Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T09:38:04.579Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Consistency in adult reporting of adverse childhood experiences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

I. Colman*
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology, Public Health & Preventive Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada School of Public Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
M. Kingsbury
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology, Public Health & Preventive Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
Y. Garad
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
Y. Zeng
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
K. Naicker
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology, Public Health & Preventive Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
S. Patten
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Community Health Sciences, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
P. B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
T. C. Wild
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
A. H. Thompson
Affiliation:
Institute of Health Economics, Edmonton, Canada
*
*Address for correspondence: I. Colman, School of Epidemiology, Public Health & Preventive Medicine, University of Ottawa, 451 Smyth Road, RGN 3230C, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1H 8M5. (Email: icolman@uottawa.ca)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Many studies have used retrospective reports to assess the long-term consequences of early life stress. However, current individual characteristics and experiences may bias the recall of these reports. In particular, depressed mood may increase the likelihood of recall of negative experiences. The aim of the study was to assess whether specific factors are associated with consistency in the reporting of childhood adverse experiences.

Method

The sample comprised 7466 adults from Canada's National Population Health Survey who had reported on seven childhood adverse experiences in 1994/1995 and 2006/2007. Logistic regression was used to explore differences between those who consistently reported adverse experiences and those whose reports were inconsistent.

Results

Among those retrospectively reporting on childhood traumatic experiences in 1994/1995 and 2006/2007, 39% were inconsistent in their reports of these experiences. The development of depression, increasing levels of psychological distress, as well as increasing work and chronic stress were associated with an increasing likelihood of reporting a childhood adverse experience in 2006/2007 that had not been previously reported. Increases in mastery were associated with reduced likelihood of new reporting of a childhood adverse experience in 2006/2007. The development of depression and increases in chronic stress and psychological distress were also associated with reduced likelihood of ‘forgetting’ a previously reported event.

Conclusions

Concurrent mental health factors may influence the reporting of traumatic childhood experiences. Studies that use retrospective reporting to estimate associations between childhood adversity and adult outcomes associated with mental health may be biased.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Introduction

Adverse childhood experiences are common (Thompson & Cui, Reference Thompson and Cui2000) and have been associated with many unfavourable psychological and physiological outcomes such as depression (Bernet & Stein, Reference Bernet and Stein1999; Heim & Nemeroff, Reference Heim and Nemeroff2001; Hovens et al. Reference Hovens, Wiersma, Giltay, Van Oppen, Spinhoven, Penninx and Zitman2010; Nelson et al. Reference Nelson, Baldwin and Taylor2012; Colman et al. Reference Colman, Garad, Zeng, Naicker, Weeks, Patten, Jones, Thompson and Wild2013), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Widom, Reference Widom1999; Heim & Nemeroff, Reference Heim and Nemeroff2001; Moffitt et al. Reference Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, Milne, Melchior, Goldberg and Poulton2007; Hovens et al. Reference Hovens, Wiersma, Giltay, Van Oppen, Spinhoven, Penninx and Zitman2010), cardiovascular disease (Dong et al. Reference Dong, Giles, Felitti, Dube, Williams, Chapman and Anda2004) and chronic pain. Despite the accumulation of evidence on the adverse long-term effects of such experiences, controversy exists around the validity of information from the retrospective reports largely used to generate these findings (Widom & Morris, Reference Widom and Morris1997). Suggested problems in establishing accuracy in the retrospective recall of childhood adverse experiences include the fallibility of memory for such experiences (Williams, Reference Williams1994; Widom & Morris, Reference Widom and Morris1997; Brennen et al. Reference Brennen, Hasanović, Zotović, Blix, Solheim Skar, Prelić, Mehmedović, Pajević, Popović and Gavrilov-Jerković2010), measurement bias (Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Woodward2000) and intentionally false reporting (Widom & Morris, Reference Widom and Morris1997). Despite these criticisms of the reliability and validity of retrospective reporting, several studies have found it to be accurate for assessing childhood maltreatment (Paivio, Reference Paivio2001; Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). However, individual characteristics and experiences may influence the reporting of adverse experiences – a concept with potentially serious implications for reporting accuracy. For example, it has been shown that current mood can influence the likelihood of a negative experience being recalled (Clark & Teasdale, Reference Clark and Teasdale1982; Dalgleish & Watts, Reference Dalgleish and Watts1990). In particular, depression has been associated with a tendency to recall more negative experiences (Gotlib & Joormann, Reference Gotlib and Joormann2010; Whalley et al. Reference Whalley, Rugg and Brewin2012).

A handful of studies have assessed cross-temporal stability in retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). Overall, these studies suggest a good deal of variability between retrospective reports at different times (e.g. 50–73% stability in reporting of separation from parents; Finlay-Jones et al. Reference Finlay-Jones, Scott, Duncan-Jones, Byrne and Henderson1981), but have not found evidence of an association between inconsistency of reporting and mental state at the time of recall (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). However, these studies are limited by relatively small sample sizes and short follow-up periods (i.e. 18 months, Finlay-Jones et al. Reference Finlay-Jones, Scott, Duncan-Jones, Byrne and Henderson1981; 3 years, Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Woodward2000).

The objective of this study was to examine the association between inconsistencies in reporting of childhood adverse experiences and several mental health factors, using a large longitudinal study of Canadian adults with a follow-up time of 12 years. We hypothesized that increases in adversity during adulthood, particularly depression and psychological distress, would be associated with new reports of childhood adversity that had not been previously reported 12 years earlier.

Method

Study population

This study used data from the Canadian National Population Health Survey (NPHS), which is a nationally representative cohort of 17 276 Canadians followed by Statistics Canada every 2 years between 1994 and 2010. The response rate was 83.6% in the 1994/1995 cycle of data collection. In the seventh cycle (2006/2007), 63.6% of participants from the original cohort were still involved in the study (Statistics Canada, 2007). The study sample included members of the NPHS who were over 18 years of age in 1994/1995, and who reported on childhood adverse experiences in both 1994/1995 and 2006/2007. The final study sample included 7466 participants.

Childhood adverse experiences

A seven-item index asked whether participants had experienced the following as a child or teenager: (1) 2 weeks or longer in hospital; (2) parental divorce; (3) parental unemployment; (4) a frightening experience that was thought about for years after; (5) being sent away from home for wrongdoing; (6) family problems due to parental substance abuse; and (7) physical abuse by someone ‘close’ (Thompson & Cui, Reference Thompson and Cui2000).

The primary outcome measure was consistency of recall for childhood adverse experiences. We examined consistency in two subcohorts. The first subcohort consisted of those who did not report an adverse childhood experience in 1994/1995. Among participants who did not report a childhood adverse experience in 1994/1995, inconsistency was defined as reporting a new childhood adverse experience in 2006/2007 (‘new remembering’). The second subcohort consisted of those who reported an adverse childhood experience in 1994/1995. For this subcohort, inconsistency was defined as failing to report the same experience in 2006/2007 (‘forgetting’).

Mental health factors potentially associated with consistency of recall

Several measures of mental health and stress were investigated for their association with consistency of reporting of childhood adverse experiences. Mental health measures assessed included depression, psychological distress, mastery, work stress and chronic stress. The presence of a major depressive episode was assessed using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form for Major Depression (CIDI-SFMD) (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Andrews, Mroczek, Ustun and Wittchen1998). Depression was separated into four time-sensitive categories for analysis: those who reported depression at both time points, those who were depressed only in 1994/1995 (remitted depression), those who reported depression only in 2006/2007 (developed depression), and those who were not depressed at either time point.

Distress was assessed at both time points using the Kessler K6 psychological distress score (Statistics Canada, 2007). Mastery was measured by a seven-item scale based on the work of Pearlin & Schooler (Reference Pearlin and Schooler1978) in which a higher value indicates a stronger sense of mastery. Work stress was measured by 13 questions that assess job security, autonomy, conflict and satisfaction (Karasek et al. Reference Karasek, Brisson, Kawakami, Houtman, Bongers and Amick1998). Chronic stress was measured by 18 questions that assess stress in personal life, with a primary focus on relationship and family strife (Colman et al. Reference Colman, Garad, Zeng, Naicker, Weeks, Patten, Jones, Thompson and Wild2013). Both stress measures were operationalized on continuous scales. Baseline levels (1994/1995) and changes (i.e. increases or decreases) in these factors between 1994/1995 and 2006/2007 were assessed, in line with our hypothesis that changes in mental health factors would be associated with increased likelihood of inconsistency in reporting adverse childhood experiences from one time point to the next.

Covariates

We included as covariates several demographic factors including old age (above age 60 years), gender, socio-economic status (ratio of family income to Statistics Canada's low-income cut-off score), employment status (currently unemployed or not), education (high school completed or not), marital status and immigration status (immigrant or Canadian-born). As with depression, time-sensitive categories were created to capture differences in employment status, education level and marital status between the two time points (e.g. married at both time points, married only in 1994/1995, married only in 2006/2007, or unmarried at both time points).

Statistical analysis

Logistic regression was used to investigate the association between each of the mental health factors outlined above and consistency in the reporting of childhood adverse experiences. Separate models were run to predict ‘new remembering’ in the first subcohort (with those who consistently reported no adverse experiences as the reference category) and ‘forgetting’ in the second cohort (with consistent reporters of adverse experiences in both 1994/1995 and 2006/2007 as the reference category).

Secondary analyses were also conducted to examine the consistency of reporting for individual events. For each event, logistic regression was used to predict ‘new remembering’ (with those who did not report that specific event at either time point as reference) and ‘forgetting’ (with consistent reporters of that event as reference) from mental health variables. All analyses included demographic factors as covariates.

The NPHS used a multi-stage sampling design with unequal selection probabilities. Normalized sampling weights were used to adjust for unequal selection probabilities and cluster sampling. SAS 9.2 (USA) was used for the analyses. This study was approved by the University of Alberta Health Research Ethics Board.

Results

Of the 3728 participants who reported no adverse experiences in 1994/1995, 802 reported a new event in 2006/2007. Of the 3738 who reported at least one childhood adverse experience in 1994/1995, 2145 responded ‘no’ to the same event in 2006/2007. Of the sample, 54% of the sample was female, and 81% attained a greater than high school level of education (Table 1). At study baseline in 1994/1995, 5.6% of the sample was depressed.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of study participants in 1994/1995 a

s.d., Standard deviation; LICO, low-income cut-off.

a Means and percentages are weighted.

The proportion of participants reporting each adverse experience in 1994/1995 and 2006/2007 is reported in Table 2. Long hospital stays, parental divorce, frightening experiences that were thought about for years after and family problems due to substance abuse were reported by more than 10% of the sample at both time points. The numbers of adverse events reported at each time point are compared in Table 3. It was not uncommon for the number of childhood adverse experiences reported to vary by two or more.

Table 2. Percentage of participants reporting adverse childhood experiences at each time point (weighted percentage)

Table 3. Number of adverse childhood experiences reported in 1994/1995 v. 2006/2007 (weighted frequencies) a

a Data for participants reporting more than four adverse childhood experiences have been suppressed due to low cell sizes.

In subcohort 1 (i.e. those not reporting a childhood adverse experience in 1994/1995), depression status was associated with reporting a new childhood adverse experience in 2006/2007 (χ2 = 17.33, p < 0.001). Participants who were depressed in 2006/2007 but not in 1994/1995 had increased odds of reporting a new adverse childhood experience [odds ratio (OR) 2.25, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.48–3.42]. Baseline psychological distress, chronic stress and work stress were associated with increased odds of reporting a new adverse childhood experience, as were increases in these three variables over time (Table 4). Baseline mastery and increases in mastery were associated with reduced odds of reporting a new childhood adverse experience.

Table 4. Associations between mental health factors and consistency in reporting of childhood adverse experiences a

CI, Confidence interval.

a Models adjusted for gender, age, race, immigration status, socio-economic position, marital status and education.

The converse was observed in subcohort 2 (i.e. those who reported a childhood adverse experience in 1994/1995). The development of depression was associated with reduced odds of answering ‘no’ in 2006/2007 to any previously reported experience (χ2 = 16.20, p < 0.01; OR 0.57, 95% CI 0.41–0.80). Baseline levels and increases in psychological distress, as well as increases in chronic stress, were associated with reduced odds of ‘forgetting’ any event (Table 4). An increase in mastery was associated with increased odds of answering ‘no’ in 2006/2007 to a previously reported adverse childhood experience.

Associations between mental health variables and consistency of reporting for individual events are presented in online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2. Predictors of inconsistency were similar across the individual events.

Discussion

This study prospectively assessed a range of psychological factors potentially associated with recall of childhood adverse experiences, and found several indications that concurrent mental health may influence the recall of childhood adversity. Notably, depression and psychological distress appeared to explain a tendency towards reporting of childhood adverse experiences that had not been reported 12 years earlier, as were increases in work and chronic stress. Conversely, those with an improved sense of mastery appeared less likely to report childhood adverse experiences that had not been previously reported 12 years earlier. In contrast, the development of depression, and increases in psychological distress and chronic stress, were associated with decreased odds of responding ‘no’ to a previously reported event.

These findings run counter to the few studies which have assessed associations between mental state and temporal stability of retrospective reporting of adverse childhood experiences (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004); however, these studies have been limited by relatively small sample sizes and short follow-up periods.

Depression has been previously associated with negative memory bias, especially for information that is emotionally toned (Dalgleish & Watts, Reference Dalgleish and Watts1990); both clinical depression and induced negative mood have been found to lead to a higher probability of recalling negative personal memories (Clark & Teasdale, Reference Clark and Teasdale1982; Dalgleish & Watts, Reference Dalgleish and Watts1990; Whalley et al. Reference Whalley, Rugg and Brewin2012). This is known as the mood-congruency hypothesis, which posits that mood-congruent information is more readily recalled than mood-incongruent information (Blaney, Reference Blaney1986; Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Critchley, Smith and Dolan2005). In line with this hypothesis is our finding that individuals who developed depression, or reported increased psychological distress, were more likely to report an adverse childhood experience in 2006/2007 that was not previously reported.

A similar pattern was observed in this study in relation to stress; participants who recalled new childhood adverse experiences in 2006/2007 were significantly more likely to have suffered increases in either work or chronic stress during the reporting period. Stress may contribute to high state anxiety (Meijer, Reference Meijer2001), which may in turn facilitate retrieval of negative memories (Richards & Whittaker, Reference Richards and Whittaker1990). Anxiety, like depression, has been associated with biases in memory, favouring recall of threat-related information (Mitte, Reference Mitte2008).

Mastery, a psychological concept defined as a sense of control over one's life circumstances (Smeets, Reference Smeets2011), has been found to reduce stress (Wallace & Swaney, Reference Wallace and Swaney2008) and the negative psychiatric effects of stress (Mausbach et al. Reference Mausbach, Patterson, von Känel, Mills, Ancoli-Israel, Dimsdale and Grant2006; Ben-Zur, Reference Ben-Zur2008). Notably, in our assessment of the impact of mastery, those with higher levels of mastery, and who increased their levels of mastery by 2006/2007, were found to be more consistent in their reporting of childhood adversity.

Of those who reported at least one childhood adverse experience in 1994/1995, the majority failed to report the same experience(s) 12 years later. This ‘forgetting’ of negative experiences may in fact be psychologically adaptive (Anderson & Levy, Reference Anderson and Levy2009). Indeed, the persistence of negative memories has been associated with major depression (Joormann et al. Reference Joormann, Nee, Berman, Jonides and Gotlib2010). Results from the present study support this notion – those who developed depression, or experienced increased stress and psychological distress were less likely to ‘forget’ previously reported adverse experiences.

Chronic stress has also been associated with impairments in the extinction of negative memories and fear responses to traumatic events (Miracle et al. Reference Miracle, Brace, Huyck, Singler and Wellman2006; Milad et al. Reference Milad, Pitman, Ellis, Gold, Shin, Lasko, Zeidan, Handwerger, Orr and Rauch2009). Consistent with these findings, increases in chronic stress in the present study were associated with reduced odds of ‘forgetting’ a previously reported adverse experience.

The childhood adverse experiences reported in the NPHS were varied in terms of severity. One possibility is that consistency of recall may be influenced by different factors depending on the severity of the event. Analyses of individual events revealed similar patterns of association between psychological variables and consistency of recall, lending support to our conclusions regarding the impact of these psychological variables on recall. However, relatively low sample sizes for certain events may have limited power to detect effects, and we therefore urge caution in interpreting discrepancies in results between specific events.

Another important methodological concern in the present study is that the age at which an adverse experience occurs has also been found to have an impact on the recall of such experiences (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004), as well as on the presence of depression and PTSD (Schoedl et al. Reference Schoedl, Costa, Mari, Mello, Tyrka, Carpenter and Price2010). Since childhood adverse experiences were retrospectively assessed in this survey, and participants were not asked about the time at which the adverse experience occurred, we could not assess the impact of age at the time of the experience on recall consistency.

In the present study, it is unknown whether new events reported in 2006/2007 represent real recovered memories or false memories. Research suggests that false memories may differ qualitatively from real ones – for example, in vividness, detail and confidence (Porter et al. Reference Porter, Yuille and Lehman1999). Participants in the NPHS were asked to report whether each event had occurred, but did not supply further details about the event.

Our study also had several notable strengths. It was conducted using longitudinal data from a large, nationally representative sample, and controlled for both baseline levels of and changes over time in key explanatory variables. Importantly, we also controlled for old age, which may have confounded the assessment of predictive factors in the consistency in the recall of childhood adverse experiences in previous studies, due the effect of older age on memory deficits (Nielsen et al. Reference Nielsen, Lolk and Kragh-Sørensen1998; Ros & Latorre, Reference Ros and Latorre2010).

In conclusion, these results show that cross-temporal inconsistencies in the reporting of adverse childhood events are common, and may be associated with factors describing concurrent mental state. These findings have important implications for studies conducted to date, many of which have relied on the recall of adverse childhood experiences without controlling for factors associated with current mental state in adulthood.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002032

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Norlien Foundation. This research was undertaken, in part, with funding from the Canada Research Chairs programme for I.C. The authors would like to thank Ms Irene Wong of Statistics Canada for her assistance with data access and use, and Dr Sonja Swanson for her advice regarding the analytical approach. The research and analysis are based on data from Statistics Canada and the opinions expressed do not represent the views of Statistics Canada.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Anderson, MC, Levy, BJ (2009). Suppressing unwanted memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18, 89194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Zur, H (2008). Personal resources of mastery–optimism, and communal support beliefs, as predictors of posttraumatic stress in uprooted Israelis. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping 21, 295307.Google Scholar
Bernet, CZ, Stein, MB (1999). Relationship of childhood maltreatment to the onset and course of major depression in adulthood. Depression and Anxiety 9, 169174.Google Scholar
Blaney, PH (1986). Affect and memory: a review. Psychological Bulletin 99, 229246.Google Scholar
Brennen, T, Hasanović, M, Zotović, M, Blix, I, Solheim Skar, AM, Prelić, NK, Mehmedović, I, Pajević, I, Popović, N, Gavrilov-Jerković, V (2010). Trauma exposure in childhood impairs the ability to recall specific autobiographical memories in late adolescence. Journal of Traumatic Stress 23, 240247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, DM, Teasdale, JD (1982). Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 91, 8795.Google Scholar
Colman, I, Garad, Y, Zeng, Y, Naicker, K, Weeks, M, Patten, SB, Jones, PB, Thompson, AH, Wild, TC (2013). Stress and development of depression and heavy drinking in adulthood: moderating effects of childhood trauma. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 48, 265274.Google Scholar
Dalgleish, T, Watts, FN (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review 10, 589604.Google Scholar
Dong, M, Giles, WH, Felitti, VJ, Dube, SR, Williams, JE, Chapman, DP, Anda, RF (2004). Insights into causal pathways for ischemic heart disease: adverse childhood experiences study. Circulation 110, 17611766.Google Scholar
Fergusson, DM, Horwood, LJ, Woodward, LJ (2000). The stability of child abuse reports: a longitudinal study of the reporting behaviour of young adults. Psychological Medicine 30, 529544.Google Scholar
Finlay-Jones, R, Scott, R, Duncan-Jones, P, Byrne, D, Henderson, S (1981). The reliability of reports of early separations. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 15, 2731.Google Scholar
Gotlib, IH, Joormann, J (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annual Review in Clinical Psychology 6, 285312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardt, J, Rutter, M (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 45, 260273.Google Scholar
Heim, C, Nemeroff, CB (2001). The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biological Psychiatry 49, 10231039.Google Scholar
Hovens, JGFM, Wiersma, JE, Giltay, EJ, Van Oppen, P, Spinhoven, P, Penninx, BWJH, Zitman, FG (2010). Childhood life events and childhood trauma in adult patients with depressive, anxiety and comorbid disorders vs. controls. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 122, 6674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J, Nee, DE, Berman, MG, Jonides, J, Gotlib, IH (2010). Interference resolution in major depression. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 10, 2133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karasek, R, Brisson, C, Kawakami, N, Houtman, I, Bongers, P, Amick, B (1998). The Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ): an instrument for internationally comparative assessments of psychosocial job characteristics. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 3, 322355.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Andrews, G, Mroczek, D, Ustun, B, Wittchen, HU (1998). The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short-Form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 7, 171185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, PA, Critchley, HD, Smith, AP, Dolan, RJ (2005). Brain mechanisms for mood congruent memory facilitation. NeuroImage 25, 12141223.Google Scholar
Mausbach, BT, Patterson, TL, von Känel, R, Mills, PJ, Ancoli-Israel, S, Dimsdale, JE, Grant, I (2006). Personal mastery attenuates the effect of caregiving stress on psychiatric morbidity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 194, 132134.Google Scholar
Meijer, J (2001). Stress in the relation between trait and state anxiety. Psychological Reports 88, 947964.Google Scholar
Milad, MR, Pitman, RK, Ellis, CB, Gold, AL, Shin, LM, Lasko, NB, Zeidan, MA, Handwerger, K, Orr, SP, Rauch, SL (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 66, 10751082.Google Scholar
Miracle, AD, Brace, MF, Huyck, KD, Singler, SA, Wellman, CL (2006). Chronic stress impairs recall of extinction of conditioned fear. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 85, 213218.Google Scholar
Mitte, K (2008). Memory bias for threatening information in anxiety and anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 134, 886911.Google Scholar
Moffitt, TE, Caspi, A, Harrington, H, Milne, BJ, Melchior, M, Goldberg, D, Poulton, R (2007). Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: childhood risk factors in a birth cohort followed to age 32. Psychological Medicine 37, 441452.Google Scholar
Nelson, S, Baldwin, N, Taylor, J (2012). Mental health problems and medically unexplained physical symptoms in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse: an integrative literature review. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 19, 211220.Google Scholar
Nielsen, H, Lolk, A, Kragh-Sørensen, P (1998). Age-associated memory impairment – pathological memory decline or normal aging? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 39, 3337.Google Scholar
Paivio, SC (2001). Stability of retrospective self-reports of child abuse and neglect before and after therapy for child abuse issues. Child Abuse and Neglect 25, 10531068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlin, LI, Schooler, C (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 19, 221.Google Scholar
Porter, S, Yuille, JC, Lehman, DR (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: implications for the recovered memory debate. Law and Human Behavior 23, 517537.Google Scholar
Richards, A, Whittaker, TM (1990). Effects of anxiety and mood manipulation in autobiographical memory. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 29, 145153.Google Scholar
Ros, L, Latorre, JM (2010). Gender and age differences in the recall of affective autobiographical memories using the autobiographical memory test. Personality and Individual Differences 49, 950954.Google Scholar
Schoedl, AF, Costa, MCP, Mari, JJ, Mello, MF, Tyrka, AR, Carpenter, LL, Price, LH (2010). The clinical correlates of reported childhood sexual abuse: an association between age at trauma onset and severity of depression and PTSD in adults. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse 19, 156170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, T (2011). Acute stress impairs memory retrieval independent of time of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 495501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada (2007). National Population Health Survey Cycle 7 (2006–2007). Public Use Microdata Files Documentation: Ottawa.Google Scholar
Thompson, AH, Cui, X (2000). Increasing childhood trauma in Canada: findings from the National Population Health Survey, 1994/95. Canadian Journal of Public Health 91, 197200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, K, Swaney, G (2008). Resilience in American Indian older adults: an examination of the stress buffering roles of personal and communal mastery. Gerontologist 48, 605605.Google Scholar
Whalley, MG, Rugg, MD, Brewin, CR (2012). Autobiographical memory in depression: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Research 201, 98106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widom, CS (1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder in abused and neglected children grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 12231229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widom, CS, Morris, S (1997). Accuracy of adult recollections of childhood victimization, part 2: childhood sexual abuse. Psychological Assessment 9, 3446.Google Scholar
Williams, LM (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: a prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62, 11671176.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of study participants in 1994/1995a

Figure 1

Table 2. Percentage of participants reporting adverse childhood experiences at each time point (weighted percentage)

Figure 2

Table 3. Number of adverse childhood experiences reported in 1994/1995 v. 2006/2007 (weighted frequencies)a

Figure 3

Table 4. Associations between mental health factors and consistency in reporting of childhood adverse experiencesa

Supplementary material: File

Colman supplementary material S1

Colman supplementary material

Download Colman supplementary material S1(File)
File 56.3 KB