Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T01:16:56.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Scandal, Hypocrisy, and Resignation: How Partisanship Shapes Evaluations of Politicians’ Transgressions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 December 2020

Adam D. Wolsky*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37203, USA
*
Email: adam.d.wolsky@vanderbilt.edu, Twitter: @adamdwolsky
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Hypocrisy is a common feature of political scandal. Yet, it is unclear how individuals evaluate hypocritical misconduct differently based on a transgressing politician’s partisan identity. Using survey experiments, this article assesses how exposure to different frames of wrong doing involving actual members of congress spill over on to the evaluation of parties distinctly among co-partisans and out-partisans. I find that Republicans feel more positive towards their party after reading about the resignation of a hypocritical co-partisan politician compared to merely reading about the politician’s hypocrisy. In addition, Republicans feel warmer about their party when reading about a hypocritical versus non-hypocritical out-party transgression. However, Democrats do not change their party evaluations after being exposed to different scandal frames involving co-partisan and out-partisan politicians. This suggests that Republicans and Democrats have different attitudes towards hypocrisy and/or differently apply information about individuals when evaluating parties.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Experimental Research Section of the American Political Science Association

In a 2009 interview with CNBC, then-congressmen Tom Price admonished his Democratic colleagues for the use of expensive private jets, stating “this is just another example of fiscal irresponsibility run amok in Congress” (Pramuk Reference Pramuk2017). Eight years later, the media revealed that Price, acting as Donald Trump’s first Secretary of Health and Human Services, had taken at least two dozen trips with private and military jets to Europe and Asia at the cost of around $1 million dollars in taxpayer money. Left-leaning journalist Rachel Maddow gleefully reported the story, including details about Tom Price using the private jets to have lunch with his son (Maddow Reference Maddow2017). Meanwhile, a slew of Republican politicians criticized Price’s actions (Fox Reference Fox2017), and within two weeks of the first story, Price resigned from his position (Gambino Reference Gambino2017).

The journalists who uncovered Tom Price’s use of taxpayer-funded private jets created a mediated political scandal, which can be defined as an action or event that transgresses values, norms or moral codes that is made public by the news media and provokes reactions from disapproval to outrage (Esser and Hartung Reference Esser and Hartung2004; Lee Reference Lee2018; Thompson Reference Thompson2000). As the scandal unfolded, journalists who referenced Price’s prior statements on private jets highlighted his hypocrisy, or “inconsistency between claimed behavior and actual behavior” (Powell and Smith Reference Powell and Smith2013, 414). Hypocrisy is a common feature of political scandal. It can characterize misconduct from politicians of all ideological and partisan stripes from “family values” Republican and former Louisiana Senator David Vitter, who faced revelations that he had solicited prostitutes, to former Florida Democratic congresswoman and public servant Corrine Brown, who took $1 million from a fake charity meant to provide scholarships for poor children.

Drawing from two actual political scandals involving US members of congress from both major political parties, this paper tests how partisans evaluate parties after being exposed to the different stages of a hypocritical scandal – an initial scandal, revelations of hypocrisy, and when a hypocritical scandal is followed by a censuring response by same-party leaders. This article makes two primary contributions to the literature on the spillover effects of political scandals. First, I test how different features of scandal spill over on to party evaluations distinctly among co-partisans and out-partisans. Second, I assess how party evaluations change after a scandal ends in party-sanctioned resignation. I find that Republicans feel more positive toward their party after reading about the resignation of a hypocritical co-partisan politician compared to merely reading about the politician’s hypocrisy. Furthermore, I find that Republicans feel warmer about their party when reading about a hypocritical versus non-hypocritical out-party transgression. However, Democrats do not change their party evaluations after being exposed to different scandal frames involving co-partisan and out-partisan politicians. This suggests that Republicans and Democrats have different attitudes toward hypocrisy and/or they differently apply information about individuals when evaluating parties.

Political scandal, spillover effects, and hypocrisy

There is widespread evidence that political scandals may damage the evaluations of politicians involved in scandal (Carlson, Ganiel, and Hyde Reference Carlson, Ganiel and Hyde2000; Doherty, Dowling, and Miller Reference Doherty, Dowling and Miller2011, Reference Doherty, Dowling and Miller2014; Funk Reference Funk1996; McDermott, Schwartz, and Vallejo Reference McDermott, Schwartz and Vallejo2015; Żemojtel-Piotrowska et al. Reference Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Marganski, Baran and Piotrowski2017). The negative effects of scandals involving individual politicians can also have consequences for evaluations of political parties associated with scandal-ridden politicians (Lee Reference Lee2018; Maier Reference Maier2011; Schwarz and Bless Reference Schwarz and Bless1992; von Sikorski, Heiss, and Matthes Reference von Sikorski, Heiss and Matthes2020). These “spillover effects” of political scandals occur when an individual’s interconnected cognitions are activated and people associate one target with another (Lee Reference Lee2018). For example, when a politician is hypocritical, people not only evaluate the individual poorly but they also negatively appraise the politician’s party (von Sikorski and Herbst Reference von Sikorski and Herbst2020).

Spillover effects of scandals may not be uniformly distributed in the United States as political evaluations are increasingly viewed through a partisan lens (Iyengar, Sood, and Lelkes Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012; Iyengar and Westwood Reference Iyengar and Westwood2015; Jacobson Reference Jacobson2013; Mason Reference Mason2015). Scandals represent actions or behavior that may be incongruent to co-partisans, who routinely engage in motivated reasoning, rejecting incongruous information, and only accepting information which conforms to their beliefs (Erisen, Redlawsk, and Erisen Reference Erisen, Redlawsk and Erisen2018; Redlawsk Reference Redlawsk2002; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006). Previous literature on hypocrisy and political scandal has found that hypocritical scandals are judged more harshly than non-hypocritical scandals (McDermott, Schwartz, and Vallejo Reference McDermott, Schwartz and Vallejo2015), but it is unclear whether partisans would evaluate hypocritical and non-hypocritical scandals differently. Some suggest that scandals may erode support among a politician’s followers (von Sikorski, Heiss, and Matthes Reference von Sikorski, Heiss and Matthes2020), but others find politicians who are involved in scandal retain their support (Fischle Reference Fischle2000). The additional information of hypocrisy in a scandal may cause co-partisans to reach an affective tipping point (Redlawsk, Civettini, and Emmerson Reference Redlawsk, Civettini and Emmerson2010), or co-partisans may discount hypocrisy (Redlawsk Reference Redlawsk2002). This leads to two competing expectations: compared to a scandal without hypocritical information, exposure to a scandal with hypocritical information will result in lower party evaluations among co-partisans (H1a), or hypocritical information will be disregarded and will not significantly lower party evaluations among co-partisans (H1b).

As a scandal involving an individual politician develops, party leaders sometimes encourage her to resign. In theory, co-partisans will assimilate the congruent information about the removal of a hypocritical politician and dismiss the incongruent information about the hypocritical scandal itself (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006). Therefore, I expect that co-partisans will evaluate their party more positively when they are exposed to the successful purge of a hypocritical politician from office compared to when they are merely exposed to a politician involved in a hypocritical political scandal (H2).

Exposure to information about out-party scandals, especially those that feature hypocrisy, ought to be perceived as a boon for out-partisans in the context of an affectively polarized society. Out-partisans can even experience schadenfreude – pleasure at another’s misfortune – when exposed to embarrassing mishaps involving out-party politicians (Schurtz et al. Reference Schurtz, Combs, Hoogland, Smith, van Dijk and Ouwerkerk2014), which can boost group esteem (Ouwerkerk and van Dijk Reference Ouwerkerk, van Dijk, van Dijk and Ouwerkerk2014). This suggests that, while a scandal committed by a politician from one party may increase party evaluations of a different party (Lee Reference Lee2018), a hypocritical scandal could bolster party evaluations even more. Thus, I hypothesize that exposure to information about a hypocritical scandal committed by an out-partisan politician will enhance positive feelings toward one’s party more than exposure to a scandal without hypocrisy (H3).

Research design

To test my hypotheses about scandals, hypocrisy, and resignation, I fielded two studies on Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) that exposed participants to information about actual members of congress who had each been involved in a sex scandal.Footnote 1 After participants consented to taking a Qualtrics survey, they were asked their party identification and level of news consumption.Footnote 2 Then participants were randomly exposed to one of three experimental treatments. Study 1’s (N = 507) experimental stimulus focused on a scandal involving Tim Murphy, a conservative Republican congressman from Pennsylvania, and Study 2 (N = 504) exposed participants to information about a scandal involving John Conyers, a liberal Democratic congressman from Michigan.Footnote 3

Participants in each study were randomly assigned to parts or all of the following text:

Study 1:

Tim Murphy, a Republican congressman from Pennsylvania, admitted to having an extramarital affair with a personal friend in September 2017. Murphy is a member of the House Pro-Life Caucus in Congress and has voted to impose more restrictions on abortion access. News emerged that he pressured his mistress to get an abortion during a pregnancy scare. After House GOP leaders and senior Republicans put pressure on Murphy to step down, he resigned in early October.

Study 2:

John Conyers, a Democratic congressman from Michigan, was accused of sexual harassment by several women in November and December 2017. Conyers had previously cosponsored legislation which helped assist victims of sexual assault and domestic violence. The women, some of whom had worked for Conyers, say that among other things, Conyers groped and propositioned them for sex. After House Democratic leaders and other senior Democrats put pressure on Conyers to step down, he resigned in early December.

The first group was randomly assigned to only receive the underlined text (scandal condition), the second group was given the underlined text and the bold text (hypocrisy condition), and the third group was given the entire text (resignation condition).Footnote 4 Thus, in Study 1, all participants were aware of Murphy’s party affiliation and extramarital affair, but only some participants saw Murphy’s incongruity of past actions and current behavior. Further, only those in the third group saw that members of his party effectively pushed him to resign.Footnote 5 These two studies were meant to complement each other, and I did not have any expectations as to differences between how co-partisans of different parties would react given the disdain hypocrites face in general (Jordan et al. Reference Jordan, Roseanna, Paul and Rand2017; Laurent et al. Reference Laurent, Clark, Walker and Wiseman2014; Powell and Smith Reference Powell and Smith2013).Footnote 6

After reading the text, all participants were asked to evaluate the political parties on a “feeling thermometer” – a 101-point scale where 0 indicates very cold feelings toward the party, 50 indicates indifference, and 100 indicates feeling very warm toward the party. Participants were first asked about the party of the target politician, so those in Study 1 were first asked about the Republican Party and those in Study 2 were first asked about the Democratic Party. Participants were then asked the same feeling thermometer question about the out-party of the target politician. These questions serve as my main dependent variables.

This study builds on previous experimental work manipulating frames from real political scandals (Cortina and Rottinghaus Reference Cortina and Rottinghaus2017; Maier Reference Maier2011), but I employ a vignette approach similar to scholars who have investigated the effects of political scandals committed by hypothetical politicians (Bhatti, Hansen, and Olsen Reference Bhatti, Hansen and Olsen2013; Botero et al. Reference Botero, Cornejo, Gamboa, Pavao and Nickerson2015, Reference Botero, Cornejo, Gamboa, Pavao and Nickerson2019a, Reference Botero, Cornejo, Gamboa, Pavao and Nickerson2019b; Doherty, Dowling, and Miller Reference Doherty, Dowling and Miller2011, Reference Doherty, Dowling and Miller2014; McDermott, Schwartz, and Vallejo Reference McDermott, Schwartz and Vallejo2015; Winters and Weitz-Shapiro Reference Winters and Weitz-Shapiro2013, Reference Winters and Weitz-Shapiro2015; Żemojtel-Piotrowska et al. Reference Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Marganski, Baran and Piotrowski2017). The vignette format exposes experimental participants to the most pertinent information, akin to headlines or sub-headlines, at different stages of scandal – the initial scandal, second-order transgressions, and resolution. Furthermore, this format may reduce recall of other details of the scandal because of the brevity of the treatment compared to a newspaper article.Footnote 7 Nevertheless, using actual political events may run the risk of pretreatment, which may bias against finding significant effects (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012).

Results

I first test if individuals feel less warm toward their own political party after being exposed to a co-partisan politician engaging in hypocritical transgression compared to a co-partisan politician who merely transgressed without apparent hypocrisy. To assess this proposition (H1), I conducted difference-in-means tests of feeling thermometer evaluations of the target’s co-partisans in the first scandal condition to co-partisans in the hypocrisy condition.Footnote 8 In Study 1, I find that, on average, Republican identifiers and leaners feel cooler toward their own party in the hypocrisy condition (56.9 degrees) compared to the baseline scandal condition (62.3 degrees), but this difference is shy of statistical significance (t = 1.19 and p = .10 in a one-tailed test). Similarly, I find that Democrats and leaners are slightly cooler upon being exposed to the hypocrisy condition (59.3 degrees) compared to in the baseline scandal condition (62.6 degrees) in Study 2, but again this difference is not statistically significant (t = 1 and p = .16 in a one-tailed test). These results indicate that hypocrisy did not create an affective tipping among partisans of the target, suggesting that partisans discounted the additional negative information of hypocrisy.Footnote 9

To test if co-partisans feel better about their party after they are exposed to party leaders’ successful purging of the hypocritical politician (H2), I again compared the means of feeling thermometer ratings of the target’s co-partisans, but this time in the hypocrisy condition to the resignation condition. In Study 1, I find that Republicans feel 8.9 degrees more warmly toward their own party (t = −2.28 and p < .05 in a one-tailed t-test) between evaluations of the Republican Party in the hypocrisy versus the resignation treatment (see Figure 1). In Study 2, however, the resignation treatment and the hypocrisy treatment produce nearly identical feeling thermometer ratings for Democrats (t = −0.03 and p = .49 in a one-tailed t-test), suggesting that Democratic leaders’ pressure and Conyers’ resignation was not enough to make them feel more warmly toward their party (see Figure 2).

Figure 1 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Republican Party Among Republicans (Study 1).

Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Figure 2 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Democratic Party Among Democrats (Study 2).

Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Why were Republicans more sensitive to the treatments than Democrats? Various studies have found that conservatives and Republicans value loyalty and authority more than liberals and Democrats (Graham, Haidt, and Nosek Reference Graham, Haidt and Nosek2009; Rempala, Okdie, and Garvey Reference Rempala, Okdie and Garvey2016; Severson Reference Severson2018). Thus, Republicans may have felt a strong sense of betrayal after exposure to group norm violations when reading about hypocrisy, but a sense of justice after observing co-partisan leaders “correcting” the violation. Differences in the stimulus may have also contributed to diverging results in the two studies. The hypocrisy treatment in Study 1 included information about an affair and novel information about the congressmen pressuring his mistress to have an abortion. However, in Study 2, the hypocrisy treatment included information about harassment, but the additional information was about the severity of the transgression. Furthermore, going against the congressional caucus of which one is a member (Study 1) may be seen as a greater betrayal than behaving inconsistently with a single piece of legislation (Study 2).

My final hypothesis (H3) anticipates that out-party partisans evaluate their own party more positively when observing a hypocritical scandal versus a non-hypocritical scandal by an out-partisan politician. I compared feeling thermometers of the out-party for out-partisans of the target in the scandal and hypocrisy conditions using difference-in-means tests. Figures 3 and 4 present the results for Study 1 and Study 2, respectively. While in both studies, out-partisans move in a positive direction, only in Study 2 do Republicans experience a statistically significant boost in feelings of their party, moving from 59.3 to 68 degrees on the feeling thermometer (t = −1.87 and p < .05 in a one-tailed t-test). Again, I find that Republicans appear more sensitive to the treatment than Democrats. Despite the evidence that both Democrats and Republicans “loathe” their opponents (Iyengar, Sood, and Lelkes Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012), some work has found that Republicans have stronger bias against Democrats and feel more positive when seeing the Democratic Party fail than vice versa (Iyengar and Westwood Reference Iyengar and Westwood2015; Zillmann and Knobloch Reference Zillmann and Knobloch2001).Footnote 10

Figure 3 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Democratic Party Among Democrats (Study 1).

Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Figure 4 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Republican Party Among Republicans (Study 2).

Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Finally, I explore two additional tests of my theory. First, I test how strength of partisanship conditions reactions to scandal, hypocrisy, and resignation of scandalous politicians. Those with strong priors (i.e. strong partisans) have been shown to be stronger motivated reasoners than those with weaker priors (i.e. weak partisans and partisan leaners) and more resilient to change in attitudes toward their party after experiencing incongruent information (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006). I anticipate that strong partisans will discount a hypocritical political scandal compared to a non-hypocritical political scandal, leading to no difference in party evaluations between the two conditions (extending H1b). However, weak partisans and leaners will be more likely to change and reach an affective tipping point (Bisgaard Reference Bisgaard2015; Gaines et al. Reference Gaines, Kuklinski, Quirk, Buddy and Verkuilen2007; Redlawsk, Civettini, and Emmerson Reference Redlawsk, Civettini and Emmerson2010) (extending H1a). I expect that strong partisans will boost their party evaluations more than weak partisans and leaners in the resignation condition compared to the hypocrisy condition (extending H2), and as out-partisans in the hypocrisy condition compared to the scandal condition (extending H3) because strong partisans prioritize directional goals more than weak partisans and leaners (Schaffner and Roche Reference Schaffner and Roche2017).

I test these extensions of my hypotheses by regressing the feeling thermometers on the interaction between the treatment and strength of partisanship.Footnote 11 The results are underpowered, but reveal some interesting differences between strong and weak partisans and leaners. First, the results of H2 in Study 1 appear to be driven by weak partisans (increasing from 53.8 to 60.9 degrees, t = −1.36 and p < .1 in a one-tailed test). Next, although I found no evidence for H3 in Study 1 among all Democrats, leaners experience a statistically significant increase in party evaluations from 45.7 to 56.4 degrees (t = 1.62 and p < .1 in a one-tailed test). In Study 2, I found no evidence for H1a for all Democrats, but breaking the results down by strength of partisanship, I find that those leaning Democratic experience a huge drop in party evaluations from the scandal condition to the hypocrisy condition (moving from 55.5 to 36.3, t = 3.4 and p < .01 in a one-tailed test). Finally, I find that the boost out-group members feel toward their party after exposure to a hypocritical scandal compared to a non-hypocritical scandal of an out-party politician is driven by weak Republicans (increasing from 51.3 to 64.5 degrees, t = 1.5 and p < .05 in a one-tailed test) in Study 2. Altogether this suggests that those with weaker attachments are more sensitive to congruent and incongruent information than strong partisans.

Second, I probe how news exposure affects how partisans interpret the different treatments. The most politically interested and knowledgeable tend to double down on their attitudes and are more likely to engage in motivated reasoning (Erisen, Redlawsk, and Erisen Reference Erisen, Redlawsk and Erisen2018; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006).Footnote 12 Thus, I expect that co-partisans who pay closer attention to the news are more likely to discount information on hypocrisy than those who pay less attention (H1b), are more likely to embrace information about a co-partisan hypocrite’s resignation (compared to hypocritical scandal) (H2), and are more likely to boost party evaluations when exposed to a hypocritical (versus non-hypocritical) out-party politician (H3). To test this, first I created a dummy variable of news exposure where those who say they follow the news very closely are coded as 1, and those who follow the news somewhat, not very or not at all closely, are coded as 0. Then I regressed the feeling thermometer evaluations for each hypothesis on the interaction of news exposure and the treatments. Only one of the interactions is statistically significant in a one-tailed test (p < .05). For H1 in Study 1, Republicans who follow the news very closely had a higher feeling thermometer of the Republican Party in the non-hypocritical scandal condition (70.8) compared to the hypocrisy condition (56.1), but those who did not follow the news very closely had nearly identical feeling thermometer ratings for the two treatments (57.3 and 57.5). This suggests that politically aware individuals were more sensitive to treatments, but this pattern does not hold for the other hypotheses (see Supplementary Appendix Figures 7A12A and Tables 11A16A).

Discussion and conclusions

Political scandals involving individual politicians have the potential to damage evaluations of a politicians’ party (Lee Reference Lee2018; von Sikorski, Heiss, and Matthes Reference von Sikorski, Heiss and Matthes2020). This study employs vignette experiments that draw on actual political scandals. Although this may enhance external validity, it also has its limitations. First, respondents may be pretreated with information about the political scandal, thus attenuating treatment effects (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012). Second, vignette experiments may not replicate how respondents would gain information about scandals in the real world despite the use of a real-world scandal. Future work could overcome these limitations by employing an article format (cf. Carlson, Ganiel, and Hyde Reference Carlson, Ganiel and Hyde2000; Cortina and Rottinghaus Reference Cortina and Rottinghaus2017; Maier Reference Maier2011; von Sikorski and Herbst Reference von Sikorski and Herbst2020). Moreover, these two studies recruited via Amazon MTurk, whose pool is more diverse than other convenience samples, but tends to be younger, more male, less religious, more educated, and more liberal than the US population (Boas, Christenson, and Glick Reference Boas, Christenson and Glick2020; Levay, Freese, and Druckman Reference Levay, Jeremy and Druckman2016).Footnote 13 While an MTurk sample produces a valid Sample Average Treatment Effect (SATE) (Coppock and McClellan Reference Coppock and McClellan2019), future work should test the hypotheses on a more representative sample.

In the polarized two-party system of the United States, spillover effects of scandals differ significantly depending not only on the politician involved but also who is evaluating the politician. There was a large gap in party evaluations between Republicans who were exposed to the hypocritical transgression and those who read about the resignation of the hypocritical co-partisan politician. Furthermore, Republicans felt significantly warmer toward their own party when reading about a hypocritical transgression than non-hypocritical scandal committed by a Democratic politician. Yet, Democrats did not feel significantly different about their party after exposure to both in-party and out-party hypocritical scandals. Future work should investigate if attitudes toward hypocrisy differ by partisans from the two parties, how strength of partisanship shapes evaluations of hypocritical behavior from individual politicians, and how partisans from different parties may assimilate attitudes toward individuals into groups.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2020.36.

Footnotes

Funding was made possible by the Research on Individuals, Politics, and Society (RIPS) Lab at Vanderbilt University. I would like to thank Cindy Kam, Liz Zechmeister, participants at the RIPS lab, my graduate student colleagues at Vanderbilt, two anonymous reviewers, and the editors at the Journal of Experimental Political Science for their invaluable feedback on previous drafts of this manuscript. The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/P7RYPP. The author declares no conflict of interest.

1 I consulted both local and national news sources to obtain information for both studies. I read articles from CNN, NPR, NBC News, the New York Times, Washington Post, Time, Politico, and Pittsburgh-Post Gazette on Tim Murphy’s scandal. For information on John Conyers’ scandal, I read articles from The New Yorker, Atlantic, Washington Post, New York Times, Politico, Huffington Post, NBC News, LA Times, Milwaukee Journal Sentinel, and Click on Detroit. Both studies condensed the news down to 1–3 sentences depending on the treatment (more details below).

2 In order to qualify for the study, participants had to be both US citizens and at least 18 years of age. I used the standard question on partisanship from the American National Election Study (ANES). If participants reported that they were a Democrat or Republican, a follow-up question asked about strength of partisanship. If participants indicated that they were an Independent or from another party, they were subsequently asked if they thought of themselves as closer to the Democratic or Republican Party.

3 Full details of the recruitment and screening procedures for both studies can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. Following Kennedy et al. (Reference Kennedy, Clifford, Burleigh, Waggoner, Jewell and Winter2020), I eliminated all respondents whose IP addresses denoted that they were outside of the United States, and whose IP addresses indicated that they were using a Virtual Private Server (VPS).

4 Randomization appeared successful as likelihood ratio tests from multinomial logit regressions of the treatment assignment on participants’ observable characteristics were not statistically significant (Study 1: Wald χ2 (16) = 11.3, p = .88; Study 2: Wald χ2 (16) = 15.5, p = .63) see Table 4A in the Supplementary Appendix.

5 The news of Murphy’s affair first broke on September 6, 2017, subsequent developments about pressures for abortion were reported in October, and Murphy resigned on October 21, 2017. Study 1 was conducted on November 17–18, 2017, thus taking place 27 days after Murphy’s resignation. Initial reports that Conyers settled a sexual harassment case with a staffer in 2015 emerged on November 21, 2017, more women came forward in late November, and Conyers resigned on December 5, 2017. Study 2 was conducted on March 2, 2018, 88 days after Conyers’ resignation.

6 I chose cases in which politicians violated norms of the party, abortion in Study 1 and sexual harassment/assault in Study 2, which should be more relevant to co-partisans of the politician. In the 2016 ANES Election Study, a majority of Republicans and leaners (57%) said that abortion should never be permitted or only permitted in the case or rape or incest, and nearly 60% of Democrats and leaners say a woman should always be able to obtain an abortion as a matter of personal choice. Conversely, Democrats had an average of 71 on a feeling thermometer asking about the #metoo movement, far warmer than Republicans who averaged 26 according to the 2018 ANES Pilot Survey. If I had swapped the parties, for example, having a non-prototypical pro-life Democrat pressure a mistress to get an abortion, I would not expect Democrats to feel worse about their party, and I would expect that Democrats would feel far better about their party after this hypothetical Democrat resigned. Furthermore, I might anticipate that Republicans would feel better about their own party after witnessing out-party hypocrisy, but experiencing out-party hypocrisy of a pro-life Democrat might not generate warmer feelings of the Republican Party among Republicans.

7 Maier (Reference Maier2011) exposed experimental participants to a bogus news article about an actual scandal that broke a few months before his study was carried out. He found that few participants mentioned the scandal in an open-ended question about scandal recall, and removing participants who indicated they knew about the scandal prior to the treatment did not affect the results of his study. Unfortunately, I did not ask about knowledge of the scandals in either study, but following the treatment and questions about the dependent variables for Study 2, I asked if participants could identify Conyers’ race. I assume that those who knew his race were more likely to be familiar with the scandal. To test if knowledge about Conyers’ race affected the results, I created and interacted a dummy variable where 1 was equal to correctly identifying Conyers’ race and 0 was equal to incorrectly identifying Conyers’ race, saying they do not know or having an invalid response. Removing respondents who knew about Conyers’ race did not affect the results of Study 2 (see Supplementary Appendix Tables 20A–22A).

8 In all subsequent analyses, I pooled together participants who report that they were partisans and partisan leaners, excluding pure independents and those who support other parties from the analysis. In Study 1, 85.2% of eligible participants (N = 432/507) and in Study 2, 85.9% of eligible participants (N = 433/504) were partisans or partisan leaners. Summary statistics are included in the Supplementary Appendix (Table 1A-2A).

9 While one might expect the initial scandal condition to have lower ratings of the in-party for co-partisans of the target because they are exposed to negative information about him, I find that there are no significant differences in feeling thermometer means for partisans in the scandal condition of the two studies. That is, Republicans and leaners average feeling thermometer ratings are 62.2 in Study 1 and 59.3 in Study 2 (t = .66 and p = .75 in a one-tailed t-test). Similarly, Democrats and leaners average feeling thermometer ratings are 62.5 in Study 2 and 64.1 in Study 1 (t =.48 and p = .32 in a one-tailed t-test). Thus, the baseline scandal treatment does not appear to boost feeling thermometer evaluations of the out-party of the scandalous politician nor dampen feeling thermometer evaluations of the in-party among co-partisans.

10 The Supplementary Appendix contains additional tests for robustness. In general, results remain robust to regressing the interaction between a dummy variable for Democrats and Republicans (excluding pure independents, but including leaners) and the treatments for each hypothesis (a) including demographic controls, (b) excluding respondents from the media market of the member of congress, (c) interacting for knowledge of the race of Conyers in Study 2, and (d) removing respondents who knew of Conyers race in Study 2 (Tables 17A–22A).

11 For ease of interpretation, I present the results from t-tests comparing the treatment conditions to each other among strong, weak, and leaning partisans. See Tables 5A–10A for full results of the regressions and Figures 1A-6A for predicted values from regressions in the Supplementary Appendix.

12 Ideally, I would include a measure of political awareness or political knowledge through a battery of factual knowledge questions. However, unfortunately, my studies did not include these kinds of questions. While self reports of news consumption may be an unsatisfying proxy for political awareness (Price and Czilli Reference Price and Czilli1996; Zaller Reference Zaller1992), I expect that those who pay closer attention to the news are more likely to be political interested and politically aware.

13 See Table 3A in the appendix that compares the pooled MTurk sample to the 2018 ANES Pilot Study.

References

Bhatti, Yosef, Hansen, Kasper M., and Olsen, Asmus Leth. 2013. Political Hypocrisy: The Effect of Political Scandals on Candidate Evaluations. Acta Politica 48(4): 408–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisgaard, Martin. 2015. Bias Will Find a Way: Economic Perceptions, Attributions of Blame, and Partisan-Motivated Reasoning during Crisis. Journal of Politics 77(3): 849–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, Taylor C., Christenson, Dino P., and Glick, David M.. 2020. Recruiting Large Online Samples in the United States and India: Facebook, Mechanical Turk, and Qualtrics. Political Science Research and Methods 8(2): 232–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botero, Sandra, Cornejo, Rodrigo Castro, Gamboa, Laura, Pavao, Nara, and Nickerson, David W.. 2015. Says Who? An Experiment on Allegations of Corruption and Credibility of Sources. Political Research Quarterly 68(3): 493504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botero, Sandra, Cornejo, Rodrigo Castro, Gamboa, Laura, Pavao, Nara, and Nickerson, David W.. 2019a. Are All Types of Wrongdoing Created Equal in the Eyes of Voters? Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. Published online first 12 August 2019. doi: 10.1080/17457289.2019.1651322.Google Scholar
Botero, Sandra, Cornejo, Rodrigo Castro, Gamboa, Laura, Pavao, Nara, and Nickerson, David W.. 2019b. Under Friendly Fire: An Experiment on Partisan Press, Fragmented Opposition and Voting Behavior. Electoral Studies 60: 102044. doi: 10.1016/j.electstud.2019.04.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, James, Ganiel, Gladys, and Hyde, Mark S.. 2000. Scandal and Political Candidate Image. Southeastern Political Review 28(4): 747–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, Alexander and McClellan, Oliver A.. 2019. Validating the Demographic, Political, Psychological, and Experimental Results Obtained from a New Source of Online Survey Respondents. Research and Politics 6(1): 2053168018822174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, Jeronimo and Rottinghaus, Brandon. 2017. Does Partisanship Stop at Scandal’s Edge? Partisan Resiliency and the Survival of Political Scandal. American Review of Politics 36(1): 229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, David, Dowling, Conor M., and Miller, Michael G.. 2011. Are Financial or Moral Scandals Worse? It Depends. Political Science & Politics 44(4): 749–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, David, Dowling, Conor M., and Miller, Michael G.. 2014. Does Time Heal All Wounds? Sex Scandals, Tax Evasion, and the Passage of Time. Political Science & Politics 47(2): 357–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N. and Leeper, Thomas J.. 2012. Learning More from Political Communication Experiments: Pretreatment and Its Effects. American Journal of Political Science 56(4): 875–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erisen, Cengiz, Redlawsk, David P., and Erisen, Elif. 2018. Complex Thinking as a Result of Incongruent Information Exposure. American Politics Research 46(2): 217–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esser, Frank and Hartung, Uwe. 2004. Nazis, Pollution, and No Sex: Political Scandals as a Reflection of Political Culture in Germany. American Behavioral Scientist 47(8): 1040–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischle, Mark. 2000. Mass Response to the Lewinsky Scandal: Motivated Reasoning or Bayesian Updating? Political Psychology 21(1): 135–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Lauren. 2017. ‘I think the President’s mad as hell’: Hill Republicans fume over Price’s travel costs. CNN (September 29). Retrieved from https://www.cnn.com/2017/09/28/politics/congress-reaction-tom-price-flights/index.html Google Scholar
Funk, Carolyn L. 1996. The Impact of Scandal on Candidate Evaluations: An Experimental Test of the Role of Candidate Traits. Political Behavior 18(1): 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaines, Brian J., Kuklinski, James H., Quirk, Paul J., Buddy, Peyton, and Verkuilen, Jay. 2007. Same Facts, Different Interpretations: Partisan Motivation and Opinion on Iraq. Journal of Politics 69(4): 957–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gambino, Lauren. 2017. Tom Price Resigns as Health Secretary Over Private Flights and Trump Criticism. The Guardian (September 29). Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/sep/29/tom-price-resigns-health-secretary-private-flights-trump Google Scholar
Graham, Jesse, Haidt, Jonathan, and Nosek, Brian A.. 2009. Liberals and Conservatives Rely on Different Sets of Moral Foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96(5): 1029–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyengar, Shanto, Sood, Gaurav, and Lelkes, Yphtach. 2012. Affect, Not Ideology: A Social Identity Perspective on Polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly 76(3): 405–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto and Westwood, Sean J.. 2015. Fear and Loathing across Party Lines: New Evidence on Group Polarization. American Journal of Political Science 59(3): 690707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, Gary C. 2013. Partisan Polarization in American Politics: A Background Paper. Presidential Studies Quarterly 43(4): 688708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, Jillian J., Roseanna, Sommers, Paul, Bloom, and Rand, David G.. 2017. Why Do We Hate Hypocrites? Evidence for a Theory of False Signaling. Psychological Science 28(3): 356–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, Ryan, Clifford, Scott, Burleigh, Tyler, Waggoner, Philip D., Jewell, Ryan, and Winter, Nicholas J. G.. 2020. The Shape of and Solutions to the MTurk Quality Crisis. Political Science Research and Methods. Published online first 24 April 2020. doi: 10.1017/psrm.2020.6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laurent, Sean M., Clark, Brian A. M., Walker, Stephannie, and Wiseman, Kimberly D.. 2014. Punishing Hypocrisy: The Roles of Hypocrisy and Moral Emotions in Deciding Culpability and Punishment of Criminal and Civil Moral Transgressors. Cognition and Emotion 28(1): 5983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, Francis L. F. 2018. The Spillover Effects of Political Scandals: The Moderating Role of Cynicism and Social Media Communications. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 95(3): 714–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levay, Kevin E., Jeremy, Freese, and Druckman, James N.. 2016. The Demographic and Political Composition of Mechanical Turk Samples. SAGE Open 6(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maddow, Rachel. 2017. Tom Price Private Plane Scandal Snowballs with New Revelations. MSNBC (September 28). Retrieved from https://www.msnbc.com/rachel-maddow/watch/tom-price-private-plane-scandal-snowballs-with-new-revelations-1057591363968 Google Scholar
Maier, Jürgen. 2011. The Impact of Political Scandals on Political Support: An Experimental Test of Two Theories. International Political Science Review 32(3): 283302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, Lilliana. 2015. ‘I Disrespectfully Agree’: The Differential Effects of Partisan Sorting on Social and Issue Polarization. American Journal of Political Science 59(1): 128–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, Monika L., Schwartz, Douglas, and Vallejo, Sebastian. 2015. Talking the Talk but Not Walking the Walk: Public Reactions to Hypocrisy in Political Scandal. American Politics Research 43(6): 952–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouwerkerk, Jaap W. and van Dijk, Wilco W.. 2014. Intergroup Rivalry and Schadenfreude. In Schadenfreude: Understanding Pleasure at the Misfortune of Others, eds. van Dijk, Wilco W. and Ouwerkerk, Jaap W.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 186–99.Google Scholar
Powell, Caitlin A.J. and Smith, Richard H.. 2013. Schadenfreude Caused by the Exposure of Hypocrisy in Others. Self and Identity 12(4): 413–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, Vincent and Czilli, Edward J.. 1996. Modeling Patterns of News Recognition and Recall. Journal of Communication 46(2): 5578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, David P. 2002. Hot Cognition or Cool Consideration ? Testing the Effects of Motivated Reasoning on Political Decision Making on Political Decision Making. Journal of Politics 64(4): 1021–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, David P., Civettini, Andrew J.W., and Emmerson, Karen M.. 2010. The Affective Tipping Point: Do Motivated Reasoners Ever ‘Get It’? Political Psychology 31(4): 563–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rempala, Daniel M., Okdie, Bradley M., and Garvey, Kilian J.. 2016. Articulating Ideology: How Liberals and Conservatives Justify Political Affiliations Using Morality-Based Explanations. Motivation and Emotion 40(5): 703–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pramuk, Jacob. 2017. Tom Price, who reportedly used costly private jets, once slammed government planes as ‘fiscal irresponsibility run amok.’ Cnbc.com (September 20). Retrieved from https://www.cnbc.com/2017/09/20/hhs-sec-tom-price-once-slammed-congress-private-jets.html Google Scholar
Schaffner, Brian F. and Roche, Cameron. 2017. Misinformation and Motivated Reasoning: Responses to Economic News in a Politicized Environment. Public Opinion Quarterly 81(1): 86110.Google Scholar
Schurtz, D. Ryan, Combs, David, Hoogland, Charles, and Smith, Richard H.. 2014. Schadenfreude in Sports and Politics: A Social Identity Perspective. In Schadenfreude: Understanding Pleasure at the Misfortune of Others, eds. van Dijk, W. W. and Ouwerkerk, J. W. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 170–85.Google Scholar
Schwarz, Norbert and Bless, Herbert. 1992. Scandals and the Public’s Trust in Politicians: Assimilation and Contrast Effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18(5): 574–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Severson, Alexander W. 2018. Partisan Affiliation and the Evaluation of Non-Prototypical Candidates. Journal of Experimental Political Science 5(2): 121–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Sikorski, Christian, Heiss, Raffael, and Matthes, Jörg. 2020. How Political Scandals Affect the Electorate. Tracing the Eroding and Spillover Effects of Scandals with a Panel Study. Political Psychology 41(3): 549–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Sikorski, Christian and Herbst, Christina. 2020. Not Practicing What They Preached! Exploring Negative Spillover Effects of News about Ex-Politicians’ Hypocrisy on Party Attitudes, Voting Intentions, and Political Trust. Media Psychology 23(3): 436–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taber, Charles S. and Lodge, Milton. 2006. Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50(3): 755–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, John B. 2000. Political Scandal: Power and Visibility in the Media Age. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Winters, Matthew S. and Weitz-Shapiro, Rebecca. 2013. Lacking Information or Condoning Corruption: When Do Voters Support Corrupt Politicians? Comparative Politics 45(4): 418–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, Matthew S. and Weitz-Shapiro, Rebecca. 2015. Political Corruption and Partisan Engagement: Evidence from Brazil. Journal of Politics in Latin America 7(1): 4581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolsky, Adam D. 2020. Replication Data for: Scandal, Hypocrisy, and Resignation: How Partisanship Shapes Evaluations of Politicians’ Transgressions. Harvard Dataverse. doi: 10.7910/DVN/P7RYPP Google Scholar
Zaller, John. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Magdalena, Marganski, Alison, Baran, Tomasz, and Piotrowski, Jarosław. 2017. Corruption and Sexual Scandal: The Importance of Politician Gender. Anales de psicología 33(1): 133–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, Dolf and Knobloch, Silvia. 2001. Emotional Reactions to Narratives about the Fortunes of Personae in the News Theater. Poetics 29(3): 189206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Republican Party Among Republicans (Study 1).Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Democratic Party Among Democrats (Study 2).Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Figure 2

Figure 3 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Democratic Party Among Democrats (Study 1).Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Figure 3

Figure 4 Predicted Values of Ratings of the Republican Party Among Republicans (Study 2).Note: Confidence intervals for means are at the 90% level.

Supplementary material: Link

Wolsky Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: File

Wolsky Supplementary Materials

Wolsky Supplementary Materials

Download Wolsky Supplementary Materials(File)
File 398.7 KB