Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:43:40.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive sequelae of blast-related versus other mechanisms of brain trauma

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2009

HEATHER G. BELANGER*
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences, James A. Haley Veterans Hospital, Tampa, Florida Department of Psychology, University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center (DVBIC)
TRACY KRETZMER
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences, James A. Haley Veterans Hospital, Tampa, Florida
RUTH YOASH-GANTZ
Affiliation:
Mid-Atlantic (VISN 6) Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), North Carolina, Virginia, West Virginia Department of Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences, Hefner Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, North Carolina Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina
TREVEN PICKETT
Affiliation:
Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center (DVBIC) Mid-Atlantic (VISN 6) Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), North Carolina, Virginia, West Virginia Mental Health Service, McGuire Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Richmond, Virginia Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, Richmond, Virginia
LARRY A. TUPLER
Affiliation:
Mid-Atlantic (VISN 6) Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), North Carolina, Virginia, West Virginia Mental Health Service, Durham Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Heather G. Belanger, James A. Haley Veterans Hospital Psychology Service, 116B 13000 Bruce B. Downs Boulevard, Tampa, Florida 33612. E-mail: heather.belanger@va.gov.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The use of improvised explosive devices has become the hallmark of modern warfare and has resulted in an ever-increasing number of blast-related traumatic brain injuries (TBIs). Despite this fact, very little is actually known about the cognitive sequelae of blast-related TBIs. The purpose of the current study was to compare patterns of performance on neuropsychological measures in subjects who have sustained TBIs as a result of blast (or explosion) with those who have sustained TBIs from non-blast or blunt force trauma (motor vehicle accident, fall, assault, etc.). Participants were categorized as blast-related TBI or non-blast-related TBI and according to severity of injury (mild or moderate-to-severe). No main effects were observed in analysis of covariance between blast-related TBI participants and non-blast-related TBI participants across any of the neuropsychological variables, although an interaction was observed on a visual memory test showing stronger performance for mild blast-related and poorer performance for moderate-to-severe blast-related participants compared with both non-blast groups. Overall, the results do not provide any strong evidence that blast is categorically different from other TBI mechanisms, at least with regard to cognitive sequelae on select measures. Additional findings included a marginally increased incidence of reported posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among blast-injured participants. (JINS, 2009, 15, 1–8.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © INS 2009

INTRODUCTION

Explosive mechanisms (e.g., improvised explosive devices [IEDs], landmines, rocket-propelled grenades) account for 78% of injuries in servicemen and women injured in Afghanistan and Iraq, which is the highest proportion seen in any large-scale conflict (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Kragh, Wenke, Macaitis, Wade and Holcomb2008). A recent study found that 88% of military personnel treated at a medical unit in Iraq had been injured by IEDs or mortar (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Reynolds, Schroeder, Harrison, Evans and Hospenthal2005). Furthermore, a Veterans Affairs (VA)–based study found that 56% of its war-injured sample had been injured by blasts (Sayer et al., Reference Sayer, Chiros, Sigford, Scott, Clothier, Pickett and Lew2008). While blast-related injuries are not new (Coupland & Meddings, Reference Coupland and Meddings1999), it is the use of IEDs with ever-increasing amounts of explosive (and sometimes toxic) materials that have become the hallmark of these wars. IEDs and other explosive munitions can cause injury via high-force blast waves (primary blast injuries) or by the mechanical aftermath of the explosion such as expelled missile fragments, being forcefully thrown, or being crushed by collapsing objects (secondary, tertiary, or quaternary blast injuries) (DePalma et al., Reference DePalma, Burris, Champion and Hodgson2005; Taber et al., Reference Taber, Warden and Hurley2006). As such, exposure to blast-level forces can result in a multitude of injuries, including damage to internal organs, multiple fractures, amputations, burns, and traumatic brain injury (TBI).

The potential neuropsychological implications of exposure to blast are still uncertain. The Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center reported that 59% of an “at-risk” group of injured soldiers returning from Afghanistan or Iraq from 2003 to 2004 suffered at least a mild TBI while in combat (Okie, Reference Okie2005; Warden et al., Reference Warden, Ryan, Helmick, Schwab, French, Lu, Lux, Ling and Ecklund2005). A recent survey of returnees from Iraq revealed that nearly 15% reported an injury involving loss of consciousness (LOC) or altered mental status (Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008). Of those, blast or explosion was the most common mechanism of injury. So, for instance, 79% of those reporting an injury involving LOC were injured via blast explosion. Clearly, blast-related TBI is not uncommon.

Our understanding of how blast affects the brain is limited. An explosive blast, with its over-pressurization or primary blast wave, is a different mechanism of injury than an acceleration–deceleration injury. Injuries caused by primary blast waves may result from discrepancies between the external atmospheric pressure and the internal pressure of air-filled organs such as the ear, lungs, and gastrointestinal tract or organs surrounded by fluid-filled cavities such as the brain and spinal cord (Elsayed, Reference Elsayed1997; Mayorga, Reference Mayorga1997). Despite limited human studies, an increasing number of animal studies suggest that primary blast waves can cause structural injuries to the brain. For example, animal exposure to primary blast waves results in damaged brain tissue and consequent cognitive deficits (Cernak et al., Reference Cernak, Savic, Malicevic, Zunic, Radosevic, Ivanovic and Davidovic1996; Kaur et al., Reference Kaur, Singh, Lim, Ng, Yap and Ling1995). More specifically, recent studies have found the formation of cytoplasmic vacuoles and myelin alterations in the hippocampus of primary blast-exposed rodents (Cernak et al., Reference Cernak, Wang, Jiang, Bian and Savic2001). Such changes were even noted in rodents with blast to the thorax while the head was protected.

In contrast to injury from the primary blast, secondary, tertiary, and quaternary blast injuries are mechanical injuries and would therefore likely be similar to brain injuries sustained from falls or motor vehicle accidents (MVAs). For example, secondary blast injuries occur when flying debris or projectiles strike or penetrate any part of the body, while tertiary blast injuries can result when an individual is thrown by high-energy shock waves. Finally, quaternary blast injuries include a wide variety of conditions such as burns, toxic inhalations, or crush injuries from collapsed structures or displaced heavy objects (DePalma et al., Reference DePalma, Burris, Champion and Hodgson2005; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Vanderploeg, Belanger and Scholten2005; Warden, Reference Warden2006).

Very little is known about the cognitive sequelae of blast-related brain injuries in humans. The existing TBI literature was created almost exclusively using data from patients having sustained TBIs from blunt force trauma, such as those resulting from MVAs. With regard to mild TBI, the literature has consistently demonstrated improvement to baseline in the majority of cases by 3–6 months post-injury (Belanger et al., Reference Belanger, Curtiss, Demery, Lebowitz and Vanderploeg2005; Binder et al., Reference Binder, Rohling and Larrabee1997; Schretlen & Shapiro, Reference Schretlen and Shapiro2003). Moderate-to-severe injury, on the other hand, tends to result in long-standing impairment despite initial recovery (Dikmen et al., Reference Dikmen, Machamer, Powell and Temkin2003), although this is not uniformly the case (Barnfield & Leathem, Reference Barnfield and Leathem1998; Dikmen et al., Reference Dikmen, Machamer, Winn and Temkin1995; Millis et al., Reference Millis, Rosenthal, Novack, Sherer, Nick, Kreutzer, High and Ricker2001). To date, no human studies have been published on the cognitive sequelae of blast-induced TBI. Sayer et al. (Reference Sayer, Chiros, Sigford, Scott, Clothier, Pickett and Lew2008) found that mechanism of injury did not predict outcomes such as changes in motor or cognitive functioning as measured by the Functional Independence Measure. However, performance on neuropsychological tests was not examined in that study. The purpose of this study is to compare performance on neuropsychological measures in patients who have sustained a TBI primarily due to blast (or explosion) to performance in demographically comparable patients who have sustained TBI due to other, non-blast-related mechanisms (MVA, fall, assault, etc.).

METHODS

Participants

Participants were consecutively assessed individuals from the Tampa VA Medical Center and selected research volunteers from the Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), which includes the Richmond, Salisbury, and Durham VA medical centers. Participants were excluded (n = 31) if they were suspected of poor effort or malingering based on clinical presentation and/or if they failed certain measures of symptom validity (Word Memory Test [Green et al., Reference Green, Allen and Astner1996] at the Mid-Atlantic MIRECC and Medical Symptom Validity Test [Green, Reference Green2003] and California Verbal Learning Test-II [CVLT-II] Long-Delay Forced-Choice Recognition [Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000] at Tampa). Participants were additionally excluded if they were known to suffer from other neurological disorders (e.g., stroke) apart from TBI (n = 1) or if they sustained a brain injury due to gunshot (n = 3). The final sample (N = 102) consisted primarily of returning active-duty or veteran military personnel who were injured in Afghanistan or Iraq (i.e., 68 were active duty [67%]). The average participant was 28.7 ± 7.7 years of age (range = 19–57 years), had 12.9 ± 2.0 years of education (range = 7–18 years), and had a Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2001) predicted full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) of 97.2 ± 13.7 (range = 68–125, n = 95). Additional basic demographic information is provided in Table 1. Patients recruited from the Tampa (n = 56) and Richmond (n = 27) VAs were participants in brain injury rehabilitation programs through their Polytrauma/TBI Rehabilitation Centers (P/TRCs), either inpatient or outpatient, and had been referred for a neuropsychological evaluation. Subjects from the Salisbury (n = 12) and Durham (n = 7) VAs were all post-deployed military personnel or VA outpatients. Participants were consented under research protocols approved by each facility’s institutional review board.

Table 1. Demographic and injury information

TBI severity

Severity of TBI was based on self-report and/or documentation of LOC and length of posttraumatic amnesia (PTA). Self-report of PTA is often necessary in returning combat personnel due to the lack of detailed medical records from the theatre, although this means of assessment occurs almost exclusively in the case of mild TBI. In contrast, LOC is generally documented, although again momentary LOC is often only ascertained through questioning the patient about the events surrounding the injury. Only participants who had LOC of less than 30 min and PTA of less than 24 hr were included in the mild TBI group (n = 51). This categorization is based on widely accepted criteria of mild TBI (Kay et al., Reference Kay, Harrington, Adams, Anderson, Berrol, Cicerone, Dahlberg, Gerber, Goka, Harley, Hilt, Horn, Lehmkuhl and Malec1993). Participants meeting criteria for mild TBI with abnormal neuroimaging findings constituted 18% of that group (n = 9: six blast and three non-blast). These findings on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or computed tomography included variants of the following: questionable low signal suggesting possible shear injury, a cyst, nonspecific hyperintensity, abnormal morphology of the amygdala, and possible small-focus subarachnoid hemorrhage. Participants experiencing LOC greater than 30 min and/or PTA greater than 24 hr were included in the moderate-to-severe TBI group (n = 51). Demographic breakdowns as a function of TBI severity are presented in Table 2. The mild TBI group was significantly older at the time of evaluation (p < .005), and a significantly longer time had elapsed since injury (p < .01). No significant differences were observed for age at time of injury, level of education, or predicted FSIQ based on the WTAR (all ps > .05).

Table 2. Demographic variables by TBI severity

a Satterthwaite t-tests due to unequal variance.

TBI etiology

Sixty-one subjects were exposed to blast as their primary mechanism of injury. Of these, 12 also sustained secondary injuries due to MVA. No differences were observed on any variable between those with pure injuries and those with secondary injuries from MVAs; subjects were therefore combined for the remaining analyses. Fifty-one subjects in the blast-related TBI group had closed-head injury; 10 subjects had open-head injury. All but one of the subjects with open-head injury was in the moderate-to-severe group. No differences were observed between subjects with open- versus closed-head injury on any variable, and thus, groups were combined for the remaining analyses. Forty-one subjects suffered injuries from non-blast types of mechanisms, including MVA (n = 26), fall (n = 7), motorcycle accident (n = 5), assault (n = 2), and helicopter crash (n = 1). Demographic information on groups as a function of blast versus non-blast etiology is presented in Table 3. No significant differences were observed for any variable. However, whereas blast victims were relatively evenly distributed between P/TRC (50/83) and non-P/TRC (11/19; χ 2 = 0.04, df = 1, p > .85) facilities, a significantly larger percentage of moderate-to-severe TBI was observed in P/TRCs (48/83) compared with non-P/TRCs (3/19; Fisher’s exact p = .002).

Table 3. Demographic variables by TBI etiology

a Satterthwaite t-tests due to unequal variance.

Procedures

The participants were administered a battery of neuropsychological tests. For the purposes of this study, measures known to have greater sensitivity to brain injury and administered in largest number across all sites were analyzed to maximize power to detect potential group differences. These measures included the Trail Making Test (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1985), Digit Symbol-Coding subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, Reference Benedict1997), and CVLT-II (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000). T scores or age-corrected scaled scores based on normative comparisons presented in the manuals were used as dependent measures to control for age and education effects. For the Trail Making Test, Heaton et al.’s (Reference Heaton, Grant and Matthews1991) normative data were used. Participants also completed the WTAR (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2001) and a self-report measure of current posttraumatic symptomatology: the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) CheckList (PCL; Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Huska and Keane1991). Except for the PCL, not all subjects received each measure, resulting in differing cell sizes for analyses.

Statistics

Group differences on demographic variables and the WTAR were examined using t-tests or contingency table analysis depending on the variable’s level of measurement. To determine whether the groups differed on cognitive functioning, we compared normative scores on the four neuropsychological tests using general linear model (GLM) multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA): Trail Making Test Part A and Part B time to completion, Digit Symbol-Coding total symbols, BVMT-R Total Recall and Delayed Recall, and CVLT-II Total Recall (i.e., Trials 1–5 Free Recall Total) and Long-Delay Free Recall (LDFR) Total. TBI etiology (blast, non-blast) and TBI severity (mild, moderate-to-severe) were entered as class variables, and the etiology × severity interaction was examined. Repeated-measures modeling was applied for the Trail Making Test (Part A, Part B), BVMT-R (Total Recall, Delayed Recall), and CVLT-II (Total Recall, LDFR). To control for significant differences between groups, time since injury (<90 days = 1, 90 days to 1 year = 2, >1 year = 3) was entered as a covariate.

RESULTS

Participants with blast injury were much more likely to be categorized with mild (n = 38) as opposed to moderate-to-severe TBI (n = 23), whereas subjects with non-blast etiologies were more likely to be categorized with moderate-to-severe (n = 28) as opposed to mild TBI (n = 13) (χ 2 = 9.18, df = 1, p < .005). Twenty participants had history of concussion prior to the index TBI for this study, but no differences were observed in the number suffered by participants with blast injury (n = 13) versus those with non-blast etiologies (n = 7; χ 2 = 0.28, df = 1, p > .59).

MANCOVA conducted on PCL scores yielded a highly significant GLM (F [4,97] = 21.35, p < .0001). Participants with blast injury exhibited a marginally higher score on the PCL (41.1 ± 18.0) compared with subjects with non-blast etiologies (32.9 ± 17.2; F [1,97] = 3.60, p < .07). The effect of time since injury was also significant (F [1,97] = 49.97, p < .0001), reflecting progressively more severe PTSD symptomatology with the passage of time. Due to the near-significant difference between groups on this variable, a highly significant difference between severity groups (mild: 45.5 ± 17.2, moderate-to-severe: 30.1 ± 15.5; t [100] = 4.76, p < .0001), and the potential for PTSD symptoms to influence neuropsychological performance, particularly memory (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Kleiner, Vasterling and Field2007), PCL score was entered as a covariate in all subsequent analyses.

Unadjusted means and standard deviations for neurocognitive variables as a function of TBI etiology and severity are presented in Table 4. Statistically significant results for TBI severity were found for all dependent measures at p < .05 with the exception of the Trail Making Test Part B, which achieved borderline significance at p < .09. Outcomes for covariate-adjusted analyses were as follows.

Table 4. Cognitive variables by TBI severity and etiology group

Note. Values presented are mean T scores (with standard deviations in parentheses).

a Digit Symbol-Coding scores represent WAIS-III age-corrected scaled scores (mean = 10, standard deviation = 3).

Speed/flexibility

Results for the Trail Making Test (n = 92) revealed no significant between-subjects difference or interactive effect for any variable (all ps > .10). The repeated-measures effect for task difficulty was marginally significant, reflecting slightly higher T scores for Part B compared with Part A (F [1,86] = 3.47, p < .07). Similarly, for WAIS-III Digit Symbol-Coding (n = 83), the overall model was nonsignificant (F [5,77] = 1.28, p > .28).

Learning/memory

Results for the BVMT-R (n = 93) failed to show main effects for TBI etiology or severity but did yield a significant interaction between TBI etiology and TBI severity (F [1,87] = 4.31, p < .05). Inspection of results indicated that blast-injured patients with mild TBI had the highest means, whereas blast-injured patients with moderate-to-severe TBI had the lowest means (i.e., worst performance) and non-blast patients had means intermediate between the two. Interactions with the repeated measure (Total Recall vs. Delayed Recall) were also significant in relation to PCL score (F [1,87] = 6.07, p < .05) and time since injury (F [1,87] = 4.15, p < .05). The interaction with PCL score reflected a significant correlation with Total Recall (r = .21, p < .05) but not Delayed Recall (r = .08, p > .42), and the interaction with time since injury reflected a somewhat steeper increase in performance with greater time since injury for Delayed Recall (T = 44.7 ± 14.7 to 46.0 ± 14.7 to 51.2 ± 12.4) compared with Total Recall (T = 43.2 ± 13.3 to 43.6 ± 16.1 to 49.0 ± 10.8). Finally, results for the CVLT-II (n = 94) indicated no between-subjects effect for TBI etiology (F [1,88] = 0.75, p > .38) but did reveal a significant effect for TBI severity (F [1,88] = 6.63, p < .05), reflecting better performance for the mild group compared with the moderate-to-severe group. The effect was noted for both Total Recall (F [1,88] = 8.09, p < .01) and LDFR (F [1,88] = 4.44, p < .05). A significant effect of the repeated measure was also observed (F [1,88] = 12.75, p < .001), reflecting substantially better performance for learning trials (Total Recall) compared with LDFR.

DISCUSSION

To our knowledge, this is the first study comparing cognitive profiles of blast versus non-blast mechanisms of TBI. Our results suggest that cognitive sequelae following TBI are determined more by severity of injury than mechanism of injury on verbal learning and memory measures. Inspection of means indicated that cognitive performance was uniformly worse for moderate-to-severe TBI compared with mild TBI, with statistically significant results observed for all measures except for Part B of the Trail Making Test and significant findings when covariates were included for the CVLT-II, a verbal learning and memory measure. Statistical significance of TBI severity on neuropsychological measures other than the CVLT-II was lost with the addition of covariates (i.e., time since injury and PCL scores), illustrating the importance of these variables on outcomes. Other studies have found no differences on neuropsychological variables between mild and moderate-to-severe TBI groups (Barnfield & Leathem, Reference Barnfield and Leathem1998). Most outcome studies have found significant heterogeneity on neuropsychological measures across TBI severity, with many TBI survivors performing within normatively normal ranges at follow-up (Millis et al., Reference Millis, Rosenthal, Novack, Sherer, Nick, Kreutzer, High and Ricker2001) and with many showing significant recovery months to years post-injury (Dikmen et al., Reference Dikmen, Machamer, Winn and Temkin1995). Our sample was nearly 2 years post-injury, on average, which may have attenuated any effect of TBI severity or mechanism of injury on neuropsychological outcomes when time since injury was entered into the model.

Comparison of blast TBI with non-blast etiologies did not yield significant differences with the exception of an etiology × severity interaction on the BVMT-R. For Total Recall and Delayed Recall, blast-injured participants with mild TBI had the highest means, whereas blast-injured participants with moderate-to-severe TBI had the lowest means (i.e., worst performance) and non-blast participants had means intermediate between the two. Overall, the results do not provide any strong evidence that blast is categorically different from other mechanisms, at least with regard to cognitive sequelae on select measures.

Our sample of blast-injured participants was more likely to have mild rather than moderate-to-severe TBI. This pattern is consistent with findings of a recent survey of Iraq war returnees with a history of mild TBI who were more likely to have been injured in a blast/explosion than any other mechanism (Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008). Clearly, additional efforts should focus on identifying the cognitive sequelae related to the more common cause of mild TBI in returning veterans: exposure to blast.

An important finding of this study was that participants with blast injury compared with non-blast etiologies showed a near-significant tendency to endorse a greater number of PTSD symptoms on the PCL. Various contextual and neural underpinnings may help explain why blast-related TBI victims in our sample more frequently evidenced PTSD symptomatology compared with non-blast victims. With the increasing threat of IEDs, the physiological level of arousal of these veterans would be expected to be heightened at the time when the blast event transpired (as compared with MVA where the victim is less likely to be hyperaroused pre-event). A heightened limbic arousal and multisensory inputs in the time immediately preceding the blast (“fight or flight response”) may strengthen the encoding of survival-based memories following the blast, even with momentary LOC (Glaesser et al., Reference Glaesser, Neuner, Lütgehetmann, Schmidt and Elbert2004; van der Kolk, Reference van der Kolk, van der Kolk, McFarlane and Weisaeth1996). Because most blasts in our sample resulted in a mild TBI, it stands to reason that these individuals had an increased likelihood of being able to encode memories immediately following the blast. The events following blasts are often marked by perceptions of horror, vulnerability to attack (e.g., by snipers or additional blasts), or by witnessing injury/death of others in the blast vicinity. These experiences, more so than MVA and other blunt force mechanisms, are beyond the realm of ordinary human experience.

The high prevalence of PTSD and other psychiatric disturbances secondary to the Afghanistan and Iraq wars has been well documented (e.g., Erbes et al., Reference Erbes, Westermeyer, Engdahl and Johnsen2007; Friedman, Reference Friedman2006; Hoge et al., Reference Hoge, Auchterlonie and Milliken2006, Reference Hoge, McGurk, Thomas, Cox, Engel and Castro2008; Jakupcak et al., Reference Jakupcak, Conybeare, Phelps, Hunt, Holmes, Felker, Klevins and McFall2007; Kolkow et al., Reference Kolkow, Spira, Morse and Grieger2007; Milliken et al., Reference Milliken, Auchterlonie and Hoge2007). Future studies are required to determine whether the rates of PTSD following blast-induced TBI are greater (or diminished) relative to individuals exposed to blast but without suffering TBI. With regard to PTSD symptoms, it has long been debated whether PTSD can follow TBI given the frequent period of retrograde and anterograde amnesia surrounding the traumatic event (Glaesser et al., Reference Glaesser, Neuner, Lütgehetmann, Schmidt and Elbert2004; Greenspan et al., Reference Greenspan, Stringer, Phillips, Hammond and Goldstein2006; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Harvey and Brewin2005; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Terryberry-Spohr and Hope2006; Warden et al., Reference Warden, Labbate, Salazar, Nelson, Sheley, Staudenmeier and Martin1997). In this sample, those injured via explosions were more likely to have sustained a mild TBI and presumably therefore better able to remember the traumatic event. Given the limitations of a singular self-report measure, a diagnosis of PTSD cannot be made, nor should it be inferred in interpreting these results. Rather, these findings suggest that individuals suffering from mild blast injuries tend to endorse more PTSD-related symptomology than individuals from other groups, including those suffering from mild injuries from non-blast-related events.

A number of possible hypotheses may explain the increasing PCL scores over time. First, recovery following brief LOC may be associated with recovery of memories surrounding the event. When these memories return, they may be experienced as intrusive and distressing, whether they are factually based or otherwise. Second, physiological changes (e.g., vestibular changes, hearing loss, tinnitus) may persist and trigger increased anxiety symptoms over time (e.g., increased startle response, hypervigilance). Finally, PCL scores may increase over time due to increased awareness and recognition of post-injury sequelae affecting quality of life, particularly as the combatant transitions back into civilian life. Indeed, Milliken et al. (Reference Milliken, Auchterlonie and Hoge2007) found that soldiers reported more mental health concerns several months after their return from Iraq as compared with their initial report upon returning home. Obviously, these hypotheses are speculative and require further exploration.

Several weaknesses of the current study warrant attention. By necessity, the presence of LOC and/or PTA was often based on self-report in cases of mild TBI. While careful questioning about the details surrounding the event can frequently shed light on these variables, objective data from emergency personnel (e.g., Glasgow Coma Scale scores) are obviously preferable. Furthermore, participants were recruited from different sites, possibly leading to unwanted variance across sites. Our numbers were too small and institutional settings too aligned with severity groupings (most moderate-to-severe subjects came from P/TRCs) to correct for these potential differences. A larger sample, with greater geographic diversity, may elucidate effects that were too small to detect in the current sample. Participants from Tampa were also treatment-seeking individuals evaluated under clinical protocols, whereas those from Richmond were treatment-seeking individuals who consented to participate in prospectively designed research studies and those from Salisbury and Durham were non-treatment-seeking individuals who consented to research-driven protocols.

Also, given that the blast-injured group included some patients with open-head injury and the non-blast-injured group did not, a potential confound may be present, although statistical analyses did not reveal formal differences. As this was a sample of convenience, a more controlled study will be needed to confirm our findings. Finally, TBI that is exclusively due to blast may be unusual. The force of the blast may propel the individual or nearby objects, thereby increasing the likelihood of secondary injuries (that are due to blunt force trauma rather than the pressure wave). Indeed, in the present study, 12 of 61 participants injured primarily due to blast exposure were also injured via other mechanisms. We would expect these cases to generate more cognitive sequelae relative to non-blast-injured, but this was not the case.

Overall, however, it appears that mechanism of injury does not have a significant differential effect on cognitive performance, at least on selected measures of speed/flexibility and learning/memory. It may be that while pathological changes differ within the brain due to mechanism, the behavioral manifestation of those changes is similar and varies more as a result of the extent or severity of injury to the brain. These findings are consistent with a recent study suggesting that mechanism of injury does not predict functional outcomes (Sayer et al., Reference Sayer, Chiros, Sigford, Scott, Clothier, Pickett and Lew2008). On the other hand, more physiologically based measures (like functional MRI [fMRI]) might reveal differences not apparent on neuropsychological measures, particularly in those cases with both brain injury and psychiatric sequelae (Trudeau et al., Reference Trudeau, Anderson, Hansen, Shagalov, Schmoller, Nugent and Barton1998). For instance, Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Johnston, Petrides and Ptito2008) found no differences between concussed athletes and non-concussed athletes with and without depressive symptoms on behavioral cognitive measures but did find differences in brain activation on fMRI measures. Larger studies with all cognitive domains assessed are still needed and are underway in some centers (Mid-Atlantic MIRECC). These studies are specifically addressing the functional, neuroanatomical, and cognitive correlates in blast injury. Additional research focus should include much larger sample sizes in light of the heterogeneity of TBI in the current combat theatre. In the current study, those injured via blast were more likely to have sustained mild injuries and to report PTSD-related symptoms. Given that blast-injured soldiers constitute the largest portion of TBI survivors in the current military conflicts, efforts directed at mild TBI education and therapy, as well as prevention of PTSD symptom escalation, seem warranted.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This material is the result of work supported with resources and the use of facilities at the James A. Haley Veterans’ Hospital (Tampa VA), Mid-Atlantic (VISN 6) MIRECC, the W.G. (Bill) Hefner VA Medical Center (Salisbury VA), the Hunter Holmes McGuire VA Medical Center (Richmond VA), and the Durham VA Medical Center. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs.

References

REFERENCES

Barnfield, T.V. & Leathem, J.M. (1998). Neuropsychological outcomes of traumatic brain injury and substance abuse in a New Zealand prison population. Brain Injury, 12, 951962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belanger, H.G., Curtiss, G., Demery, J.A., Lebowitz, B.K., & Vanderploeg, R.D. (2005). Factors moderating neuropsychological outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 215227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, R.D. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Binder, L.M., Rohling, M.L., & Larrabee, G.J. (1997). A review of mild head trauma. Part I: Meta-analytic review of neuropsychological studies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 421431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C.R., Kleiner, J.S., Vasterling, J.J., & Field, A.P. (2007). Memory for emotionally neutral information in posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 448463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cernak, I., Savic, J., Malicevic, Z., Zunic, G., Radosevic, P., Ivanovic, I., & Davidovic, L. (1996). Involvement of the central nervous system in the general response to pulmonary blast injury. Journal of Trauma-Injury Infection & Critical Care, 40, 100104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cernak, I., Wang, Z., Jiang, J., Bian, X., & Savic, J. (2001). Cognitive deficits following blast injury-induced neurotrauma: Possible involvement of nitric oxide. Brain Injury, 15, 593612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.-K., Johnston, K.M., Petrides, M., & Ptito, A. (2008). Neural substrates of symptoms of depression following concussion in male athletes with persisting postconcussion symptoms. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 8189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coupland, R.M. & Meddings, D.R. (1999). Mortality associated with use of weapons in armed conflicts, wartime atrocities, and civilian mass shootings: Literature review. British Medical Journal, 319, 407410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test, (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.Google Scholar
DePalma, R.G., Burris, D.G., Champion, H.R., & Hodgson, M.J. (2005). Blast injuries. New England Journal of Medicine, 352, 13351342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dikmen, S.S., Machamer, J.E., Powell, J.M., & Temkin, N.R. (2003). Outcome 3 to 5 years after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84, 14491457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dikmen, S.S., Machamer, J.E., Winn, H.R., & Temkin, N.R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 8090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsayed, N.M. (1997). Toxicology of blast overpressure. Toxicology, 121, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erbes, C., Westermeyer, J., Engdahl, B., & Johnsen, E. (2007). Post-traumatic stress disorder and service utilization in a sample of service members from Iraq and Afghanistan. Military Medicine, 172, 359363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, M.J. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder among military returnees from Afghanistan and Iraq. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 586593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaesser, J., Neuner, F., Lütgehetmann, R., Schmidt, R., & Elbert, T. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with traumatic brain injury. Bmc Psychiatry, 4, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, P. (2003). Medical symptom validity test for windows: User’s manual and program. Edmonton, Canada: Green’s Publishing.Google Scholar
Green, P., Allen, L., & Astner, K. (1996). Manual for computerized Word Memory Test. Durham, NC: CogniSystem.Google Scholar
Greenspan, A.I., Stringer, A.Y., Phillips, V.L., Hammond, F.M., & Goldstein, F.C. (2006). Symptoms of post-traumatic stress: Intrusion and avoidance 6 and 12 months after TBI. Brain Injury, 20, 733742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C.G. (1991). Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead–Reitan Battery. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Hoge, C.W., Auchterlonie, J.L., & Milliken, C.S. (2006). Mental health problems, use of mental health services, and attrition from military service after returning from deployment to Iraq or Afghanistan. JAMA, 295, 10231032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoge, C.W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J.L., Cox, A.L., Engel, C.C., & Castro, C.A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 453463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakupcak, M., Conybeare, D., Phelps, L., Hunt, S., Holmes, H.A., Felker, B., Klevins, M., & McFall, M.E. (2007). Anger, hostility, and aggression among Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans reporting PTSD and subthreshold PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 945954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, C., Harvey, A.G., & Brewin, C.R. (2005). Traumatic brain injury, dissociation, and posttraumatic stress disorder in road traffic accident survivors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 181191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaur, C., Singh, J., Lim, M.K., Ng, B.L., Yap, E.P., & Ling, E.A. (1995). The response of neurons and microglia to blast injury in the rat brain. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, 21, 369377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, T., Harrington, D.E., Adams, R.E., Anderson, T.W., Berrol, S., Cicerone, K., Dahlberg, C., Gerber, D., Goka, R., Harley, P., Hilt, J., Horn, L., Lehmkuhl, D., & Malec, J. (1993). Definition of mild traumatic brain injury: Report from the Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Committee of the Head Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group of the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 8687.Google Scholar
Kolkow, T.T., Spira, J.L., Morse, J.S., & Grieger, T.A. (2007). Post-traumatic stress disorder and depression in health care providers returning from deployment to Iraq and Afghanistan. Military Medicine, 172, 451455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayorga, M.A. (1997). The pathology of primary blast overpressure injury. Toxicology, 121, 1728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milliken, C.S., Auchterlonie, J.L., & Hoge, C.W. (2007). Longitudinal assessment of mental health problems among active and reserve component soldiers returning from the Iraq war. JAMA, 298, 21412148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millis, S.R., Rosenthal, M., Novack, T.A., Sherer, M., Nick, T.G., Kreutzer, J.S., High, W.M., & Ricker, J.H. (2001). Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 343355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, E.L., Terryberry-Spohr, L., & Hope, D.A. (2006). Mild traumatic brain injury and anxiety sequelae: A review of the literature. Brain Injury, 20, 117132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, C.K., Reynolds, J.C., Schroeder, J.M., Harrison, M.B., Evans, O.M., & Hospenthal, D.R. (2005). Spectrum of care provided at an echelon II Medical Unit during Operation Iraqi Freedom. Military Medicine, 170, 516520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okie, S. (2005). Traumatic brain injury in the war zone. New England Journal of Medicine, 352, 20432047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, B.D., Kragh, J.F., Wenke, J.C., Macaitis, J., Wade, C.E., & Holcomb, J.B. (2008). Combat wounds in Operation Iraqi Freedom and Operation Enduring Freedom. Journal of Trauma, 64, 295299.Google ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R.M. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Sayer, N.A., Chiros, C.E., Sigford, B., Scott, S., Clothier, B., Pickett, T., & Lew, H.L. (2008). Characteristics and rehabilitation outcomes among patients with blast and other injuries sustained during the Global War on Terror. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 89, 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schretlen, D.J. & Shapiro, A.M. (2003). A quantitative review of the effects of traumatic brain injury on cognitive functioning. International Review of Psychiatry, 15, 341349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, S.G., Vanderploeg, R.D., Belanger, H.G., & Scholten, J.D. (2005). Blast injuries: Evaluating and treating the postacute sequelae. Federal Practitioner, 22, 6775.Google Scholar
Taber, K.H., Warden, D.L., & Hurley, R.A. (2006). Blast-related brain injury: What is known? Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 18, 141145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trudeau, D.L., Anderson, J., Hansen, L.M., Shagalov, D.N., Schmoller, J., Nugent, S., & Barton, S. (1998). Findings of mild traumatic brain injury in combat veterans with PTSD and a history of blast concussion. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 10, 308313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Kolk, B.A. (1996). The body keeps score: Approaches to the psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In van der Kolk, B.A., McFarlane, A.C., & Weisaeth, L. (Eds.), Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society (pp. 214241). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Warden, D. (2006). Military TBI during the Iraq and Afghanistan wars. Journal of Head Trauma and Rehabilitation, 21, 398402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warden, D.L., Labbate, L.A., Salazar, A.M., Nelson, R., Sheley, E., Staudenmeier, J., & Martin, E. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with traumatic brain injury and amnesia for the event? Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 9, 1822.Google ScholarPubMed
Warden, D.L., Ryan, L.M., Helmick, K.M., Schwab, K., French, L., Lu, W., Lux, W., Ling, G., & Ecklund, J. (2005). War neurotrauma: The Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center (DVBIC) experience at Walter Reed Army Medical Center (WRAMC). Journal of Neurotrauma, 22, 1178.Google Scholar
Weathers, F.W., Huska, J.A., & Keane, T.M. (1991). PCL-C for DSM-IV. Boston: National Center for PTSD—Behavioral Science Division.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Administration and Scoring Manual (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and injury information

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographic variables by TBI severity

Figure 2

Table 3. Demographic variables by TBI etiology

Figure 3

Table 4. Cognitive variables by TBI severity and etiology group