Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:28:59.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Construct validity of cognitive reserve in a multiethnic cohort: The Northern Manhattan Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2009

KAREN L. SIEDLECKI
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
YAAKOV STERN*
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York Department of Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
AARON REUBEN
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
RALPH L. SACCO
Affiliation:
Division of Cognitive Disorders, Department of Neurology, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida Evelyn F. McKnight Center for Age Related Memory Loss, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida
MITCHELL S.V. ELKIND
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
CLINTON B. WRIGHT
Affiliation:
Division of Cognitive Disorders, Department of Neurology, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida Evelyn F. McKnight Center for Age Related Memory Loss, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Yaakov Stern, Cognitive Neuroscience Division, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, 630 West 168th Street, New York, New York 10032. E-mail: ys11@columbia.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Cognitive reserve is a hypothetical construct that has been used to inform models of cognitive aging and is presumed to be indicative of life experiences that may mitigate the effects of brain pathology. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the construct validity of cognitive reserve by examining both its convergent and its discriminant validity across three different samples of participants using structural equation modeling. The cognitive reserve variables were found to correlate highly with one another (thereby providing evidence of convergent validity), but demanding tests of discriminant validity indicated that, in two of the samples, the cognitive reserve construct was highly related to an executive functioning construct. (JINS, 2009, 15, 558–569.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2009

INTRODUCTION

The concept of reserve emerged as a mechanism to explain why there is not a perfect relationship between brain pathology postmortem and the severity of a clinical condition, such as vascular dementia (VaD) or Alzheimer disease (AD). Research has indicated that individuals with the same amount of brain pathology may have very different clinical manifestations of dementia. Increased reserve is hypothesized to be associated with protection against clinical expression of dementia because those individuals with higher reserve may be more efficient or better able to compensate for the pathological changes accumulating in the brain. An initial model of reserve indicated the existence of two classifications of reserve—brain reserve and cognitive reserve. Both brain reserve and cognitive reserve are hypothetical constructs that have been used to inform models of aging. Our purpose here was to examine whether it is meaningful to refer to cognitive reserve as a distinct construct that represents a unique dimension of individual differences.

Although brain reserve is an important theoretical construct in its own right, in this article, we are primarily concerned with the concept of cognitive reserve that, Stern (Reference Stern2002) has argued, is an active model of reserve comprising two main components—neural reserve and neural compensation. The neural reserve component of cognitive reserve refers to the efficiency or capacity of the brain prior to pathological damage. The neural compensation component of cognitive reserve refers to the recruitment of brain areas not typically used by individuals without brain damage to compensate for brain pathology.

Cognitive reserve is typically operationalized with variables that are reflective of lifetime experiences that may provide individuals with a set of skills to better manage pathological brain damage, as seen in diseases like AD and VaD. To that end, education level, literacy, occupational attainment, and performance on tests of knowledge (such as vocabulary) have been used as surrogates of cognitive reserve.

Evidence that greater educational attainment is associated with a reduced relative risk of developing AD has been demonstrated in the United States (e.g., Evans et al., Reference Evans, Hebert, Beckett, Scherr, Albert, Chown, Pilgrim and Taylor1997; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Gurland, Tatemichi, Tang, Wilder and Mayeux1994; White et al., Reference White, Katzman, Losonczy, Salive, Wallace, Berkman, Taylor, Fillenbaum and Havlik1994), China (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Katzman, Salmon, Jin, Cai, Wang, Qu, Grant, Yu, Levy, Klauber and Liu1990), France (Letenneur et al., Reference Letenneur, Commenges, Dartigues and Barberger-Gateau1994), and Sweden (Qiu et al., Reference Qiu, Backman, Winblad, Aguero-Torres and Frtiglioni2001). An inverse relationship between occupational attainment and incident dementia has been found in a number of studies as well (see Valenzuela & Sachdev, Reference Valenzuela and Sachdev2005, for a review).

Research has indicated that those individuals who have greater cognitive reserve tend to decline more quickly once diagnosed with AD. This finding supports the idea that at any given level of clinical severity, patients with higher cognitive reserve have more pathological brain damage (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Tang, Denaro and Mayeux1995).

Although cognitive reserve is typically invoked as an important concept in dementia research, there is also evidence that cognitive reserve may play a protective role against normal age-related declines. Age-related declines in measures of memory, processing speed, reasoning, and spatial ability have been well documented both cross-sectionally (e.g., Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2004) and longitudinally (e.g., Christensen, Reference Christensen2001). More education has been linked with slower declines in both cognition and function in cognitively healthy older adults over short intervals of 1 year (Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Kittner, Rae, Bartko and Regier1995) or 2–2.5 years (e.g., Albert et al., Reference Albert, Jones, Savage, Berkman, Seeman, Blazer and Rowe1995) using general measures of cognition such as the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) and a composite created from several neuropsychological tests. In a prospective study of older adults, Christensen et al. (Reference Christensen, Korten, Jorm, Henderson, Jacomb, Rodgers and MacKinnon1997) found that education was inversely related to the rate of decline in measures of language and knowledge (i.e., crystallized ability) but not in measures of memory, processing speed, or reaction time. Likewise, Salthouse (Reference Salthouse2006) reported that in a cross-sectional study, individuals in a high–cognitive stimulation group (i.e., individuals who engaged in mentally stimulating activities) were not significantly different from a low–cognitive stimulation group on measures of visual-spatial ability, memory, and processing speed. However, declines with age were different across the low- and high–cognitive stimulation groups for a vocabulary test such that scores decreased less for individuals in the high–cognitive stimulation group. Together, these findings suggest that high cognitive reserve (as measured by education or leisure activity) may be protective against declines in measures of crystallized ability but not protective against declines in other cognitive (fluid) domains. This finding is not surprising considering that vocabulary, for example, is sometimes used as a measure of cognitive reserve.

The validity of referring to cognitive reserve as a coherent construct has not been tested. In order to continue to refer to cognitive reserve as an independent construct, it is important to establish that the variables typically used as measures of cognitive reserve are correlated with one another and that they represent a unique dimension of individual differences that are distinct from other cognitive constructs.

Both convergent validity and discriminant validity are essential to establishing construct validity. Convergent validity can be evaluated by examining whether the variables hypothesized to represent a latent construct have significant variance in common, as determined by assessing the magnitudes of the factor loadings in a confirmatory factor analysis. The loadings of the observed cognitive reserve variables on the latent construct (i.e., cognitive reserve) should be moderately large in magnitude and significantly different from 0. Discriminant validity is evaluated by examining whether the construct of interest is distinct from other non-target constructs, as indicated by the interfactor correlations, which should be significantly less than 1.0. Using an individual differences approach, we evaluated the convergent and discriminant validity of the cognitive reserve construct with a set of analytic models designed to assess construct validity (Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Atkinson and Berish2003, Reference Salthouse, Berish and Siedlecki2004; Siedlecki et al., Reference Siedlecki, Salthouse and Berish2005).

In this project, the construct validity of the cognitive reserve construct was evaluated with four structural equation models described in detail in Salthouse et al. (Reference Salthouse, Atkinson and Berish2003). The models represent progressively more demanding tests of construct validity. Model A assesses only convergent validity by evaluating whether the cognitive reserve variables have significant variance in common. Model B assesses discriminant validity by examining the correlations among the cognitive reserve construct and the non-target cognitive constructs (represented by verbal memory, processing speed, and executive functioning/fluid ability in the current study). Model C extends Model B by allowing the non-target constructs to be related to each of the cognitive reserve variables, one at a time. If the relationship between a non-target construct and an observed cognitive reserve variable results in a significantly improved fit of the overall model (as determined by ∆χ 2/∆df), then that relationship is retained in the final Model C. Model D is the most demanding test of construct validity because each of the non-target constructs is allowed to be related to each of the observed cognitive reserve variables simultaneously. Convergent validity is assessed in Model D by examining whether there is significant residual variance among the cognitive reserve variables, after allowing them to be related to the non-target cognitive constructs. Discriminant validity is assessed by examining the magnitude of the relations among the cognitive reserve variables and the non-target constructs.

It should be noted that Salthouse et al. (Reference Salthouse, Atkinson and Berish2003) found that after examining constructs hypothesized to represent different aspects of executive functioning with the four construct validity models, there was only weak evidence that these constructs represented distinct dimensions of individual differences. An executive functioning construct comprising a set of neuropsychological variables was found to lack convergent and discriminant validity. Specifically, the executive functioning variables were highly related to measures of fluid ability. In the current project, executive functioning is included as one of the non-target constructs (in Samples 1 and 2). In terms of individual differences, we consider executive functioning and fluid ability to be highly related to one another, representing a common dimension of individual differences, but because they are different both practically and semantically, we will continue to refer to them as separate constructs. Also, the source population for Samples 1 and 2 is one for which executive function measures have been helpful in discriminating those with vascular cognitive dysfunction (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Festa, Paik, Schmiedigen, Brown, Yoshita, DeCarli, Sacco and Stern2008).

The purposes of this project were to evaluate the construct validity of cognitive reserve using structural equation modeling (SEM) in a population-based clinically stroke-free sample of cognitively healthy older adults and to examine the generalizability of the findings by evaluating the construct validity of the cognitive reserve in two additional samples of healthy adults.

METHOD

Participants

Three samples were selected for the current project: the primary sample and two replication samples. All participants had to be cognitively normal at baseline as measured by a Clinical Dementia Rating of 0. The data included in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with regulations of the local institutional review board.

Participants in Samples 1 and 2 were part of a magnetic resonance imaging substudy recruited from the larger Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS), a prospective population-based study designed to study stroke incidence, risk factors, and prognosis in a sample from a multiethnic urban community. Details regarding recruitment into NOMAS and the substudy have been documented elsewhere (Prabhakaran et al. Reference Prabhakaran, Wright, Yoshita, Delapaz, Brown, DeCarli and Sacco2008; Sacco et al., Reference Sacco, Boden-Albala, Abel, Lin, Elkind, Hauser, Paik and Shea2001). Sample 1 participants were native English speakers, and Sample 2 participants were native Spanish speakers. Demographic details of the primary sample and the two replication samples are presented in Table 1. Sample 2 had significantly lower mean education, age, and MMSE scores than the participants in the primary sample.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the three samples

Sample 3 comprised individuals who were not NOMAS subjects but had participated in other brain imaging studies at the Columbia University Medical Center and had completed tests of cognitive reserve. The individuals in Sample 3 were significantly younger than those in either the primary sample or the Sample 2.

Neuropsychological Evaluation for Samples 1 and 2

Participants in Samples 1 and 2 were given identical neuropsychological evaluations with the exception of the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT; Wilkinson, Reference Wilkinson1993), which was only administered to the primary sample, and Word Accentuation Test (WAT; Del Ser et al., Reference Del Ser, González-Montalvo, Martínez-Espinosa, Delgado-Villapalos and Bermejo1997), which was only administered to Sample 2. The language of administration was different between the two groups: English in Sample 1 and Spanish in Sample 2.

Memory

Memory was assessed with a modified California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000) in which participants heard a prerecorded list of 12 unrelated nouns across five trials and were asked to recall the words at the end of each trial. Participants were scored on the total number of words remembered across the five trials (CVLT-total), the total number of words recalled after a 20-min delay (CVLT-recall), and the total number of correctly recognized words in a recognition test comprising 12 words and 12 foils (CVLT-recognition).

Processing speed

Processing speed was assessed with the Grooved Pegboard task (Matthews & Klove, Reference Matthews and Klove1964) that measures the total time it takes the participant to place 25 small metal-grooved pegs into an array of holes with different orientations, requiring the subject to rotate the peg correctly in order to insert it into the pegboard, using both dominant and nondominant hands.

Executive functioning

Executive functioning was assessed with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997) Letter Number Sequencing subtest, the Odd-Man-Out task (Flowers & Robertson, Reference Flowers and Robertson1985), and a difference score calculated from the Color Trails Test (CTT; D’Elia et al., Reference D’Elia, Satz, Uchiyama and White1996).

In the Letter Number Sequencing subtest,Footnote a participants are presented verbally with strings of intermixed letters and numbers (that become increasingly longer on a trial basis) and are asked to repeat the items back in a specific sequence.

The Odd-Man-Out task consists of four sets of subtests and involves the participant selecting which item in a set of three does not belong with the other items. Scores of each of the subtests were summed to create an Odd-Man-Out total score.

The CTT requires participants to connect numbers (Form A) or numbers alternating in color (Form B) in numerical order as quickly as possible. The time it takes the participants to complete Trails B minus the time to complete Trails A (Trails difference) is thought to be indicative of cognitive flexibility (ability to switch between sets) irrespective of general speed or ability to perceive and respond to stimuli and, as such, was used as the measure in this study.

Cognitive reserve

Three variables were hypothesized to represent the cognitive reserve construct—education (as defined in terms of level of educational attainment in which 20 is the highest level, representing a doctoral degree), performance on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-third edition (PPVT-III; Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn1997), and performance on the reading subtest of either the WRAT or the WAT. Each of these variables presumably reflects life experiences, above and beyond that of age, that have the potential to provide protection against clinical manifestation of disease in the brain and has been used in the literature as a proxy for cognitive reserve.

The PPVT comprises 204 black-and-white line drawings of items that participants are required to name. We used the total number of correct responses as the measure. The WRAT Reading subtest (completed by the participants in Sample 1) comprises 55 words that the participant must read out loud to the examiner. The WAT (completed by the participants in Sample 2) comprises 30 infrequent Spanish words written without an accentuation mark. The participants are instructed to read the words out loud. The total number pronounced correctly was used as the measure in both tests.

Neuropsychological Evaluation for Sample 3

The three non-target constructs included in these analyses were speed, memory, and fluid ability (Gf). The speed and memory constructs in this sample comprised different, albeit similar, variables compared to Samples 1 and 2.

Memory

Memory was assessed with three subscores of the Selective Reminding Test (SRT; Buschke & Fuld, Reference Buschke and Fuld1974) in which participants were read a list of 12 words and asked to recall the words after each of six trials. After each recall attempt, participants were reminded of the words they failed to recall. SRT-total refers to the total number of words out of a possible 72 that the participant remembered. SRT-delayed refers to the number correct that the participant recalled after a 15-min delay. SRT-recognition is the total correctly recognized words in a recognition test in which each of the 12 words is presented with three distracters.

Processing speed

Processing speed was assessed with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1981) Digit Symbol subtest and the Trail Making Test (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993). The Digit Symbol subtest involves writing symbols that correspond with single-digit numbers, as provided by a key at the top of the test form, as quickly as possible. The time to complete Trails A (numbers only) was also included as a speed measure.

Fluid ability

The construct of fluid ability (Gf) comprised the WAIS-III Letter Number Sequencing subtest (described above), as well as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay and Curtiss1993) and the Matrix Reasoning Test (Raven, Reference Raven1962). In the WCST, participants must sort cards into one of four piles based on one dimension of the cards (e.g., color, number). After sorting the cards correctly for 10 trials, the rule for sorting changes without the subject being informed, and the rule may change as many as six times. The total number of correct responses was the measure used. The Matrix Reasoning task involves determining which pattern out of a set of eight possible patterns best completes the missing cell of a matrix.

Cognitive reserve

Cognitive reserve was assessed with the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, Reference Nelson1982), WAIS-R Vocabulary subtest (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1981), and years of education. The NART requires participants to pronounce words that do not follow normal grapheme and phoneme rules (e.g., superfluous, epitome). The Vocabulary subtest requires participants to define words out loud to the examiner. Education was defined as the total number of years of formal classroom education.

Modeling Procedure

SEM was used to examine the construct validity of the cognitive reserve construct. The fit of the models was evaluated with several fit statistics. These fit statistics include the chi-square (χ 2), the critical ratio (χ 2/df), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) for which numbers closer to 0 indicate a better fit and Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI) for which values closer to 1.0 indicate a better fit (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999).

Full-information maximum likelihood estimation was used to deal with missing data. Amos 5.0 (Arbuckle, Reference Arbuckle2003) was used for all SEM analyses.

RESULTS

Means and SDs of each variable for the primary sample and Sample 2 are presented in Table 2. Inspection of the table indicates that each of the memory and executive functioning variables was significantly negatively correlated with age. The two speed variables were significantly positively correlated with age, and given that smaller values indicate faster performance, increased age was associated with slower processing speed. The relationship between the cognitive reserve variables and the age was mixed. Education was significantly negatively correlated with age in both samples, whereas the WRAT/WAT was not significantly related to age in either sample. The PPVT was negatively correlated with age in Sample 2 only. Participants in Sample 2 performed significantly worse than those in Sample 1 on all measures except the speed measures (as determined with independent samples t tests, p value less than .01 for each significant comparison). See Appendices A1–A3 for complete correlation matrices for each sample.

Table 2. Means (SDs) of cognitive variables for Samples 1 and 2

Note

Age r, age correlation.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

Prior to evaluating the construct validity of the cognitive reserve construct, a three-factor model comprising the nontarget cognitive constructs was examined in the primary sample. This three-factor model fits the data well (χ 2 = 44.20; df = 17; χ 2/df = 2.60; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.07), and inspection of Table 3 indicates that the loadings of each variable on its respective construct were moderate to large and significantly different from 0.

Table 3. Standardized coefficients from the confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor model of non-target constructs

Note

All values are significantly different from 0 at the p < .001 level.

Construct Validity

Initial construct validity analysis of the cognitive reserve construct consisted of examining a simple one-factor model comprising the three hypothesized target variables and the latent construct (i.e., Model A). The results of this model are summarized in the top half of column A in Table 4. The overall fit of this model cannot be ascertained because there were no degrees of freedom, but the three variables hypothesized to represent cognitive reserve all had significant loadings on the latent construct. This finding suggests that the variables hypothesized to represent cognitive reserve have convergent validity.

Table 4. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in the primary sample

Note

In the top part of the table, each column corresponds to a model (A–D). Values of goodness-of-fit indices are reported in the first five rows. The values in the second set of rows correspond to the loadings of each target variable on the cognitive reserve construct (when a loading is significant, it means that the value is significantly greater than 0). The values in the third set of rows correspond to the interfactor correlations between the cognitive reserve construct and each of the non-target constructs. In the bottom part of the table, each column corresponds to each of the non-target constructs included in the analyses. The values reported in the rows are the loadings from each of the cognitive reserve variables to the non-target constructs. For Model C, these are the loadings that significantly improved the fit of the model when included in the model. In Model D, each of the cognitive reserve variables loads simultaneously on each of the non-target constructs (as well as on the cognitive reserve construct). CR, cognitive reserve.

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

In Model B, the cognitive reserve construct is examined in the context of the three non-target constructs (memory, speed, and executive functioning) to evaluate discriminant validity. This model, depicted in Figure 1, fits well (see column B of Table 4), and inspection of the estimated correlation coefficients suggests that although the cognitive reserve construct was only moderately correlated with memory and speed, it was fairly highly correlated with executive functioning (r = .77). However, the 95% confidence interval (0.69–0.84) did not include 1.0, indicating the correlation was significantly less than 1.0.

Fig. 1 Four-factor Model B of the construct validity model for Samples 1 and 2. Double-headed arrows connecting the latent variables (represented by circles) represent correlations among the constructs. The observed variables are represented by rectangles. The single-headed arrows from the latent ability constructs to the observed variables represent the loading of each task on the latent variable. The latent variables labeled “e” represent the unique and error variance associated with each observed variable.

In Model C, each of the non-target constructs is allowed to be related to each of the target cognitive reserve variables, one at a time. If including the path in the model significantly improves the fit of the model (as determined by ∆χ 2/∆df), then that relation is retained in the final Model C. Three additional loadings were retained in Model C compared to Model B. As can be seen in the bottom half of Table 4, the WRAT variable loaded on the speed and executive functioning constructs and the education variable also loaded on the executive functioning construct. Although the cognitive reserve variables were allowed to be related to other constructs, the three variables still loaded significantly on the cognitive reserve construct, providing additional evidence of convergent validity. In addition, the correlations among the cognitive reserve construct and the non-target variables were still moderate or in the case of executive functioning, large but still significantly less than 1.0.

Model D is the most demanding test of both convergent and discriminant validity. In Model D, the correlations among the cognitive reserve construct and the non-target constructs are no longer estimated. Instead, in this model, each of the non-target constructs is allowed to be related to each of the observed cognitive reserve variables simultaneously. Inspection of column D in Table 4 indicates that although the loadings of the observed cognitive reserve variables on the cognitive reserve construct are still significant, the cognitive reserve variables also all load significantly on the executive functioning construct. In addition, the PPVT and WRAT load significantly on the speed construct as well.

Construct Validity: Sample 2

All the same tasks were completed in Sample 2 as in the primary sample except in this sample, the participants completed the WAT in Spanish (instead of the WRAT). The results of the construct validity analyses are presented in Table 5.

Table 5. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in Sample 2

Note

See Table 4 note for a detailed description of what the values in each row and column represent. CR, cognitive reserve.

*** p < .001.

In Model A, the loadings on the cognitive reserve construct are all significant and large, suggesting that the variables share a large amount of common variance. Model B fits the data well, despite the large correlation (r = .84) between the cognitive reserve and the executive functioning construct. Model B provides some evidence of discriminant validity because although the correlation with executive functioning is very large, it is significantly less than 1.0. In Model C, the inclusion of the paths to PPVT from the speed and executive functioning constructs and the paths to the WAT variable also from the speed and executive functioning constructs significantly improved the fit of the model. With the inclusion of these four additional relations in the model, there is still significant common variance among the observed cognitive reserve variables. In Model C, the 95% confidence interval around the correlation estimate (r = .90) between the cognitive reserve and the executive functioning constructs includes 1.0. Evaluation of Model D indicates that each of the observed cognitive reserve variables loads significantly on the cognitive reserve construct, and they also all load significantly on the executive functioning construct. The loadings on the executive functioning construct are all the same general magnitude, or larger, than those of the observed cognitive reserve variables on the cognitive reserve construct.

Construct Validity: Sample 3

As described in the Method section, the tasks completed by the participants in Sample 3 were not identical to those completed by the participants in the primary sample and in Sample 2. Means and SDs for each variable are presented in Table 6.

Table 6. Means (SDs) of measures obtained from Sample 3

Note

Age r, age correlation.

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

Evaluation of Model A indicates that the three observed cognitive reserve variables have significant variance in common (Table 7). Model B fits the data very well, and the correlations between the cognitive reserve construct and the non-target constructs are only moderate in magnitude, providing evidence of discriminant validity. Model C was identical to Model B since no path between the observed cognitive reserve variables and the non-target constructs significantly improved the fit of the model. Model D also fits the data well, and most striking in this sample is that none of the loadings of the observed cognitive reserve variables on the non-target constructs were significant. In addition, there was substantial common residual variance among the three target variables in Model D.

Table 7. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in Sample 3

Note

See Table 4 note for a detailed description of what the values in each row and column represent. CR, cognitive reserve.

a The error variance on the vocabulary variable was negative in Model B and was therefore set to .02.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

DISCUSSION

Cognitive reserve is a theoretical construct that has been used to inform models of cognitive aging and is presumed to be indicative of life experiences that may help in the management of brain pathology. The purpose of this project was to evaluate the appropriateness of referring to cognitive reserve as reflecting a distinct dimension of individual differences.

Analyses in the current study indicated that the variables hypothesized to reflect cognitive reserve have moderate to large loadings on the latent construct, providing evidence of convergent validity across three samples. The finding that the cognitive reserve variables are correlated with one another is not surprising when one considers that most cognitive variables are correlated with one another, a finding first reported by Spearman (Reference Spearman1904). To further establish construct validity, it is therefore necessary to also establish discriminant validity: that is, that the construct hypothesized to represent a unique dimension of individual differences (i.e., cognitive reserve) is only moderately correlated with other non-target constructs. We examined discriminant validity across three progressively more demanding models. Because the models are increasingly stringent in their test of discriminant validity, it is possible to find evidence of discriminant validity in Models B and C but not in Model D, for example. Ideally, the magnitude of the loadings on the cognitive reserve construct from the observed variables should be the same or larger than the correlations among the target and the non-target constructs. Across all three samples, we found that this was generally the case; the magnitude of the cognitive reserve loadings was similar or larger than the correlation between the cognitive reserve construct and the non-target constructs (see Models B and C). These findings from Models B and C provide evidence of discriminant validity.

However, examination of the results of Model D suggests that in Samples 1 and 2, the cognitive reserve variables are highly related to the executive functioning variables. Thus, the findings reported for Model D in Samples 1 and 2 fail to provide support for discriminant validity of the cognitive reserve construct. Furthermore, in Sample 2, the correlation estimate between the cognitive reserve and the executive functioning construct was not significantly less than 1.0, suggesting that the two constructs were not distinct from one another, at least in terms of individual differences. However, there was still significant positive residual variance among the cognitive reserve variables in Model D, which provides additional evidence of convergent validity. Stereotypical executive functioning tasks typically capture the ability to use flexible strategies to solve problems. The construct of cognitive reserve presumably reflects the cumulative effect of life experiences to provide individuals with a set of skills to better manage pathological damage through the use of alternative or compensatory strategies. Some aspects of executive function, such as flexible cognition, may even be considered part of cognitive reserve such that it may influence life experiences. It is therefore not surprising that these two constructs would be highly related to one another (in Samples 1 and 2). Furthermore, previous work by Salthouse et al. (Reference Salthouse, Atkinson and Berish2003) has suggested that the construct of executive functioning may not represent a unique dimension of individual differences. It therefore may not be realistic to expect the cognitive reserve variables to demonstrate evidence of discriminant validity in Model D (the most stringent test) from a set of variables that have been shown previously to lack discriminant validity.

This was not the case for Model D in Sample 3; evaluation of these results indicates that there was no significant overlap between the cognitive reserve and the Gf constructs (the Gf construct being similar to the executive functioning construct examined in the other samples). In this sample, discriminant validity of the cognitive reserve construct was strongly supported. The cognitive reserve variables were not significantly related to any of the non-target constructs, and the cognitive reserve variables still had substantial and significant residual variance in common after being allowed to be related to the non-target constructs.

To summarize, results of Models A through C provided evidence of both convergent and discriminant validity of cognitive reserve across all three samples. Results from Model D, the most demanding test of construct validity, failed to provide evidence of discriminant validity for Samples 1 and 2 but did provide additional evidence of discriminant validity in Sample 3.

The cognitive reserve constructs in this study were fairly narrowly defined (e.g., occupation was not included in the construct), and therefore, in many ways, the cognitive reserve construct was akin to what cognitive psychologists refer to as crystallized ability (Gc; Cattell, Reference Cattell1987). It is well established that Gc represents a unique dimension of individual differences, and researchers often partition intelligence (g) into Gc and Gf components (e.g., Cattell, Reference Cattell1987; Horn, Reference Horn and Wolman1982). Consequently, the finding of strong construct validity in a model in which there are constructs closely related to both Gc and Gf (Sample 3) is not surprising. However, although some of the cognitive reserve tasks (used in the current project and also in the literature at large) are often considered measures of semantic knowledge (i.e., the PPVT), the theoretical concept of reserve refers to variables that are reflective of lifetime experiences that promote cognitive flexibility (Stern, in press). Each of the variables in this project presumably provides an indication of these lifetime experiences.

Collectively, these findings suggest that it may be reasonable to refer to cognitive reserve as a distinct construct (based on strong convergent validity and moderate discriminant validity) as long as it is acknowledged that the cognitive reserve variables are highly related to executive functioning. Another possible consequence of these findings is that the concept of cognitive reserve should be reconsidered to include measures of executive functioning. However, because prior research has indicated that executive functioning does not likely reflect a distinct dimension of individual differences (Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Atkinson and Berish2003), it is probably unrealistic to expect strong evidence of construct validity when an executive construct is included in the models. In fact, the results of the analyses in Sample 3 suggest that without the inclusion of an executive functioning construct, the cognitive reserve construct demonstrates strong discriminant validity, even in Model D.

For the most part, the same general pattern was found across the primary sample (which consisted of cognitively healthy community-dwelling English speakers with an average age of 74 years and an average education of 13.5 years), Sample 2 (which consisted of Spanish speakers who were significantly younger and less educated than the primary sample), and Sample 3 (who were much younger and more educated than the other two samples). Our findings suggest that the main results of our study can be replicated across samples that differ along age, language, and education levels. This suggests that the findings presented here are fairly generalizable, at least in terms of healthy nondemented adults.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank the staff of the NOMAS, especially Yeseon Moon for compiling the data set and Janet DeRosa, the project manager. This work is supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 NS 29993; K02 NS059729). We acknowledge the support of the National Institute of Mental Health (K.L.S., T32MH020004-09) and the Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Research Foundation (C.B.W. and R.L.S.).

APPENDIX

Table A1. Correlation matrix for Sample 1

Note

All correlations significant at the p < .01 level.

Table A2. Correlation matrix for Sample 2

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

Table A3. Correlation matrix for Sample 3

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

Footnotes

a Not all the participants completed the Letter Number subtest because it was removed from the test battery. Fifty-one percent of the English speakers and 33.2% of the Spanish speakers completed the task. Maximum likelihood estimation was used to deal with the missing data in the SEM analyses.

Note

All correlations significant at the p < .01 level.

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

References

REFERENCES

Albert, M.S., Jones, K., Savage, C.R., Berkman, L., Seeman, T., Blazer, D., & Rowe, J.W. (1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 10, 578589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbuckle, J.L. (2003). Amos 5.0. Chicago, IL: SPSS.Google Scholar
Buschke, H. & Fuld, P.A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 10191025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R.B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Christensen, H. (2001). What cognitive changes can be expected with normal ageing? The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 768775.Google Scholar
Christensen, H., Korten, A.E., Jorm, A.F., Henderson, A.S., Jacomb, P.A., Rodgers, B., & MacKinnon, A.J. (1997). Education and decline in cognitive performance: Compensatory but not protective. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 323330.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Elia, L.F., Satz, P., Uchiyama, C.L., & White, T. (1996). Color Trails Test. Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test-second edition: Adult version. Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Del Ser, T., González-Montalvo, J., Martínez-Espinosa, S., Delgado-Villapalos, C., & Bermejo, F. (1997). Estimation of pre-morbid intelligence in Spanish people with the Word Accentuation Test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition, 33, 343356.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, L.M. (1997). Examiner’s manual for the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-third edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Evans, D.A., Hebert, L.E., Beckett, L.A., Scherr, P.A., Albert, M.S., Chown, M.J., Pilgrim, D.M., & Taylor, J.O. (1997). Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older person. Archives of Neurology, 54, 13991405.Google Scholar
Farmer, M.E., Kittner, S.J., Rae, D.S., Bartko, J.J., & Regier, D.A. (1995). Education and change in cognitive function: The epidemiologic catchment area study. Annals of Epidemiology, 5, 17.Google Scholar
Flowers, K.A. & Robertson, C. (1985). The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 48, 517529.Google Scholar
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Chelune, G.J., Talley, J.L., Kay, G.G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual-revised and expanded. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resource.Google Scholar
Horn, J.L. (1982). The aging of human abilities. In Wolman, B.B. (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 847870). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Hu, L.-T. & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155.Google Scholar
Letenneur, L., Commenges, D., Dartigues, J.F., & Barberger-Gateau, P. (1994). Incidence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in elderly community residents of south-western France. International Journal of Epidemiology, 23, 12561261.Google Scholar
Matthews, C.G. & Klove, H. (1964). Instruction manual for the Adult Neuropsychology Test Battery. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Medical School.Google Scholar
Nelson, H.E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Prabhakaran, S., Wright, C.B., Yoshita, M., Delapaz, R., Brown, T., DeCarli, C., & Sacco, R.L. (2008). Prevalence and determinants of subclinical brain infarction: The Northern Manhattan Study. Neurology, 70, 425430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qiu, C.X., Backman, L., Winblad, B., Aguero-Torres, H., & Frtiglioni, L. (2001). The influence of education on clinical diagnosed dementia: Incidence and mortality data from the Kungsholmen Project. Archives of Neurology, 58, 20342039.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (1962). Advanced progressive matrices, set II. London, UK: H.K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Reitan, R.M. & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Sacco, R.L., Boden-Albala, B., Abel, G., Lin, I-F., Elkind, M., Hauser, W.A., Paik, M.C., & Shea, S. (2001). Race-ethnic disparities in the impact of stroke risk factors: The northern Manhattan stroke study. Stroke, 32, 17251731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T.A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 140144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 6887.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Atkinson, T.M., & Berish, D.E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 132, 566594.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Berish, D.E., & Siedlecki, K.L. (2004). Construct validity and age sensitivity of prospective memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 11331148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siedlecki, K.L., Salthouse, T.A., & Berish, D.E. (2005). Is there anything special about the aging of source memory? Psychology and Aging, 20, 1932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology, 15, 820.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (in press ). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia.Google Scholar
Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T.K., Tang, M.X., Wilder, D., & Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, 10041010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Tang, M.X., Denaro, J., & Mayeux, R. (1995). Increased risk of mortality in Alzheimer’s disease patients with more advanced educational and occupational attainment. Annals of Neurology, 37, 590595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valenzuela, M.J. & Sachdev, P. (2005). Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 35, 114.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
White, L., Katzman, R., Losonczy, K., Salive, M., Wallace, R., Berkman, L., Taylor, J., Fillenbaum, G., & Havlik, R. (1994). Association of education with incidence of cognitive impairment in three established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 47, 363374.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G.S. (1993). The Wide Range Achievement Test: Manual (3rd ed.). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.Google Scholar
Wright, C.B., Festa, J.R., Paik, M.C., Schmiedigen, A., Brown, T.R., Yoshita, M., DeCarli, C., Sacco, R., & Stern, Y. (2008). White matter hyperintensities and subclinical infarction: Associations with psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility. Stroke, 39, 800805.Google Scholar
Zhang, M., Katzman, R., Salmon, D., Jin, H., Cai, G., Wang, Z., Qu, G., Grant, I., Yu, E., Levy, P., Klauber, M.R., & Liu, W.T. (1990). The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in Shanghai, China: Impact of age, gender and education. Annals of Neurology, 27, 428437.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the three samples

Figure 1

Table 2. Means (SDs) of cognitive variables for Samples 1 and 2

Figure 2

Table 3. Standardized coefficients from the confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor model of non-target constructs

Figure 3

Table 4. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in the primary sample

Figure 4

Fig. 1 Four-factor Model B of the construct validity model for Samples 1 and 2. Double-headed arrows connecting the latent variables (represented by circles) represent correlations among the constructs. The observed variables are represented by rectangles. The single-headed arrows from the latent ability constructs to the observed variables represent the loading of each task on the latent variable. The latent variables labeled “e” represent the unique and error variance associated with each observed variable.

Figure 5

Table 5. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in Sample 2

Figure 6

Table 6. Means (SDs) of measures obtained from Sample 3

Figure 7

Table 7. Statistics for construct validity models for the variables hypothesized to reflect CR in Sample 3

Figure 8

Table A1. Correlation matrix for Sample 1

Figure 9

Table A2. Correlation matrix for Sample 2

Figure 10

Table A3. Correlation matrix for Sample 3