Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T10:53:43.091Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can Risk-Taking Be an Endophenotype for Bipolar Disorder? A Study on Patients with Bipolar Disorder Type I and Their First-Degree Relatives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2013

Ceren Hıdıroğlu*
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Health Sciences Institute, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
Özlem Demirci Esen
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Health Sciences Institute, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
Zeliha Tunca
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
Şehnaz Neslihan Gűrz Yalçìn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey Etlik Zubeyde Hanim Women's Health Teaching and Research Hospital, Ankara, Turkey
Lauren Lombardo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
David C. Glahn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut Olin Neuropsychiatric Research Center, Institute of Living Whitehall Research Building, Hartford, Connecticut
Ayşegül Özerdem
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Health Sciences Institute, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey Brain Dynamics and Research Center, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Ceren Hıdıroğlu, Dokuz Eylul University, Health Sciences Institute, Department of Neuroscience, Izmir, Turkey. E-mail: cerennh@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Risk-taking behavior and impulsivity are core features of bipolar disorder. Whether they are part of the inherited aspect of the illness is not clear. We aimed to evaluate risk-taking behavior as a potential endophenotype for bipolar disorders, and its relationship with impulsivity and illness features. The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) were used to assess risk-taking behavior and impulsivity respectively in 30 euthymic bipolar I patients (BD), their 25 asymptomatic first-degree relatives (BD-R), and 30 healthy controls (HC). The primary BART outcome measure was the behavioral adjustment score (number of pumps after trials where the balloon did not pop minus the number of pumps after trials where the balloon popped). BD (p < .001) and BD-R (p = .001) had similar and significantly lower adjustment scores than HC. Only BD scored significantly higher on BIS-11 total (p = .01) and motor (p = .04) subscales than HC. Neither the BART, nor impulsivity scores associated with illness features. A limitation of this study is medicated patients and a heterogeneous BD-R were included. Riskiness may be a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder as it appears independently from illness features, presents similarly in BD and BD-R groups and differs from impulsivity. (JINS, 2013, 19, 1–9)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2013

Introduction

Increased risk-taking behavior is one of the more recognized symptoms of bipolar disorder and is included in the diagnostic criteria for mania (APA, 2000). Risk-taking is defined as engaging in behaviors with a high potential for harm or danger and simultaneous opportunity for reward (Leigh, Reference Leigh1999). The operational definitions of impulsivity and risk-taking behavior are not straightforward. Although risk-taking propensity seems to have similar features with impulsivity and the two are oftentimes difficult to be delineated within a particular behavior, they are not synonymous constructs. Risk-taking behavior is assumed to be a selected strategic response (Floden, Alexander, Kubu, Katz, & Stuss, Reference Floden, Alexander, Kubu, Katz and Stuss2008). It is a situationally determined behavior (Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman, & LaForge, Reference Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman and LaForge2005), whereas impulsivity reflects reduced control over behavior and appears upon evoking (Floden et al., Reference Floden, Alexander, Kubu, Katz and Stuss2008). Whether risk-taking results from a deficit in impulse control is in dispute (Kreek et al., Reference Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman and LaForge2005). In general, impulsivity refers to a predisposition and an overall pattern of behavior (Kreek et al., Reference Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman and LaForge2005).

Brain imaging data can be considered to be in line with the notion that impulsivity and risk-taking are two separate constructs. The role of the frontal lobe in impulsive behavior is explained in detail in an extensive review by Evenden (Reference Evenden1999). Findings from an animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), where impulsivity is a core symptom involved brain regions such as the nucleus accumbens and amygdala. As these regions are linked to reward, reinforcement, and decision making the authors concluded that the involvement of such locations may be critical to the expression of impulsivity (King, Tenney, Rossi, Colamussi, & Burdick, Reference King, Tenney, Rossi, Colamussi and Burdick2003). On the other hand, several recent studies have demonstrated specific anatomical and functional locations contributing significantly to the risk behavior in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), a computer-based and valid measure of risk-taking propensity. These locations include dorso-lateral frontal (Lighthall et al., Reference Lighthall, Sakaki, Vasunilashorn, Nga, Somayajula, Chen and Mather2011; Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang, & Detre, Reference Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang and Detre2008; Rao et al., Reference Rao, Mamikonyan, Detre, Siderowf, Stern, Potenza and Weintraub2010), ventral and dorsal striatal (Jentsch, Woods, Groman, & Seu, Reference Jentsch, Woods, Groman and Seu2010; Rao et al., Reference Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang and Detre2008) brain regions. Data suggest an association between striatal function and risk-taking in the BART.

Increasing levels of impulsivity in bipolar disorders are present across all states of the illness (Peluso et al., Reference Peluso, Hatch, Glahn, Monkul, Sanches, Najt and Soares2007; Swann, Dougherty, Poazzaglia, Pham, & Moeller, Reference Swann, Dougherty, Pazzaglia, Pham and Moeller2004; Swann, Pazzaglia, Nicholls, Dougherty, & Moeller, Reference Swann, Pazzaglia, Nicholls, Dougherty and Moeller2003). Literature on the relationship between impulsivity and clinical aspects of the illness is contradictory. Impulsivity was shown to be independent of symptom severity and mood episodes (Holmes et al., Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009; Swann, Lijffit, Lane, Steinberg, & Moeller, Reference Swann, Lijffijt, Lane, Steinberg and Moeller2009), therefore, supporting the proposition that it is a core trait of bipolar disorder (Najt et al., Reference Najt, Perez, Sanches, Peluso, Glahn and Soares2007). More recently, impulsivity, especially rapid-response, is stated to be more severe with a highly recurrent course of illness (Swann, Reference Swann2010).

Research on risk-taking behavior in general and in bipolar disorder is minimal although it is often part of the clinical presentation of bipolar disorder. The BART has been applied to study risk-taking in clinical populations, including schizophrenia (Cheng, Tang, Li, Lau, & Lee, Reference Cheng, Tang, Li, Lau and Lee2012), Parkinson's disease (Rao et al., Reference Rao, Mamikonyan, Detre, Siderowf, Stern, Potenza and Weintraub2010), and ADHD (Weafer, Milich, & Fillmore, Reference Weafer, Milich and Fillmore2011). In the ADHD study, although patients did not differ from controls in terms of risk-taking, there was a trend toward greater risk-taking on the BART and the quantity of alcohol consumption in the patient population (Weafer et al., Reference Weafer, Milich and Fillmore2011). On the contrary, patients with schizophrenia performed suboptimally on the BART which was related to a possible abnormal tendency to engage in safe actions rather than risky actions as determined by the Risky-Gains Task response rates (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Tang, Li, Lau and Lee2012). In an attempt to conceptualize risk-taking propensity and impulsivity in bipolar disorder, Holmes et al. (Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009) demonstrated a link between risk-taking and previous alcohol abuse in bipolar disorder, and showed that risk-taking propensity and impulsivity are separable constructs that tap distinct aspects of the bipolar phenotype.

Studies focusing on the neurobiology of impulsivity and risk-taking show that genetic variation may partially underlie these complex traits (Anokhin, Golosheykin, Grant, & Heath, Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin, Grant and Heath2009; Congdon & Canli, Reference Congdon and Canli2008). Coupled with the evidence that impulsivity and risk-taking are important features of bipolar disorder, a better understanding of the familial transmission of risk-taking and/or impulsivity has implications for identifying endophenotypes of the illness. To consider a feature as an endophenotype, it should be associated with the disease in that population, be heritable, and be independent from clinical state; additionally, the marker and the disorder should co-exist among family members (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). Endophenotypes can be useful in defining subtypes of the illness, and developing new approaches in treatment of bipolar disorder (Frantom, Allen, & Cross, Reference Frantom, Allen and Cross2008; Najt et al., Reference Najt, Perez, Sanches, Peluso, Glahn and Soares2007). Very few studies investigated impulsivity as an endophenotype of bipolar disorder (Chamberlain & Sahakian, Reference Chamberlain and Sahakian2007) and no study to date has focused on risk-taking behavior as an endophenotype of bipolar disorder.

The primary objective of this study was to assess risk-taking behavior in patients with bipolar disorder (BD) and their asymptomatic first-degree relatives (BD-R) in comparison to healthy control participants (HC) as a potential endophenotype. Our secondary objective was to investigate the relationship between risk-taking and impulsivity in the same population. Third, we aimed to investigate the relationship between clinical characteristics and risk-taking as well as impulsivity in the patient group. The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) (Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Ramsey, Stuart, Strong and Brown2002) and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) (Patton; Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) were used for measuring risk-taking behavior and trait impulsivity, respectively. We hypothesized that individuals with bipolar disorder and their asymptomatic first-degree relatives would present a similar propensity for risk-taking behavior and that both groups would differ in risk-taking compared to the healthy controls. We anticipated an association between scores of riskiness on the BART and BIS-11 scores. We anticipated an association between a more severe course of illness and higher levels of risk-taking and impulsivity in the patient group.

Methods

Participants

The study was carried out at the neurocognitive research laboratories of Department of Neuroscience and the bipolar disorders outpatient unit of Department of Psychiatry, Dokuz Eylul University. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Clinical Trials of Dokuz Eylul University Hospital which follows Helsinki criteria. Euthymic patients with a previously diagnosed bipolar type I disorder who were being followed at the bipolar outpatient unit were referred to the study by their physician. All living first-degree relatives (parents, siblings, children) of the enrolled patients were invited to participate in the study. Healthy volunteers were recruited through announcements at the university hospital and on the medical school campus. All participants provided written informed consent. Thirty consecutive euthymic patients with DSM-IV BD type-I who were found to be eligible were enrolled in the study. Due to either lack or unavailability of all first-degree relatives (parent, sibling, and children) in each individual patient any consented first-degree relative was included for the screening. Those who met the inclusion criteria were enrolled in the study. In this group there were 25 asymptomatic first-degree relatives three of whom (12%) were offspring, nine (36%) were parents, and 13 (52%) were siblings. Thirty sex-, age-, and educationally matched healthy controls with no personal or family history of psychiatric illness were included in the study.

All participants (patients, first-degree relatives, and healthy controls) were interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996). SCID interviews were completed by three experienced psychiatry residents who were formally trained for structured interviews and rating scales as a part of their psychiatry training. Information on demographics, clinical characteristics and treatment history of the patients was registered by using the standardized registration program for bipolar disorders-Turkey (SKIP-TURK) forms by the same clinicians (Özerdem et al., Reference Özerdem, Yazici, Oral, Tunca, Vahip and Kurt2004).

To be included in the study, patients needed to be euthymic for at least 6 months with no subclinical symptoms, score 7 or less on the validated and reliable Turkish version of the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (Karadag, Oral, Yalcin, & Erten, Reference Karadag, Oral, Yalcin and Erten2002), and score 7 or less on the validated and reliable Turkish version of the 21-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D 21) (Aydemir & Deveci, Reference Aydemir and Deveci2003). Scales were given by the same clinician who ran the structured interviews. Patients with history of any comorbid Axis I disorder, neurodegenerative disease, mental retardation, epilepsy, cerebral tumor, cerebrovascular disease, or head trauma were excluded. The same exclusion criteria applied to the first-degree relatives and healthy controls.

Assessment Instruments

The Balloon Analogue Risk Task is a computer-based measure of risk-taking propensity. It is designed to examine risk-taking behavior in similar to real-world situations, where taking risk is rewarded until a certain point, after which more riskiness results in poorer outcomes (Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Ramsey, Stuart, Strong and Brown2002). As there is no specific and established behavioral test that has been used to measure risk-taking behavior in bipolar disorder, the BART was the most appropriate choice of selection for the present study, although balloon popping may not generalize to most risk-taking behaviors. It is of note that the task includes other cognitive features such as learning, planning, and cognitive flexibility.

BIS-11 is a widely used self rating test which is considered to be a good reference for studying impulsivity and related properties in psychiatric illnesses. It measures three theoretical subtraits of impulsivity: motor impulsiveness, non-planning impulsiveness, and attentional impulsiveness (Patton et al., Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995).

A single investigator introduced BIS-11 and administered the BART to all participants. The BART and BIS-11 were given during one session and in the same order to each participant to provide standardization of the procedure across all participants.

The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)

The BART is presented on a computer screen which includes a small simulated balloon accompanied by a balloon pump button, a “collect” button for stopping and getting points earned until that time (temporary bank) and an indicator presenting permanent points (permanent bank). The balloon inflates and the participant earns one point with each pump, and the task includes 30 trials. In each trial, the participant can stop pumping at any point and click on the collect button for transferring all points from the temporary bank to permanent bank. If the balloon explodes, the participant loses all collected points. Information on the balloons’ breakpoints is not given to the participants to allow for testing both participants’ initial responses to the task and changes in responding as they gain experience with the task contingencies.

Construct validity of the BART was provided by Lejuez et al. (Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Ramsey, Stuart, Strong and Brown2002). Reliability of the test has been established across a range of samples and testing conditions. Split-third reliability examination (i.e., comparing scores across the first block, middle, and third blocks of 10 balloons each), revealed strong reliability (> 0.7) (White, Lejuez, & de Wit, Reference White, Lejuez and de Wit2008). Its performance was shown to be stable across several days, with test–retest reliability correlation coefficients approximating 0.77 (White et al., Reference White, Lejuez and de Wit2008).

The permission to use the BART was provided from Dr. Lejuez and his team before starting the project. To run the task in Turkish, translation and back translation of the participant instructions and button contents were completed independently by two experienced team members who were in good command of English and Turkish. The process was completed in collaboration with the Lejuez group. Before beginning the task, all participants were given full information on the procedure and told they would receive a symbolic reward proportional to the total points they earned at the completion of the task. No practice was allowed before beginning the task.

Lejuez et al. (Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Ramsey, Stuart, Strong and Brown2002) used the total number of times balloons were pumped on trials where the balloon did not pop, (adjusted pumps for successful trials) as a measure of risk-taking. This approach does not account for the number of pumps on trials where the balloon that exploded (e.g., unsuccessful trials), and thus may not adequately model those trials where individuals adopt a risky strategy. For this reason, our primary dependent measure was the average number of pumps after successful trials minus the average number of pumps after unsuccessful trials. We believe that this within subject “adjustment score” indexes change in behavior after a popped trial and reflects a behavioral adjustment in risk-taking behavior (i.e., the smaller the difference is, the poorer the adjustment). To assess riskiness in a broader sense, we used the total number of exploded balloons as a secondary outcome measure.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 is a 30-item self-report measure of impulsivity. The BIS-11 items are measured on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = Rarely/Never; 2 = Occasionally; 3 = Often; 4 = Almost Always/Always). Some items are scored inversely to avoid response bias. The BIS-11 total score indicates level of impulsiveness.

The BIS-11 includes three sub-scales: (1) Attentional Impulsiveness (eight items; racing thoughts, impatience to cognitive complexity, concentration, and attention problems), (2) Motor Impulsiveness (11 items; fast reactions or restlessness), (3) Non-planning Impulsiveness (11 items; lack of future orientation) (Patton et al., Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995).

Cronbach's alpha for internal consistency of the original BIS-11 scores are as follows: for total score: 0.83, for attentional subscale score 0.74, for motor subscale score 0.59, and for non-planning score 0.72. These second order factors have consistently been found (Stanford et al., Reference Stanford, Mathias, Dougherty, Lake, Anderson and Patton2009) with test–retest reliability figures of r = 0.72, 0.61, and 0.67, respectively (Stanford et al., Reference Stanford, Mathias, Dougherty, Lake, Anderson and Patton2009).

Permission for using the BIS-11 was obtained from the contact person (Marijn Lijffijt, PhD) of the test before beginning the project. Translation and back translation were completed independently by two experienced team members with good command of English and Turkish. The process was completed in collaboration with Dr. Marijn Lijffijt. For validation, BIS-11 Turkish version was administered to a sample of 105 volunteers. Cronbach's alpha for internal consistency was 0.83 which shows the reliability of the Turkish version. This is consistent with the previously reported and acceptable internal consistencies (Cronbach's alpha) for other translated versions of BIS-11 total score (0.71–0.83) (Stanford et al., Reference Stanford, Mathias, Dougherty, Lake, Anderson and Patton2009).

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using Statistical Package for Social Sciences 15.0. Before analyses, all variables were shown to conform to normality (Shapiro-Wilk test p > .05 for all variables). The adjustment score (average number of pumps after successful trials - average number of pumps after unsuccessful trials), the total number of exploded balloons on the BART (total, motor, non-planning, and attentional) were examined with a linear mixed model with group as a between-subjects factor and random family effects taken into account. Bonferroni test was used for all post hoc analyses.

To examine the relationship between impulsivity and riskiness in general, Pearson correlations were run between BIS-11 scores and the number of exploded balloons on the BART first for the entire sample (n = 85), and then individually for each group (BD, BD-R, and HC). Before running the correlation analysis, we performed a linear mixed model analysis as explained above for BIS-11 scores to see whether the three groups differed in terms of impulsivity.

The difference in age and years of education between the three groups was assessed using one-way analysis of variance. Chi-square test was used to compare groups for sex. Regression analysis was used to examine the relationship between clinical characteristics of the patient group (duration of illness, number of months in euthymia, total number of past episodes, and number of hospitalizations) and the BART adjustment score, total number of exploded balloons on the BART as well as BIS-11 total score. In all tests, two-tailed p values < .05 were accepted as significant. Results in the manuscript are given as mean ± standard deviation (SD).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Groups did not differ in terms of age, sex, or years of education (Table 1). In the patient group, the number of past episodes was 5.30 ± 3.84 (range, 1–21), duration of illness in years was 9.67 ± 7.76 (range, 1–41 years), euthymia duration (in months) was 23.62 ± 17.59 (range, 7–72 months), number of hospitalizations was 1.53 ± 2.06 (range, 0–11), and age of onset of illness was 25.77 ± 10.14 years (range, 13–57 years). Only one patient (3.3%) was medication free, nine (30.0%) were on monotherapy with either lithium (n = 4) or valproate (n = 5). Thirteen patients (43.3%) were on combination treatment with a mood stabilizer plus atypical antipsychotic. The remaining sample (n = 7; 16.3%) was on combination treatment with various medications. SCID-I interview of the BD-Rs and HCs confirmed that none of the participants in these groups had any Axis I diagnosis.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the participants

aChi-square test.

bOne-way analysis of variance.

BD = bipolar disorder; BD-R = bipolar disorder first-degree relatives; HC = healthy controls; SD = standard deviation.

Risk-Taking Behavior (BART) Results

Linear mixed model analysis revealed significant group difference on the adjustment score (F 2,82 = 12.26; p < .001). Post hoc Bonferroni analysis revealed that both patients (1.72 ± 1.70) and relatives (2.06 ± 2.34) had significantly lower adjustment scores than controls (4.31 ± 2.45) (patients vs. controls p < .001; relatives vs. controls p = .001). However, patients and relatives had similar adjustment scores (p > .05) (Table 2 and Fig. 1). Two sided power analysis for the difference between patients and healthy controls with an α (type I error) value of 0.05 revealed a power of 1.0. The effect size (Cohen's d: 1.22) for the provided difference was large by the criteria of Cohen (Reference Cohen1977). The power of the difference between relatives and healthy controls was 0.91 with a corresponding large effect size (Cohen's d: 0.93) by the Cohen criteria.

Table 2 Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) scores of the participants

*Average number of pumping after successful trials - Average number of pumping after unsuccessful trials.

aLinear Mixed Model.

bPost-hoc Bonferroni test.

BD = bipolar disorder; BD-R = bipolar disorder first-degree relatives; HC = healthy controls; SD = standard deviation.

Fig. 1 Adjustment patter in the three groups. Both BDs and BD-Rs show alike and less adjustment, therefore higher risk taking propensity compared to HCs. BD = patients with bipolar disorder; BD-R = first-degree relatives; HC = healthy controls.

Groups did not differ significantly in the total number of exploded balloons (F2,82 = 0.14; p = .86) (Table 2).

Impulsivity (BIS-11) Results

In the linear mixed model analysis groups differed significantly on the BIS-11 total (F 2,82 = 4.51; p = .01) and motor impulsivity subscale scores (F 2,82 = 3.30; p = .04). Post hoc analysis showed that patients had significantly higher BIS-11 total (59.90 ± 9.93) and motor impulsivity scores (19.70 ± 4.26) compared to controls (52.90 ± 7.25, and 17.30 ± 3.70, respectively) (p = .01, power 0.88 and p = .04, power 0.65, respectively). Effect size of the difference between patients and controls for the BIS-11 total score (Cohen's d: 0.81) was large and for the motor impulsivity scores was medium (Cohen's d: 0.60) by the criteria of Cohen (Reference Cohen1977). BD-Rs did not significantly differ from either BDs or HCs on BIS-11 total (57.40 ± 10.10) or motor impulsivity (18.24 ± 2.63) scores. Neither the non-planning (F2,82 = 2.74; p = .07), nor the attentional subscale scores (F2,82 = 2.54; p = .09) showed significant difference among the three groups (Table 3).

Table 3 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS–11) scores of the participants

Note. Statistical significance: p values<.05.

aLinear Mixed Model.

bPost-hoc Bonferroni test.

BD = bipolar disorder; BD-R = bipolar disorder first degree relatives; HC = healthy controls; SD = standard deviation.

In the whole group (n = 85) the number of exploded balloons was significantly correlated with the BIS-11 total (r = 0.37; p<.001), attentional (r = 0.28; p = .01), motor (r = 0.25; p = .02), and non-planning (r = 0.31; p = .004) subscale scores. There was no correlation between adjustment scores and either the total BIS-11 scores or BIS-11 subscores in the entire group (n = 85) (Table 4). Only the control group (n = 30) showed a statistically significant medium correlation between adjustment and BIS-11 total (r = 0.397; p = .030) and BIS-11 motor impulsivity scores (r = 0.436; p = .016).

Table 4 Correlations between BART and BIS-11 scores in the entire group (n = 85)

r = Pearson correlation coefficiency.

*Indicates statistical significance.

Relationship Between Clinical Features and Risk-taking and Impulsivity

Linear regression analysis showed that the BART adjustment score (R = 0.317), total number of exploded balloons on the BART (R = 0.405) and BIS-11 total score (R = 0.113) did not have any significant relationship with either duration of illness (p values for the BART adjustment score, total number of exploded balloons on the BART and BIS-11 total scores are 0.57, 0.07, 0.96, respectively) or duration of euthymia (p values for BART adjustment score, total number of exploded balloons on the BART and BIS-11 total scores are; 0.36, 0.77, 0.76, respectively), or total number of past episodes (p values for BART adjustment score, total number of exploded balloons on the BART and BIS-11 total scores are; 0.26, 0.31, 0.72, respectively) or number of hospitalizations (p values for BART adjustment score, total number of exploded balloons on the BART and BIS-11 total scores are; 0.77, 0.32, 0.71, respectively).

Discussion

The primary finding of this study is that euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder and their asymptomatic first-degree relatives showed similar adjustment propensity in an experimentally probed risky condition. These two groups adjusted significantly less compared to healthy controls after the loss of a temporary gain on the BART. All participants reported reduced pumping after facing loss on the BART. However, the magnitude of this adjustment was similar in the patients and their relatives and it was significantly smaller compared to that of healthy controls. This magnitude difference in the behavioral change is what delineated healthy controls from patients as well as from their first-degree relatives. This may suggest a particular deficiency in learning behavior on a risk-taking construct by both patients and their first-degree relatives, suggesting that liability for bipolar disorder may be linked to a reduced ability to modulate risky activity in the face of negative consequences.

Early studies on risk-taking behavior by using or using the BART generally targeted populations with either nicotine (Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Aklin, Jones, Richards, Strong, Kahler and Read2003; Lejuez, Aklin, Bornovalova, & Moolchan, Reference Lejuez, Aklin, Bornovalova and Moolchan2005) or alcohol use (Skeel, Pilarski, Pytlak, & Neudecker, Reference Skeel, Pilarski, Pytlak and Neudecker2008) problems. To our knowledge, there is only one published study (Holmes et al., Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009) investigating risk-taking behavior with the BART in patients with BD in which a group of patients with prior history of alcohol abuse or dependence was compared to another group of bipolar patients without such a history. Our study is the first and only study where risk-taking behavior is investigated in a group of medicated patients who were otherwise homogeneous with regard to the illness subtype and state (all bipolar disorder type I, euthymic) and absence of present or past alcohol related problems. In the study by Holmes et al. (Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009), only the group with past alcohol abuse/dependence showed impaired BART performance. These patients popped significantly more balloons than the bipolar patients with no prior alcohol related problems and failed to adjust their performance after popping balloons. Our euthymic patients who had no prior history of alcohol related problems did not differ from healthy controls in the number of exploded balloons which is similar to the non-alcohol group of the previous study. However, our patients still displayed significantly less adjustment on the BART than healthy controls, which contrasts with the finding of Holmes et al. (Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009). This discrepancy may reflect differences in outcome measures (e.g., pumps after unsuccessful-pumps after successful trials) that had been used in these two studies. With our new outcome measure, we have been able to assess the behavioral change probed by a changing riskiness condition (pumps after unpopped trials vs. pumps after popped trials) instead of assessing a single measure (pumps only on successful trials). However, this new parameter seems to be more related to learning from past experience, presenting a new feature that has not much to do with risk-taking per se. It is also of note that we studied a homogeneous group of patients (all bipolar I and euthymic) in contrast to the study by Holmes et al. (Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009), where both bipolar I and II patients in different states of the illness were included in assessments.

The significantly elevated BIS-11 total and motor subscale scores in BDs compared to HCs is consistent with previous studies (Swann, Anderson, Dougherty, and Moeller, Reference Swann, Anderson, Dougherty and Moeller2001; Swann et al., Reference Swann, Pazzaglia, Nicholls, Dougherty and Moeller2003). However, there was no significant difference between relatives and either controls or patients on the BIS-11, despite the finding that they showed significantly lower adjustment than healthy controls (and similar adjustments to patients) on the BART. The fact that there was no significant difference between relatives and controls on the BIS-11 is important. Our finding suggests that diminished ability to adjust distinguishes relatives from healthy controls in contrast to self-reported impulsivity. A measure such as the BART may be able to pick up on an important endophenotype that would be missed by BIS-11, or, these two measures may be sensitive to two fundamentally different types of impulsivity.

In a previous study Jollant et al. (Reference Jollant, Guillaume, Jaussent, Bellivier, Leboyer, Castelnau and Courtet2007) found no correlation between impulsivity and decision-making performance in asymptomatic patients with bipolar disorder. In this study decision-making performance was tested using Iowa Gambling Task which relates to risk-taking in decision making and capitalizes on the learning of reward and punishment associations (Jollant et al., Reference Jollant, Guillaume, Jaussent, Bellivier, Leboyer, Castelnau and Courtet2007). Christodoulou, Lewis, Ploubidis, and Frangou (Reference Christodoulou, Lewis, Ploubidis and Frangou2006) suggested that self-reported impulsivity as measured by BIS-11 may reflect rather specific aspects of impulse control and decision-making but not necessarily of sustained attention in remitted patients with bipolar I disorder. Abovementioned findings and related arguments are indicative of a fact that a multifaceted construct such as risk-taking warrants assessment using a multi-method, multi-informant approach.

There was a modest but significant correlation between the number of exploded balloons and the BIS-11 scores in the entire group (n = 85). However, no significant correlation between adjustment and BIS-11 scores was assessed in the same group. The dissociation between the number of popped balloons-BIS-11 score relationship versus adjustment score-BIS-11 score relationship may be generated by the difference between what the two BART measures actually represent. As the adjustment score used in this study intends to reveal more sophisticated parts of risk-taking behavior such as learning, and cognitive flexibility, correlation analysis may not accurately reflect its relation to the level of impulsivity as measured by BIS-11. Additionally, in a recent study where Ryan, Mackillop, and Carpenter (Reference Ryan, Mackillop and Carpenter2013) investigated the relationship between impulsivity, risk-taking propensity and nicotine dependence among older adolescent smokers, greater risk-taking was found to be predictive of less nicotine dependence. The authors considered the finding as a further evidence that these neurobehavioral constructs are separable characteristics that may have unique contributions to smoking behavior. Our finding and the finding of the Ryan et al. study are in line with a recent meta-analysis where relationships among unidimensional impulsivity self-report and lab task conceptualizations indicated very little overlap in self-report and behavioral lab task constructs (Cyders & Coskunpinar, Reference Cyders and Coskunpinar2011).

We found no relationship between clinical characteristics such as duration of illness, duration of euthymia, total number of past episodes, and number of hospitalizations, and either the total number of exploded balloons on the BART or the BIS-11 scores. These clinical features did not show any significant interaction with the adjustment behavior on the BART either. Previous studies comparing manic and euthymic patients found more commission errors during manic episodes (Bora, Vahip, & Akdeniz, Reference Bora, Vahip and Akdeniz2006; Fleck, Shear, & Strakowski, Reference Fleck, Shear and Strakowski2005). Also high trait impulsivity was associated with a more severe course of illness and linked to a more severe suicide attempt history (Swann et al., Reference Swann, Dougherty, Pazzaglia, Pham, Steinberg and Moeller2005, Reference Swann, Lijffijt, Lane, Steinberg and Moeller2009). On the other hand, other studies (Holmes et al., Reference Kathleen Holmes, Bearden, Barguil, Fonseca, Serap Monkul, Nery and Glahn2009; Swann et al., Reference Swann, Lijffijt, Lane, Steinberg and Moeller2009) did not find a significant relationship between symptoms and impulsivity. However, unlike direct manic versus euthymic comparisons, these studies do not have sufficient variance of symptoms to show such an effect. The absence of a correlation between clinical features and impulsivity and adjustment scores in our study may be due to absence of patients in manic or depressive episodes. It is also worth noting that the state factors do not explain all impulsivity that is present in bipolar disorder, as there are also trait related aspects of impulsivity and risk-taking.

The main limitation of this study was that first-degree relatives group was heterogeneous and included parents, offspring and siblings. Having only the siblings would be ideal for studying any genetic predisposition. Despite this limitation, findings on the BART do reflect a familial risk-taking behavior pattern. As the impact of medication use on risk-taking and impulsivity is not established, having patients on various medications may be considered another limitation of the study. We suggest that working with drug free euthymic patients as well as medicated patients in large sample sizes would be beneficial to rule out the potential effect of medication use on results. Similarly, assessment of specific effect of illness duration or length of euthymia would contribute to understanding how course of illness impacts risk-taking and/or impulsivity beyond the preexisting neurobiological infrastructure. Studies addressing these questions require larger patient populations to override the limitation of high variability in the course of illness as seen in the present study.

In conclusion, this is the first study showing evidence for decreased adjustment pattern in riskiness in a homogeneous group of euthymic bipolar I patients with no psychiatric or medical co-morbidities and in their asymptomatic family members in comparison to healthy controls. Our finding supports the idea that decreased adjustment pattern in riskiness may be a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder as it is present in the patient population (association to the given illness) in the asymptomatic first-degree relatives (co-existance among family members), and independent from clinical state (persists in the absence of symptoms) as defined by Gottesman and Gould (Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). Further studies on the underlying genetic make-up of risk-taking and related behavioral pattern in bipolar disorder will provide us with an answer for the question whether this trait is a true endophenotype.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Marijn Lijffijt, PhD, for his support and collaboration in the permission and validation process of BIS-11; Omer Aydemir, MD, for advising us on the validation of Turkish version of BIS-11; Carl W. Lejuez, PhD, and his team for their support and collaboration in setting up the BART and its use in this study; Gul Ergor, MD, for her contributions in the statistical analysis; Lithium Society, the first advocacy group in Turkey, for their financial support for the study; and our patients and their families for their participation in the study. Funding for this study was provided by the Lithium Society, Izmir, Turkey; the Lithium Society had no further role in either design or conduct of the study; in the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. Conflict of interest: Ayşegül Özerdem has been a principle investigator in clinical trials sponsored by Astra Zeneca and Eli-Lilly; received speaker honorarium from Astra Zeneca, Bristol Myers Squibb (BMS), Janssen-Cilag, Eli-Lilly, Sanofi-Aventis, Glaxo Smith Kleine (GSK); has been on the advisory boards of Astra Zeneca, Janssen-Cilag, Eli-Lilly BMS, EGİS, GSK, Shering Plaugh, TEVA during last five years. She has no direct commercial associations that might pose a conflict of interest in connection with the submitted manuscript. Authors Ceren Hìdìroğlu, Özlem Demirci Esen, Zeliha Tunca, Şehnaz Neslihan Gűrz Yalçìn, Lauren Lombardo, David C. Glahn do not have any commercial associations that might pose a conflict of interest in connection with the submitted manuscript. Contributors: Ceren Hìdìroğlu wrote the protocol, recruited the patients, first-degree relatives and the healthy controls, ran the BART and BIS-11 tests, collected participant data, attended the statistical analysis, wrote the first draft of the manuscript and finalized it with supervision. Özlem Demirci Esen helped patient and healthy control recruitment, and assisted writing the first draft of the manuscript. Zeliha Tunca helped patient recruitment, contributed actively in interpreting the data and finalization of the manuscript. Şehnaz Neslihan Gűrz Yalçìn did the clinical interviews with all participants, applied the rating scales, contributed to the data interpretation. Lauren Lombardo helped statistical analysis and finalization of the manuscript. David C. Glahn helped statistical analysis, reviewed the manuscript and actively participated in finalization of the manuscript. Ayşegül Özerdem supervised the first author in building the protocol. She also supervised clinical data collection, managed the literature searches, undertook the statistical analysis and finalization of the manuscript.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. DSM-IV-TR. (Revised 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.Google Scholar
Anokhin, A.P., Golosheykin, S., Grant, J., Heath, A.C. (2009). Heritability of risk-taking in adolescence: A longitudinal twin study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12(4), 366371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aydemir, O., Deveci, A. 2003. Validity and reliability of structured interview for Hamilton Depression Rating Scale Seasonal Affective Disorders Version. VIIth Annual Spring Symposium of Psychiatric Association of Turkey Abstract Book, p. 187.Google Scholar
Bora, E., Vahip, S., Akdeniz, F. (2006). Sustained attention deficits in manic and euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 30(6), 10971102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, S.R., Sahakian, B.J. (2007). The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 255261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, G.L.F., Tang, J.C.Y., Li, F.W.S., Lau, E.Y.Y., Lee, T.M.C. (2012). Schizophrenia and risk-taking: Impaired reward but preserved punishment processing. Schizophrenia Research, 136, 122127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christodoulou, T., Lewis, M., Ploubidis, G.B., Frangou, S. (2006). The relationship of impulsivity to response inhibition and decision-making in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. European Psychiatry, 21, 270273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for behavioral sciences (Revised ed.). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Congdon, E., Canli, T. (2008). A neurogenetic approach to impulsivity. Journal of Personality, 76(6), 14471484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyders, M.A., Coskunpinar, A. (2011). Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 965982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evenden, J. (1999). Impulsivity: A discussion of clinical and experimental findings. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 13(2), 180192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J.B.W. (1996). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Fleck, D.E., Shear, P.K., Strakowski, S.M. (2005). Processing efficiency and sustained attention in bipolar disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(1), 4957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floden, D., Alexander, M.P., Kubu, C.S., Katz, D., Stuss, D.T. (2008). Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 46, 213223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frantom, L.V., Allen, D.N., Cross, C.L. (2008). Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 387399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, I.I., Gould, T.J. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry. Etymology and strategic intentions. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jentsch, J.D., Woods, J.A., Groman, S.M., Seu, E. (2010). Behavioral characteristics and neural mechanisms mediating performance in a rodent version of the balloon analogue risk task. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 17971806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jollant, F., Guillaume, S., Jaussent, I., Bellivier, F., Leboyer, M., Castelnau, D., Courtet, P. (2007). Psychiatric diagnoses and personality traits associated with disadvantageous decision-making. European Psychiatry, 22, 455461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karadag, F., Oral, E.T., Yalcin, F.A., Erten, E. (2002). Validity and reliability of Young Mania Rating Scale in Turkey. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 107114.Google Scholar
Kathleen Holmes, M., Bearden, C.E., Barguil, M., Fonseca, M., Serap Monkul, E., Nery, F.G., Glahn, D.C. (2009). Conceptualizing impulsivity and risk taking in bipolar disorder: Importance of history of alcohol abuse. Bipolar Disorders, 11, 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, J.A., Tenney, J., Rossi, V., Colamussi, L., Burdick, S. (2003). Neural substrates underlying impulsivity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008, 160169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreek, M.J., Nielsen, D.A., Butelman, E.R., LaForge, K.S. (2005). Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 14501457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leigh, B.C. (1999). Peril, chance, and adventure: Concepts of risk, alcohol use, and risky behavior in young adults. Addiction, 94, 371383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejuez, C.W., Aklin, W.M., Bornovalova, M., Moolchan, E.T. (2005). Differences in risk-taking propensity across inner-city adolescent ever- and never-smokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 7(1), 7179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejuez, C.W., Aklin, W.M., Jones, H.A., Richards, J.B., Strong, D.R., Kahler, C.W., Read, J.P. (2003). The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 2633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejuez, C.W., Read, J.P., Kahler, C.W., Ramsey, S.E., Stuart, G.L., Strong, D.R., Brown, R.A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology (Applied), 8(2), 7584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lighthall, N.R., Sakaki, M., Vasunilashorn, S., Nga, L., Somayajula, S., Chen, E.Y., Mather, M. (2011). Gender differences in reward-related decision processing under stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(4), 476484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Najt, P., Perez, J., Sanches, M., Peluso, M.A., Glahn, D., Soares, J.C. (2007). Impulsivity and bipolar disorder. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 17, 313320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özerdem, A., Yazici, O., Oral, E.T., Tunca, Z., Vahip, S., Kurt, E. (2004). The Mood Disorders Study Group Psychiatric Association of Turkey. Establishment of a registry program for bipolar illness in Turkey. Journal of Affective Disorders, 78(Suppl. 1), 86.Google Scholar
Patton, J.H., Stanford, M.S., Barratt, E.S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768774.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peluso, M.A., Hatch, J.P., Glahn, D.C., Monkul, E.S., Sanches, M., Najt, P., Soares, J.C. (2007). Trait impulsivity in patients with mood disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 100, 227231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Pluta, J., Hoang, A., Detre, J.A. (2008). Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: An fMRI study of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Neuroimage, 42, 902910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rao, H., Mamikonyan, M.S., Detre, J.A., Siderowf, A.D., Stern, M.B., Potenza, M.N., Weintraub, D. (2010). Decreased ventral striatal activity with impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 25, 16601669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, K.K., Mackillop, J., Carpenter, M.J. (2013). The relationship between impulsivity, risk-taking propensity and nicotine dependence among older adolescent smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 38, 14311434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeel, R.L., Pilarski, C., Pytlak, K., Neudecker, J. (2008). Personality and performance-based measures in the prediction of alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22(3), 402409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C. (2010). Mechanisms of impulsivity in bipolar disorder and related illness. Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale, 19(2), 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C., Anderson, J.C., Dougherty, D.M., Moeller, F.G. (2001). Measurement of inter-episode impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 101(2), 195197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C., Dougherty, D.M., Pazzaglia, P.J., Pham, M., Moeller, F.G. (2004). Impulsivity: A link between bipolar disorder and substance abuse. Bipolar Disorders, 6, 204212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C., Dougherty, D.M., Pazzaglia, P.J., Pham, M., Steinberg, J.L., Moeller, F.G. (2005). Increased impulsivity associated with severity of suicide attempt history in patients with bipolar disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(9), 16801687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C., Lijffijt, M., Lane, S.D., Steinberg, J.L., Moeller, F.G. (2009). Severity of bipolar disorder is associated with impairment of response inhibition. Journal of Affective Disorders, 116(1–2), 3036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, A.C., Pazzaglia, P., Nicholls, A., Dougherty, D.M., Moeller, F.G. (2003). Impulsivity and phase of illness in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 73, 105111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanford, M.S., Mathias, C.W., Dougherty, D.M., Lake, S.L., Anderson, N.E., Patton, J.H. (2009). Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(5), 385395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weafer, J., Milich, R., Fillmore, M.T. (2011). Behavioral components of impulsivity predict alcohol consumption in adults with ADHD and healthy controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 15, 113(2–3), 139146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, T.L., Lejuez, C.W., de Wit, H. (2008). Test-retest characteristics of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 565570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the participants

Figure 1

Table 2 Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) scores of the participants

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Adjustment patter in the three groups. Both BDs and BD-Rs show alike and less adjustment, therefore higher risk taking propensity compared to HCs. BD = patients with bipolar disorder; BD-R = first-degree relatives; HC = healthy controls.

Figure 3

Table 3 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS–11) scores of the participants

Figure 4

Table 4 Correlations between BART and BIS-11 scores in the entire group (n = 85)