdivisions to capture thickness changes in both directions.
The limitations of the present study include the lack of longitudinal data to estimate illness onset, exposure to recreational substances and antipsychotic medications in a systematic manner. Structural changes are suspected to be linked to exposure to antipsychotics (Ho et al. Reference Ho, Andreasen, Ziebell, Pierson and Magnotta2011). We did not have the data for cumulative antipsychotic exposure for this sample. Nevertheless, when the effect due to the prescribed antipsychotic dose in our sample of clinically stable patients (dose unchanged at least for a period of 6 weeks at the time of scan) was removed from the analysis, the results were not altered (Supplementary Fig. S3). We cannot conclude whether the putative ameliorative changes reported here depend on exposure to antipsychotics. Longitudinal follow-up, ideally starting during the prodrome, is the best design to address the question of compensatory brain changes; cross-sectional data such as the one presented here are likely to be affected by a confound between generational or cohort differences and age. We controlled for age in our analyses, but acknowledge that this may not be sufficient to eliminate the cohort effects in a cross-sectional design. To date, most longitudinal structural studies have had a modest range of follow-up periods (median time of 2.4 years), and rarely capture the brain changes right from the onset or beyond 7 years of illness (Vita et al. Reference Vita, Peri, Deste and Sacchetti2012). In addition, multiple changes in image acquisition procedures that often take place during longitudinal studies greatly affect the ability to observe spatially circumscribed changes (as opposed to total brain volume or lobar volume) in a reliable manner (Takao et al. Reference Takao, Hayashi and Ohtomo2013). The neurohistological basis of changes in MRI-derived cortical thickness is unclear, although this measure corresponds closely to biopsy-based measurement of cortical thickness (Cardinale et al. Reference Cardinale, Chinnici, Bramerio, Mai, Sartori, Cossu, Lo Russo, Castana, Colombo, Caborni, De Momi and Ferrigno2014). Nevertheless, in the wake of the limitations discussed here, our results regarding putative ‘normalization’ at later stages of schizophrenia must be considered preliminary until examined in a follow-up design.
Neuroimaging-based machine-learning approaches are considered as promising tools for a stratified approach in psychiatry (Lawrie et al. Reference Lawrie, Olabi, Hall, McIntosh, Sm, BO and Am2011; Borgwardt & Fusar-Poli, Reference Borgwardt and Fusar-Poli2012; Koutsouleris et al. Reference Koutsouleris, Davatzikos, Borgwardt, Gaser, Bottlender, Frodl, Falkai, Riecher-Rössler, Möller, Reiser, Pantelis and Meisenzahl2014). Considering the neuroanatomy of schizophrenia as a dynamic reorganization rather than static alteration will greatly improve the accuracy of diagnostic/outcome prediction and the clinical utility of MRI scans. Our observations also raise the possibility that if the compensatory brain changes can be further characterized in schizophrenia, harnessing brain's plasticity optimally for therapeutic purposes may become feasible in the near future.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000994.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful for the assistance of Dr Pavan Mallikarjun, Dr Vijender Balain and other colleagues of Centre for Translational Neuroimaging in Mental Heath for helping with recruitment and clinical data acquisition. Professor Dorothee Auer (Division of Radiological Sciences, University of Nottingham) and Professor Penny Gowland (Sir Peter Mansfield MR Centre) advised on image acquisition. We also extend our gratitude to all the patients who took part in the study.
L.P. was originally supported by a Grant from the Wellcome Trust (grant no. 076448/Z/05/Z) and currently by the Department of Psychiatry & Schulich School of Medicine Dean's Support funds, Western University, Ontario. Data acquisition for this study was in part supported by Medical Research Council Grant: G0601442. J.F.F. is a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award holder. J.F.F. is also partially supported by the National High Technology Research and Development Program of China (No. 2015AA020507) and the key project of Shanghai Science & Technology Innovation Plan (No. 15JC1400101). The research was partially supported by the National Centre for Mathematics and Interdisciplinary Sciences (NCMIS) of the Chinese Academy of Sciences and Key Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 91230201). S.G. is supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC) (grant no. 11271121), the Program for New Century Excellent Talents in University (NCET-13-0786) and the Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province (2015JJ1010).
Declaration of Interest
All authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.