Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:13:55.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural Correlates of Interference Control in Adolescents with Traumatic Brain Injury: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Counting Stroop Task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2010

Sarah J. Tlustos
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio
C.-Y. Peter Chiu*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio
Nicolay Chertkoff Walz
Affiliation:
Division of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Department of Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio
Scott K. Holland
Affiliation:
University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Research Foundation, Department of Radiology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center
Lori Bernard
Affiliation:
Division of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio
Shari L. Wade
Affiliation:
University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio Division of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Peter Chiu, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, 101A Dyer Hall, ML 0376, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0376. E-mail: peter.chiu@uc.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Difficulty in inhibition or cognitive control is a common and significant sequela of pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI). The present study used functional MRI to examine one specific inhibitory function, interference control, in 11 adolescents, aged 12–16 years, (mean age, 15.7 years) with TBI who were at least 1 year postinjury and 11 age-matched typically developing control participants (TC) (mean age, 15.2 years). Participants completed a Counting Stroop task with 2 main conditions: (1) a neutral condition requiring the counting of animal words and (2) an interference condition in which mismatched number words were counted. Both TBI and TC adolescents activated similar networks of brain regions relevant to interference control, but the TBI group showed higher levels of activation relative to the TC group in multiple brain areas within this network, including predominantly right frontal and parietal regions. Findings of greater activation of the relevant neural network in the TBI group are consistent with recent fMRI findings using other interference control paradigms with individuals with a history of TBI. (JINS, 2011, 17, 000–000.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is one of the most common causes of acquired disability in childhood and adolescence (Keenan & Bratton, Reference Keenan and Bratton2006), with the highest peak in incidence of TBI occurring during the adolescent and young adult years (Langlois, Rutland-Brown, & Thomas, Reference Langlois, Rutland-Brown and Thomas2006). Adolescence also constitutes a critical time for brain development (Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, Reference Casey, Tottenham, Liston and Durston2005) and an important transitional period during which the child is expected to assume increasing responsibility for self-management (Crosnoe & Trinitapoli, Reference Crosnoe and Trinitapoli2008; Wigfield & Eccles, Reference Wigfield and Eccles1994). Deficits in executive functions (EF) are commonly observed in pediatric TBI (Max et al., Reference Max, Schachar, Levin, Ewing-Cobbs, Chapman, Dennis and Landis2005a, Reference Max, Schachar, Levin, Ewing-Cobbs, Chapman, Dennis and Landis2005b; Max, Manes, et al., Reference Max, Manes, Robertson, Mathews, Fox and Lancaster2005). In general, children with severe TBI display poorer performance on EF measures, including attention, working memory, behavioral inhibition, and cognitive control, than less-severely injured or healthy control children (e.g., Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005; Anderson, Fenwick, Manly, & Robertson, Reference Anderson, Fenwick, Manly and Robertson1998; Catroppa & Anderson, Reference Catroppa and Anderson2005; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Fletcher, Levin, Miner and Eisenberg1998; Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Armstrong, Janusz, Taylor, Wade, Stancin and Drotar2005). Deficits in EF may help explain the discrepancy between seemingly normal performance on many standard non-EF neurocognitive measures (as in areas of verbal and non-verbal abilities), despite observed impairments in everyday social and academic functioning (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005; Catroppa & Anderson, Reference Catroppa and Anderson2003; Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer, & DeLeon, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeon2004). Intact EF skills play an important role in interpersonal, school, and vocational success (Levin & Hanten, Reference Levin and Hanten2005; Pennington, Reference Pennington1994; Schachar, Levin, Max, Purvis, & Chen, Reference Schachar, Levin, Max, Purvis and Chen2004).

One EF domain in which children and adolescents with TBI have shown deficits concerns inhibitory or interference control. Whereas cognitive control has been used to refer broadly to the ability to select, sustain, filter, or optimize information processing (e.g., Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, Reference Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz and Carter2004), regulate and reconcile our goals and actions (e.g., Aarts, Roelofs, & van Turennout, Reference Aarts, Roelofs and van Turennout2008), and monitor and resolve conflicts (e.g., Posner & Rothbart, Reference Posner and Rothbart2007), interference control refers to the specific ability to select information from competing sources to minimize interference or conflict (e.g., Aarts et al., Reference Aarts, Roelofs and van Turennout2008; Fan, Flombaum, McCandliss, Thomas, & Posner, Reference Fan, Flombaum, McCandliss, Thomas and Posner2003; Levin, Hanten, & Li, Reference Levin, Hanten and Li2009). Thus, a better understanding of the neural substrates of interference control has the potential to inform our understanding of one of the core deficits arising from TBI in childhood and adolescence.

Extant studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine EF and TBI have focused largely on adults (e.g., Chen, Johnston, Collie, McCroy, & Ptito, Reference Chen, Johnston, Collie, McCroy and Ptito2007; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Mannell, Ling, Elgie, Gasparovic, Phillips and Yeo2009; McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Saykin, Flashman, Sparling, Johnson, Guerin and Yanofsky1999, Reference McAllister, Sparling, Flashman, Guerin, Mamourian and Saykin2001; Newsome, Scheibel, Steinberg, et al., Reference Newsome, Scheibel, Steinberg, Troyanskaya, Sharma, Rauch and Levin2007; Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009; Smits et al., Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009), with many focusing on working memory as a domain of interest. A few fMRI studies have examined attention and working memory functions in pediatric TBI but did not focus on interference control (e.g., Kramer, Chiu, Shear, & Wade, Reference Kramer, Chiu, Shear and Wade2009; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Chiu, Walz, Holland, Yuan, Karunanayaka and Wade2008; Newsome, Scheibel, Hunter, et al., Reference Newsome, Scheibel, Hunter, Wang, Chu, Li and Levin2007; Newsome et al., Reference Newsome, Steinberg, Scheibel, Troyanskaya, Chu, Hanten and Levin2008). Scheibel and colleagues (Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009) investigated interference control in adults with TBI at 3 months post-injury using a stimulus response compatibility task. In this task, participants pressed keys on the side indicated by the direction of arrows on the screen (neutral) or on the side opposite to that indicated by the arrows (interference). These authors reported higher levels of inhibition-related activation (i.e., incompatible minus compatible) in adults with TBI relative to adults with orthopedic injuries (OI) across an extensive range of brain regions including the left precentral gyrus, midline cingulate region, medial frontal, middle frontal, and superior frontal gyri bilaterally. Participants with more severe TBI had higher levels of activation than those with milder injuries or OI. Higher levels of activation were reported to be associated with higher levels of task performance in the incompatible condition in participants with TBI (e.g., Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009). Smits and colleagues (Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009) studied interference control in healthy controls and in adult patients (age 18–50) who suffered minor head injuries, as defined by GCS scores of 13–15 with accompanying post-concussive symptoms, approximately 1 month post-injury using the Counting Stroop task developed by Bush and colleagues (Reference Bush, Whalen, Rosen, Jenike, McInerney and Rauch1998, Reference Bush, Frazier, Rauch, Seidman, Whalen, Jenike and Biederman1999). In this task, participants were shown several words on screen and asked to count the number of words and respond in two critical conditions. The words were non-number words (e.g., animal names) in the Neutral condition, whereas in the Interference condition, there were number names that mismatched the number of words on screen (e.g., “four four” or “two two two two”). For the Interference minus Neutral contrast, Smits and colleagues observed overall activation broadly in lateral prefrontal areas (including superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyrus) with rightward bias, medial superior frontal gyrus, right inferior and superior parietal lobules, and other posterior parietal areas. In addition, activation levels in the insula, inferior frontal gyrus, pre- and post-central gyrus on the left, as well as posterior parietal cortex, anterior cingulate, and posterior cingulate bilaterally were associated with the number of self-reported post-concussion symptoms (with control = 0 symptoms), with higher activation levels corresponding to greater numbers of post-concussion symptoms.

The current study sought to examine the neural substrates associated with interference control in adolescents with complicated mild to severe TBI relative to a cohort of age and sex-matched healthy controls. Consistent with Scheibel and colleagues (Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007), we hypothesized that adolescents with and without TBI would activate similar frontal and parietal regions when engaging interference control processes during the Interference condition to a greater extent than during the Neutral condition, but that the adolescents with TBI would demonstrate greater activation of brain regions supporting interference control compared to controls.

Methods

Participants

Participants with TBI, who were in ongoing behavioral intervention studies, were identified as eligible for participation based on current age and time since injury. All adolescent participants with TBI were required to have sustained a TBI requiring overnight hospitalization with a lowest Glasgow Coma Scale score of ⩽12, or a GCS of 13–15 accompanied by abnormalities on imaging. Functional imaging was conducted ⩾12 months post-injury because we were interested in the longer-term effects of TBI on developing brain networks during adolescence. Eighteen consented to be enrolled in the study, but 1 was unable to be scheduled during the period of the study, 1 declined to complete the scan, 3 were excluded due to metal in their bodies, and 2 were excluded due to excessive motion (>3 mm, see fMRI method below), leaving a total of 11 participants (mean age, 15.7 years; range, 12–16 years) with usable fMRI data. A typically developing comparison cohort of 15 adolescents (TC group) matched with the TBI group on sex, handedness, and race/ethnicity, were recruited through an institutional review board-approved electronic notice to employees of a large, urban hospital. Individuals who expressed interest in participation were screened for eligibility and matched to the adolescents with TBI until 15 eligible healthy adolescents consented to participate. Of this group of 15, 2 had excessive motion during the fMRI session under the same criterion, 1 declined to finish a scan, and Counting Stroop data for 1 participant was lost due to equipment malfunction, leaving 11 participants (mean age, 15.2 years; range, 12–16 years), with usable fMRI data. Eligibility criteria for both groups included English as the primary language spoken in the home. Adolescents were excluded if they had a diagnosis of significant developmental disability (i.e., placement in a special classroom and IQ < 70), significant psychiatric/behavioral disturbances (e.g., bipolar, major depression, autism), or extreme vision and hearing deficits. Additional exclusion criteria for the control group included any prior diagnosis of a neurological disorder or any indications of a history of TBI. Both the intervention project and the current imaging study were approved by the Institutional Review Board. Thus, a total of 33 participants completed informed consent to participate in the study, 28 were assessed and provided behavioral data, and 22 (67%) yielded behavioral and usable fMRI data. Those with usable fMRI data did not differ from others who were excluded in terms of age, IQ, injury severity, or self- or parent-report of EF skills.

Procedures

After informed consent was obtained, adolescents completed a brief neuropsychological battery including an abbreviated assessment of intelligence and measures of various aspects of EF. Participants then completed several fMRI tasks including the Counting Stroop, described in the present report.

Cognitive and behavioral functioning

Receptive vocabulary was assessed using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth Edition (PPVT-IV, Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn2007). Single word reading skills, which have been used as a proxy for pre-injury cognitive status (Orme, Johnstone, Hanks, & Novack, Reference Orme, Johnstone, Hanks and Novack2004), were measured using the Wide Range Achievement Test-Fourth Edition (WRAT-4, Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006). The Working Memory (WM) and Processing Speed (PS) Indices from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV, Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2003) were administered to assess domains of EF known to be vulnerable to TBI. Interference control was assessed using the Color-Word Interference Test of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS, Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001). In this Stroop-like task, the participant is presented with a color word that is printed in a font color that is different from the word and asked to name the color of the font. Parents and adolescents completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF, Gioia, Isquith, & Guy, Reference Gioia, Isquith and Guy2000) and BRIEF-self report (BRIEF-SR, Guy, Isquith, & Gioia, Reference Guy, Isquith and Gioia2004), respectively.

Stimuli and behavioral tasks used for fMRI

In the variant of the Counting Stroop Task used in the present study (cf. Bush et al., Reference Bush, Whalen, Rosen, Jenike, McInerney and Rauch1998, Reference Bush, Frazier, Rauch, Seidman, Whalen, Jenike and Biederman1999), one to four identical words were presented in a vertical column on the screen on each trial of 1.5 s. Participants were asked to note the number of words that appeared on the screen and press one of four corresponding response buttons on a button box. For example, if the word appeared four times on the screen, the correct response would be “4.” The Interference condition contained all number words (i.e., “one,” “two,” “three,” or “four”), with each appearing 2 to 5 times in each block in a random manner. The Neutral condition contained names of common animals (i.e., “dog,” “cat,” “mouse,” or “bird”), again each appearing 2 to 5 times in each block in a random manner. The two conditions were balanced for the number and length of the words. There were fourteen trials in each of the two conditions. Additionally, we also included a third, baseline fixation condition. In this condition, participants were asked to monitor a row of “+” signs presented at the center of the screen, and the number of “+” signs on screen changed every 2 s (randomly from two to six “+” signs). The baseline condition was included for the following reasons. First, if the interference control comparison (Interference minus Neutral) turned out to be non-significant or sub-threshold in either group, a baseline condition may allow us to check whether the effect was genuine or whether it was due to equipment failure, subject non-compliance, or the presence of other artifacts. Second, if a group difference in the interference control comparison was observed (as in Smits et al., Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009), it might have been difficult to interpret, without a baseline condition, because this between-group difference might have been due to different levels of activation in the Neutral condition (relative to some baseline state) across groups. Thus, a baseline condition was selected that would engage attention to stimuli and interference control as little as possible to accentuate its difference from the Neutral and Interference conditions. Participants were given one example of each condition during a pre-scan training session, and all indicated understanding of the instructions. No further training was provided. Video and audio monitoring were used to observe whether there were any problems during the administration of the task and track whether participants appeared to be responding appropriately. Stimulus administration and response logging was accomplished using the experimental software E-Prime with magnet compatible goggles and a response system.

fMRI data acquisition and analyses

MR scanning was performed on a 3 Tesla Philips Achieva MR scanner. A T2*-weighted, gradient-echo EPI sequence was used for fMRI scans (repetition time/echo time [TR/TE] = 2000/30 ms, field of view [FOV] = 240 × 240 mm, matrix = 80 × 80, slice thickness = 4 mm, flip angle = 90°). Forty-one slices were acquired at 210 time points for a total imaging time of 432 s, beginning with a 12 s (6 volumes) fixation period to allow for T1 relaxation effects that were subsequently discarded before statistical post-processing of the fMRI data. Functional imaging time consisted of six cycles, each 66-s long with fixation periods of 12 s and condition periods of 21 s alternating with one another (i.e., fixation-neutral-fixation-interference). A T1-weighted, three-dimensional (3D) MPRAGE whole brain scan was also obtained for anatomical co-registration (TR/TE = 8.2/3.7 ms, FOV = 25 cm × 25 cm, matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1 mm, scan time = 355 s) before the functional scans.

Following Schmithorst and Holland (Reference Schmithorst and Holland2007), fMRI image post-processing was accomplished using in-house software written in IDL™ (ITT visual information solutions). The reconstructed EPI data were corrected for drift using quadratic baseline correction on a pixel-by-pixel basis (Hu, Le, Parrish, & Erhard, Reference Hu, Le, Parrish and Erhard1995; Le & Hu, Reference Le and Hu1996), co-registered to the reference volume to reduce the effects of motion artifacts using a pyramid iterative algorithm (Thevenaz, Ruttimann, & Unser, Reference Thevenaz, Ruttimann and Unser1998), and transformed into Talairach coordinates using landmarks (anterior commissure, posterior commissure, inter-hemispheric plane, and bounding volume) obtained from the T1-weighted anatomic images (Talairach & Tournoux, Reference Talairach and Tournoux1988) using a linear affine transformation shown previously to be valid for individuals 5 to 18 years of age (Muzik & Chugani, Reference Muzik and Chugani2000; Wilke, Schmithorst, & Holland, Reference Wilke, Schmithorst and Holland2002). During the co-registration procedure, which produced one point-estimate of movement in 3-D space per time point, excessive motion was defined as a median voxel displacement of 3 mm. The fMRI data from four participants were deemed to show excessive motion for > 10% of the EPI data set from Counting Stroop and were subsequently discarded. This cutoff was selected to allow for inclusion of as many data sets as possible, while excluding poor data that were unusable even after motion correction. Once subjects who exceeded criteria for excessive movement were excluded, the TBI and TC group did not differ in mean movement, median movement, and proportion of time points where movement exceeded 3 mm, all ps > .11.

For each participant, Pearson’s partial correlation analyses were computed on a voxel-wise basis between EPI data and task reference functions corresponding to the Neutral (21 s), Interference (21 s), and Control (12 + 12 = 24 s) conditions, using the motion correction parameter as a covariate. All task reference functions were box-car convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. A 6-s delay was applied to the reference function to allow for the canonical hemodynamic response to peak. Correlation coefficients were then transformed into Z-score maps using Fisher’s Z-transformation. Group level random effects analyses were performed on these Z-maps from individual participants in the context of the general linear model (GLM). A post-processing filter (full-width, half maximum [FWHM] = 8 mm) was then applied before significant regions of activation on a voxel-by-voxel basis were identified (Worsley & Friston, Reference Worsley and Friston1995), generating statistical parameter maps. A clustering threshold of 100 contiguous voxels (corresponding to 3600 mL of brain tissue in the Talairach space) was used unless otherwise stated (Xiong, Gao, Lancaster, & Fox, Reference Xiong, Gao, Lancaster and Fox1995) to improve visualization of the parameter maps and to reduce the severity of the corrections that were made for multiple comparisons. Based on the method of Ledberg, Akerman, and Roland (Reference Ledberg, Akerman and Roland1998), Monte Carlo simulations were used to estimate corrected p values and performed in the following manner. The spatial autocorrelations present in the fit residuals were used to estimate the intrinsic smoothness in the data. “Null” activation maps were generated from spatially auto-correlated Gaussian noise generated using the previously found smoothness estimates and post-processing parameters (e.g., threshold intensity, cluster size, and exogenous spatial filtering). The simulations were repeated, and the corrected p values estimated by computing the proportion of null maps with spurious activated clusters detected. Monte-Carlo simulation was performed to assure p < .001 after adjusting for multiple comparisons. For each cluster, the Talairach coordinates of the pixel that showed the maximum Z-value before filtering (i.e., the maxima) is reported here.

Results

Group Comparisons on Demographic, Cognitive, and Behavioral Data

Table 1 provides the demographic, cognitive, and behavioral data for the 22 adolescents with usable fMRI data. The mean Glasgow Coma Scale score for the TBI group was 12.6 (SD = 3.6). Eight of 11 children had complicated mild TBI characterized by GCS scores of 13–15 with abnormal imaging. The other three children (1 severe) had GCS scores of <13 with no abnormalities on clinical CT or MRI at the time of injury. The average time since injury was 1.8 years (SD = 0.45). The groups were comparable with respect to age, family income, race/ethnicity, sex, and handedness. Adolescents in the TC group showed numerically better performance on the WRAT and PPVT than the adolescents with TBI (with effect sizes indexed by Cohen’s d = 0.68 and 0.41, respectively), but this difference did not reach statistical significance (both ps > .10). There were also no significant group differences on the WISC PSI and WMI, although effect size for WISC WMI (Cohen’s d = 0.68) was in the medium range. Groups were also comparable on the D-KEFS Color-Word Inhibit task. Adolescents with TBI had significantly higher levels of parent-rated executive dysfunction (i.e., on the BRIEF) than did the TC group, although means in both groups were within the normal range. Parents of adolescents with TBI rated their children as having significantly greater difficulties with shifting from one task to another, behavior regulation, and meta-cognition than the TC group, all ps < .05.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological performance by group

Note. TBI = Traumatic Brain Injuries; TC = Typically Developing Controls; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Version 4; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test 4th Edition; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th Edition; PSI = Processing Speed Index; WMI = Working Memory Index; BRIEF = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC = Global Executive Composite; SR = Self Report; BRI = Behavior Regulation Index; MI = Metacognition Index DKEFS CW = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Color Word Interference Task; RT = median reaction time.

*Only participants who completed all tests and contributed useable functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data are included here.

Counting Stroop: Behavioral and fMRI results

Performance data for the Counting Stroop task were recorded during the fMRI scan. All but one participant showed higher accuracy and faster median reaction time in the Neutral condition than the Interference condition. Mean accuracy for participants with TBI was not significantly lower than that for participants in the TC group, all ps > .11. Median reaction time (RT) calculated for each participant, excluding data from all error trials, showed that adolescents with TBI were slower than those in the TC group in the Neutral condition, t(20) = 2.08, p = .05, but not in the Interference condition, t(20) = 1.59, p > .13. Focusing on interference control, defined as the difference between the Neutral and the Interference conditions, the two groups were well matched and not different in RT Difference (79 ms for the TBI group and 86 ms for the TC group) or in Accuracy Difference (10.4% for the TBI group and 9.3% for the TC group), both ps > .7 and Cohen’s d < 0.17. Note that the magnitude of the interference control effects in RT and accuracy were numerically larger than has been demonstrated in studies of adults (range of RT Diff. = 29 to 46 +ms, see Bush et al., Reference Bush, Shin, Holmes, Rosen and Vogt2003; range of Accuracy Diff. = 0 to 3%, see Bush et al., Reference Bush, Whalen, Rosen, Jenike, McInerney and Rauch1998, Reference Bush, Frazier, Rauch, Seidman, Whalen, Jenike and Biederman1999).

Figure 1 presents the composite Z-score map of brain regions that were significantly more active during the Interference condition compared to the Neutral condition in the entire sample of 22 participants. Predominantly bilateral activation was seen in broad extent of the brain. These include dorsal as well as ventrolateral prefrontal cortices, anterior cingulate, posterior parietal cortex, inferior and middle temporal gyri, lingual and fusiform gyrus, cerebellum, and other occipital regions. Group-related differences in brain activity were computed on a voxel-wise basis and are displayed in Figure 2 with the Talairach coordinate of the centroids of significant differences listed in Table 2. Participants in the TBI group had higher levels of interference-control related activation relative to the TC group in frontal and parietal areas including the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG, Brodmann area [BA] 6), medial frontal areas including dorsal anterior cingulate (BA 32) and frontal cortex (BA 8), and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9). Activation differences were also seen in the right parietal areas including the superior (BA 7) and inferior (BA 40) parietal lobule. There were no areas in which participants in the TC group demonstrated greater inhibition-related activation than participants in the TBI group.

Fig. 1 Brain activation map for the Interference Control comparison across the entire group of participants. Only positive activation foci (Interference > Neutral) are shown here. Images are horizontal slices 4 mm apart and start at Z = –29 mm (top left) to Z = +63 mm (bottom right). Images are in radiological convention: left side of the images corresponds to the right hemisphere. Image parameters are as follows: nominal Z threshold = 10.0, cluster size = 100, corrected p < .001 for multiple comparisons.

Fig. 2 Group difference map in the Interference Control comparison. The traumatic brain injury (TBI) group had significantly higher levels of activation in multiple brain regions relative to the typically developing control participants (TC) group (warm color). The TC group had no regions with higher levels of activation than the TBI group. Nominal Z threshold = 8.0, cluster size = 100, corrected p < .001 for multiple comparisons.

Table 2 Regions of interest (ROIs) showing significantly different levels of activation between adolescents with TBI (TBI) and typically developing controls (TC) in the interference minus neutral contrast during the counting stroop task

Given that the imaging findings showed that the TBI group had a larger difference in the Interference minus Neutral contrast than the TC group, whereas the groups were largely comparable on measures (both RT diff. and Accuracy diff) indexing interference control (Interference minus Neutral) on the Stroop task, it is possible that the between-group difference in imaging might have arisen from between-group differences in the Neutral condition (relative to baseline). We, therefore, repeated the above analyses with the Neutral minus Fixation Baseline contrast. At the same nominal thresholds (i.e., Z = 8.0, cluster size = 100), no activation differences between groups were detected anywhere. Only when the nominal threshold was made much less stringent (i.e., to Z = 4.0, cluster size = 50) was there a trend toward a larger Neutral minus Baseline contrast in the TC group than the TBI group, but the few scattered foci were found around the left parietal areas (BA 7 and 40), the cerebellum, and left dorsolateral prefrontal areas; none of these areas overlapped with the ones identified in Figure 2. At this more lenient threshold, there were no areas in which the TBI group showed a larger Neutral minus Baseline difference than the TC group. Thus, imaging data did not provide support for the notion that group differences in the interference control contrast was due solely to differences in the Neutral minus Baseline Fixation contrast.

Discussion

The findings from this study are consistent with our hypotheses that adolescents with TBI would activate similar networks of brain regions during the Counting Stroop task relative to typically developing controls, but would demonstrate stronger activation of brain regions supporting inhibition. The regions with higher levels of activation in the TBI group are in the frontal and parietal cortices (including BA 6, 8, 32, 7, 9, & 40) and right-lateralized and belong to the overall neural network that supports interference control more broadly (Figure 1). Our findings are consistent with extant studies that observed higher levels of activation in adults with TBI relative to controls (either healthy adults or adults with less severe TBI) when they engage in interference control in similar frontal and parietal areas (Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009; Smits et al., Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009).

It is particularly noteworthy that this altered neural activation pattern persists in our sample of adolescents with TBI despite their participation in ongoing behavioral intervention, relatively normal level of performance on the interference control task, and at least 12 months post-injury. Whether the observation of increased activation is specific to adolescents with complicated mild TBI remains to be determined, since the adults with TBI with GCS in the mild range in Smits and colleagues (Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009) were studied only 1-month post-injury. It should also be noted that other fMRI studies in adults with TBI have reported a more complicated relationship between levels of brain activation and injury status or severity that may vary depending on task difficulty or time since injury. For example, fMRI studies have also reported higher or lower levels (or even absence) of activation in brain regions in adults with TBI relative to controls in working memory tasks that depended for instance on working memory load (e.g., Chen et al., Reference Chen, Johnston, Frey, Petrides, Worsley and Ptito2004; McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Saykin, Flashman, Sparling, Johnson, Guerin and Yanofsky1999, Reference McAllister, Sparling, Flashman, Guerin, Mamourian and Saykin2001). In addition, brain activation in right superior frontal areas initially showed a reduced level of activation in adults with TBI compared to healthy controls, but activation increased in this area over a 6-month period such that the between-group differences were much reduced (e.g., Sanchez-Carrion et al., Reference Sanchez-Carrion, Fernandez-Espejo, Junque, Falcon, Bargallo, Roig and Vendrell2008).

The current study demonstrates the feasibility of assessing interference control with fMRI in adolescents following TBI using healthy adolescents as controls. The adolescents with TBI successfully completed the scanning protocol and were largely equivalent to healthy controls on task performance with minimal training before scanning, as well as on most demographic measures. In conjunction with other studies using healthy controls (e.g., Smits et al., Reference Smits, Dippel, Houston, Wielopolski, Koudstaal, Hunink and van der Lugt2009), the results provide preliminary evidence that the altered pattern of brain activation in individuals with TBI may not be limited to cases where OI participants were used as a comparison standard (e.g., Scheibel et al., Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao and Levin2007, Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Troyanskaya, Steinberg, Goldstein, Mao and Levin2009). In the current study, we focused on examining interference control in adolescents with TBI within a relatively restricted range of age and IQ that was generally comparable to the healthy controls. Future investigations using a more diverse sample with a broad GCS range could also examine the relationship between GCS and brain activation in children as has been done for adults with TBI.

Care must be taken in generalizing from the current findings given the small and heterogeneous nature of the sample of adolescents with TBI. All but three children with TBI had injuries that would be termed complicated mild. Moreover, although most measures of cognitive performance fell within the average range overall, they exhibit considerable variability across participants, which was not directly examined in the current study. The effect size estimates (Cohen’s d) for some neuropsychological and behavioral measures, including estimates of overall cognitive ability, were indicated in the “moderate” to “large” range numerically, which suggests that statistically significant differences between groups may emerge with a larger sample size. The small sample size and the issue of insufficient statistical power clearly limit the generalizability of results from the present preliminary study to the broader population.

Future research is needed to determine the neural substrates of inhibition and cognitive control in children with more significant cognitive and behavioral impairment following TBI. Additionally, the current study examined only one aspect of inhibition at a single point in time during the chronic phase of recovery. Additional studies will be required to understand the neural substrates of other aspects of inhibition and related executive functions, whether the process of recovery results in neural remodeling over time, and how neural functioning relates to functional outcomes. Nonetheless, this study provides important new information about neural differences in the network subserving interference control and inhibition following TBI in adolescence.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by (1) NIH grant RO1-MH073764 from the National Institute of Mental Health; (2) H133G050239 from the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research in the Department of Education, and (3) EMS/Trauma grant from the Ohio Department of Public Safety. The information in this manuscript is an original contribution to the literature and has not previously been published, electronically or in print. The authors certify that we know of no conflict of interest, and we adhere to the ethical guidelines of the Helsinki Declaration. We thank three anonymous reviewers for providing numerous helpful comments and suggestions for improving the manuscript. The order of the first two authors was determined by a coin toss.

References

REFERENCES

Aarts, E., Roelofs, A., van Turennout, M. (2008). Anticipatory activity in anterior cingulate cortex can be independent of conflict and error likelihood. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 46714678. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4400-07.2008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., Rosenfeld, J. (2005). Attentional and processing skills following traumatic brain injury in early childhood. Brain Injury, 19, 699710. doi:10.1080/02699050400025281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V., Fenwick, T., Manly, T., Robertson, I. (1998). Attentional skills following traumatic brain injury in childhood: A componential analysis. Brain Injury, 12, 937949. doi:10.1080/026990598121990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Frazier, J.A., Rauch, S.L., Seidman, L.J., Whalen, P.J., Jenike, M.A., Biederman, J. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting stroop. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 15421552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Shin, L.M., Holmes, J., Rosen, B.R., Vogt, B.A. (2003). The multi-source interference task: A validation study with fMRI in individual subjects. Molecular Psychiatry, 8, 6070. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Whalen, P.J., Rosen, B.R., Jenike, M.A., McInerney, S.C., Rauch, S.L. (1998). The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging – validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270282. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:4<270::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-03.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B.J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 104110. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catroppa, C., Anderson, V. (2003). Children’s attentional skills 2 years post-traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 359373. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2303_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catroppa, C., Anderson, V. (2005). A prospective study of the recovery of attention from acute to 2 years following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 8498. doi:10.1017/S1355617705050101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnston, K.M., Collie, A., McCroy, P., Ptito, A. (2007). A validation of the post concussion symptom scale in the assessment of complex concussion using cognitive testing and functional MRI. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78, 12311238. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2006.110395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnston, K.M., Frey, S., Petrides, M., Worsley, K., Ptito, A. (2004). Functional abnormalities in symptomatic concussed athletes: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 22, 6882. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crosnoe, R., Trinitapoli, J. (2008). Shared family activities and the transition from childhood into adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 2348. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00549.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT4) (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
E-Prime [Computer software]. Sharpsburg, PA: Psychological Software Tools, Inc.Google Scholar
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M., Fletcher, J.M., Levin, H.S., Miner, M.E., Eisenberg, H.M. (1998). Attention after pediatric traumatic brain injury: A multidimensional assessment. Child Neuropsychology, 4, 3548. doi:10.1076/chin.4.1.35.3194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M.R., Landry, S.H., Kramer, L., DeLeon, R. (2004). Executive functions following traumatic brain injury in young children: A preliminary analysis. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 487512. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2601_7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Flombaum, J.I., McCandliss, B.D., Thomas, K.M., Posner, M.I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage, 18, 4257. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C. (2000). Behavior rating of executive function (BRIEF). Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guy, S.C., Isquith, P.K., Gioia, G.A. (2004). BRIEF-SR: Behavior rating inventory of executive function--Self-report version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Hu, X., Le, T.H., Parrish, T., Erhard, P. (1995). Retrospective estimation and correction of physiological fluctuation in functional MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34, 201212. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, H.T., Bratton, S.L. (2006). Epidemiology and outcomes of pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuroscience, 28, 256263. doi:10.1159/000094152CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, M.E., Chiu, C.Y., Shear, P.K., Wade, S.L. (2009). Neural correlates of verbal associative memory and mnemonic strategy use following childhood traumatic brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Rehabilitation Medicine, 2, 255271. doi:10.3233/PRM-2009-0091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, M.E., Chiu, C.Y., Walz, N.C., Holland, S.K., Yuan, W., Karunanayaka, P., Wade, S.L. (2008). Long-term neural processing of attention following early childhood traumatic brain injury: fMRI and neurobehavioral outcomes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 424435. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langlois, J.A., Rutland-Brown, W., Thomas, K.E. (2006). Traumatic brain injury in the United States: Emergency department visits, hospitalizations, and deaths. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Retrieved from CDC website: www.cdc.gov/ncipc/pub-res/tbi../tbi%20in%20the%20us_jan_2006.pdfGoogle Scholar
Le, T.H., Hu, X. (1996). Retrospective estimation and correction of physiological artifacts in fMRI by direct extraction of physiological activity from MR data. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 35, 290298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ledberg, A., Akerman, S., Roland, P.E. (1998). Estimation of the probabilities of 3D clusters in functional brain images. Neuroimage, 8, 113128. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, H.S., Hanten, G. (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatric Neurology, 33, 7993. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, H.S., Hanten, G., Li, X. (2009). The relation of cognitive control to social outcome after paediatric TBI: Implications for intervention. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 320329. doi:10.3109/17518420903087673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Max, J.E., Manes, F.F., Robertson, B.A.M., Mathews, K., Fox, P.T., Lancaster, J. (2005). Prefrontal and executive attention network lesions and the development of attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptomatology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 443450. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000156661.38576.0fCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Max, J.E., Schachar, R.J., Levin, H.S., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Chapman, S.B., Dennis, M., Landis, J. (2005a). Predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder within 6 months after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 10321040. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000173293. 05817.b1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Max, J.E., Schachar, R.J., Levin, H.S., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Chapman, S.B., Dennis, M., Landis, J. (2005b). Predictors of secondary attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents 6 to 24 months after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 10411049. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000173292.05817.f8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, A.R., Mannell, M.V., Ling, J., Elgie, R., Gasparovic, C., Phillips, J.P., Yeo, R.A. (2009). Auditory orienting and inhibition of return in mild traumatic brain injury: A fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 41524166. doi:10.1002/hbm.20836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T.W., Saykin, A.J., Flashman, L.A., Sparling, M.B., Johnson, S.C., Guerin, S.J., Yanofsky, N. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain injury: A functional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 13001308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T.W., Sparling, M.B., Flashman, L.A., Guerin, S.J., Mamourian, A.C., Saykin, A.J. (2001). Differential working memory load effects after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage, 14, 10041012. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0899CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muzik, O., Chugani, D.C. (2000). Statistical parametric mapping: Assessment of application in children. Neuroimage, 12, 538549. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newsome, M.R., Scheibel, R.S., Hunter, J.V., Wang, Z.J., Chu, Z., Li, X., Levin, H.S. (2007). Brain activation during working memory after traumatic brain injury in children. Neurocase, 13, 1624. doi:10.1080/13554790601186629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newsome, M.R., Scheibel, R.S., Steinberg, J.L., Troyanskaya, M., Sharma, R.G., Rauch, R.A., Levin, H.S. (2007). Working memory brain activation following severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 43, 95111. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70448-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newsome, M.R., Steinberg, J.L., Scheibel, R.S., Troyanskaya, M., Chu, Z., Hanten, G., Levin, H.S. (2008). Effects of traumatic brain injury on working memory-related brain activation in adolescents. Neuropsychology, 22, 419425. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orme, D.R., Johnstone, B., Hanks, R., Novack, T. (2004). The WRAT-3 reading subtest as a measure of premorbid intelligence among persons with brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 49, 250253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B.F. (1994). The working memory function of the prefrontal cortices: Implications for developmental and individual differences in cognition. In M.M. Haith, J. Benson, R.J. Roberts, & B.F. Pennington (Eds.), Future oriented processes in development (pp. 243289). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 123. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K.R., van den Wildenberg, W.P.M., Segalowitz, S.J., Carter, C.S. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of the prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain and Cognition, 56, 129140. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Carrion, R., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., Falcon, C., Bargallo, N., Roig, T., Vendrell, P. (2008). A longitudinal fMRI study of working memory in severe TBI patients with diffuse axonal injury. Neuroimage, 43, 421429. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schachar, R., Levin, H.S., Max, J.E., Purvis, K., Chen, S. (2004). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and response inhibition after closed head injury in children: Do preinjury behavior and injury severity predict outcome? Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 179198. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2501&2_10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheibel, R.S., Newsome, M.R., Steinberg, J.L., Pearson, D.A., Rauch, R.A., Mao, H., Levin, H.S. (2007). Altered brain activation during cognitive control in patients with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 21, 3645. doi:10.1177/1545968306294730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheibel, R.S., Newsome, M.R., Troyanskaya, M., Steinberg, J.L., Goldstein, F.C., Mao, H., Levin, H.S. (2009). Effects of severity of traumatic brain injury and brain reserve on cognitive-control related brain activation. Journal of Neurotrauma, 26, 14471461. doi:10.1089/neu.2008.0736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmithorst, V.J., Holland, S.K. (2007). Sex differences in the development of neuroanatomical functional connectivity underlying intelligence found using Bayesian connectivity analysis. Neuroimage, 35, 406419. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smits, M., Dippel, D.W., Houston, G.C., Wielopolski, P.A., Koudstaal, P.J., Hunink, M.G., van der Lugt, A. (2009). Postconcussion syndrome after minor head injury: Brain activation of working memory and attention. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 27892803. doi:10.1002/hbm.20709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talairach, J., Tournoux, P. (1988). Coplanar stereotactic atlas of the human brain. New York, NY: Thieme.Google Scholar
Thevenaz, P., Ruttimann, U.E., Unser, M. (1998). A pyramid approach to subpixel registration based on intensity. IEEE Transactions in Image Processing, 7, 2741. doi:10.1109/83.650848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children (WISC-IV) (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. (1994). Middle grades schooling and early adolescent development: An introduction. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 102106. doi:0.1177/027243169401400202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilke, M., Schmithorst, V.J., Holland, S.K. (2002). Assessment of spatial normalization of whole-brain magnetic resonance images in children. Human Brain Mapping, 17, 4860. doi:10.1002/hbm.10053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, G.S., Robertson, G.J. (2006). Wide range achievement test (WRAT-4) (4th ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Worsley, K.J., Friston, K.J. (1995). Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited – Again. Neuroimage, 2, 173181. doi:10.1006/nimg.1995.1023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiong, J., Gao, J.H., Lancaster, J.L., Fox, P.T. (1995). Clustered pixels analysis for functional MRI activation studies of the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 3, 287301. doi:10.1002/hbm.460030404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Armstrong, K., Janusz, J., Taylor, H.G., Wade, S., Stancin, T., Drotar, D. (2005). Long-term attention problems in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 574584. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000159947.50523.64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological performance by group

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Brain activation map for the Interference Control comparison across the entire group of participants. Only positive activation foci (Interference > Neutral) are shown here. Images are horizontal slices 4 mm apart and start at Z = –29 mm (top left) to Z = +63 mm (bottom right). Images are in radiological convention: left side of the images corresponds to the right hemisphere. Image parameters are as follows: nominal Z threshold = 10.0, cluster size = 100, corrected p < .001 for multiple comparisons.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Group difference map in the Interference Control comparison. The traumatic brain injury (TBI) group had significantly higher levels of activation in multiple brain regions relative to the typically developing control participants (TC) group (warm color). The TC group had no regions with higher levels of activation than the TBI group. Nominal Z threshold = 8.0, cluster size = 100, corrected p < .001 for multiple comparisons.

Figure 3

Table 2 Regions of interest (ROIs) showing significantly different levels of activation between adolescents with TBI (TBI) and typically developing controls (TC) in the interference minus neutral contrast during the counting stroop task