Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T05:18:07.702Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

School success in childhood and subsequent prodromal symptoms and psychoses in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2019

M. Lassila*
Affiliation:
Center for Life Course Health Research, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Center Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
T. Nordström
Affiliation:
Center for Life Course Health Research, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Center Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
T. Hurtig
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Research Unit of Clinical Neuroscience, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland PEDEGO Research Unit, Child Psychiatry, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Clinic of Child Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
P. Mäki
Affiliation:
Medical Research Center Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Research Unit of Clinical Neuroscience, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Länsi-Pohja healthcare district Department of Psychiatry, The Middle Ostrobothnia Central Hospital, Soite; Mental Health Services, Joint Municipal Authority of Wellbeing in Raahe District; Mental Health Services and Basic Health Care District of Kallio, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Kainuu Central Hospital, Kainuu Social and Healthcare District, Finland
E. Jääskeläinen
Affiliation:
Center for Life Course Health Research, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Center Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
E. Oinas
Affiliation:
Center for Life Course Health Research, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
J. Miettunen
Affiliation:
Center for Life Course Health Research, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Center Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
*
Author for correspondence: M. Lassila, E-mail: meri.s.lassila@student.oulu.fi
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Low IQ is a risk factor for psychosis, but the effect of high IQ is more controversial. The aim was to explore the association of childhood school success with prodromal symptoms in adolescence and psychoses in adulthood.

Methods

In the general population-based Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 (n = 8 229), we studied the relationship between teacher-assessed learning deficits, special talents and general school success at age 8 years and both prodromal symptoms (PROD-screen) at age 15–16 years and the occurrence of psychoses by age 30 years.

Results

More prodromal symptoms were experienced by those talented in oral presentation [boys: adjusted odds ratio (OR) 1.49; 95% confidence interval 1.14–1.96; girls: 1.23; 1.00–1.52] or drawing (boys: 1.44; 1.10–1.87). Conversely, being talented in athletics decreased the probability of psychotic-like symptoms (boys: OR 0.72; 0.58–0.90). School success below average predicted less prodromal symptoms with boys (OR 0.68; 0.48–0.97), whereas above-average success predicted more prodromal symptoms with girls (OR 1.22; 1.03–1.44). The occurrence of psychoses was not affected. Learning deficits did not associate with prodromal symptoms or psychoses.

Conclusions

Learning deficits in childhood did not increase the risk of prodromal symptoms in adolescence or later psychosis in this large birth cohort. Learning deficits are not always associated with increased risk of psychosis, which might be due to, e.g. special support given in schools. The higher prevalence of prodromal symptoms in talented children may reflect a different kind of relationship of school success with prodromal symptoms compared to full psychoses.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Early school success may be used to predict individuals' later life. School success is affected by many factors, for instance motivation, attention or memory, learning deficits or IQ. Special talents in certain subjects require also creativity. The onset of psychosis is often in early adolescence when major physical and behavioral changes occur (Paus et al., Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008). Males have a higher risk of onset of schizophrenia in early adulthood (van der Werf et al., Reference van der Werf, Hanssen, Köhler, Verkaaik, Verhey, van Winkel, van Os and Allardyce2014). The onset of psychosis is often preceded by premorbid prodromal symptoms. Changes in cognition or IQ manifesting already in childhood or adolescence before the prodrome have been reported (Kremen et al., Reference Kremen, Buka, Seidman, Goldstein, Koren and Tsuang1998; Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Reichenberg, Rabinowitz, Weiser, Kaplan and Mark1999). Information on school success could be used to search these predictors: such information can easily be obtained on a population level and is thus applicable in large epidemiological studies.

Sometimes poor school success may indicate cognitive impairment characteristic for the trajectory to psychosis. Low and especially deteriorating premorbid IQ in childhood or puberty has been seen to precede the appearance of psychotic symptoms in adulthood (Kremen et al., Reference Kremen, Buka, Seidman, Goldstein, Koren and Tsuang1998; Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Nopoulos, Arndt, O'Leary, Ho and Andreasen2002; Matheson et al., Reference Matheson, Shepherd, Laurens and Carr2011). Some studies show a linear dose–response relationship between decreasing IQ and increased risk of psychosis (David et al., Reference David, Malmberg, Brandt, Allebeck and Lewis1997; Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Reichenberg, Rabinowitz, Weiser, Kaplan and Mark1999; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones2011), whereas others suggest that IQ influences the risk of psychosis most strongly at the lowest range of IQ distribution (Schulz et al., Reference Schulz, Sundin, Leask and Done2014). The relationship between lower than average IQ and psychosis might depend on a third factor, such as obstetric complications (Sussmann et al., Reference Sussmann, McIntosh, Lawrie and Johnstone2009; Di Prinzio et al., Reference Di Prinzio, Morgan, Björk, Croft, Lin, Jablensky and McNeil2018).

Bipolar disorder has been associated with excellent school performance (MacCabe et al., Reference MacCabe, Lambe, Cnattingius, Sham, David, Reichenberg, Murray and Hultman2010). Excellent grades are not solely due to high IQ but also motivation and productivity, which is also higher in hypomania. Less evidence exists about good school performance and the risk of schizophrenia and other psychoses (Isohanni et al., Reference Isohanni, Järvelin, Jones, Jokelainen and Isohanni1999; MacCabe et al., Reference MacCabe, Lambe, Cnattingius, Torrång, Björk, Sham, David, Murray and Hultman2008). The occurrence of different prodromal symptoms and types of psychosis seems to differ by gender. Gender differences in cognition in the premorbid phase and during illness have been suggested but less studied (Barajas et al., Reference Barajas, Ochoa, Obiols and Lalucat-Jo2015). One study suggested that men with first-episode psychosis had a higher IQ than women (Hui et al., Reference Hui, Leung, Chang, Chan, Lee and Chen2016). High IQ might affect the course of illness, e.g. suicide risk in psychotic disorder has been found to be higher in those with good school success (Alaräisänen et al., Reference Alaräisänen, Miettunen, Lauronen, Räsänen and Isohanni2006; Nordentoft, Reference Nordentoft2007). However, high childhood IQ predicts a better outcome in subjects who later develop schizophrenia (Munro et al., Reference Munro, Russell, Murray, Kerwin and Jones2002).

In order to obtain more information about the association between school success and psychoses, it is worthwhile to explore both superior and inferior performers. The aim of this study was to study how teacher-assessed learning deficits or special talents in certain school subjects are associated with later self-reported psychotic-like symptoms and with the occurrence of psychoses in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 among boys and girls. We hypothesized that learning deficits would associate with more psychotic-like symptoms and with psychosis, whereas special talents would not associate with the outcomes.

Methods and material

Study population

The Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 is a longitudinal birth cohort including 9362 mothers and their 9432 live-born children with an expected date of birth between 1 July 1985 and 30 June 1986 in Northern Finland (Järvelin et al., Reference Järvelin, Hartikainen-Sorri and Rantakallio1993). Subjects who denied the use of their data (n = 256) and those with a known diagnosis of intellectual disability (n = 115) were excluded. This was done based on knowledge concerning the association between intellectual disability and psychoses reported previously (Myrbakk and von Tetzchner, Reference Myrbakk and von Tetzchner2008).

Learning deficits and special talents in childhood

At the age of 8 years, a postal questionnaire concerning school behavior and performance was sent to the teachers with permission from the parents (Taanila et al., Reference Taanila, Ebeling, Kotimaa, Moilanen and Järvelin2004). Teachers replied with information on learning deficits and special talents about 4193 (86.0%) of the boys and 4036 (88.9%) of the girls. School success was assessed based on this questionnaire, and the final sample included 8229 individuals.

Learning deficits and special talents of the subjects were assessed on the basis of no/yes questions. Learning deficits included difficulties in reading, writing, or mathematics. Special talents were assessed both in academic and non-academic subjects. Special talents included creativity-related talents: music, drawing, athletics, craft, oral presentation, and acting and written presentation. Information about mathematical talent was gathered from answers to open questions about pupils' talents. The general success in school was graded to be either below average, average, or above average.

Prodromal symptoms in adolescence

A questionnaire about subjects' lifestyles and habits was given to them during their clinical 16-year examination in 2001–2002 (Mäki et al., Reference Mäki, Koskela, Murray, Nordström, Miettunen, Jääskeläinen and Veijola2014).

Prodromal symptoms include both mild psychotic-like and unspecific psychiatric symptoms, which are associated with increased risk of transition to psychosis. The lack of specificity makes it challenging to separate the pre-psychotic state from some other mental disorders. The PROD-screen is an easily applicable tool for assessing the prodromal symptoms in clinical work and also for screening healthy individuals for prospective population-based study. The simplicity of the PROD-screen is however to the detriment of its power to detect specific pre-psychotic symptoms. The questionnaire included the PROD-screen, which consists of 21 questions (no/yes) screening psychotic-like (prodromal) symptoms, of which 12 are considered as specific symptoms for psychosis (Heinimaa et al., Reference Heinimaa, Salokangas, Ristkari, Plathin, Huttunen, Ilonen, Suomela, Korkeila and McGlashan2003). Others include symptoms such as depression and anxiety that are also associated with pre-psychotic states, although they are less specific (Therman et al., Reference Therman, Heinimaa, Miettunen, Joukamaa, Moilanen, Mäki and Veijola2011). The total score of the specific symptoms was used in the current study and the score was dichotomized (0–3 v. over 3 points), based on previous research. About half of the future psychotic cases scored over 3 points in the PROD-screen, odds ratio (OR) (95% confidence interval, CI) for psychosis risk being 2.36 (95% CI 1.68–3.31) (unpublished result). PROD-screen has been found to classify correctly 77% of prodromal cases (Heinimaa et al., Reference Heinimaa, Salokangas, Ristkari, Plathin, Huttunen, Ilonen, Suomela, Korkeila and McGlashan2003). The 12 specific items have Cronbach's α value of 0.69 in the current sample. The missing information due to replying only to some of the PROD-screen questions was taken into consideration in the analyses by assuming that the subject replied to the missing items with the same yes/no percentages as in the filled part of the questionnaire. Ruling out the subjects (0.5%) who did not reply to more than two specific items or to three of the total items, the screening information was available from 3225 (68.7%) boys and 3341 (75.7%) girls.

Psychoses in adolescence and early adulthood by 30 years of age

The information about psychoses was derived from multiple sources of register data: the Care Register for Healthcare (1998–2015), Primary Healthcare Outpatient Registers (2011–2015), Specialized Care Outpatient Registers (1998–2015), register for disability pensions from the Finnish Centre for Pensions (1998–2013), and register for reimbursable medication from the Social Insurance Institute (1998–2005).

Possible confounding factors

The distribution of family background factors by gender is seen in Table 1. The presented data consist of possible confounding factors which could affect the rates of prodromal symptoms or psychoses; these were selected based on earlier studies. These items were included in the regression models as covariates. The data included gender, parental psychosis (no/yes) (Keskinen et al., Reference Keskinen, Marttila, Marttila, Jones, Murray, Moilanen, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Mäki, Isohanni, Jääskeläinen and Miettunen2015), family type (married/cohabiting or single/divorced) (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Wang, Heeramun-Aubeeluck, Wang, Shi, Yuan and Zhao2014), and parental education (<9 years of comprehensive school/comprehensive or elementary school/matriculation examination) (Frissen et al., Reference Frissen, Lieverse, Marcelis, Drukker and Delespaul2015). Family type information was collected from a questionnaire made during pregnancy by the 24th gestational week. Information about parental education was collected from a questionnaire delivered to the parents when the subjects were 16 years old. Information on parental psychosis was collected from different registers [Care Register for Healthcare (1998–2015), Primary Healthcare Outpatient Registers (2011–2015), Specialized Care Outpatient Registers (1998–2015), and Finnish Center for Pensions (1998–2013)].

Table 1. Family characteristics [n (%)] by gender in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

a Calculated using Pearson's χ2-test.

Statistical methods

The statistical difference between genders regarding family background factors and the prevalence of learning deficits and special talents was assessed by using Pearson χ2 tests. OR were calculated using binary logistic regression for having more than three specific PROD-screen items and for having psychoses for those with learning deficits or special talents. Hazard ratios (HR) were calculated using Cox regression models with time of death and emigration as censoring points. Adjusted OR and HR were calculated using parental marital status, basic education, psychosis, and family's place of residence as covariates. All results are presented by gender because school variables and outcome variables differed by gender. IBM SPSS Statistics 24.0 was used in the analyses. P-values <0.05 were considered as statistically significant.

Attrition analysis

Attrition in the questionnaire to teachers (pupils' age 8) and the self-reported PROD-screen (pupils' age 15–16) was assessed regarding family type (marital status of the mother), type of residence (urban/rural), mother's and father's education (<9 years/9–12 years/more than 12 years), and parental psychosis (no/yes). Teachers' assessment was available for 91.0% of the boys and 92.4% of the girls. PROD-screen was available for 70.4% of the boys and 76.7% of the girls. Regarding boys, participants and non-participants differed significantly in most family background factors in both follow-up surveys, whereas among girls the only significant finding was that those with married mothers participated more at age 15–16 years. Differences between the groups of participants and non-participants can be seen in online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.

Results

Prevalence of special talents and learning deficits

The prevalence of learning deficits and special talents by gender is presented in Table 2. Overall, boys had more learning deficits and less special talents than girls. The prevalence of different learning deficits differed between 9.2% and 22.2% in boys and between 8.8% and 12.3% in girls. Regarding special talents, the range was between 6.9% and 27.4% in boys and between 3.2% and 27.1% in girls. In athletics and mathematics boys were more frequently talented than girls. However, boys were assessed to perform generally less well at school than girls. Of the boys, 13.1% were assessed to be below average while the percentage for girls was 6.9%. Above average performance was registered in 30.4% of boys and 42.4% of girls.

Table 2. Learning deficits, special talents, and general school success at the age of 8 years by gender in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

a Calculated using Pearson's χ2-test.

Prodromal symptoms in adolescence

The number of subjects who had at least three specific PROD-screen items with unadjusted and adjusted OR according to childhood school variables is presented in Table 3. Learning deficits in childhood were not associated with having a PROD-score above 3 reported in adolescence. Those with special talents in oral presentation, written presentation, and drawing had higher ORs for having a PROD-score above 3. After controlling for confounding factors, the association remained with oral presentation for boys and girls and drawing for boys. The adjusted ORs for oral presentation were 1.49 (95% CI 1.14–1.96) for boys and 1.23 (1.00–1.52) for girls and for drawing 1.44 (1.10–1.87) for boys. Talent in athletics appeared to be protecting for boys lowering the adjusted OR to 0.72 (0.58–0.90). The results with general school success showed the same trend; the prevalence of prodromal symptoms decreased with below average boys and increased with above average girls: the adjusted ORs were 0.68 (0.48–0.97) for boys and 1.22 (1.03–1.44) for girls.

Table 3. The psychotic-like symptoms (PROD-screen) at the age of 15–16 years according to scholastic traits at age 8 years in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.

Statistically significant (p < 0.05) findings are in bold.

a Calculated by using binary logistic regression model.

b Adjusted for parental marital status, basic education, psychosis, and family's place of residence as covariates.

Psychoses in adulthood

Table 4 shows the number of psychoses experienced by age 30 with unadjusted and adjusted HR according to school variables. In total, 2.4% of boys and 2.1% of girls received a psychosis diagnosis during the follow-up until the age of 30 years. Of the 214 cases with psychoses, 69 (46 boys) had schizophrenia, 49 (18 boys) affective psychosis, and 96 (55 boys) other psychoses. Learning deficits and special talents in childhood were not associated significantly with psychoses in adulthood (until the age of 30 years). However, some gender differences were observed. In girls, a higher number of psychoses was observed with succeeding below average at school, having problems in reading and being talented in oral presentation. With boys the trend was opposite; the same variables were associated with lower psychosis rates.

Table 4. The occurrence of psychoses by the age of 30 years according to scholastic traits at age 8 years in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

CI, confidence interval; HR, hazard ratio.

Statistically significant (p < 0.05) findings are in bold.

a Calculated by using Cox regression model.

b Adjusted for parental marital status, basic education, psychosis, and family's place of residence as covariates.

Discussion

Main findings

Surprisingly, learning deficits in childhood were not associated with prodromal symptoms in adolescence or with subsequent psychoses in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 sample. Prodromal symptoms appeared to be associated only with good school success (such as talent in oral presentation and drawing). Special talent in athletics decreased the prevalence of psychotic-like symptoms in boys.

Learning deficits

In this general population-based birth cohort, about every 10th child or up to every fifth boy and every eighth girl had experienced some learning deficits at the age of 8 years. None of the learning deficits were associated with prodromal symptoms in adolescence or with later psychoses. Sometimes learning deficits reflect low IQ, but social, motivational, attention problems, and personality-related factors are also important aspects affecting learning deficits. Similarly, in the meta-analysis of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia, some studies have not found a significant connection (Walker et al., Reference Walker, McConville, Hunter, Deary and Whalley2002; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones2011). Consistently but not significantly lower prodromal symptom scores among subjects with learning deficits and respectively high scores among talented subjects show a trend that may arise from the subjects' ability to describe their symptoms.

In the Finnish school system, many different forms of special support are directed toward children with problems in learning and could possibly act as a protecting factor (Ström and Hannus-Gullmets, Reference Ström, Hannus-Gullmets, Cameron and Thygesen2015). In personalized curriculums, learning goals are set to be realistic concerning the students' abilities and restrictions. Special support can give students with learning deficits the experience of succeeding, and thus could prevent social isolation which could be assumed to be positive for mental health. Unfortunately, studies about the possible connections between learning deficits, special education, and mental health are scarce.

Ruling out subjects with known intellectual disability from the analyses could explain the results of this study, this was done based on the significant number of intellectually disabled subjects in the learning deficits group and on evidence showing that intellectual disability is a risk factor in itself (Jacobson, Reference Jacobson1990). In studies including intellectually disabled subjects, the risk-increasing effect of learning deficits may be overestimated.

In this study, we investigated early learning deficits detected at the age of 8 years. Early learning deficits were chosen to reveal their effect on psychosis risk, avoiding the confounding effect of cognitive deterioration characteristics on the trajectory of the illness itself.

Special talents

Several special talents in childhood were associated with subsequent increased prodromal symptoms in adolescence. These talents (oral and written presentation and drawing) can be representations of intelligence but also of creativity. Higher performance in verbal learning and fluency is associated with a liability to bipolar disorder, but not to schizophrenia. Verbal fluency could have improved evolutionary fitness and its association with bipolar disorder could offer one explanation of why bipolar disorder has persisted in the population (Higier et al., Reference Higier, Jimenez, Hultman, Borg, Roman, Kizling, Larsson and Cannon2014). In one study, subjects who later developed schizophrenia excelled in drawing and arts at the age of 12 (Helling et al., Reference Helling, Ohman and Hultman2003).

The link between creativity and mental disorders is supported by many studies, suggesting that the link is strongest with affective disorders (Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1987). A particular link to affective disorders could explain why only prodromal symptoms but not psychoses were affected. Because of the non-specific character of the PROD-screen discussed later in this article, subjects with high scores also include those experiencing affective rather than psychotic symptoms. Different special talents (or types of creativity) are suggested to have separate links to bipolarity and schizotypy–schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Richards, Reference Richards2001). Intelligent and verbally talented individuals can also be thought to be more able to describe their sensations leading to higher self-reporting of symptoms. The self-reporting of symptoms has found to be affected by cognitive ability and personality factors (Enns et al., Reference Enns, Larsen and Cox2000).

Most previous studies about creativity and mental disorders have been in relation to adults, showing that some mental disorders are more frequent in artists (Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1987; Jamison, Reference Jamison1989; Kyaga et al., Reference Kyaga, Lichtenstein, Boman, Hultman, Långström and Landen2011). In a recent large Swedish register study, it was found that artistic creativity at high school or university associated with later mental health problems (MacCabe et al., Reference MacCabe, Sariaslan, Almqvist, Lichtenstein, Larsson and Kyaga2018). Interestingly, they found visual arts to associate with psychosis as we did in the current study. Special talents emerging during childhood could allow the assessment of causality but have been less studied. Our result suggests that special talents emerging already during childhood could relate to mental health in adolescence.

Some theories have been suggested to explain the mechanism underlying the link between creativity (or special talents) and mental disorder, one being the shared genetic vulnerability model. This model suggests that some hereditary traits such as reduced latent inhibition are common for creativity and psychopathology, increasing the amount of information processed consciously (Baruch et al., Reference Baruch, Hemsley and Gray1988; Carson, Reference Carson2011). Simultaneous manifestation of protective factors, such as high IQ, controls the processing of this information increasing creativity and protecting against the formation of mental disorders (Carson, Reference Carson2011). Horwood et al. (Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell, Hollis, Lewis, Menezes, Thompson, Wolke, Zammit and Harrison2008) found in their ALSPAC birth cohort study that low IQ measured at the age of 8 years was associated with increased psychotic-like symptoms at the age of 12 years, but a weaker association was also observed with high IQ. However, the risk of schizophrenia declines with increasing IQ (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Allebeck, David, Dalman, Hemmingsson, Lundberg and Lewis2004). In our study, the increase was also observed only in psychotic-like symptoms, not in actual psychoses.

The status of athletics as a protecting factor could be explained by the known positive effect of physical activities on mood and mental health (Wiles et al., Reference Wiles, Jones, Haase, Lawlor, Macfarlane and Lewis2008; Griffiths et al., Reference Griffiths, Kouvonen, Pentti, Oksanen, Virtanen, Salo, Väänänen, Kivimäki and Vahtera2014) or as selection process relating to minor motor difficulties associating also with psychosis risk (Filatova et al., Reference Filatova, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Khandaker, Lowry, Nordström, Hurtig, Moilanen and Miettunen2018). Physical activity has been linked with psychosis risk also in the current birth cohort (Koivukangas et al., Reference Koivukangas, Tammelin, Kaakinen, Mäki, Moilanen, Taanila and Veijola2010).

Psychotic-like symptoms

Despite its simplicity, PROD-screen has been shown to be useful in evaluating psychosis risk (Heinimaa et al., Reference Heinimaa, Salokangas, Ristkari, Plathin, Huttunen, Ilonen, Suomela, Korkeila and McGlashan2003). The four prodromal symptoms regarding social withdrawal are more frequent in patients with first-episode psychosis than in patients with non-psychotic disorders and in controls. Although the patients had experienced less of these symptoms compared with patients with psychosis, those with non-psychotic disorder had experienced more social withdrawal-associated symptoms in adolescence than the controls (Mäki et al., Reference Mäki, Koskela, Murray, Nordström, Miettunen, Jääskeläinen and Veijola2014).

The selection of the cut-off point of 3/12 specific symptoms can be justified based on the proven consistency with the more complex SIPS screening tool. However, the screen is designed for clinical settings, and it remains unclear what should be the cut-off point when the screen is applied to epidemiological settings.

Psychoses

To our surprise, we did not find any school variables in childhood to correlate with an increase in later psychoses despite the results concerning the prodromal symptoms. One explaining factor could be the young age of our sample, in which not all the vulnerable individuals have yet become ill. Symptoms screened in the PROD-screen are also much more common in the population than psychosis diagnoses consistently with the continuum hypothesis of psychotic symptoms. According to this hypothesis, the occurrence of psychotic symptoms is continuous in the population, varying from mild forms to more severe ones (Verdoux and van Os, Reference Verdoux and van Os2002). The increase in PROD-score but not in the psychosis rate could be a manifestation of milder, but still clinically relevant symptomatology. It is also possible that other factors such as student's temperament affected teachers' ratings (Mullola et al., Reference Mullola, Ravaja, Lipsanen, Alatupa, Hintsanen, Jokela and Keltikangas-Järvinen2012). Regarding talented individuals, it has been found that creative individuals have more some positive schizotypal symptoms more frequently than others (Nelson and Rawlings, Reference Nelson and Rawlings2010).

In our study, all psychosis diagnoses were included, not only schizophrenia. With regard to premorbid intelligence as a risk factor, some studies have shown that low premorbid intelligence is a risk factor only for schizophrenia, not for affective psychoses (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Seidman, Fitzmaurice, Smoller, Goldstein and Buka2017). In general, it has been found that schizophrenia and affective psychoses have slightly different risk profiles (see e.g. Jääskeläinen et al., Reference Jääskeläinen, Juola, Korpela, Lehtiniemi, Nietola, Korkeila and Miettunen2017). Although most risk factors overlap, the level of the risk increasing effect is often found to be greater with schizophrenia. Furthermore, less evidence is available concerning antecedents for affective psychoses (Laurens et al., Reference Laurens, Luo, Matheson, Carr, Raudino, Harris and Green2015). This could partly explain why early learning deficits, most probably also associated with lower intelligence, did not predict psychoses in our sample.

Strengths and limitations

One strength of our study is the extensive data, the included survey data have good participation rates and the follow-up for psychosis diagnoses was based on nationwide registers with no attrition. Also the prospective design of the study is an advantage; information about the large general population-based study sample has been gathered starting from birth, resulting in an extensive follow-up time of 30 years. The study has very good participation rates both in childhood and adolescent surveys and we were able to use extensive nationwide registers to detect psychoses. Teachers are in general well trained and competent to evaluate pupils' school performance such as talents (Bracken and Brown, Reference Bracken and Brown2006), although there can be factors such as gender and student's temperament which may affect the ratings (Mullola et al., Reference Mullola, Ravaja, Lipsanen, Alatupa, Hintsanen, Jokela and Keltikangas-Järvinen2012).

Limitations of our study include the young age of the sample which explains the small number of psychosis cases. Evaluating the risk by PROD-screen also imposes some limitations. These are the PROD-screens' non-specificity in predicting psychoses and its use as a screening tool rather than in the clinical setting for which it was originally designed. Screening these symptoms on a population level does not have as strong predictive power for psychoses as using the screen in clinical settings and with chosen subjects. It is also possible that response bias may explain some of the results as those children are more diligent (and thus rated highly by teachers) and may be more likely to report psychotic experiences.

Conclusion

Surprisingly, learning deficits in childhood did not increase psychotic-like symptoms in adolescence or later psychosis rate in this large general population-based birth cohort, unlike in most previous studies. Learning problems are not always associated with increased risk of psychosis, which might be due to the special support given in school, such as remedial instruction, studying in small groups or guidance of a school helper, designing personalized curriculums or giving challenged students more time to finish compulsory education. The higher prevalence of psychotic-like symptoms in talented children may reflect a different kind of relationship of school success with psychotic-like symptoms compared with full psychoses.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000825

Acknowledgements

We thank all cohort members and researchers who have participated in the study.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Brain and Behavior Research Fund, the Academy of Finland (grant numbers 268336, 278286); EU QLG1-CT-2000-01643 (EUROBLCS) (grant number E51560); NorFA (grant numbers 731, 20056, 30167); and USA/NIHH 2000 G DF682 (grant number 50945).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Agnew-Blais, J, Seidman, LJ, Fitzmaurice, GM, Smoller, JW, Goldstein, JM and Buka, SL (2017) The interplay of childhood behavior problems and IQ in the development of later schizophrenia and affective psychoses. Schizophrenia Research 184, 4551.10.1016/j.schres.2016.12.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alaräisänen, A, Miettunen, J, Lauronen, E, Räsänen, P and Isohanni, M (2006) Good school performance is a risk factor of suicide in psychoses: a 35-year follow up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 114, 357362.10.1111/j.1600-0447.2006.00800.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC (1987) Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. The American Journal of Psychiatry 144, 12881292.Google ScholarPubMed
Barajas, A, Ochoa, S, Obiols, JE and Lalucat-Jo, L (2015) Gender differences in individuals at high-risk of psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. The Scientific World Journal 2015, 430735.10.1155/2015/430735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baruch, I, Hemsley, DR and Gray, JA (1988) Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 176, 598606.10.1097/00005053-198810000-00004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken, BA and Brown, EF (2006) Behavioral identification and assessment of gifted and talented students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 24, 112122.10.1177/0734282905285246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, SH (2011) Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne De Psychiatrie 56, 144153.10.1177/070674371105600304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F, Wang, L, Heeramun-Aubeeluck, A, Wang, J, Shi, J, Yuan, J and Zhao, X (2014) Identification and characterization of college students with attenuated psychosis syndrome in China. Psychiatry Research 216, 346350.10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, AS, Malmberg, A, Brandt, L, Allebeck, P and Lewis, G (1997) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine 27, 13111323.10.1017/S0033291797005680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, M, Reichenberg, A, Rabinowitz, J, Weiser, M, Kaplan, Z and Mark, M (1999) Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. The American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13281335.Google ScholarPubMed
Di Prinzio, P, Morgan, VA, Björk, J, Croft, M, Lin, A, Jablensky, A and McNeil, TF (2018) Intellectual disability and psychotic disorders in children: association with maternal severe mental illness and exposure to obstetric complications in a whole-population cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry 175, 12321242.10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enns, MW, Larsen, DK and Cox, BJ (2000) Discrepancies between self and observer ratings of depression. The relationship to demographic, clinical and personality variables. Journal of Affective Disorders 60, 3341.10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00156-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filatova, S, Koivumaa-Honkanen, H, Khandaker, GM, Lowry, E, Nordström, T, Hurtig, T, Moilanen, K and Miettunen, J (2018) Early motor developmental milestones and schizotypy in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort Study 1966. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 11511158.10.1093/schbul/sbx165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frissen, A, Lieverse, R, Marcelis, M, Drukker, M and Delespaul, P and GROUP Investigators (2015) Psychotic disorder and educational achievement: a family-based analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 50, 15111518.10.1007/s00127-015-1082-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R, Nopoulos, P, Arndt, and , S, O'Leary, D, Ho, BC and Andreasen, NC (2002) Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia through examination of standardized scholastic test performance. The American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 11831189.10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, A, Kouvonen, A, Pentti, J, Oksanen, T, Virtanen, M, Salo, P, Väänänen, A, Kivimäki, M and Vahtera, J (2014) Association of physical activity with future mental health in older, mid-life and younger women. European Journal of Public Health 24, 813818.10.1093/eurpub/ckt199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinimaa, M, Salokangas, RK, Ristkari, T, Plathin, M, Huttunen, J, Ilonen, T, Suomela, T, Korkeila, J and McGlashan, TH (2003) PROD-screen – a screen for prodromal symptoms of psychosis. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 12, 92104.10.1002/mpr.146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helling, I, Ohman, A and Hultman, CM (2003) School achievements and schizophrenia: a case-control study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108, 381386.10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00151.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higier, RG, Jimenez, AM, Hultman, CM, Borg, J, Roman, C, Kizling, I, Larsson, H and Cannon, TD (2014) Enhanced neurocognitive functioning and positive temperament in twins discordant for bipolar disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 11911198.10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwood, J, Salvi, G, Thomas, K, Duffy, L, Gunnell, D, Hollis, C, Lewis, G, Menezes, P, Thompson, A, Wolke, D, Zammit, S and Harrison, G (2008) IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 193, 185191.10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hui, CL, Leung, CM, Chang, WC, Chan, SK, Lee, EH and Chen, EY (2016) Examining gender difference in adult-onset psychosis in Hong Kong. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 10, 324333.10.1111/eip.12167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isohanni, I, Järvelin, MR, Jones, P, Jokelainen, J and Isohanni, M (1999) Can excellent school performance be a precursor of schizophrenia? A 28-year follow-up in the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 100, 1726.10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10909.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jääskeläinen, E, Juola, T, Korpela, H, Lehtiniemi, H, Nietola, M, Korkeila, J and Miettunen, J (2017) Epidemiology of psychotic depression - systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 48, 905918.10.1017/S0033291717002501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, JW (1990) Do some mental disorders occur less frequently among persons with mental retardation? American Journal of Mental Retardation 94, 596602.Google ScholarPubMed
Jamison, KR (1989) Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychiatry 52, 125134.10.1080/00332747.1989.11024436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Järvelin, MR, Hartikainen-Sorri, AL and Rantakallio, P (1993) Labour induction policy in hospitals of different levels of specialisation. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology 100, 310315.10.1111/j.1471-0528.1993.tb12971.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, GM, Barnett, JH, White, IR and Jones, PB (2011) A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 132, 220227.10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koivukangas, J, Tammelin, T, Kaakinen, M, Mäki, P, Moilanen, I, Taanila, A and Veijola, J (2010) Physical activity and fitness in adolescents at risk for psychosis within the Northern Finland 1986 Birth Cohort. Schizophrenia Research 116, 152158.10.1016/j.schres.2009.10.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keskinen, E, Marttila, A, Marttila, R, Jones, PB, Murray, GK, Moilanen, K, Koivumaa-Honkanen, H, Mäki, P, Isohanni, M, Jääskeläinen, E and Miettunen, J (2015) Interaction between parental psychosis and early motor development and the risk of schizophrenia in a general population birth cohort. European Psychiatry 30, 719727.10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kremen, WS, Buka, SL, Seidman, LJ, Goldstein, JM, Koren, D and Tsuang, MT (1998) IQ decline during childhood and adult psychotic symptoms in a community sample: a 19-year longitudinal study. The American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 672677.10.1176/ajp.155.5.672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyaga, S, Lichtenstein, P, Boman, M, Hultman, C, Långström, N and Landen, M (2011) Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300000 people with severe mental disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 373379.10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurens, KR, Luo, L, Matheson, SL, Carr, VJ, Raudino, A, Harris, F and Green, MJ (2015) Common or distinct pathways to psychosis? A systematic review of evidence from prospective studies for developmental risk factors and antecedents of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders and affective psychoses. BMC Psychiatry 15, 205.10.1186/s12888-015-0562-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, JH, Lambe, MP, Cnattingius, S, Torrång, A, Björk, C, Sham, PC, David, AS, Murray, RM and Hultman, CM (2008) Scholastic achievement at age 16 and risk of schizophrenia and other psychoses: a national cohort study. Psychological Medicine 38, 11331140.10.1017/S0033291707002048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, JH, Lambe, MP, Cnattingius, S, Sham, PC, David, AS, Reichenberg, A, Murray, RM and Hultman, CM (2010) Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study. The British Journal of Psychiatry 196, 109115.10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060368CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, JH, Sariaslan, A, Almqvist, C, Lichtenstein, P, Larsson, H and Kyaga, S (2018) Artistic creativity and risk for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and unipolar depression: a Swedish population-based case-control study and sib-pair analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry 212, 370376.10.1192/bjp.2018.23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäki, P, Koskela, S, Murray, GK, Nordström, T, Miettunen, J, Jääskeläinen, E and Veijola, JM (2014) Difficulty in making contact with others and social withdrawal as early signs of psychosis in adolescents – the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986. European Psychiatry 29, 345351.10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matheson, SL, Shepherd, AM, Laurens, KR and Carr, VJ (2011) A systematic meta-review grading the evidence for non-genetic risk factors and putative antecedents of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 133, 133142.10.1016/j.schres.2011.09.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullola, S, Ravaja, N, Lipsanen, J, Alatupa, S, Hintsanen, M, Jokela, M and Keltikangas-Järvinen, L (2012) Gender differences in teachers’ perceptions of students’ temperament, educational competence, and teachability. The British Journal of Educational Psychology 82(Pt 2), 185206.10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02017.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munro, JC, Russell, AJ, Murray, RM, Kerwin, RW and Jones, PB (2002) IQ in childhood psychiatric attendees predicts outcome of later schizophrenia at 21 year follow-up. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 106, 139142.10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.02030.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myrbakk, E and von Tetzchner, S (2008) Psychiatric disorders and behavior problems in people with intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities 29, 316332.10.1016/j.ridd.2007.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B and Rawlings, D (2010) Relating schizotypy and personality to the phenomenology of creativity. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 388399.10.1093/schbul/sbn098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordentoft, M (2007) Prevention of suicide and attempted suicide in Denmark. Epidemiological studies of suicide and intervention studies in selected risk groups. Danish Medical Bulletin 54, 306369.Google ScholarPubMed
Paus, T, Keshavan, M and Giedd, JN (2008) Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9, 947957.10.1038/nrn2513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, R (2001) Creativity and the schizophrenia spectrum: more and more interesting. Creativity Research Journal 13, 111132.10.1207/S15326934CRJ1301_13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, J, Sundin, J, Leask, S and Done, DJ (2014) Risk of adult schizophrenia and its relationship to childhood IQ in the 1958 British birth cohort. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 143151.10.1093/schbul/sbs157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ström, K and Hannus-Gullmets, B (2015) From special (class) teacher to special educator – the Finnish case. In Cameron, DL and Thygesen, R (eds), Transitions in the Field of Special Education. Theoretical Perspectives and Implications for Practice. Münster: Waxmann, pp. 115134.Google Scholar
Sussmann, JE, McIntosh, AM, Lawrie, SM and Johnstone, EC (2009) Obstetric complications and mild to moderate intellectual disability. The British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 224228.10.1192/bjp.bp.106.033134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taanila, A, Ebeling, H, Kotimaa, A, Moilanen, I and Järvelin, MR (2004) Is a large family a protective factor against behavioural and emotional problems at the age of 8 years? Acta Paediatrica 93, 508517.10.1111/apa.2004.93.4.508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Therman, S, Heinimaa, M, Miettunen, J, Joukamaa, M, Moilanen, I, Mäki, P and Veijola, J (2011) Symptoms associated with psychosis risk in an adolescent birth cohort: improving questionnaire utility with a multidimensional approach. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 5, 343348.10.1111/j.1751-7893.2011.00290.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Werf, M, Hanssen, M, Köhler, S, Verkaaik, M, Verhey, FR, RISE Investigators, van Winkel, R, van Os, J and Allardyce, J (2014) Systematic review and collaborative recalculation of 133693 incident cases of schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 44, 916.10.1017/S0033291712002796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdoux, H and van Os, J (2002) Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations and the continuum of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 54, 5965.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00352-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, NP, McConville, PM, Hunter, D, Deary, IJ and Whalley, LJ (2002) Childhood mental ability and lifetime psychiatric contact: a 66-year follow-up study of the 1932 Scottish mental ability survey. Intelligence 30, 233245.10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00098-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiles, NJ, Jones, GT, Haase, AM, Lawlor, DA, Macfarlane, GJ and Lewis, G (2008) Physical activity and emotional problems amongst adolescents: a longitudinal study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 43, 765772.10.1007/s00127-008-0362-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S, Allebeck, P, David, AS, Dalman, C, Hemmingsson, T, Lundberg, I and Lewis, G (2004) A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ Score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 354360.10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Family characteristics [n (%)] by gender in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

Figure 1

Table 2. Learning deficits, special talents, and general school success at the age of 8 years by gender in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

Figure 2

Table 3. The psychotic-like symptoms (PROD-screen) at the age of 15–16 years according to scholastic traits at age 8 years in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

Figure 3

Table 4. The occurrence of psychoses by the age of 30 years according to scholastic traits at age 8 years in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986

Supplementary material: File

Lassila et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Lassila et al. supplementary material(File)
File 19.3 KB