Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T05:59:24.231Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changes in cocaine consumption are associated with fluctuations in self-reported impulsivity and gambling decision-making

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 June 2015

L. M. Hulka*
Affiliation:
Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland
M. Vonmoos*
Affiliation:
Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland
K. H. Preller
Affiliation:
Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland
M. R. Baumgartner
Affiliation:
Center of Forensic Hairanalytics, Institute of Forensic Medicine, University of Zurich, Switzerland
E. Seifritz
Affiliation:
Director of the Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich, and Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, Switzerland
A. Gamma
Affiliation:
Division of ADHD Research, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland
B. B. Quednow
Affiliation:
Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich, and Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Address for correspondence: L. M. Hulka, PhD, Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Zurich, Lenggstrasse 31, CH-8032 Zurich, Switzerland. (Email: lea.hulka@bli.uzh.ch) [L.M.H.] (Email: matthias.vonmoos@bli.uzh.ch) [M.V.]
*Address for correspondence: L. M. Hulka, PhD, Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Zurich, Lenggstrasse 31, CH-8032 Zurich, Switzerland. (Email: lea.hulka@bli.uzh.ch) [L.M.H.] (Email: matthias.vonmoos@bli.uzh.ch) [M.V.]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

In cross-sectional studies, cocaine users generally display elevated levels of self-reported and cognitive impulsivity. To what extent these impairments are stable v. variable markers of cocaine use disorder, and, thus, are pre-existing or drug-induced, has not yet been systematically investigated.

Method.

We conducted a longitudinal study with cocaine users who changed or maintained their consumption intensity, measuring self-reported impulsivity with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), and cognitive impulsivity with the Rapid Visual Processing task (RVP), Iowa Gambling task (IGT), and Delay Discounting task (DD) at baseline and at 1-year follow-up. We assessed 48 psychostimulant-naive controls and 19 cocaine users with decreased, 19 users with increased, and 19 users with unchanged cocaine intake after 1 year as confirmed by hair analysis.

Results.

Results of linear multilevel modelling showed significant group × time interactions for the BIS-11 total score and the IGT total card ratio. Increasers showed a trend for elevated scores, whereas decreasers exhibited reduced self-reported impulsivity scores within 1 year. Surprisingly, increasers’ IGT performance was improved after 1 year, whereas decreasers’ performance deteriorated. By contrast, neither RVP response bias B″ nor DD total score showed substantial group × time interactions. Importantly, BIS-11 and DD revealed strong test–retest reliabilities.

Conclusion.

Self-reported impulsivity (BIS-11) and decision-making impulsivity (IGT) covary with changing cocaine use, whereas response bias and delay discounting remain largely unaffected. Thus, self-reported impulsivity and gambling decision-making were strongly state-dependent in a stimulant-using population and may be suitable to monitor treatment success, whereas delay of gratification was confirmed as a potential endophenotype of stimulant addiction.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Introduction

Cocaine addiction is a debilitating chronically relapsing disorder that is characterized by persistent and compulsive drug-seeking despite harmful consequences (APA, 1994). Cocaine is the most commonly used stimulant drug in Europe and the United States (EMCDDA, 2014; UNODC, 2014) and current lifetime prevalence rates in the populations are estimated at 4.2% in Europe and 14.3% in the United States (EMCDDA, 2014; NSDUH, 2014). Despite the high addiction liability of cocaine (Nutt et al. Reference Nutt, King, Saulsbury and Blakemore2007), only a relatively small fraction of users (5–6%) made the transition from controlled drug use to drug dependence within the first year of use, whereas 15–16% developed dependency in the long term (Wagner & Anthony, Reference Wagner and Anthony2002).

Converging evidence points to a complex interplay of inherited genetic predispositions and environmental, social, and neurobiological factors contributing to the vulnerability to develop an addiction (Kreek et al. Reference Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman and LaForge2005; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2007). It has been postulated that chronic drug use is associated with neuroadaptations in fronto-limbic brain circuits mediating reward, motivation, and memory processes, presumably resulting in behavioural inflexibility such as disrupted self-control and compulsive drug use (Goldstein & Volkow, Reference Goldstein and Volkow2011). However, the emerging concept of addiction endophenotypes also highlights the role of pre-existing psychological, cognitive, and neurobiological factors that may render an individual more susceptible to initiate drug use and develop an addiction (Ersche et al. Reference Ersche, Turton, Chamberlain, Muller, Bullmore and Robbins2012, Reference Ersche, Jones, Williams, Smith, Bullmore and Robbins2013; Mahoney & Olmstead, Reference Mahoney and Olmstead2013). Important and somewhat conceptually overlapping key factors implicated in the initiation and maintenance of drug use are maladaptive decision-making, increased impulsivity propensities, and implicit cognitive processes such as attentional bias and implicit memory associations (Field et al. Reference Field, Schoenmakers and Wiers2008; Lucantonio et al. Reference Lucantonio, Stalnaker, Shaham, Niv and Schoenbaum2012; Marhe et al. Reference Marhe, Waters, van de Wetering and Franken2013; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Verdejo-Garcia, Goudriann, Royers, Dom and Vanderplasschen2014; Wiers & Stacy, Reference Wiers and Stacy2006). Overall, drug users attribute excessive attention to drug-related stimuli, rate them more positively than non-users, and exhibit reduced inhibitory control and increased impulsivity, leading to harmful compulsive drug use despite the occurrence of negative consequences. To what extent impaired decision-making and increased impulsivity are drug-induced, pre-existing vulnerability factors, or a combination of both is, however, not entirely clear so far.

Decision-making refers to the ability to select an optimal course of action from multiple options, requiring ongoing updating and integrating of the value of present and potential actions (Fellows, Reference Fellows2004; Lucantonio et al. Reference Lucantonio, Stalnaker, Shaham, Niv and Schoenbaum2012). Prior studies have consistently shown that dependent cocaine users exhibit disadvantageous decision-making strategies in tasks such as the Iowa Gambling task (IGT) and preferences for smaller immediate over larger but delayed rewards as measured with the Delay Discounting task (DD; Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Dolan and Hindes2002; Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Benbrook, Funderburk, David, Cadet and Bolla2007; Hulka et al. Reference Hulka, Eisenegger, Preller, Vonmoos, Jenni, Bendrick, Baumgartner, Seifritz and Quednow2014; Kjome et al. Reference Kjome, Lane, Schmitz, Green, Ma, Prasla, Swann and Moeller2010). Impulsivity, a multi-faceted concept, is generally defined as a predisposed tendency toward rapid and unplanned reaction patterns without much foresight regarding possible consequences (Moeller et al. Reference Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz and Swann2001a ). Impulsive tendencies or trait impulsivity is generally assessed with self-report questionnaires, such as the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), which presumably focus on stable personality characteristics (Patton et al. Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Verdejo-Garcia, Goudriann, Royers, Dom and Vanderplasschen2014). Although self-report measures can provide valuable information, they can be limited if the subject lacks insight or answers in a socially desirable manner (Moeller et al. Reference Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz and Swann2001a ). Therefore, using an experimental approach to complement self-report measures of impulsivity with behavioural performance measures is essential. Behavioural or neurocognitive impulsivity has been proposed to include the two main components of impulsive action representing motor impulsivity and impulsive choice reflecting more cognitive parts of impulsivity (Winstanley et al. Reference Winstanley, Olausson, Taylor and Jentsch2010). Impulsive action is generally measured with tasks assessing the ability to inhibit motor responses [e.g. Stop-Signal, Stroop, Go/No-go, and sustained attention tasks such as the Rapid Visual Processing (RVP)], while impulsive choice is either assessed with tasks quantifying the ability to tolerate delay of gratification such as the DD or with tasks measuring responses to reward and loss contingencies such as assessed in the IGT (Winstanley et al. Reference Winstanley, Olausson, Taylor and Jentsch2010; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Verdejo-Garcia, Goudriann, Royers, Dom and Vanderplasschen2014). It has been suggested that impulsive action might be more associated with the development of cocaine addiction, while impulsive choice may contribute specifically to relapse of cocaine use (Winstanley et al. Reference Winstanley, Olausson, Taylor and Jentsch2010). However, this hypothesis has been recently challenged as we did not find elevated motor impulsivity in cocaine dependence (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ), while especially DD – a prototype of impulsive choice – has been proposed as a critical risk factor for the development of drug addiction as well (MacKillop, Reference MacKillop2013).

Elevated trait impulsivity has consistently been reported for dependent and recreational stimulant users (Reske et al. Reference Reske, Delis and Paulus2011; Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ), whereas results on behavioural impulsivity are somewhat mixed: some studies found dependent (Perry & Carroll Reference Perry and Carroll2008; Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Lawrence and Clark2008; Ersche et al. Reference Ersche, Turton, Pradhan, Bullmore and Robbins2010) and recreational cocaine users (CU) (Colzato et al. Reference Colzato, van den Wildenberg and Hommel2007) to be impaired in Stop-Signal or Go/No-go tasks, whereas we recently concluded from data of a large sample of relatively pure CU that neither dependent nor recreational cocaine use was associated with impairment in the Stop-Signal task (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ). However, the inconsistent results regarding performance in Stop-Signal tasks may be explained by different task designs and parameter calculation procedures as well as different exclusion criteria of polytoxic drug use and psychiatric comorbidities (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ).

Importantly, impaired decision-making and social cognition in CU have been associated with worse self-reported real-life functioning (Cunha et al. Reference Cunha, Bechara, de Andrade and Nicastri2011; Preller et al. Reference Preller, Hulka, Vonmoos, Jenni, Baumgartner, Seifritz, Dziobek and Quednow2014) and more frequent drug relapse and treatment attrition (Bechara, Reference Bechara2003; Aharonovich et al. Reference Aharonovich, Hasin, Brooks, Liu, Bisaga and Nunes2006; Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Albein-Urios, Martinez-Gonzalez, Civit, de la Torre and Lozano2014). Moreover, elevated levels of impulsivity have been associated with an increased risk to initiate and continue drug use and a greater likelihood of relapse (for review see Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Verdejo-Garcia, Goudriann, Royers, Dom and Vanderplasschen2014). Considering the impact of impaired decision-making and elevated impulsivity on real-life functioning and treatment outcome, it is important to better understand if the different facets of impulsivity are stable antecedent vulnerability factors – predisposing individuals to develop and maintain cocaine addiction – or rather drug-induced consequences. Moreover, it is not clear whether these behavioural deviances are reversible when drug use is reduced or completely ceased. Recently, we reported that CU, who had substantially increased cocaine use over the course of 1 year, exhibited worse cognitive performance compared to baseline, whereas those who decreased or ceased their cocaine use showed substantially improved cognitive performance, suggesting that cognitive impairment is partially cocaine-induced but also reversible (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Minder, Baumgartner and Quednow2014).

The primary goal of the present investigation was, thus, to determine which specific decision-making and impulsivity components are stable (addiction endophenotypes) v. variable (drug-induced) markers of cocaine use disorder over the time-course of one year. For this purpose, we conducted a longitudinal study assessing trait impulsivity with the BIS-11, impulsive action with the RVP response bias B″, and impulsive choice with the main IGT and DD parameters, at baseline and after 1 year. CU of the Zurich Cocaine Cognition Study (ZuCo2St) were categorized as decreasers, increasers, or equal users after the 1-year follow-up based on a toxicological quantification of cocaine levels in hair samples. With regard to the BIS-11 and DD, we expected that scores would remain stable over the two assessments because the BIS-11 is regarded as a trait measure and also discounting preferences appear to be relatively stable over time (for review see MacKillop, Reference MacKillop2013). For the RVP B″ we expected stable results as none of the few existing previous studies with CU displayed a substantial cocaine-associated effect on this parameter (Ersche et al. Reference Ersche, Barnes, Jones, Morein-Zamir, Robbins and Bullmore2011; Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ). By contrast, we expected that the IGT performance would improve with decreased cocaine use and worsen with increased cocaine use as prior studies reported dose-dependent relationships between cocaine use and IGT performance (Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Benbrook, Funderburk, David, Cadet and Bolla2007).

Materials and method

Participants

The eligible baseline sample of the ZuCo2St comprised 234 participants [138 CU, 96 healthy psychostimulant-naive controls (HC)] (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Baumgartner, Stohler, Bolla and Quednow2013a ; Hulka et al. Reference Hulka, Eisenegger, Preller, Vonmoos, Jenni, Bendrick, Baumgartner, Seifritz and Quednow2014; Preller et al. Reference Preller, Hulka, Vonmoos, Jenni, Baumgartner, Seifritz, Dziobek and Quednow2014). One hundred and thirty-two subjects [79 CU (57.3% of baseline), 53 HC (55.2%)] participated in the 1-year-follow-up. Hereof, 57 CU and 48 matched HC could be included in the longitudinal analysis (for details see Supplementary Method S1).

At baseline, exclusion criteria for all participants were clinically significant somatic diseases, neurological disorders, head injuries, family history of schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive disorder, or bipolar disorder, or any medication affecting the CNS. Further exclusion criteria for CU were opioid use, polytoxic drug use patterns, or DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders (except for cocaine, nicotine, and alcohol abuse/dependence, former affective disorders, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder). At baseline, all CU met the following inclusion criteria: cocaine as primary drug of choice, cocaine use >0.5 g/month, and abstinence duration of <6 months. Exclusion criteria for HC were DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders (excluding nicotine dependence) and regular illegal drug use (>15 lifetime occasions, except occasional cannabis use).

Participants were asked to abstain from illegal substances for >72 h and from alcohol for >24 h. Compliance was controlled by urine screens (semi-quantitative enzyme multiplied immunoassay method) and 6-month hair toxicology (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Baumgartner, Stohler, Bolla and Quednow2013a ). The study was approved by the Cantonal Ethics Committee of Zurich. All participants provided written informed-consent statements and were compensated for their participation.

Cocaine user groups

Cocaine user groups were determined based on a combination of absolute (±0.5 ng/mg cocaine) (Bush Reference Bush2008; Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Kronstrand and Kintz2012) and relative (>10% increase/decrease in the robust hair toxicology parameter cocainetotal) (Hoelzle et al. Reference Hoelzle, Scheufler, Uhl, Sachs and Thieme2008) changes in cocaine concentration in the hair samples between baseline and follow-up. CU who met both criteria were categorized as increasers or decreasers accordingly, whereas CU who met none or only one criterion were classified as equal users. Accordingly, CU were divided into three equally sized groups: Whereas 19 CU with increased cocaine use and 19 CU with decreased cocaine use met the according change criteria, a third group of 19 CU displayed equal cocaine use and met neither the cut-off-criteria for increased nor decreased cocaine use [increaser: mean±s.d. + 30.4 ± 61.9 ng/mg (+297%), range +0.5 to +268.5 ng/mg (+20% to +5374%); decreaser: −10.6 ± 26.7 ng/mg (−72%), −116.9 to −0.6 ng/mg (−100% to −12%); equal user: −0.1 ± 0.5  ng/mg (−2%), −1.9 to +0.5 ng/mg (−100% to +720%)].

Procedure

The test procedure was similar in both test sessions. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID-I; APA, 1994) was conducted by trained psychologists. Drug use was assessed by means of a structured and standardized interview for psychotropic drug consumption (Quednow et al. Reference Quednow, Kuhn, Hoenig, Maier and Wagner2004). For matching reasons, pre-morbid verbal intelligence was measured with a German version of a multiple choice vocabulary intelligence test (MWT-B; Lehrl, Reference Lehrl1999). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms were assessed with the ADHD Self-Rating Scale (ADHD-SR; Roesler et al. Reference Roesler, Retz, Retz-Junginger, Thome, Supprian, Nissen, Stieglitz, Blocher, Hengesch and Trott2004), and symptoms of depression with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961). Smoking severity was assessed with the Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al. Reference Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker and Fagerstrom1991) and craving for cocaine with the Brief Cocaine Craving Questionnaire (CCQ; Sussner et al. Reference Sussner, Smelson, Rodrigues, Kline, Losonczy and Ziedonis2006). Smoking intensity and cocaine craving were assessed to control for their potential acute effects on test performance (Franken, Reference Franken2003). Trait impulsivity was assessed with the BIS-11 (Patton et al. Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995). The two components of cognitive impulsivity impulsive action and impulsive choice were measured by means of the response bias B’ (an analogue to the response bias beta from the signal detection theory; Green & Swets, Reference Green and Swets1966) drawn from the sustained attention task RVP of the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB; Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, McKay, Herrod, Robbins and Paykel1996) as well as the discounting rate described by parameter k from the DD task (Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999), and the IGT parameter total ratio (number of good cards – number of bad cards) (Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Dolan and Hindes2002), respectively. At follow-up, a parallel test version was used for the IGT. Detailed task descriptions for the RVP, IGT, and DD can be found in Supplementary Method S2.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses regarding demographic data and drug use patterns (Table 1) as well as baseline test scores were performed with SPSS Statistics 19.0 (IBM, Switzerland). Frequency data were analysed by means of Pearson's χ2 test and quantitative data by analyses of variance (ANOVA) or Student's t tests. All other analyses derived from the xtmixed command in Stata 12.1 for Macintosh (StataCorp. 2011). Regression modelling involved a series of linear multilevel models (also known as linear mixed, random-effects, or random-coefficient models) (Tabachnik & Fidell, Reference Tabachnik and Fidell2006; Gelman & Hill, Reference Gelman and Hill2007; Locascio & Atri, Reference Locascio and Atri2011).

Table 1. Demographic data and pattern of drug use

Values given are means and standard deviations.

Significant p values are shown in bold.

a ANOVA (all groups, with significant Sidak post-hoc test v. control group: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; v. cocaine increasers: †p < 0.05).

b χ² test (all groups/cocaine users only) for frequency data.

c ANOVA (cocaine users only, with significant Sidak post-hoc test v. cocaine increasers: †p < 0.05; †††p < 0.001).

d Average use during the last 6 months.

e Craving for cocaine was assessed by the CCQ.

f Last consumption is averaged only for persons who used the drug in the last 6 months.

g Urine toxicology (neg/pos) is based on cut-off value for cocaine = 150 ng/ml and for tetrahydrocannabinol = 50 ng/ml (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2008). The χ² test for cocaine includes only cocaine users, the χ² test for cannabis includes all groups.

h Hair samples were voluntary and data are missing for three controls.

i Parameters at follow-up refer to the 1-year period between t1 and t2.

j SCID-I diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria (APA, 1994).

k Self-reported maximum dose of cocaine within 24 h (in grams).

All dependent variables were divided by their respective standard deviations to aid in model interpretation. Predictors evaluated were: group (increasers, decreasers, equal users, controls), time (dummy variable with levels baseline and follow-up), group × time interaction, female sex, age (in 10-year steps), level of education (years), age of onset of cocaine use (in 5-year steps), verbal IQ (in steps of 5), amount of Swiss francs available in the last 12 months (ordinal variable with levels 0–15000, 15000–30000, 30000–60000, 60000–90000, 90000–120000, >120000), FTND score (divided by standard deviation), smoker (yes/no), ADHD-SR score (divided by standard deviation), adult ADHD (yes/no, DSM-IV criteria), BDI score (divided by standard deviation), BDI depression (yes/no, cut-off ≥18), CCQ score (divided by standard deviation), years of cocaine use, cumulative cocaine use in grams (transformed by natural logarithm because of violation of normality distribution), cocaine in hair samples (pg/mg), positive urine screen for cocaine (yes/no), and cocaine level in urine toxicology (ng/ml, log-transformed). Continuous predictors were centred on their grand means. In addition to some of the predictors (see above), the dependent variable DD k total was log-transformed as well (further details of the statistical analysis are given in Supplementary Method S3).

Fig. 1 shows the effects of group and time for the four main dependent variables, even if the main effects and/or interactions of group and time were not statistically significant in the final models that are reported in Table 2. In such cases, these effects were added to the final model and the figures were derived from these extended models.

Fig. 1. Predicted values for baseline (t1) and follow-up (t2) are shown. (a) Development of Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) total scores within 1 year. Control group data in raw values (predicted data are not available because this group was dropped from the regression model due to collinearity). Group  ×  time interaction (increaser × decreaser) p  <  0.01. *p <  0.05 time effect for cocaine decreasers. (b) Development of the Rapid Visual Processing task (RVP) response bias B″ within 1 year. (c) Development of delay discounting (DD) parameter k within 1 year. Control group data in raw values (predicted data are not available because this group was dropped from the regression model due to collinearity). (d) Development of the Iowa Gambling task (IGT) total ratio within 1 year. Group × time interaction (increaser × decreaser) p  <  0.05.

Table 2. Coefficients (standard errors) of final multilevel regression models with standardized dependent variables

RVP, Rapid Visual Processing task; BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; DD, delay discounting task; IGT, Iowa Gambling task; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; FTND, Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Significance levels: †p  <  0.10; *p <  0.05; **p  < 0 .01; ***p < 0 .001.

a All dependent variables divided by standard deviation.

b Increasers.

c Values at baseline (t1).

d No longer significant when excluding controls.

e Only trend-level significant when excluding controls.

The table shows all coefficients that are relevant for the final models. Principally, final models included only predictors at p < 0.10. However, in case of any significant interaction effects (group × time), the table additionally displays the single terms (group, time).

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics and drug use

The four groups did not significantly differ regarding age, sex distribution, verbal IQ, years of education, smoking status, and length of interval between the two study assessments (Table 1). Moreover, all three CU groups showed similar BDI (p Sidak post-hoc   >  0.97) and ADHD-SR (p Sidak post-hoc   >  0.99) sum scores, but exhibited distinctively higher values than HC (p BDI  =  0.08 – 0.006, p ADHD-SR  =  0.03 – 0.007).

At baseline, all three CU groups did not display significant differences in the self-reported cocaine use parameters and hair concentrations. However, the increaser and decreaser groups clearly featured a stronger current and cumulative cocaine use than the equal user group although the differences were not statistically significant. Furthermore and in accordance with the inclusion criteria, the data showed a clear domination of cocaine compared to other illegal drug use. At follow-up, increasers had a three- to four-fold higher cocaine concentration in the hair samples compared to the baseline value, whereas decreasers’ values dropped by about three quarters, while the equal users’ values remained relatively stable.

Impulsivity and decision-making scores

BIS-11

At baseline, a combined CU group (including increasers, decreasers, and equal users) differed significantly from the HC group in the total score (T 103  =  2.98, p  <  0.01) and all three subscales FI–FII (T 103  =  2.32 – 2.83, p  =  0.02 – 0.006) as previously shown in Vonmoos et al. (Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ). The most striking finding was the significant group × time interaction (p  <  0.01) in the BIS-11 total score with increasers showing a trend for elevated self-reported impulsivity over time (p  =  0.12), whereas decreasers displayed significantly reduced self-reported impulsivity within 1 year (p  <  0.05). Equal users’ (p =  0.86) and HCs’ total scores remained comparatively stable (Fig. 1a ; a p value for HC is not available because this group was dropped from the regression model due to collinearity). Table 2 shows that this pattern of significant group × time interactions (increaser v. decreaser) can also be seen in the BIS-11 primary subscales FI (attentional impulsiveness, p <  0.05) and FII (motor impulsiveness, p  < 0.01). Whereas none of the groups revealed significant time effects in attentional impulsiveness (p dec  =  0.17, p inc =  0.08, p equal =  0.28, p HC  =  0.48), decreasers showed a significant reduction in the self-reported motor impulsiveness (p dec <  0.01, p inc  =  0.13, p equal  =  0.92, p HC  =  0.18). For the BIS-11 subscale FIII (non-planning impulsiveness) no significant group × time interaction (increaser v. decreaser) was found (p  =  0.15).

Additionally, BIS-11 total, attentional, and motor impulsiveness scores correlated strongly with ADHD-SR sum scores (all p  <  0.002). Furthermore, BIS-11 total, attentional, and non-planning scores were positively associated with BDI depression (all p  <  0.05). BIS-11 total (p  <  0.05) and non-planning scores (p  <  0.001) positively covaried with craving scores, and a later age of cocaine onset was linked to reduced motor impulsiveness (p  <  0.01). However, this latter effect was no longer significant when controls were excluded from the analysis.

RVP response bias B″

For B, no baseline differences between CU and HC occurred (T 103  =  − 1.04, p  =  0.30) (see also Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ) and in the longitudinal analysis there was no significant effect regarding group, time, or their interaction. The only significant finding was a weak positive association with duration of cocaine use (p < 0.05), which was no longer significant when excluding the control group from the analysis (Table 2, Fig. 1b ).

DD k total

At baseline, CU and HC strongly differed regarding k total (T 103  =  − 3.40, p  <  0.001), as shown before (Hulka et al. Reference Hulka, Eisenegger, Preller, Vonmoos, Jenni, Bendrick, Baumgartner, Seifritz and Quednow2014). Longitudinally, k total showed no statistically significant group and/or time effects. However, a 100% increase in the cumulative lifetime dose of cocaine was associated with roughly a 20% decline in scores of the ability to delay gratification – i.e. to forego immediate smaller in favour of delayed larger rewards. Moreover, a 100% raise in the FTND score was associated with a 30% decrease in scores for delayed gratification (Table 2, Fig. 1c ).

IGT total ratio

Baseline scores of the IGT total ratio did not differ between CU and HC (T 102  =  0.72, p  =  0.47). In the longitudinal analysis, a significant group × time interaction was found for the IGT total ratio (p  <  0.05, Table 2, Fig. 1d , for a detailed distribution of IGT scores for each group at t1 and t2 see Supplementary Fig. S1). Without any statistically significant main effect of group, decreasers’ scores fell during the 1-year-interval (p  =  0.11), whereas those of the increasers improved (p  =  0.09). Concretely, this means that decreasers chose significantly fewer favourable cards at follow-up. Equal users (p  =  0.34) and HC (p  =  0.54) total ratio scores remained fairly stable. Age (p  <  0.01), an ADHD diagnosis (p  <  0.10), and positive smoking status (p  <  0.01) were related to decreases in IGT performance of roughly 1/3 s.d.

Test–retest reliability

In three of the four main variables, HC displayed slightly higher test–retest reliability scores than the combined CU group (Table 3) but the differences were not significant (z  <  0.80, p  >  0.20). Moreover, the BIS-11 total score showed a remarkably strong test-retest reliability, whereas the response bias B’ featured the smallest but nonetheless significant test-retest reliability in HC and CU.

Table 3. One-year test–retest reliability between baseline and 1-year follow-up in controls and cocaine users

BIS, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; RVP, Rapid Visual Processing task; DD, delay discounting task; IGT, Iowa Gambling task.

Pearson's product-moment correlation.

Significance levels: *p  <  0.05, **p  <  0.01, ***p < 0 .001.

Discussion

The present longitudinal study investigated to what extent impaired decision-making and elevated impulsivity levels in CU may represent stable and pre-existing addiction endophenotypes v. variable state markers depending on the quantity of cocaine used. The most striking finding was that self-reported levels of impulsivity strongly covaried with changing cocaine use patterns. Accordingly, CU who substantially decreased their cocaine intake within one year reported significantly lower levels of impulsive behaviour, whereas CU who increased their consumption reported by trend higher levels of subjectively perceived impulsivity. By contrast, cognitive impulsivity measures capturing impulsive action (response bias B’) and impulsive choice (delay discounting) did not significantly differ with increasing or decreasing cocaine use over the course of one year. Contrary to our expectations, CU who decreased their cocaine intake during the 1-year interval chose fewer favourable cards in the IGT at the second study assessment, whereas CU who increased their consumption showed a slightly improved performance. Nonetheless, there seemed to be a linkage between worse decision-making at baseline and a subsequently increased cocaine use pattern suggesting that impaired decision-making might indeed be a risk factor for increasing cocaine use.

The results of the present study are somewhat surprising and call some existing assumptions into question. The BIS has been regarded to reflect a relatively stable trait component of subjectively experienced impulsivity levels (Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Verdejo-Garcia, Goudriann, Royers, Dom and Vanderplasschen2014). However, our results revealed that CU who increased their consumption over the time-course of 1 year perceived themselves by trend as more impulsive, whereas CU who decreased their use rated themselves as less impulsive. Although the BIS reliably differentiated CU from HC, and, thus, might overall reflect a trait component, it is not as stable in drug using populations as previously assumed. Therefore, the BIS may be useful to assess how subjectively perceived levels of impulsiveness in CU covary with drug consumption. Moreover, Littlefield et al. (Reference Littlefield, Sher and Wood2009) have also provided evidence that impulsivity measures assessed in questionnaires change over time. Previously, we have already shown that BIS ratings were elevated in dependent CU using substantially higher amounts of cocaine than in recreational CU with a much less pronounced cocaine use pattern (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ). It is noteworthy that differences (Δ t2–t1) in self-reported impulsivity between the two testing sessions did not significantly correlate with changes in cognitive impulsivity tasks (CU: r  <  0.30, p  >  0.05; HC: r  <  −0.30, p  >  0.05; total sample: r  <  −0.15, p  >  0.25). This finding is in line with prior studies conducted in our laboratory where trait impulsivity was only weakly correlated with behavioural impulsivity task measures (Quednow et al. Reference Quednow, Kuhn, Hoppe, Westheide, Maier, Daum and Wagner2007; Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ; Hulka et al. Reference Hulka, Eisenegger, Preller, Vonmoos, Jenni, Bendrick, Baumgartner, Seifritz and Quednow2014) and, thus, underlines the multi-faceted concept of the impulsivity construct. The reasons why users, who changed their cocaine use pattern, perceived their impulsivity levels to be more or less pronounced, remains to be determined in further investigations. One could speculate though that the subjective recognition of changes in impulsivity is more sensitive than the behavioural measures or that specific facets of impulsivity were not covered by our behavioural tasks. Alternatively, CUs’ personal thoughts, attitudes, and intentions to change their cocaine use might have influenced subjectively reported impulsivity levels. Accordingly, increasers may be more concerned with their loss of control over drug intake and/or may also experience more aversive consequences due to their increased cocaine use fostering worries. Decreasers on the other hand may have perceived themselves as less impulsive because they had a desire to reduce their cocaine use and made a conscious effort to control their impulsivity. Correspondingly, research on attentional bias modification and substance use has shown that individuals who express a desire to reduce their use respond much better to treatment and show less impulsivity (Wiers & Stacy, Reference Wiers and Stacy2006). Moreover, it has been shown that increased impulsivity in the BIS is associated with more treatment dropouts and shorter treatment duration in CU (Moeller et al. Reference Moeller, Dougherty, Barratt, Schmitz, Swann and Grabowski2001b ; Patkar et al. Reference Patkar, Murray, Mannelli, Gottheil, Weinstein and Vergare2004). Therefore, treatment approaches should foster awareness in CU that they often act impulsively and without foresight for negative consequences, but that these cognitive-behavioural tendencies can be reduced with mindfulness-based and other cognitive-behavioural relapse prevention strategies (Dakwar & Levin, Reference Dakwar and Levin2013).

It is also noteworthy that self-reported attentional and motor impulsivity were significantly associated with more pronounced ADHD symptoms. These results are in line with findings from our cross-sectional study (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ) as well as with previous studies postulating a substantial comorbidity of ADHD with substance use disorders (van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen et al. Reference van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen, van de Glind, van den Brink, Smit, Crunelle, Swets and Schoevers2012). Unfortunately, we omitted to assess the ADHD-SR questionnaire at the follow-up test session and are therefore not able to directly analyse the covariation of changes in ADHD symptoms with changes of impulsivity measures.

In the present study, temporal discounting preferences as measured by DD did not vary with increased or decreased cocaine use, which is in line with previous propositions that intertemporal discounting preferences reflect a stable aspect of personality and can be considered as a trait marker (Odum Reference Odum2011a , Reference Odum b ). A growing body of evidence corroborates our finding by showing that individual discounting preferences in adolescents and adults are relatively stable over numerous time intervals ranging from 1 week to multiple years (for review see MacKillop, Reference MacKillop2013). In addition, more pronounced delay discounting preferences have been suggested to be predictive of the initiation and escalation of drug use (Ersche et al. Reference Ersche, Turton, Chamberlain, Muller, Bullmore and Robbins2012, Reference Ersche, Turton, Pradhan, Bullmore and Robbins2013; Mahoney & Olmstead, Reference Mahoney and Olmstead2013) as well as poor treatment responses in human CU (Washio et al. Reference Washio, Higgins, Heil, McKerchar, Badger, Skelly and Dantona2011). Furthermore, a prospective study yielded evidence that stronger tendencies to prefer immediate smaller over larger but later available amounts are associated with negative outcomes in the financial, academic, and health domains (Mischel et al. Reference Mischel, Ayduk, Berman, Casey, Gotlib, Jonides, Kross, Teslovich, Wilson, Zayas and Shoda2011). Altogether, the results of the present study and prior findings suggest that more pronounced delay discounting rates may be regarded as a relatively stable addiction endophenotype increasing the risk to initiate and maintain drug use (Odum Reference Odum2011a , Reference Odum b ; MacKillop, Reference MacKillop2013).

Impulsive action measured by the RVP response bias B″ was not significantly shifted in our CU and did not covary with changing drug use patterns. These findings are consistent with the previously published results from the cross-sectional part of the ZuCo2St showing that neither recreational nor dependent CU differed from controls regarding RVP B″ and Stop-Signal task performance (Vonmoos et al. Reference Vonmoos, Hulka, Preller, Jenni, Schulz, Baumgartner and Quednow2013b ). Accordingly, the ecological validity of the RVP response bias B’ regarding the measurement of addiction-related impulsive action might be questioned.

The finding that CU who decreased their consumption chose fewer favourable cards in the IGT at the second assessment is somewhat puzzling. In the cross-sectional analysis published previously (Hulka et al. Reference Hulka, Eisenegger, Preller, Vonmoos, Jenni, Bendrick, Baumgartner, Seifritz and Quednow2014), we found that although dependent CU performed slightly worse than HC, the effect was not as pronounced as shown in previous studies (Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Dolan and Hindes2002; Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Benbrook, Funderburk, David, Cadet and Bolla2007; Kjome et al. Reference Kjome, Lane, Schmitz, Green, Ma, Prasla, Swann and Moeller2010). We speculated that these differences might be explained by the fact that we paid our participants with real money according to their performance, which is in contrast to most prior studies reporting substantial differences between CU and HC. In line with our hypothesis are the results obtained in another study conducted by Vadhan et al. (Reference Vadhan, Hart, Haney, van Gorp and Foltin2009) where CU were paid in real money and also did not perform significantly worse than HC in the IGT, suggesting that motivational factors may influence IGT performance in drug users. It is further noteworthy that while cocaine use patterns did not significantly differ between the user groups at baseline, decreasers had a higher cumulative cocaine usage than equal users and increasers. Therefore, we cannot fully rule out that decreasers were overall more impaired in their IGT performance due to their more pronounced cocaine use, which may not have been detected in the baseline assessment because of low statistical power.

There are some limitations inherent to this study: first, the sample sizes are relatively small for multilevel analyses. However, the data were also analysed with mixed design analyses of covariance yielding comparable results (not reported here). Second, in addition to the increased cocaine consumption, increasers also drank more alcohol than decreasers and tested positive for recent cocaine use more frequently than decreasers. However, the rise in weekly alcohol use was based on an increased alcohol intake in only three of 19 cocaine increasers and excluding these subjects did not alter the main results. Third, although the group assignment of increasers, decreasers, and equal users was based on objective hair toxicology capturing the past 6 months, for the first 6 months of the time interval we had to rely on self-reported drug measures. Fourth, although our sample consisted of CU with relatively little polytoxic drug use, it should be mentioned that at baseline, cocaine increasers used low but significantly higher amounts of MDMA (0.04 v. 0.01 tablet per week) and used amphetamine (3.3 v. 1.3 years) for a longer period than decreasers. Furthermore, at follow-up cocaine increasers revealed a slightly higher use of MDMA and methylphenidate compared to baseline and had an additional augmentation in weekly alcohol use. Whereas the change in MDMA use was less than half a tablet per week, the difference in methylphenidate consumption was explained by a single individual. However, exclusion of the single methylphenidate user did not change the main results in separate analyses. Thus, although changes in other drugs should be considered as a contributing factor to our results, it seems reasonable that compared to the strong increase in cocaine use, the effect of changed use of other drugs is likely rather small. Fifth, our CU groups differed from controls regarding ADHD and depressive symptoms, which is in accordance with the current literature (Swendsen & Merikangas, Reference Swendsen and Merikangas2000; Wilson, Reference Wilson2007). However, we tried to overcome this constraint by introducing these variables in the multilevel regression analyses.

These 1-year longitudinal data indicate for the first time that self-reported impulsivity and impulsive choice requiring the integration of risk, reward, and loss contingencies (as measured with the IGT) covary with changing cocaine use, while a response bias measure and delay discounting preferences remain largely unaffected. Accordingly, self-reported impulsivity and gambling decision-making measures are less suitable for predicting the risk to develop a cocaine use disorder but might be used to monitor treatment success. By contrast, the preference for immediate smaller rewards over larger delayed rewards seems to be a stable trait marker for cocaine use and, thus, may represent a suitable endophenotype for stimulant addiction.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001063

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Daniela Jenni, Kathrin Küpeli, and Franziska Minder for excellent support of recruitment and assessment of the participants. The study was supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF; grant No. PP00P1-123516/1 and PP00P1-146326/1) and the Olga Mayenfisch Foundation.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Aharonovich, E, Hasin, DS, Brooks, AC, Liu, X, Bisaga, A, Nunes, EV (2006). Cognitive deficits predict low treatment retention in cocaine dependent patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 81, 313322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (1994). American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. American Psychiatric Association (APA): Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bechara, A (2003). Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. Journal of Gambling Studies 19, 2351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A, Dolan, S, Hindes, A (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 40, 16901705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Ward, CH, Mendelson, M, Mock, J, Erbaugh, J (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, DM (2008). The U.S. mandatory guidelines for federal workplace drug testing programs: current status and future considerations. Forensic Science International 174, 111119.Google ScholarPubMed
Colzato, LS, van den Wildenberg, WP, Hommel, B (2007). Impaired inhibitory control in recreational cocaine users. PLoS ONE 2, e1143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, GA, Kronstrand, R, Kintz, P, Society of Hair T (2012). Society of hair testing guidelines for drug testing in hair. Forensic Science International 218, 2024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunha, PJ, Bechara, A, de Andrade, AG, Nicastri, S (2011). Decision-making deficits linked to real-life social dysfunction in crack cocaine-dependent individuals. American Journal on Addictions 20, 7886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dakwar, E, Levin, FR (2013). Individual mindfulness-based psychotherapy for cannabis or cocaine dependence: a pilot feasibility trial. American Journal of Addiction 22, 521526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R, Sahakian, BJ, McKay, AP, Herrod, JJ, Robbins, TW, Paykel, ES (1996). Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychological Medicine 26, 975989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
EMCDDA (2014). European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction. European Drug Report 2014. Trends and Developments. p. 88. Publications Office of the European Union: Luxembourg.Google Scholar
Ersche, KD, Barnes, A, Jones, PS, Morein-Zamir, S, Robbins, TW, Bullmore, ET (2011). Abnormal structure of frontostriatal brain systems is associated with aspects of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependence. Brain 134, 20132024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ersche, KD, Jones, PS, Williams, GB, Smith, DG, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW (2013). Distinctive personality traits and neural correlates associated with stimulant drug use versus familial risk of stimulant dependence. Biological Psychiatry 74, 137144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ersche, KD, Turton, AJ, Chamberlain, SR, Muller, U, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW (2012). Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry 169, 926936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ersche, KD, Turton, AJ, Pradhan, S, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW (2010). Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits. Biological Psychiatry 68, 770773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fellows, LK (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3, 159172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, M, Schoenmakers, T, Wiers, RW (2008). Cognitive processes in alcohol binges: a review and research agenda. Current Drug Abuse Reviews 1, 263279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franken, IH (2003). Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 27, 563579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, A, Hill, J (2007). Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. Cambridge University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12, 652669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DM, Swets, JA (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Heatherton, TF, Kozlowski, LT, Frecker, RC, Fagerstrom, KO (1991). the fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction 86, 11191127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoelzle, C, Scheufler, F, Uhl, M, Sachs, H, Thieme, D (2008). Application of discriminant analysis to differentiate between incorporation of cocaine and its congeners into hair and contamination. Forensic Science International 176, 1318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulka, LM, Eisenegger, C, Preller, KH, Vonmoos, M, Jenni, D, Bendrick, K, Baumgartner, MR, Seifritz, E, Quednow, BB (2014). Altered social and non-social decision-making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychological Medicine 44, 10151028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Myers, J, Prescott, CA (2007). Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for symptoms of cannabis, cocaine, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 13131320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, KN, Petry, NM, Bickel, WK (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 128, 7887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjome, KL, Lane, SD, Schmitz, JM, Green, C, Ma, L, Prasla, I, Swann, AC, Moeller, FG (2010). Relationship between impulsivity and decision making in cocaine dependence. Psychiatry Research 178, 299304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreek, MJ, Nielsen, DA, Butelman, ER, LaForge, KS (2005). Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neuroscience 8, 14501457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrl, S (1999). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest (MWT-B), 4th edn. Spitta: Balingen.Google Scholar
Littlefield, AK, Sher, KJ, Wood, PK (2009). Is ‘maturing out’ of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 118, 360374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locascio, JJ, Atri, A (2011). An overview of longitudinal data analysis methods for neurological research. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra 1, 330357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucantonio, F, Stalnaker, TA, Shaham, Y, Niv, Y, Schoenbaum, G (2012). The impact of orbitofrontal dysfunction on cocaine addiction. Nature Neuroscience 15, 358366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKillop, J (2013). Integrating behavioral economics and behavioral genetics: delayed reward discounting as an endophenotype for addictive disorders. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 99, 1431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, MK, Olmstead, MC (2013). Neurobiology of an endophenotype: modeling the progression of alcohol addiction in rodents. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23, 607614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marhe, R, Waters, AJ, van de Wetering, BM, Franken, IHA (2013). Implicit and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxification treatment are associated with drug relapse: an ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 81, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W, Ayduk, O, Berman, MG, Casey, BJ, Gotlib, IH, Jonides, J, Kross, E, Teslovich, T, Wilson, NL, Zayas, V, Shoda, Y (2011). ‘Willpower’ over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6, 252256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Barratt, ES, Dougherty, DM, Schmitz, JM, Swann, AC (2001a). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 17831793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Dougherty, DM, Barratt, ES, Schmitz, JM, Swann, AC, Grabowski, J (2001b). The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse and Treatment 21, 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NSDUH (2014). National survey of drug use and health. trends in prevalence of various drugs for ages 12 or older, ages 12 to 17, ages 18 to 25, and ages 26 or older. http://www.drugabuse.gov/national-survey-drug-use-health. Accessed 13 October 2014.Google Scholar
Nutt, D, King, LA, Saulsbury, W, Blakemore, C (2007). Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. Lancet 369, 10471053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odum, AL (2011 a). Delay discounting: I'm a k, you're a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 96, 427439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odum, AL (2011 b). Delay discounting: trait variable? Behavioural Processes 87, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patkar, AA, Murray, HW, Mannelli, P, Gottheil, E, Weinstein, SP, Vergare, MJ (2004). Pre-treatment measures of impulsivity, aggression and sensation seeking are associated with treatment outcome for African-American cocaine-dependent patients. Journal of Addictive Diseases 23, 109122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, JH, Stanford, MS, Barratt, ES (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51, 768774.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, JL, Carroll, ME (2008). The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology 200, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preller, KH, Hulka, LM, Vonmoos, M, Jenni, D, Baumgartner, MR, Seifritz, E, Dziobek, I, Quednow, BB (2014). Impaired emotional empathy and related social network deficits in cocaine users. Addiction Biology 19, 452466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quednow, BB, Kuhn, KU, Hoenig, K, Maier, W, Wagner, M (2004). Prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in male MDMA (‘ecstasy’) users, cannabis users, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 982990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quednow, BB, Kuhn, KU, Hoppe, C, Westheide, J, Maier, W, Daum, I, Wagner, M (2007). Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making cognition in heavy users of MDMA (‘Ecstasy’). Psychopharmacology 189, 517530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reske, M, Delis, DC, Paulus, MP (2011). Evidence for subtle verbal fluency deficits in occasional stimulant users: quick to play loose with verbal rules. Journal of Psychiatric Research 45, 361368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roesler, M, Retz, W, Retz-Junginger, P, Thome, J, Supprian, T, Nissen, T, Stieglitz, R-D, Blocher, D, Hengesch, G, Trott, GE (2004). Tools for the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Self-rating behaviour questionnaire and diagnostic checklist. Nervenarzt 75, 888895.Google Scholar
StataCorp. (2011). Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. StataCorp LP: Texas.Google Scholar
Stevens, L, Verdejo-Garcia, A, Goudriann, AE, Royers, H, Dom, G, Vanderplasschen, W (2014). Impulsivity as a vulnerability factor for poor addiction treatment outcomes: a review of neurocognitive findings among individuals with substance use disorders. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 47, 5872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2008). Mandatory guidelines for federal workplace drug testing programs. Federal Register 73, 7185871907.Google Scholar
Sussner, BD, Smelson, DA, Rodrigues, S, Kline, A, Losonczy, M, Ziedonis, D (2006). The validity and reliability of a brief measure of cocaine craving. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 83, 233237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swendsen, JD, Merikangas, KR (2000). The comorbidity of depression and substance use disorders. Clinical Psychology Review 20, 173189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnik, B, Fidell, L (2006). Using Multivariate Statistics. Pearson Education: Boston.Google Scholar
UNODC (2014). United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. World Drug Report 2014. p. 128. United Nations Publication: Vienna.Google Scholar
Vadhan, NP, Hart, CL, Haney, M, van Gorp, WG, Foltin, RW (2009). Decision-making in long-term cocaine users: Effects of a cash monetary contingency on Gambling task performance. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 102, 95101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen, K, van de Glind, G, van den Brink, W, Smit, F, Crunelle, CL, Swets, M, Schoevers, RA (2012). Prevalence of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in substance use disorder patients: a meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 122, 1119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Albein-Urios, N, Martinez-Gonzalez, JM, Civit, E, de la Torre, R, Lozano, O (2014). Decision-making impairment predicts 3-month hair-indexed cocaine relapse. Psychopharmacology 231, 41794187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Benbrook, A, Funderburk, F, David, P, Cadet, JL, Bolla, KI (2007). The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 90, 211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Lawrence, AJ, Clark, L (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 777810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonmoos, M, Hulka, LM, Preller, KH, Jenni, D, Baumgartner, MR, Stohler, R, Bolla, KI, Quednow, BB (2013 a). Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, craving and early age at onset. British Journal of Psychiatry 203, 3543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonmoos, M, Hulka, LM, Preller, KH, Jenni, D, Schulz, C, Baumgartner, MR, Quednow, BB (2013 b). Differences in self-reported and behavioral measures of impulsivity in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 133, 6170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonmoos, M, Hulka, LM, Preller, KH, Minder, F, Baumgartner, MR, Quednow, BB (2014). Cognitive Impairment in Cocaine Users is Drug-Induced but Partially Reversible: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 22002210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, FA, Anthony, JC (2002). From first drug use to drug dependence; developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 479488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washio, Y, Higgins, ST, Heil, SH, McKerchar, TL, Badger, GJ, Skelly, JM, Dantona, RL (2011). Delay discounting is associated with treatment response among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 19, 243248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiers, RW, Stacy, AW (2006). Implicit cognition and addiction. Current Directions in Psychological Science 15, 292296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, JJ (2007). ADHD and substance use disorders: developmental aspects and the impact of stimulant treatment. American Journal on Addictions 16 (Suppl. 1), 511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winstanley, CA, Olausson, P, Taylor, JR, Jentsch, JD (2010). Insight into the relationship between impulsivity and substance abuse from studies using animal models. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 34, 13061318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic data and pattern of drug use

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Predicted values for baseline (t1) and follow-up (t2) are shown. (a) Development of Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) total scores within 1 year. Control group data in raw values (predicted data are not available because this group was dropped from the regression model due to collinearity). Group  ×  time interaction (increaser × decreaser) p  <  0.01. *p <  0.05 time effect for cocaine decreasers. (b) Development of the Rapid Visual Processing task (RVP) response bias B″ within 1 year. (c) Development of delay discounting (DD) parameter k within 1 year. Control group data in raw values (predicted data are not available because this group was dropped from the regression model due to collinearity). (d) Development of the Iowa Gambling task (IGT) total ratio within 1 year. Group × time interaction (increaser × decreaser) p  <  0.05.

Figure 2

Table 2. Coefficients (standard errors) of final multilevel regression models with standardized dependent variables

Figure 3

Table 3. One-year test–retest reliability between baseline and 1-year follow-up in controls and cocaine users

Supplementary material: File

Hulka supplementary material

Hulka supplementary material 1

Download Hulka supplementary material(File)
File 45.7 KB