Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T08:43:27.379Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gene–environment interplay in the etiology of psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2018

Alyson Zwicker
Affiliation:
Department of Pathology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada Nova Scotia Health Authority, Halifax, NS, Canada
Eileen M. Denovan-Wright
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
Rudolf Uher*
Affiliation:
Department of Pathology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada Nova Scotia Health Authority, Halifax, NS, Canada Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Rudolf Uher, E-mail: uher@dal.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Schizophrenia and other types of psychosis incur suffering, high health care costs and loss of human potential, due to the combination of early onset and poor response to treatment. Our ability to prevent or cure psychosis depends on knowledge of causal mechanisms. Molecular genetic studies show that thousands of common and rare variants contribute to the genetic risk for psychosis. Epidemiological studies have identified many environmental factors associated with increased risk of psychosis. However, no single genetic or environmental factor is sufficient to cause psychosis on its own. The risk of developing psychosis increases with the accumulation of many genetic risk variants and exposures to multiple adverse environmental factors. Additionally, the impact of environmental exposures likely depends on genetic factors, through gene–environment interactions. Only a few specific gene–environment combinations that lead to increased risk of psychosis have been identified to date. An example of replicable gene–environment interaction is a common polymorphism in the AKT1 gene that makes its carriers sensitive to developing psychosis with regular cannabis use. A synthesis of results from twin studies, molecular genetics, and epidemiological research outlines the many genetic and environmental factors contributing to psychosis. The interplay between these factors needs to be considered to draw a complete picture of etiology. To reach a more complete explanation of psychosis that can inform preventive strategies, future research should focus on longitudinal assessments of multiple environmental exposures within large, genotyped cohorts beginning early in life.

Type
Invited Review
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Psychosis refers to a subset of severe mental illness marked by delusions, hallucinations, and disorganized behavior. Schizophrenia is the prototypical psychotic disorder and it is defined by the presence of persistent psychotic symptoms and impaired functioning. However, psychosis manifests across a spectrum of conditions including major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, delusional and schizophreniform disorder. Over 3% of individuals develop a psychotic disorder at some point during their lifetime but less than 1% are diagnosed with schizophrenia (Perala et al. Reference Perala, Suvisaari, Saarni, Kuoppasalmi, Isometsa and Pirkola2007). All forms of psychosis tend to emerge in early adulthood, cause persistent disability, and premature death (Smith, Reference Smith2011; Reininghaus et al. Reference Reininghaus, Dutta, Dazzan, Doody, Fearon and Lappin2015). Psychotic disorders are often preceded by early manifestations of psychopathology, including transitory psychotic symptoms that commonly occur in childhood (Kelleher et al. Reference Kelleher, Connor, Clarke, Devlin, Harley and Cannon2012). Childhood psychotic symptoms tend to be transient but they are associated with increased risk of psychotic disorders in adulthood (Poulton et al. Reference Poulton, Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, Murray and Harrington2000; Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Caspi, Poulton, Meier, Houts and Harrington2013) and may be considered antecedents to psychotic illness (Uher et al. Reference Uher, Cumby, Mackenzie, Morash-conway, Glover and Aylott2014). Knowledge of mechanisms involved in the etiology of psychosis may inform effective prevention. While a variety of risk factors have been identified, the causation of psychosis is still far from being understood. In this paper, we take stock of genetic and environmental factors associated with psychosis. Since there is substantial overlap of etiological factors across all types of severe mental illness (Uher & Zwicker, Reference Uher and Zwicker2017), we include evidence on the entire spectrum of psychotic disorders wherever possible. However, the largest body of data has been collected specifically on schizophrenia. We will review genetic and environmental contributions to psychosis separately before examining the joint effects of genetics and the environment. We will conclude by providing suggestions to guide future research.

The role of genetics in the development of psychosis

Psychosis runs in families. The strongest known predictor of risk is having a close biological relative who is affected (Gottesman et al. Reference Gottesman, Laursen, Bertelsen and Mortensen2010). Results from adoption and twin studies suggest that the familial clustering of psychosis is due largely to genetic factors. Among individuals with schizophrenia who were adopted at birth, increased rates of schizophrenia were found among their biological relatives and not within their adoptive families (Kety et al. Reference Kety, Wender, Jacobsen, Ingraham, Jansson and Faber1994). Additionally, twin studies show that monozygotic twins, who are genetically identical, are substantially more similar in their propensity to develop schizophrenia than are dizygotic twins, who share only half of their genetic material. The heritability of schizophrenia derived from twin studies is estimated to be 92% (Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven and Visscher2015). Twin and family studies also suggest a substantial genetic contribution to psychotic symptoms in childhood (Zavos et al. Reference Zavos, Freeman, Haworth, McGuire, Plomin and Cardno2014). Taken together, this information strongly suggests that genetic factors significantly contribute to psychosis.

The last two decades have seen major progress in the identification of specific genetic variants that contribute to the risk of psychosis. To date, most molecular genetic investigations have focused on schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is a complex disorder and no single gene or genetic variant has been implicated as a necessary and sufficient causal factor. In linkage analysis, multiple loci across the genome have met the threshold for genome-wide or suggestive significance for an association with schizophrenia (Ng et al. Reference Ng, Levinson, Faraone, Suarez, DeLisi and Arinami2009). Due to the cost and difficulty associated with comprehensive genotyping, early investigations into specific molecular genetic contributions to psychosis focused on identifying associations between schizophrenia and a small number of genes thought to play a role in leading etiological hypotheses of psychosis. Using this hypothesis-driven method, associations were identified between several of these biologically plausible genes and schizophrenia. However initial reports often failed to replicate (Farrell et al. Reference Farrell, Werge, Sklar, Owen, Ophoff and O'Donovan2015). The candidate gene era culminated in the finding that associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in top candidate genes previously reported to be associated with schizophrenia were consistent with chance expectation in the largest sample available at the time (Sanders et al. Reference Sanders, Duan, Levinson, Shi, He and Hou2008).

In the past decade, technological advances have enabled large-scale genomic studies and consortia have formed that have brought together sample sizes necessary for adequately powered genome-wide investigations (Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Borglum and Breen2017). The focus has shifted from candidate gene studies to the exploration of molecular genetic contributions to psychosis on a genome-wide scale. A case-control genome-wide association study (GWAS) conducted by the Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium identified over 100 independent loci that are associated with schizophrenia (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014). It is likely that substantially more (thousands) common variants contribute to the genetic liability for psychosis, because the predictive ability of polygenic risk scores (see Table 1 for definition) for schizophrenia improves with the inclusion of weakly associated variants in addition to variants significantly associated with the disorder (see Fig. 1). Some of the variants identified by GWAS fall within predictable genes (e.g. DRD2 – which encodes the dopamine receptor D 2, the target of most effective antipsychotic medications). However, the strongest association stemmed from variation within the major histocompatibility complex locus on chromosome 6, which is an area of the genome known for its role in immune function. This association is partly due to the many alleles of the complement component 4 (C4) genes (Sekar et al. Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond and Kamitaki2016). These alleles produce varying levels of C4A/C4B expression in the brain and alleles producing greater C4A expression were more associated with increased risk of schizophrenia. C4 is expressed in neurons and has been shown to mediate synaptic pruning during brain development in mice (Sekar et al. Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond and Kamitaki2016). These results implicate the immune system and complement activity in the causation of schizophrenia.

Fig. 1. The polygenic risk for schizophrenia. The proportion of variance explained by polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia produced using the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) sample from 2011 (The Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium 2011) and 2014 (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014) as the discovery samples, with p value thresholds (PT) at 5 × 10−8 (genome-wide significance) and 0.05. The proportion of variance explained increases with both sample size and with the inclusion of more, non-significantly associated variants. This suggests that schizophrenia is highly polygenic, and many more genetic loci contribute to disease risk than have been captured by the most recent GWAS.

Table 1. Terminology of gene–environment interplay

The results of genome-wide investigations of common SNPs suggest that genetic liability to schizophrenia arises from the accumulation of small effects of hundreds to thousands of variants across the genome. In addition to the cumulative effect of common SNPs, rare copy number variants and disruptive mutations are also enriched among individuals with schizophrenia compared with controls (Purcell et al. Reference Purcell, Moran, Fromer, Ruderfer, Solovieff and Roussos2014; Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Howrigan, Merico, Thiruvahindrapuram, Wu and Greer2016). A specific deletion on chromosome 22, leading to 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, is associated with increased risk of psychosis. Approximately one in four individuals with this deletion will develop schizophrenia (Owen & Doherty, Reference Owen and Doherty2016). However, 22q11.2 deletion syndrome is not specific to schizophrenia, and is associated with a range of medical and psychiatric phenotypes (Niarchou et al. Reference Niarchou, Zammit, Van Goozen, Thapar, Tierling and Owen2014; Owen & Doherty, Reference Owen and Doherty2016). More recently, exome sequencing has enabled identification of rare variants that may alter protein function and are associated with schizophrenia (Girard et al. Reference Girard, Dion, Bourassa, Geoffroy, Lachance-Touchette and Barhdadi2015; Genovese et al. Reference Genovese, Fromer, Stahl, Ruderfer, Chambert and Landén2016; Singh et al. Reference Singh, Kurki, Curtis, Purcell, Crooks and McRae2016). Psychosis-associated rare variants, including deletions/duplications and copy number variants, are more prevalent in genes involved in neurodevelopment, e.g. neuregulin (Walsh et al. Reference Walsh, McClellan, McCarthy, Addington, Pierce and Cooper2008). Additionally, rare copy number variants have been identified in individuals with psychosis from densely affected families (Van Den Bossche et al. Reference Van Den Bossche, Strazisar, Cammaerts, Liekens, Vandeweyer and Depreeuw2013). Rare variants directly affecting protein function could individually have larger effects on psychosis risk than the common variants detected by genome-wide SNP analysis. The evidence suggests that genetic risk of psychosis is likely due to a combination of rare and common genetic variation.

The ability to identify and quantify genetic risk for psychosis allows us to investigate how genetic liability for illness manifests before disease onset. Genetic liability for schizophrenia is enriched among individuals with a family history (Bigdeli et al. Reference Bigdeli, Ripke, Bacanu, Lee, Wray and Gejman2016). The genetic burden of both rare and common variants is higher among individuals with early-onset compared with later-onset schizophrenia (Walsh et al. Reference Walsh, McClellan, McCarthy, Addington, Pierce and Cooper2008; Ahn et al. Reference Ahn, Gotay, Andersen, Anvari, Gochman and Lee2014, Reference Ahn, An, Shugart and Rapoport2016). In a general population sample, genetic risk for schizophrenia is associated with anxiety, negative symptoms, and worse neurodevelopmental outcomes (e.g. worse language fluency) in childhood and adolescence but, surprisingly, not with adolescent psychotic symptoms (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron and Cannon2016; Riglin et al. Reference Riglin, Collishaw, Richards, Thapar, Maughan and O'Donovan2016). However, these findings warrant further investigation due to the high and non-random attrition of participants in this particular study (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron and Cannon2016; Martin et al. Reference Martin, Tilling, Hubbard, Stergiakouli, Thapar and Davey Smith2016; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Jones, Sallis, Eusden, Stergiakouli and Davies2017). To find out when in development genetic risk for psychosis first manifests, it is necessary to establish large genotyped cohorts with high retention rates over longitudinal follow-ups with comprehensive phenotyping, including putative developmental precursors of psychotic illness.

Polygenic analyses across multiple disorders suggest that the genetic loci contributing to schizophrenia risk are also implicated in other mental and physical illnesses. There is substantial overlap between genetic liability to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, and autism (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013b). To a smaller extent, schizophrenia-associated genetic variants also confer increased risk of inflammatory bowel disease, further supporting a role of the immune system in schizophrenia etiology (Pickrell et al. Reference Pickrell, Berisa, Liu, Ségurel, Tung and Hinds2016). In summary, the heritable contribution to psychosis is highly polygenic, arises from a combination of rare and common variation, and overlaps with other forms of pathology.

Environmental factors contributing to psychosis

The relative contributions of environmental and genetic factors to psychosis have been debated over time. Twin studies seem to suggest that less than 20% of the variance in liability to schizophrenia is explained by environmental influences unique to the individual and that environment shared between twins plays no role in the causation of schizophrenia (Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Kendler and Neale2003; Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven and Visscher2015). However, other typed of research studies strongly suggest a relatively smaller contribution of genetics and greater role of environment. Epidemiological studies show strong and consistent associations between multiple environmental exposures and psychosis (Marconi et al. Reference Marconi, Di Forti, Lewis, Murray and Vassos2016; Varese et al. Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster and Viechtbauer2012). Molecular genetic studies in unrelated individuals give substantially lower heritability estimates than twin studies (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013a; Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Borglum and Breen2017). In combination, the results of epidemiological research and the gap between twin and molecular heritability estimates suggest that environmental exposures likely play a more prominent role in the etiology of psychotic illness than was previously thought (Uher & Zwicker, Reference Uher and Zwicker2017).

The major environmental exposures that have been implicated in psychosis are listed in Fig. 2. They can be clustered based on the stage in development when exposure could influence risk. Complex factors, such as parental socioeconomic status (SES) and income inequality tend to remain constant throughout development and have wide-reaching implications on health across the life course (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Boyce, Chesney, Cohen, Folkman and Kahn1994; Kennedy et al. Reference Kennedy, Kawachi, Glass and Prothrow-Stith1998). Other factors, including maternal viral infection, influence psychosis risk if individuals are exposed during a ‘sensitive’ period in development (Brown & Patterson, Reference Brown and Patterson2011). Most individuals, with and without psychotic illness, are exposed to at least one risk factor, which complicates the investigation of the roles of individual environmental exposures (Van Nierop et al. Reference Van Nierop, Janssens, Bruggeman, Cahn, De Haan and Kahn2013; Stepniak et al. Reference Stepniak, Papiol, Hammer, Ramin, Everts and Hennig2014).

Fig. 2. Environmental factors associated with psychosis. The number of plus signs denotes the strength of evidence for the association: +++ indicates consistent evidence from multiple large-scale studies or a meta-analysis; ++ indicates evidence from multiple smaller studies or a strong association in a high-quality study; + indicates evidence from a single study, multiple small/low-quality studies, or few studies with conflicting reports. The reference provided for each exposure reflects a meta-analysis (where available) or the largest study available. The list is limited to environmental exposures and excludes risk factors that reflect conditions of the individual (e.g. inflammation).

In terms of pre- and perinatal risk factors, exposures that influence immune function have been linked to the development of psychotic illness (Mednick et al. Reference Mednick, Machon, Huttunen and Bonett1988; van Os & Selten, Reference van Os and Selten1998; Brown et al. Reference Brown, Schaefer, Wyatt, Goetz, Begg and Gorman2000, Reference Brown, Cohen, Harkavy-Friedman, Babulas, Malaspina and Gorman2001; Buka et al. Reference Buka, Cannon, Torrey and Yolken2008; Khashan et al. Reference Khashan, Abel, Mcnamee, Pedersen, Webb and Baker2008; Malaspina et al. Reference Malaspina, Corcoran, Kleinhaus, Perrin, Fennig and Nahon2008; Torrey et al. Reference Torrey, Bartko and Yolken2012; Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Ellman, Schaefer, Maxwell, Shen and Chaudhury2016). It has been suggested that immune activation and subsequent inflammation could mediate the effects of pre- and perinatal insults, such as stress or infection, on psychosis risk by contributing to abnormal neurodevelopment (Deverman & Patterson, Reference Deverman and Patterson2009; Holloway et al. Reference Holloway, Moreno, Umali, Rayannavar, Hodes and Russo2013; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Culpepper, Rapaport and Buckley2013; Ménard et al. Reference Ménard, Pfau, Hodes and Russo2017). Individuals exposed to high levels of inflammation in utero are at increased risk of developing schizophrenia in adulthood (Canetta & Sourander, Reference Canetta and Sourander2014). Conversely, elevated levels of anti-inflammatory molecules in utero lower the risk of developing psychosis in adulthood (Allswede et al. Reference Allswede, Buka, Yolken, Torrey and Cannon2016). Results from animal studies suggest that the unfavorable effects of prenatal immune activation may extend across multiple generations. For example, using a mouse model, some pathological traits (e.g. reduced sociability) resulting from in utero exposure to immune activation were observable for up to three generations (Weber-Stadlbauer et al. Reference Weber-Stadlbauer, Richetto, Labouesse, Bohacek, Mansuy and Meyer2017). This suggests that epigenetic mechanisms or learned behavioral transmission may influence these traits.

Early adversities occurring in childhood, including physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, neglect and exposure to violence have been strongly implicated in the development of psychotic illness (Varese et al. Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster and Viechtbauer2012). More recently, involvement in bullying, both as a victim and as a bully, has been recognized as a contributor to both psychotic illness in adulthood and subclinical psychotic symptoms in adolescence (Schreier et al. Reference Schreier, Wolke, Thomas, Horwood, Hollis and Gunnell2009; Trotta et al. Reference Trotta, Di Forti, Mondelli, Dazzan, Pariante and David2013; Wolke et al. Reference Wolke, Lereya, Fisher, Lewis and Zammit2014). Childhood trauma is associated with increased levels of inflammatory markers in adulthood, which provides a possible mechanism through which childhood adversities could impact the development of psychosis (Baumeister et al. Reference Baumeister, Akhtar, Ciufolini, Pariante and Mondelli2016).

Exposures associated with psychosis that occur later in development are largely substance use-related. Abuse of psychostimulants is typically associated with acute psychosis, however, individuals with a family history of mental illness who use stimulants recreationally appear to be more vulnerable to persistent psychotic symptoms (Curran et al. Reference Curran, Byrappa and Mcbride2004; Li et al. Reference Li, Lu, Xiao, Li, He and Mei2014; Hajebi et al. Reference Hajebi, Amini, Kashani and Sharifi2016; McKetin et al. Reference McKetin, Gardner, Baker, Dawe, Ali and Voce2016). Interestingly, the link between stimulants and psychosis extends to children with a family history of mental illness taking stimulants to treat attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (MacKenzie et al. Reference MacKenzie, Abidi, Fisher, Propper, Bagnell and Morash-Conway2016). The risk of experiencing psychotic symptoms is more than four times higher among children taking prescribed stimulant medication compared with those who have never taken stimulants (MacKenzie et al. Reference MacKenzie, Abidi, Fisher, Propper, Bagnell and Morash-Conway2016). Regardless of family history status, cannabis use has been strongly and consistently associated with psychotic disorders (Moore et al. Reference Moore, Zammit, Lingford-hughes, Barnes, Jones and Burke2007; Gage et al. Reference Gage, Hickman and Zammit2015). In the case of cannabis exposure, many criteria of causality are met including a positive dose–response relationship between cannabis use and psychotic outcomes (Marconi et al. Reference Marconi, Di Forti, Lewis, Murray and Vassos2016), the temporal sequence of cannabis use preceding the onset of psychosis (Arseneault et al. Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Poulton, Murray, Caspi and Moffitt2002; Stefanis et al. Reference Stefanis, Dragovic, Power, Jablensky, Castle and Morgan2013; Kelley et al. Reference Kelley, Wan, Broussard, Crisafio, Cristofaro and Johnson2016), and consistent evidence for an association (Hill, Reference Hill1965). Additionally, a Mendelian randomization study (see Table 1) found that use of cannabis is causally associated with risk of schizophrenia and that cannabis users had a 37% increased risk of developing schizophrenia (Vaucher et al. Reference Vaucher, Keating, Lasserre, Gan, Lyall and Ward2017). There is also evidence that tobacco use could play a role in the development of psychosis (van Gastel et al. Reference van Gastel, MacCabe, Schubart, Vreeker, Tempelaar and Kahn2013; Gurillo et al. Reference Gurillo, Jauhar, Murray and MacCabe2015; McGrath et al. Reference McGrath, Alati, Clavarino, Williams, Bor and Najman2016). The fact that cannabis and tobacco are often used by the same individuals complicates the investigation into the nature of the individual relationships between cannabis and tobacco use and psychosis (Gage et al. Reference Gage, Hickman, Heron, Munafò, Lewis and Macleod2014). This highlights the necessity of gathering information on exposure to multiple factors when examining environmental contributors to psychosis.

Broad characteristics of the physical environment during development have also been associated with psychotic illness. Upbringing in an urban center is associated with increased risk of psychosis compared with rural upbringing (Vassos et al. Reference Vassos, Pedersen, Murray, Collier and Lewis2012). Risk increases with total time spent living in an urban setting (Pedersen & Mortensen, Reference Pedersen and Mortensen2001). This finding is not exclusive to adult illness; psychotic symptoms among children and adolescents are more frequent and more likely to progress to a first episode of psychosis among youth living in an urban environment (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, Moffitt, Arseneault, Cannon, Ambler and Keefe2010; Dragt et al. Reference Dragt, Nieman, Veltman, Becker, van de Fliert and de Haan2011). The exact components of the urban environment that contribute to psychosis are not well-defined. Air pollution and toxins, such as heavy metals, are not likely to play a substantial role (Attademo et al. Reference Attademo, Bernardini, Garinella and Compton2017). A deficiency or excess of Vitamin D, which is produced in the skin when exposed to sunlight and may play a role in the regulation of gene expression, could explain a proportion of psychosis (McGrath, Reference McGrath1999; McGrath et al. Reference McGrath, Saari, Hakko, Jokelainen, Jones and Järvelin2004, Reference McGrath, Burne, Féron, MacKay-Sim and Eyles2010a, Reference McGrath, Eyles, Pedersen, Anderson, Ko and Burne2010b). It has been suggested that vitamin D deficiency is more prevalent in urban centers, possibly due to less sun exposure (Holick, Reference Holick1995). In the case of childhood and adolescent psychotic symptoms, neighborhood factors including low social cohesion and high crime rate partially explain the elevated risk for psychotic symptoms among children living in urban areas (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, Moffitt, Arseneault, Cannon, Ambler and Keefe2010; Newbury et al. Reference Newbury, Arseneault, Caspi, Moffitt, Odgers and Fisher2016).

Although some environmental risk factors for psychosis have been identified, it is difficult to separate the effects of individual factors because exposures often cluster within the same person (Gage et al. Reference Gage, Hickman, Heron, Munafò, Lewis and Macleod2014; Sideli et al. Reference Sideli, Fisher, Murray, Sallis, Russo and Stilo2015). Similar to the notion of many genetic factors of small effect sizes contributing to the genetic risk of psychosis, it has been suggested that environmental risk is also attributable to the cumulative contribution of many exposures (Van Nierop et al. Reference Van Nierop, Janssens, Bruggeman, Cahn, De Haan and Kahn2013). Exposure to a greater number of environmental risk factors was associated with earlier age at onset of psychotic symptoms and first-episode of psychosis (Stepniak et al. Reference Stepniak, Papiol, Hammer, Ramin, Everts and Hennig2014; O'Donoghue et al. Reference O'Donoghue, Lyne, Madigan, Lane, Turner and O'Callaghan2015). An aggregate ‘polyenviromic’ score predicted the onset of psychosis among youth with a family history of psychotic illness (Padmanabhan et al. Reference Padmanabhan, Shah, Tandon and Keshavan2017). Although exposure to certain environments increases the risk of psychosis, only a minority of individuals exposed to these factors will become ill. Genetic differences may render some individuals more vulnerable or resilient to the impact of environmental exposures.

Gene–environment interplay

Psychotic illness in a given individual arises due to a combination of genetics and environment. The term gene–environment interplay captures the combined contributory effects of both genetics and environment to psychosis. Gene–environment interplay encompasses gene-environment correlation (rGE) and gene–environment interaction (GxE; see Table 1 for definitions).

Gene-environment correlation

Exposure to specific environments is not random. Gene-environment correlation (rGE) refers to this non-random relationship between genotype and exposure. It is important to assess the possible contribution of rGE when investigating causal contributors to psychosis. Three forms of rGE have been widely described: passive rGE, evocative (or reactive) rGE and active rGE (Plomin et al. Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977), see Fig. 3.

Fig. 3. Gene-environment correlations and psychosis. The three panels illustrate passive, evocative and active gene-environment correlations (rGE), base on specific examples that are relevant to the etiology of psychosis. The same examples are described and referenced in the text section Gene-Environment Correlation.

Passive gene-environment correlation

Passive rGE describes the association between genotype and rearing environment, both of which are influenced by an individual's parents’ genes. For example, the association between childhood abuse and genetic risk for psychosis represents a potential passive rGE. Children of parents with psychosis are at increased risk of experiencing childhood maltreatment and individuals who experience abuse are more likely to develop psychosis (Walsh et al. Reference Walsh, MacMillan and Jamieson2002; Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan2014). However, it is unclear whether the association between childhood maltreatment and psychosis arises because abuse directly causes psychosis or because genetic factors increase both the likelihood of experiencing abuse and the propensity to develop psychosis. If the latter is true, the association between childhood abuse and psychotic illness represents a passive gene-environment correlation. Adoption studies provide a useful design to tease apart passive rGE from G × E. Using this methodology, it has been possible to demonstrate that individuals with a family history of schizophrenia are truly more vulnerable to the psychosis-inducing effects of childhood adversity than are individuals without a family history (Tienari et al. Reference Tienari, Ynne, Sorri and Lahti2004).

Evocative gene-environment correlation

Personal characteristics determine how individuals interact in social situations, and as a result, influence the responses they will elicit from others. Evocative rGE describes how differences in genotype will evoke different reactions. As an example, genetic risk of schizophrenia is associated with the presence of childhood anxiety disorders (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron and Cannon2016). Anxious children may evoke a different response from peers than their less anxious counterparts, resulting in an evocative rGE. Thus, anxious children may face rejection in social interactions with peers, which could, in turn, increase their risk of developing psychosis (Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007).

Active gene-environment correlation

Individual behaviors and preferences are also partially determined by genetics. By influencing these factors, genotype also influences the experiences that people will seek out. Active rGE describes how genetic variation can contribute to differences in the likelihood of exposure to environments. For example, individuals at high genetic risk for schizophrenia are more likely to use cannabis (Verweij et al. Reference Verweij, Abdellaoui, Nivard, Sainz Cort, Ligthart and Draisma2017). Genotype, therefore, influences both the propensity to develop psychosis and the likelihood of being exposed to cannabis. Active rGE could account for a portion of the association between cannabis use and psychosis. However, genetic risk for schizophrenia only explains a small proportion (0.5%) of the variance in cannabis use (Verweij et al. Reference Verweij, Abdellaoui, Nivard, Sainz Cort, Ligthart and Draisma2017). Therefore, it is unlikely that rGE fully explains the association between cannabis use and psychosis. It does, however, bring attention to the need to consider rGE before making causal interpretations that assume relative independence of genetic and environmental factors.

Gene–environment interaction

Exposure to certain environmental factors increases the risk of psychosis, however, these adverse environments do not affect everyone equally. Some individuals remain healthy even when exposed to multiple known risk factors whereas others will go on to develop a psychotic disorder (Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007; Tottenham, Reference Tottenham2013). Genetic factors may render certain individuals more vulnerable to the impact of environmental exposures, resulting in a gene–environment interaction. Two sets of findings suggest that G × E plays a substantial role in the development of psychosis. First, much larger heritability estimates of schizophrenia have been obtained from twin studies than from molecular genetic studies using unrelated individuals (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013a). Second, despite the fact that epidemiological studies have robustly demonstrated that risk factors shared within families (such as minority status, urbanicity, or low SES) are among the top environmental contributors to psychosis, twin studies suggest that shared environment plays a very minor or no role (Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven and Visscher2015). The best estimate of shared environment contribution across twin studies is actually a negative number (Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven and Visscher2015). These implausible findings can be explained by the manner in which heritability is calculated in twin studies: the interplay between genetic factors and environmental variables shared within a family are attributed to genetics and inflate heritability estimates while reducing the estimated contribution of shared environment (Taylor, Reference Taylor2007; Uher & Zwicker, Reference Uher and Zwicker2017). Therefore, G × E offers a plausible explanation for these discrepant findings.

In addition to providing an explanation for the conflicting heritability estimates from twin studies, molecular genetic studies and epidemiology, identification of G × E could offer additional insight into both genetic and environmental contributions to psychosis. First, G × E research can lead to the identification of novel genetic contributors to psychosis that may not otherwise be identified in case-control studies (Børglum et al. Reference Børglum, Demontis, Grove, Pallesen, Hollegaard and Pedersen2013). Additionally, identification of G × E is valuable because unlike genetics, environment is malleable and can be modified selectively among those at high genetic risk. Therefore, identifying G × E could provide the opportunity for targeted interventions to minimize or eliminate exposure to environmental contributors to psychosis, particularly exposures that are under the control of the individual, such as cannabis, tobacco and stimulant use. Despite the significant role of G × E in psychosis and the potential value of G × E knowledge, identification of G × Es to date has been slow.

Gene–environment interaction by proxy

Initial investigation of G × E in psychosis relied on the family history of severe mental illness as a proxy fo genetic risk. Using this method, interactions between ‘genetic liability’ for psychotic illness and exposure to environmental factors have been identified. Individuals with a family history of psychotic illness appear to be particularly sensitive to the effects of multiple environmental contributors to psychosis including cannabis use (GROUP investigators, 2011; van Winkel & GROUP investigators, Reference van Winkel2015), urban upbringing (Van Os et al. Reference Van Os, Hanssen, Bak, Bijl and Vollebergh2003, Reference Van Os, Pedersen and Mortensen2004), and maternal infection (Clarke et al. Reference Clarke, Tanskanen, Huttunen, Whittaker and Cannon2009). There have also been reports in which no interaction was found between family history and known environmental risk factors, such as childhood maltreatment (Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, McGuffin, Boydell, Fearon, Craig and Dazzan2014; Trotta et al. Reference Trotta, Di Forti, Iyegbe, Green, Dazzan and Mondelli2015). Other factors, such as maternal depression during pregnancy, selectively increases the risk of psychosis among those with at least one parent affected by psychotic illness (Mäki et al. Reference Mäki, Riekki, Miettunen, Isohanni, Jones and Murray2010). However, since there is overlap in the genetic factors contributing to depression and psychosis, this result could also reflect a gene-environment correlation or interaction between multiple genetic factors (Han et al. Reference Han, Pouget, Slowikowski, Stahl, Lee and Diogo2016). Gene-environment studies using proxy measures are inherently limited because a family history of illness is not equivalent to the genetic contribution to psychosis and because genetic variants that increase sensitivity to the environment may be distinct from genetic variants that directly increase the risk of illness. The applicability of findings using proxy measures of genetic contribution to illness is also limited because the comprehensive family history of mental illness is not always known.

Gene–environment interactions involving molecular genetic variants

The search for gene–environment interactions involving specific molecular genetic variants began by testing the interaction between environmental risk factors for psychosis and variants within candidate genes. These studies faced the same challenges as candidate gene association studies discussed earlier. This approach relies on correctly selecting both the genetic variant and environmental exposure of interest based on prior knowledge. Surprisingly, this approach has led to the identification of interactions between variants in a handful of genes and environmental factors influencing the risk of psychosis (see Table 2). Replication of results, however, has been inconsistent. For example, carriers of the methionine-encoding allele of BDNF [Val66Met (rs6265)] have been shown to be at increased risk of experiencing psychotic symptoms after being exposed to childhood trauma (Alemany et al. Reference Alemany, Arias, Aguilera, Villa, Moya and Ibanez2011), but attempts to replicate this finding have either found no interaction between genotype and exposure to trauma (Ramsay et al. Reference Ramsay, Kelleher, Flannery, Clarke, Lynch and Harley2013), or found discordant results regarding which allele (valine-encoding v. methionine-encoding) increases psychosis risk following exposure (de Castro-Catala et al. Reference de Castro-Catala, van Nierop, Barrantes-Vidal, Cristóbal-Narváez, Sheinbaum and Kwapil2016). More systematic searches, involving screening hundreds of polymorphisms across functionally defined groups of genes, have been carried out in search of a G × E. This methodology led to the identification of an interaction between cannabis use and a SNP in AKT1, a gene encoding a serine/threonine kinase involved in the transduction of signal following cannabinoid receptor activation (van Winkel, Reference van Winkel2011). Homozygous carriers of the C allele at rs2494732 in AKT1 appear to be more vulnerable to the psychosis-inducing properties of cannabis. In a replication study, individuals with the C/C genotype at rs2494732 who used cannabis daily were found to be at 7-fold increased risk of developing psychotic illness compared with T allele homozygotes (Di Forti et al. Reference Di Forti, Iyegbe, Sallis, Kolliakou, Falcone and Paparelli2012). This finding has been replicated in two independent samples since the original report and therefore likely represents a true G × E (Di Forti et al. Reference Di Forti, Iyegbe, Sallis, Kolliakou, Falcone and Paparelli2012; Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Freeman, Powell and Curran2016).

Table 2. Molecular gene–environment interactions in psychosis

The table lists specific molecular gene–environment interactions for which there is at least one published replication attempt. The first column classifies the G × Es based on the initial search strategy into G × Es identified based on focused candidate gene testing, systematic searches including polymorphisms that tag genetic variation in a functionally defined group of genes and genome-wide systematic search. For replication results, ‘Y’ denotes a replication, ‘Y*’ denotes a replication with a different but similar exposure or outcome from the original report, ‘N’ denotes a failed replication attempt, and ‘N*’ denotes a replication attempt where an interaction between the variant and the environment of interest was found but the sensitive allele differed from the original report.

Comprehensive genome-wide search strategies, referred to as genome–wide environment interaction studies (GWEIS), have also been conducted to systematically search for G × E. The first GWEIS in psychosis found a significant interaction in a genome-wide test examining the interaction between in utero exposure to cytomegalovirus (CMV) infection and hundreds of thousands of common SNPs across the genome (Børglum et al. Reference Børglum, Demontis, Grove, Pallesen, Hollegaard and Pedersen2013). This led to the identification of a single G × E between CMV infection and a variant within a gene (CTNNA3) not previously associated with psychosis (Børglum et al. Reference Børglum, Demontis, Grove, Pallesen, Hollegaard and Pedersen2013). Carriers of the minor allele at rs7902091 in CTNNA3 exposed to CMV in utero are at increased risk of developing schizophrenia compared with those who do not carry the minor allele and those who were not exposed to CMV. A genome-wide approach to G × E research is likely to be superior to hypothesis-driven approaches, as many schizophrenia-associated genetic loci that have been identified to date are found within genes that were not previously suspected to be implicated in psychosis (Collins et al. Reference Collins, Kim, Sklar, O'Donovan and Sullivan2012). Due to a large number of loci being tested, GWEIS require very large samples to be adequately powered – even when examining interactions between common variants and common exposures. Although it is likely that G × Es play a significant role in the development of psychosis, no individual G × E identified thus far explains a substantial proportion of cases. The potential gene–environment interactions involved in the early manifestation of psychosis in childhood and adolescence remain to be examined.

Gene–environment interactions and polygenic scores

It is possible to summarize the effects of hundreds to thousands of variants across the genome as a single polygenic score. Polygenic risk scores calculated based on the most recent case-control GWAS for schizophrenia have been used to test G × E across the genome. One study derived a score from a few thousand schizophrenia-associated genetic variants located within coding and regulatory regions and found that this score interacted with winter birth to increase the risk of developing schizophrenia (Hong Lee et al. Reference Hong Lee, Byrne, Hultman, Kahler, Vinkhuyzen and Ripke2015). Another study found no interaction between polygenic risk score for schizophrenia and childhood adversity (Trotta et al. Reference Trotta, Iyegbe, Di Forti, Sham, Campbell and Cherny2016). The authors of this study concluded that their results support a model whereby genetic and environmental factors contribute independently to psychosis risk.

Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia may not be the ideal method to detect G × E because sensitivity to the environment and risk of illness may be determined by distinct subsets of genetic variants. It has been suggested that the individuals who are most severely impacted by adverse environments may also benefit most from positive ones. This is referred to as differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009). The differential susceptibility hypothesis posits that the genetic variants that render individuals sensitive to environmental influences may be distinct from genetic factors that directly influence the risk of psychopathology (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009). Therefore, G × E involving variants contributing to differential susceptibility will not be captured when using a polygenic risk score for schizophrenia (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009). A ‘polygenic sensitivity score’ indexing overall sensitivity to the environment (both positive and negative) predicted both the effects of parenting on emotional problems and response to psychological treatment among children with anxiety disorders (Keers et al. Reference Keers, Coleman, Lester, Roberts, Breen and Thastum2016). This sensitivity score did not, however, predict psychopathology directly, thus providing evidence that environmental sensitivity is genetically distinct from illness (Keers et al. Reference Keers, Coleman, Lester, Roberts, Breen and Thastum2016). Like polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia, the predictive ability of the polygenic sensitivity score improved with the inclusion of a greater number of less significantly associated variants, suggesting that genetic contribution to environmental sensitivity is also dispersed over a large number of loci across the genome. A polygenic sensitivity score has yet to be tested in the etiology of psychosis or its early developmental antecedents.

Conclusions, gaps in evidence and future directions

The findings of the past decade have led to a better understanding of the etiology of psychosis but also highlight a need to shift the direction of future research. Advances in the availability of genome-wide technology have uncovered the massively polygenic nature of psychosis. Epidemiological investigations have identified many environmental exposures that are associated with psychosis, however, the effect of these exposures differs between individuals. It is likely that genetic factors influence the impact of environmental exposures, and that genetic sensitivity to the environment is also highly polygenic. Identification of specific gene–environment interactions has proven to be difficult and has been hindered by lack of environmental measurement in large genetic studies. GWEIS can detect multiple gene–environment interactions in a systematic manner, but this methodology is limited in terms of statistical power. Polygenic environmental sensitivity scores may facilitate future genome-wide investigation of G × E in moderately large samples.

Despite the progress of the past decade, there remain important gaps in knowledge. Although genotyping technologies have improved in efficiency and affordability, the commonly used genome-wide genotyping methods do not provide full coverage of the genome. Thus, despite their names, genomic methods such as GWAS, GWEIS, and polygenic risk scores -environment interaction studies still leave a proportion of genetic variation unexplored. Additionally, most investigations of the genetic contribution to psychosis have been performed by comparing the genotypes of individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia to controls who do not have schizophrenia. This approach, though a necessary first step, limits the generalizability of polygenic risk scores from schizophrenia to other forms of psychosis. Indeed, a polygenic risk score for schizophrenia may predict other psychotic disorders less accurately than narrowly defined schizophrenia (Vassos et al. Reference Vassos, Di Forti, Coleman, Iyegbe, Prata and Euesden2017). Additionally, most large-scale investigations of the genetic contribution to schizophrenia have relied on ethnically homogenous samples of white individuals of European descent. This limits the generalizability to other ethnicities. In a sample of individuals of African descent, the proportion of variance in psychosis case-control status explained by polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia is approximately 1/9th of the proportion of variance explained in a European sample (1.1% v. 9.4%; Vassos et al. Reference Vassos, Di Forti, Coleman, Iyegbe, Prata and Euesden2017). To improve the utility of genetic risk information, future gene-environment investigations should cover a broader range of psychosis phenotypes in ethnically diverse samples.

Samples with a thorough assessment of both psychopathology and environment at multiple points in development will likely provide the most valuable information on gene-environment causation. Psychosis typically emerges in the second decade of life and it is often preceded by different and milder forms of mental illness. It is therefore essential to examine environmental exposures and psychopathology earlier in life, prior to disease onset. These studies should begin as early as during pregnancy or infancy and continue longitudinally across development. Regular assessment across development will allow us to establish true age at onset and track the development of psychosis over time. Large cohorts with genome-wide SNP genotyping and/or whole genome sequencing and comprehensive assessment of exposures and psychopathology across development are needed for this investigation. Important steps towards establishing such cohorts are currently underway (Korver et al. Reference Korver, Quee, Boos, Simons and de Haan2012; Uher et al. Reference Uher, Cumby, Mackenzie, Morash-conway, Glover and Aylott2014; Thorup et al. Reference Thorup, Jepsen, Ellersgaard, Burton, Christiani and Hemager2015; Kooijman et al. Reference Kooijman, Kruithof, van Duijn, Duijts, Franco and van IJzendoorn2016). To expand and combine information from these cohorts, future work should focus on the formation of large-scale, transparent collaborations and data sharing mechanisms.

Acknowledgements

The work on this manuscript has been completed thanks to funding from the Canada Research Chairs Program (reference number 231397) and grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Grant reference numbers 124976, 142738 and 148394), Nova Scotia Health Research Foundation and the Dalhousie Medical Research Foundation awarded to Dr Rudolf Uher.

References

Adler, N, Boyce, T, Chesney, M, Cohen, S, Folkman, S, Kahn, R et al. (1994) Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. The American Psychologist 49, 1524.Google Scholar
Agerbo, E, Sullivan, PF, Vilhjálmsson, BJ, Pedersen, CB, Mors, O, Børglum, AD et al. (2015) Polygenic risk score, parental socioeconomic status, family history of psychiatric disorders, and the risk for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 635.Google Scholar
Ahn, K, An, SS, Shugart, YY and Rapoport, JL (2016) Common polygenic variation and risk for childhood-onset schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 9496.Google Scholar
Ahn, K, Gotay, N, Andersen, TM, Anvari, AA, Gochman, P, Lee, Y et al. (2014) High rate of disease-related copy number variations in childhood onset schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 19, 568572.Google Scholar
Alemany, S, Arias, B, Aguilera, M, Villa, H, Moya, J, Ibanez, MI et al. (2011) Childhood abuse, the BDNF-Val66Met polymorphism and adult psychotic-like experiences. The British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 3842.Google Scholar
Alemany, S, Arias, B, Fatjó-Vilas, M, Villa, H, Moya, J, Ibáñez, MI et al. (2014) Psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis are related to both childhood abuse and COMT genotypes. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 129, 5462.Google Scholar
Alemany, S, Moya, J, Ibáñez, MI, Villa, H, Mezquita, L, Ortet, G et al. (2016) Research letter: childhood trauma and the rs1360780 SNP of FKBP5 gene in psychosis: a replication in two general population samples. Psychological Medicine 46, 221223.Google Scholar
Allswede, DM, Buka, SL, Yolken, RH, Torrey, EF and Cannon, TD (2016) Elevated maternal cytokine levels at birth and risk for psychosis in adult offspring. Schizophrenia Research 172, 4145.Google Scholar
Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Poulton, R, Murray, R, Caspi, A and Moffitt, TE (2002) Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospective study. BMJ 325, 12121213.Google Scholar
Attademo, L, Bernardini, F, Garinella, R and Compton, MT (2017) Environmental pollution and risk of psychotic disorders: a review of the science to date. Schizophrenia Research 181, 5559.Google Scholar
Avramopoulos, D, Pearce, BD, McGrath, J, Wolyniec, P, Wang, R, Eckart, N et al. (2015) Infection and inflammation in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a genome wide study for interactions with genetic variation. PLOS ONE 10, e0116696.Google Scholar
Baumeister, D, Akhtar, R, Ciufolini, S, Pariante, CM and Mondelli, V (2016) Childhood trauma and adulthood inflammation: a meta-analysis of peripheral C-reactive protein, interleukin-6 and tumour necrosis factor-α. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 642649.Google Scholar
Belsky, J and Pluess, M (2009) Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin 135, 885908.Google Scholar
Bigdeli, TB, Ripke, S, Bacanu, SA, Lee, SH, Wray, NR, Gejman, PV et al. (2016) Genome-wide association study reveals greater polygenic loading for schizophrenia in cases with a family history of illness. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 171, 276289.Google Scholar
Børglum, A, Demontis, D, Grove, J, Pallesen, J, Hollegaard, M, Pedersen, C et al. (2013) Genome-wide study of association and interaction with maternal cytomegalovirus infection suggests new schizophrenia loci. Molecular Psychiatry 19, 325333.Google Scholar
Brown, A, Schaefer, C, Wyatt, R, Goetz, R, Begg, M, Gorman, J et al. (2000) Maternal exposure to respiratory infections and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective birth cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 287295.Google Scholar
Brown, AS, Cohen, P, Harkavy-Friedman, J, Babulas, V, Malaspina, D, Gorman, JM et al. (2001) Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormalities, and adult schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 49, 473486.Google Scholar
Brown, AS and Patterson, PH (2011) Maternal infection and schizophrenia: implications for prevention. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 284290.Google Scholar
Buka, SL, Cannon, TD, Torrey, EF and Yolken, RH (2008) Maternal exposure to herpes simplex virus and risk of psychosis Among adult offspring. Biological Psychiatry 63, 809815.Google Scholar
Burns, JK, Tomita, A and Kapadia, AS (2014) Income inequality and schizophrenia: increased schizophrenia incidence in countries with high levels of income inequality. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 60, 185196.Google Scholar
Byrne, M, Agerbo, E, Bennedsen, B, Eaton, WW and Mortensen, PB (2007) Obstetric conditions and risk of first admission with schizophrenia: a Danish national register based study. Schizophrenia Research 97, 5159.Google Scholar
Canetta, S and Sourander, A (2014) Elevated maternal C-reactive protein and increased risk of schizophrenia in a national birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 960968.Google Scholar
Cantor-Graae, E (2005) Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis and review. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 1224.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Cannon, M, McClay, J, Murray, R, Harrington, H et al. (2005) Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal evidence of a gene X environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry 57, 11171127.Google Scholar
Clarke, MC, Tanskanen, A, Huttunen, M, Whittaker, JC and Cannon, M (2009) Evidence for an interaction between familial liability and prenatal exposure to infection in the causation of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 10251030.Google Scholar
Collins, AL, Kim, Y, Sklar, P, O'Donovan, MC and Sullivan, PF (2012) Hypothesis-driven candidate genes for schizophrenia compared to genome-wide association results. Psychological Medicine 42, 607616.Google Scholar
Collip, D, Myin-Germeys, I, Wichers, M, Jacobs, N, Derom, C, Thiery, E et al. (2013) FKBP5 as a possible moderator of the psychosis-inducing effects of childhood trauma. British Journal of Psychiatry 202, 261268.Google Scholar
Collip, D, van Winkel, R, Peerbooms, O, Lataster, T, Thewissen, V, Lardinois, M et al. (2011) COMT Val158Met-stress interaction in psychosis: role of background psychosis risk. CNS Neuroscience and Therapeutics 17, 612619.Google Scholar
Collishaw, S, Pickles, A, Messer, J, Rutter, M, Shearer, C and Maughan, B (2007) Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse and Neglect 31, 211229.Google Scholar
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2013a) Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nature Genetics 45, 984994.Google Scholar
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2013b) Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. The Lancet 381, 13711379.Google Scholar
Curran, C, Byrappa, N and Mcbride, A (2004) Stimulant psychosis: systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry 185, 196204.Google Scholar
de Castro-Catala, M, van Nierop, M, Barrantes-Vidal, N, Cristóbal-Narváez, P, Sheinbaum, T, Kwapil, TR et al. (2016) Childhood trauma, BDNF Val66Met and subclinical psychotic experiences. Attempt at replication in two independent samples. Journal of Psychiatric Research 83, 121129.Google Scholar
Deverman, BE and Patterson, PH (2009) Cytokines and CNS development. Neuron 64, 6178.Google Scholar
Di Forti, M, Iyegbe, C, Sallis, H, Kolliakou, A, Falcone, MA, Paparelli, A et al. (2012) Confirmation that the AKT1 (rs2494732) genotype influences the risk of psychosis in cannabis users. Biological Psychiatry 72, 811816.Google Scholar
Dragt, S, Nieman, DH, Veltman, D, Becker, HE, van de Fliert, R, de Haan, L et al. (2011) Environmental factors and social adjustment as predictors of a first psychosis in subjects at ultra high risk. Schizophrenia Research 125, 6976.Google Scholar
Farrell, MS, Werge, T, Sklar, P, Owen, MJ, Ophoff, RA, O'Donovan, MC et al. (2015) Evaluating historical candidate genes for schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 555562.Google Scholar
Fearon, P, Kirkbride, JB, Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Morgan, K, Lloyd, T et al. (2006) Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic minority groups: results from the MRC AESOP study. Psychological Medicine 36, 15411550.Google Scholar
Fineberg, AM, Ellman, LM, Schaefer, CA, Maxwell, SD, Shen, L, Chaudhury, NH et al. (2016) Fetal exposure to maternal stress and risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders among offspring: differential influences of fetal sex. Psychiatry Research 236, 9197.Google Scholar
Fisher, H, Caspi, A, Poulton, R, Meier, M, Houts, R, Harrington, H et al. (2013) Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: a birth cohort study. Psychological Medicine 43, 20772086.Google Scholar
Fisher, HL, McGuffin, P, Boydell, J, Fearon, P, Craig, TK, Dazzan, P et al. (2014) Interplay between childhood physical abuse and familial risk in the onset of psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 14431451.Google Scholar
Gage, SH, Hickman, M, Heron, J, Munafò, MR, Lewis, G, Macleod, J et al. (2014) Associations of cannabis and cigarette use with psychotic experiences at age 18: findings from the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Psychological Medicine 44, 34353444.Google Scholar
Gage, SH, Hickman, M and Zammit, S (2015) Association between cannabis and psychosis: epidemiologic evidence. Biological Psychiatry 18, 18.Google Scholar
Genovese, G, Fromer, M, Stahl, EA, Ruderfer, DM, Chambert, K, Landén, M et al. (2016) Increased burden of ultra-rare protein-altering variants among 4877 individuals with schizophrenia. Nature Neuroscience 19, 14331441.Google Scholar
Girard, SL, Dion, PA, Bourassa, CV, Geoffroy, S, Lachance-Touchette, P, Barhdadi, A et al. (2015) Mutation burden of rare variants in schizophrenia candidate genes. PLOS ONE 10, 111.Google Scholar
Gottesman, II, Laursen, TM, Bertelsen, A and Mortensen, PB (2010) Severe mental disorders in offspring with 2 psychiatrically ill parents. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 252257.Google Scholar
Green, MJ, Chia, TY, Cairns, MJ, Wu, J, Tooney, PA, Scott, RJ et al. (2014) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotype moderates the effects of childhood trauma on cognition and symptoms in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 49, 4350.Google Scholar
GROUP investigators (2011) Evidence that familial liability for psychosis is expressed as differential sensitivity to cannabis. Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 138.Google Scholar
Gurillo, P, Jauhar, S, Murray, RM and MacCabe, JH (2015) Does tobacco use cause psychosis? Systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry 2, 718725.Google Scholar
Hajebi, A, Amini, H, Kashani, L and Sharifi, V (2016) Twelve-month course and outcome of methamphetamine-induced psychosis compared with first episode primary psychotic disorders. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 17.Google Scholar
Han, B, Pouget, JG, Slowikowski, K, Stahl, E, Lee, CH, Diogo, D et al. (2016) A method to decipher pleiotropy by detecting underlying heterogeneity driven by hidden subgroups applied to autoimmune and neuropsychiatric diseases. Nature Genetics 48, 803810.Google Scholar
Hill, AB (1965) The environment and disease: association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine 58, 295300.Google Scholar
Holick, F (1995) Environmental factors that influence the cutaneous production of vitamin D1-3. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 61, 638S645S.Google Scholar
Holloway, T, Moreno, JL, Umali, A, Rayannavar, V, Hodes, GE, Russo, SJ et al. (2013) Prenatal stress induces schizophrenia-like alterations of serotonin 2A and metabotropic glutamate 2 receptors in the adult offspring: role of maternal immune system. The Journal of Neuroscience 33, 10881098.Google Scholar
Hong Lee, S, Byrne, EM, Hultman, CM, Kahler, A, Vinkhuyzen, AAE, Ripke, S, et al. (2015) New data and an old puzzle: the negative association between schizophrenia and rheumatoid arthritis. International Journal of Epidemiology 44, 17061721Google Scholar
Jones, HJ, Stergiakouli, E, Tansey, KE, Hubbard, L, Heron, J, Cannon, M et al. (2016) Phenotypic manifestation of genetic risk for schizophrenia during adolescence in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 221228.Google Scholar
Keers, R, Coleman, JRI, Lester, KJ, Roberts, S, Breen, G, Thastum, M et al. (2016) A genome-wide test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis reveals a genetic predictor of differential response to psychological treatments for child anxiety disorders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 85, 146158.Google Scholar
Kelleher, I, Connor, D, Clarke, MC, Devlin, N, Harley, M and Cannon, M (2012) Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychological Medicine 42, 18571863.Google Scholar
Kelley, ME, Wan, CR, Broussard, B, Crisafio, A, Cristofaro, S, Johnson, S et al. (2016) Marijuana use in the immediate 5-year premorbid period is associated with increased risk of onset of schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia Research 171, 6267.Google Scholar
Kennedy, BP, Kawachi, I, Glass, R and Prothrow-Stith, D (1998) Income distribution, socioeconomic status, and self rated health in the United States: multilevel analysis. BMJ 317, 917921.Google Scholar
Kety, SS, Wender, PH, Jacobsen, B, Ingraham, LJ, Jansson, L, Faber, B et al. (1994) Mental illness in the biological and adoptive relatives of schizophrenic adoptees. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 442455.Google Scholar
Khashan, AS, Abel, KM, Mcnamee, R, Pedersen, MG, Webb, RT, Baker, PN et al. (2008) Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to severe adverse life events. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 146152.Google Scholar
Kooijman, MN, Kruithof, CJ, van Duijn, CM, Duijts, L, Franco, OH, van IJzendoorn, MH et al. (2016) The generation R study: design and cohort update 2017. European Journal of Epidemiology 31, 12431264.Google Scholar
Korver, N, Quee, PJ, Boos, HBM, Simons, CJP and de Haan, L (2012) Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP), a multi site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene-environment interaction: objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 21, 205221.Google Scholar
Li, H, Lu, Q, Xiao, E, Li, Q, He, Z and Mei, X (2014) Methamphetamine enhances the development of schizophrenia in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59, 107113.Google Scholar
MacKenzie, LE, Abidi, S, Fisher, HL, Propper, L, Bagnell, A, Morash-Conway, J et al. (2016) Stimulant medication and psychotic symptoms in offspring of parents with mental illness. Pediatrics 137, e20152486.Google Scholar
Mäki, P, Riekki, T, Miettunen, J, Isohanni, M, Jones, PB, Murray, GK et al. (2010) Schizophrenia in the offspring of antenatally depressed mothers in the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort: relationship to family history of psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 7077.Google Scholar
Malaspina, D, Corcoran, C, Kleinhaus, KR, Perrin, MC, Fennig, S, Nahon, D et al. (2008) Acute maternal stress in pregnancy and schizophrenia in offspring: a cohort prospective study. BMC Psychiatry 8, 19.Google Scholar
Marconi, A, Di Forti, M, Lewis, C, Murray, RM and Vassos, E (2016) Meta-analysis of the association between the level of cannabis use and risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 12621269.Google Scholar
Marshall, C, Howrigan, D, Merico, D, Thiruvahindrapuram, B, Wu, WGreer, D, et al. (2016) Contribution of novel CNVs to schizophrenia from a genome-wide study of 41321 subjects. Nature Genetics 49, 2738.Google Scholar
Martin, J, Tilling, K, Hubbard, L, Stergiakouli, E, Thapar, A, Davey Smith, G et al. (2016) Association of genetic risk for schizophrenia with nonparticipation over time in a population-based cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology 183, 11491158.Google Scholar
McGrath, J (1999) Hypothesis: is low prenatal vitamin D a risk-modifying factor for schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research 40, 173177.Google Scholar
McGrath, J, Saari, K, Hakko, H, Jokelainen, J, Jones, P, Järvelin, MR et al. (2004) Vitamin D supplementation during the first year of life and risk of schizophrenia: a Finnish birth cohort study. Schizophrenia Research 67, 237245.Google Scholar
McGrath, JJ, Alati, R, Clavarino, A, Williams, GM, Bor, W, Najman, JM et al. (2016) Age at first tobacco use and risk of subsequent psychosis-related outcomes: a birth cohort study. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 50, 577583.Google Scholar
McGrath, JJ, Burne, TH, Féron, F, MacKay-Sim, A and Eyles, DW (2010a) Developmental vitamin D deficiency and risk of schizophrenia: a 10-year update. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 10731078.Google Scholar
McGrath, JJ, Eyles, DW, Pedersen, CB, Anderson, C, Ko, P, Burne, TH et al. (2010b) Neonatal vitamin D status and risk of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 889894.Google Scholar
McKetin, R, Gardner, J, Baker, AL, Dawe, S, Ali, R, Voce, A et al. (2016) Correlates of transient versus persistent psychotic symptoms among dependent methamphetamine users. Psychiatry Research 238, 166171.Google Scholar
Mednick, SA, Machon, RA, Huttunen, MO and Bonett, D (1988) Adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to an influenza epidemic. Archives of General Psychiatry 45, 189192.Google Scholar
Ménard, C, Pfau, ML, Hodes, GE and Russo, SJ (2017) Immune and neuroendocrine mechanisms of stress vulnerability and resilience. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews 42, 6280.Google Scholar
Miller, BJ, Culpepper, N, Rapaport, MH and Buckley, P (2013) Prenatal inflammation and neurodevelopment in schizophrenia: a review of human studies. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 42, 92100.Google Scholar
Moore, THM, Zammit, S, Lingford-hughes, A, Barnes, TRE, Jones, PB, Burke, M et al. (2007) Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. The Lancet 370, 319328.Google Scholar
Morgan, CJA, Freeman, TP, Powell, J and Curran, HV (2016) AKT1 genotype moderates the acute psychotomimetic effects of naturalistically smoked cannabis in young cannabis smokers. Translational Psychiatry 6, e738.Google Scholar
Mortensen, PB, Pedersen, CB, Westergaard, T, Wohlfahrt, J, Ewald, H, Mors, O et al. (1999) Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. The New England Journal of Medicine 340, 603608.Google Scholar
Newbury, J, Arseneault, L, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Odgers, CL and Fisher, HL (2016) Why are children in urban neighborhoods at increased risk for psychotic symptoms? Findings from a UK longitudinal cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 372383.Google Scholar
Ng, MYM, Levinson, DF, Faraone, SV, Suarez, BK, DeLisi, LE, Arinami, T et al. (2009) Meta-analysis of 32 genome-wide linkage studies of schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 774785.Google Scholar
Niarchou, M, Zammit, S, Van Goozen, SHM, Thapar, A, Tierling, HM, Owen, MJ et al. (2014) Psychopathology and cognition in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry 204, 4654.Google Scholar
Nielsen, PR, Meyer, U and Mortensen, PB (2016) Individual and combined effects of maternal anemia and prenatal infection on risk for schizophrenia in offspring. Schizophrenia Research 172, 3540.Google Scholar
Nosarti, C, Reichenberg, A and Murray, RM (2012) Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, E1E8.Google Scholar
O'Donoghue, B, Lyne, J, Madigan, K, Lane, A, Turner, N, O'Callaghan, E et al. (2015) Environmental factors and the age at onset in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 168, 106112.Google Scholar
Orlovska, S, Pedersen, MS, Benros, ME, Mortensen, PB, Agerbo, E and Nordentoft, M (2014) Head injury as risk factor for psychiatric disorders: a nationwide register-based follow-up study of 113906 persons with head injury. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 463469.Google Scholar
Owen, MJ and Doherty, JL (2016) What can we learn from the high rates of schizophrenia in people with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome? World Psychiatry 15, 2223.Google Scholar
Padmanabhan, JL, Shah, JL, Tandon, N and Keshavan, MS (2017) The ‘polyenviromic risk score’: aggregating environmental risk factors predicts conversion to psychosis in familial high-risk subjects. Schizophrenia Research 181, 1722.Google Scholar
Pedersen, CB and Mortensen, PB (2001) Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 10391046.Google Scholar
Perala, J, Suvisaari, J, Saarni, SI, Kuoppasalmi, K, Isometsa, E, Pirkola, S et al. (2007) Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general population. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 1928.Google Scholar
Pickrell, JK, Berisa, T, Liu, JZ, Ségurel, L, Tung, JY and Hinds, DA (2016) Detection and interpretation of shared genetic influences on 42 human traits. Nature Genetics 48, 709717.Google Scholar
Plomin, R, DeFries, JC and Loehlin, JC (1977) Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological bulletin 84, 309322.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, G, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Ambler, A, Keefe, RSE et al. (2010) Etiological and clinical features of childhood psychotic symptoms: results from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 328338.Google Scholar
Polderman, TJC, Benyamin, B, de Leeuw, CA, Sullivan, PF, van Bochoven, A, Visscher, PM et al. (2015) Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics 47, 702709.Google Scholar
Poulton, R, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Cannon, M, Murray, RM and Harrington, H (2000) Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 10531058.Google Scholar
Purcell, SM, Moran, JL, Fromer, M, Ruderfer, D, Solovieff, N, Roussos, P et al. (2014) A polygenic burden of rare disruptive mutations in schizophrenia. Nature 506, 185190.Google Scholar
Ramsay, H, Kelleher, I, Flannery, P, Clarke, MC, Lynch, F, Harley, M et al. (2013) Relationship between the COMT-Val158Met and BDNF-Val66Met polymorphisms, childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in an adolescent general population sample. PLOS ONE 8, e79741.Google Scholar
Reininghaus, U, Dutta, R, Dazzan, P, Doody, GA, Fearon, P, Lappin, J et al. (2015) Mortality in schizophrenia and other psychoses: a 10-year follow-up of the ÆSOP first-episode cohort. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 664673.Google Scholar
Riglin, L, Collishaw, S, Richards, A, Thapar, AK, Maughan, B, O'Donovan, MC et al. (2016) Schizophrenia risk alleles and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: a population-based cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry 4, 5762.Google Scholar
Sanders, AR, Duan, J, Levinson, DF, Shi, J, He, D, Hou, C et al. (2008) No significant association of 14 candidate genes to schizophrenia in a large European-ancestry sample: implications for psychiatric genetics. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 497506.Google Scholar
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2014) Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 511, 421427.Google Scholar
Schreier, A, Wolke, D, Thomas, K, Horwood, J, Hollis, C, Gunnell, D et al. (2009) Prospective study of peer victimization in childhood and psychotic symptoms in a nonclinical population at age 12 years. Archives of General Psychiatry 66, 527536.Google Scholar
Sekar, A, Bialas, AR, de Rivera, H, Davis, A, Hammond, TR, Kamitaki, N et al. (2016) Schizophrenia risk from complex variation of complement component 4. Nature 530, 177183.Google Scholar
Sideli, L, Fisher, HL, Murray, RM, Sallis, H, Russo, M, Stilo, SA et al. (2015) Interaction between cannabis consumption and childhood abuse in psychotic disorders: preliminary findings on the role of different patterns of cannabis use. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 18.Google Scholar
Singh, T, Kurki, MI, Curtis, D, Purcell, SM, Crooks, L, McRae, J et al. (2016) Rare loss-of-function variants in SETD1A are associated with schizophrenia and developmental disorders. Nature Neuroscience 19, 571577.Google Scholar
Smith, K (2011) Trillion-dollar brain drain. Nature 478, 1515.Google Scholar
Stefanis, NC, Dragovic, M, Power, BD, Jablensky, A, Castle, D and Morgan, VA (2013) Age at initiation of cannabis use predicts age at onset of psychosis: the 7-to 8-year trend. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 251254.Google Scholar
Stefanis, NC, Henquet, C, Avramopoulos, D, Smyrnis, N, Evdokimidis, I, Myin-Germeys, I et al. (2007) COMT valmet moderation of stress-induced psychosis. Psychological Medicine 37, 16511656.Google Scholar
Stepniak, B, Papiol, S, Hammer, C, Ramin, A, Everts, S, Hennig, L et al. (2014) Accumulated environmental risk determining age at schizophrenia onset: a deep phenotyping-based study. The Lancet Psychiatry 1, 444453.Google Scholar
Sullivan, PF, Agrawal, A, Bulik, CM, Andreassen, OA, Borglum, AD, Breen, G et al. (2017) Psychiatric genomics: an update and an agenda. American Journal of Psychiatry, doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17030283.Google Scholar
Sullivan, PF, Kendler, KS and Neale, MC (2003) Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 11871192.Google Scholar
Susser, ES and Lin, SP (1992) Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944–1945. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 983988.Google Scholar
Taylor, A, Jones, H, Sallis, H, Eusden, J, Stergiakouli, E, Davies, N et al. (2017) The molecular genetics of participation in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. bioRxiv, doi: 10.1101/206698.Google Scholar
Taylor, PJ (2007) The unreliability of high human heritability estimates and small shared effects of growing up in the same family. Biological Theory 2, 387397.Google Scholar
Thorup, AAE, Jepsen, JR, Ellersgaard, DV, Burton, BK, Christiani, CJ, Hemager, N et al. (2015) The Danish High Risk and Resilience Study – VIA 7 – a cohort study of 520 7-year-old children born of parents diagnosed with either schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or neither of these two mental disorders. BMC Psychiatry 15, 233.Google Scholar
Tienari, P, Ynne, LCW, Sorri, A and Lahti, I (2004) Genotype-environment interaction in schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: long-term follow-up study of Finnish adoptees. British Journal of Psychiatry 184, 216222.Google Scholar
Torrey, EF, Bartko, JJ and Yolken, RH (2012) Toxoplasma gondii and other risk factors for schizophrenia: an update. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 642647.Google Scholar
Tottenham, N (2013) Risk and developmental heterogeneity in previously institutionalized children. Journal of Adolescent Health 51, S29S33.Google Scholar
Trotta, A, Di Forti, M, Iyegbe, C, Green, P, Dazzan, P, Mondelli, V et al. (2015) Familial risk and childhood adversity interplay in the onset of psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry Open 1, 613.Google Scholar
Trotta, A, Di Forti, M, Mondelli, V, Dazzan, P, Pariante, C, David, A et al. (2013) Prevalence of bullying victimisation amongst first-episode psychosis patients and unaffected controls. Schizophrenia Research 150, 169175.Google Scholar
Trotta, A, Iyegbe, C, Di Forti, M, Sham, PC, Campbell, DD, Cherny, SS et al. (2016) Interplay between schizophrenia polygenic risk score and childhood adversity in first-presentation psychotic disorder: a pilot study. PLOS ONE 11, e0163319.Google Scholar
Uher, R, Cumby, J, Mackenzie, LE, Morash-conway, J, Glover, JM, Aylott, A et al. (2014) A familial risk enriched cohort as a platform for testing early interventions to prevent severe mental illness. BMC Psychiatry 14, 344.Google Scholar
Uher, R and Zwicker, A (2017) Etiology in psychiatry: embracing the reality of poly-gene-environmental causation of mental illness. World Psychiatry 16, 121129.Google Scholar
Van Den Bossche, MJ, Strazisar, M, Cammaerts, S, Liekens, AM, Vandeweyer, G, Depreeuw, V et al. (2013) Identification of rare copy number variants in high burden schizophrenia families. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 162, 273282.Google Scholar
van Gastel, WA, MacCabe, JH, Schubart, CD, Vreeker, A, Tempelaar, W, Kahn, RS et al. (2013) Cigarette smoking and cannabis use are equally strongly associated with psychotic-like experiences: a cross-sectional study in 1929 young adults. Psychological Medicine 43, 23932401.Google Scholar
Van Nierop, M, Janssens, M, Bruggeman, R, Cahn, W, De Haan, L, Kahn, RS et al. (2013) Evidence that transition from health to psychotic disorder can be traced to semi-ubiquitous environmental effects operating against background genetic risk. PLOS ONE 8, e76690.Google Scholar
Van Os, J, Hanssen, M, Bak, M, Bijl, RV and Vollebergh, W (2003) Do urbanicity and familial liability coparticipate in causing psychosis? American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 477482.Google Scholar
Van Os, J, Pedersen, CB and Mortensen, PB (2004) Confirmation of synergy between urbanicity and familial liability in the causation of psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 23122314.Google Scholar
van Os, J and Selten, J-P (1998) Prenatal exposure to maternal stress and subsequent schizophrenia. The May 1940 invasion of The Netherlands. British Journal of Psychiatry 172, 324326.Google Scholar
van Winkel, R (2011) Family-based analysis of genetic variation underlying psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis: sibling analysis and proband follow-up. Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 148157.Google Scholar
van Winkel, R, GROUP investigators (2015) Further evidence that cannabis moderates familial correlation of psychosis-related experiences. PLOS ONE 10, e0137625.Google Scholar
Varese, F, Smeets, F, Drukker, M, Lieverse, R, Lataster, T, Viechtbauer, W et al. (2012) Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective-and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 661671.Google Scholar
Vassos, E, Di Forti, M, Coleman, J, Iyegbe, C, Prata, D, Euesden, J, et al. (2017) An examination of polygenic score risk prediction in individuals with first episode psychosis. Biological Psychiatry 81, 470477.Google Scholar
Vassos, E, Pedersen, CB, Murray, RM, Collier, DA and Lewis, CM (2012) Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 11181123.Google Scholar
Vaucher, J, Keating, BJ, Lasserre, AM, Gan, W, Lyall, D, Ward, J et al. (2017) Cannabis use and risk of schizophrenia: a Mendelian randomization study. Molecular Psychiatry, 16.Google Scholar
Verweij, KJH, Abdellaoui, A, Nivard, MG, Sainz Cort, A, Ligthart, L, Draisma, HHM et al. (2017) Short communication: genetic association between schizophrenia and cannabis use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 171, 117121.Google Scholar
Vinkers, CH, Van Gastel, WA, Schubart, CD, Van Eijk, KR, Luykx, JJ, Van Winkel, R et al. (2013) The effect of childhood maltreatment and cannabis use on adult psychotic symptoms is modified by the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Schizophrenia Research 150, 303311.Google Scholar
Walsh, C, MacMillan, H and Jamieson, E (2002) The relationship between parental psychiatric disorder and child physical and sexual abuse: findings from the Ontario health supplement. Child Abuse and Neglect 26, 1122.Google Scholar
Walsh, T, McClellan, JM, McCarthy, SE, Addington, AM, Pierce, SB, Cooper, GM et al. (2008) Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science 320, 539543.Google Scholar
Weber-Stadlbauer, U, Richetto, J, Labouesse, MA, Bohacek, J, Mansuy, IM and Meyer, U (2017) Transgenerational transmission and modification of pathological traits induced by prenatal immune activation. Molecular Psychiatry 22, 102112.Google Scholar
Wolke, D, Lereya, ST, Fisher, HL, Lewis, G and Zammit, S (2014) Bullying in elementary school and psychotic experiences at 18 years: a longitudinal, population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine 44, 21992211.Google Scholar
Zammit, S, Owen, MJ, Evans, J, Heron, J and Lewis, G (2011) Cannabis, COMT and psychotic experiences. British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 380385.Google Scholar
Zavos, HMS, Freeman, D, Haworth, CMA, McGuire, P, Plomin, R, Cardno, AG et al. (2014) Consistent etiology of severe, frequent psychotic experiences and milder, less frequent manifestations. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 10491057.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. The polygenic risk for schizophrenia. The proportion of variance explained by polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia produced using the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) sample from 2011 (The Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium 2011) and 2014 (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014) as the discovery samples, with p value thresholds (PT) at 5 × 10−8 (genome-wide significance) and 0.05. The proportion of variance explained increases with both sample size and with the inclusion of more, non-significantly associated variants. This suggests that schizophrenia is highly polygenic, and many more genetic loci contribute to disease risk than have been captured by the most recent GWAS.

Figure 1

Table 1. Terminology of gene–environment interplay

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Environmental factors associated with psychosis. The number of plus signs denotes the strength of evidence for the association: +++ indicates consistent evidence from multiple large-scale studies or a meta-analysis; ++ indicates evidence from multiple smaller studies or a strong association in a high-quality study; + indicates evidence from a single study, multiple small/low-quality studies, or few studies with conflicting reports. The reference provided for each exposure reflects a meta-analysis (where available) or the largest study available. The list is limited to environmental exposures and excludes risk factors that reflect conditions of the individual (e.g. inflammation).

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Gene-environment correlations and psychosis. The three panels illustrate passive, evocative and active gene-environment correlations (rGE), base on specific examples that are relevant to the etiology of psychosis. The same examples are described and referenced in the text section Gene-Environment Correlation.

Figure 4

Table 2. Molecular gene–environment interactions in psychosis