INTRODUCTION
Organizational learning, or the process through which organizations change or modify their mental models, processes or knowledge, maintaining or improving their performance (Cyert & March, Reference Cyert and March1963; Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Hedberg, Reference Hedberg1981; Senge, Reference Senge1990; Brown & Duguid, Reference Brown and Duguid1991; Dibella, Nevis, & Gould, Reference DiBella, Nevis and Gould1996), has become an important field of research in management and organization literature.
One of the most important classical typologies within the organizational learning literature is the distinction between adaptive and generative learning. However, the processes of these types of learning have not been widely analyzed and incorporated into the organizational learning process (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010); taking into account that the literature understands that their processes are different (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974; Fiol & Lyles, Reference Fiol and Lyles1985; Senge, Reference Senge1990). Thus, Crossan, Maurer, and White (Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011) stress the importance of developing organizational learning frameworks that encompass types of learning.
To address this shortcoming, we propose in this paper a framework of organizational learning that will allow us to differentiate the processes of three types of organizational learning: one for zero learning, one for adaptive learning and one for generative learning (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972; Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Senge, Reference Senge1990): the ZAG (Zero, Adaptive, Generative) organizational learning framework. Zero learning means internalizing norms, routines, procedures or knowledge; adaptive learning implies improving them and finally generative learning involves questioning them.
Moreover, most of the organizational learning models and frameworks focus on cognition and behavior (Huber, Reference Huber1991; Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999; Crossan, Maurer, & White, Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011), so their phases are cognitive or behavioral constructs (e.g., knowledge acquisition, information distribution, intuition or interpretation). Nevertheless, organizational learning literature suggests important relations with concepts such as consciousness – mindfulness – (Levinthal & Rerup, Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006; Weick & Sutcliffe, Reference Weick and Sutcliffe2006) or emotions (Vince, Reference Vince2001, Reference Vince2002; Crossan, Maurer, & White, Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011), which have scarcely been incorporated into a process of organizational learning (Crossan, Maurer, & White, Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011).
Consciousness can be defined as the degree of awareness of one’s inner and outer worlds, of being mentally perceptive, and of feeling the undivided wholeness of existence (Wilber, Reference Wilber2000). Mindfulness is considered as a state of high consciousness (Glomb, Duffy, Bono, & Yang, Reference Glomb, Duffy, Bono and Yang2011). Fischer, Shaver, and Carnochan (Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990: 84) define emotion as a ‘discrete, innate, functional, biosocial action and expression system,’ and posit that emotions can be divided into two groups: the positive emotions, like love and joy, imply goal advancement on the one hand, and the negative emotions such as anger, sadness and fear, imply goal hindrance on the other hand.
Consequently, we propose an organizational learning framework that incorporates three subprocesses: one for zero learning, one for adaptive learning and one for generative learning (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972; Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Senge, Reference Senge1990): the ZAG organizational learning framework. In order to do so, we will base the organizational learning framework on consciousness levels (Langer, Reference Langer1989, Reference Langer1997; Weick & Sutcliffe, Reference Weick and Sutcliffe2006; Dane, Reference Dane2011), emotions (Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990), and also behavioral and cognitive aspects like stress (Cangelosi & Dill, Reference Cangelosi and Dill1965; Selye, Reference Selye1987) and complex behaviors (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996; Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010).
In order to explain clearly the ZAG organizational learning framework through the paper, we will illustrate chronologically every phase or stage of the model. First, we will describe our conceptualization of organizational learning. Second, we will focus on the consciousness levels. Third, we will explain their effect on emotions, stress and complex behaviors. Fourth, we will focus on the outcomes of learning and the learning types. Finally, we suggest the framework’s implications for academics and practitioners, limitations and future research lines.
CONCEPTUALIZATION OF ORGANIZATIONAL LEARNING
Organizations are systems formed by other systems or agents like individuals and groups (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010). We consider that zero, adaptive and generative learning might happen in any agent – at individual or collective levels – which implies affirming that organizations learn through individuals (Simon, Reference Simon1991) and also through communities (Brown & Duguid, Reference Brown and Duguid1991). Learning may start in individuals and in relationships, which means following a comprehensive view or accepting the two main perspectives of organizational learning, individual and social (Örtenblad, Reference Örtenblad2002; Elkjaer, Reference Elkjaer2004; Chiva & Alegre, Reference Chiva and Alegre2005). Thus, organizational learning can occur in individuals, who then transfer it to groups, and finally to the organization (Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999), or it can be initiated in relationships in groups, and then become institutionalized (Brown & Duguid, Reference Brown and Duguid1991).
Similarly, consciousness, emotions or behaviors can happen at individual and group levels. In fact, previous research has already considered concepts like social emotions (Bernett, Bird, Moll, Frith, & Blakemore, Reference Bernett, Bird, Moll, Frith and Blakemore2009), social behaviors (Homans, Reference Homans1958) or organizational consciousness (Pandey & Gupta, Reference Pandey and Gupta2008). Thus, consciousness, emotions, stress or complex system behaviors might also take place simultaneously at individual and social levels. Accordingly, there is a certain multilevel interaction (Langley, Smallman, Tsoukas, & Van de Ven, Reference Langley, Smallman, Tsoukas and Van de Ven2013).
The organizational learning framework presented in this paper contains three related subprocesses that occur at different levels – individual, group and organization – and that incorporate different facets like consciousness, emotions, stress and complex system behaviors. These facets are essential in the framework as they bind the process together. They represent a sequence and progression through the learning process. Figure 1 describes the framework presented in this paper.
Figure 1 The zero, adaptive, generative framework of organizational learning
Organizational learning is considered as the process through which organizations change or modify their mental models, processes, rules, behaviors or knowledge, which Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) name organizational explicate order in reference to Bohm’s work (Reference Bohm1980). Therefore, we consider alterations in the organizational explicate order (Bohm, Reference Bohm1980) as the outcome of organizational learning. Organizational learning can be conceived of as a principal means of achieving improved or new organizational explicate order. But what is an explicate order? Bohm (Reference Bohm1980) considers that there are two orders in the universe: explicate and implicate orders. Within the implicate order everything is connected and enfolded into everything else. This contrasts with the explicate order or manifested world where things are unfolded. Thus, explicate order is the manifested world, which is represented through knowledge, schemas, rules, mental models, paradigms, etc. Organizational explicate order means any mental model, paradigm, idea, process, knowledge, information, schema, rule, etc., shared by most of the members in an organization. The explicate order derives or unfolds from the implicate order. These concepts are closely related to Plato’s theory of forms and appearances, respectively. Plato suggested that the world as it seems to us is not the real world, but only a shadow of the real world: the world of appearances (explicate order) is the shadow of a more profound world of forms or ideas (implicate order). Hence, when one approaches the implicate order, where everything is related, a new explicate order unfolds. Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) consider a new explicate order as the outcome of generative learning and an improved explicate order as the outcome of adaptive learning.
If organizational explicate order might be considered as the outcome of organizational learning; what provokes organizational learning? The organizational learning literature has traditionally considered that learning within organizations is generated by external or environmental factors (Hedberg, Reference Hedberg1981) or internal or organizational conditions (Brown & Duguid, Reference Brown and Duguid1991; Cook & Yanow, Reference Cook and Yanow1993). Thus, we name stressors as the catalysts of organizational learning, the external or internal forces or conditions acting on organizations. Stressors are considered to be the initial triggers of organizational learning: something different or challenging happening within or around the organization, the effects of unwanted decisions, external deadlines, shifts in decision making, new interactions within the organization or with groups external to it, etc. Stressors are anything that causes stress in an organism or a system, in our case, an organization. Once stressors emerge, individuals and groups within an organization may notice them, and then appraise them (Atkins & Parker, Reference Atkins and Parker2012). In fact, learning could be considered as the consequence of a process that starts with noticing or scanning data (Daft & Weick, Reference Daft and Weick1984; Huber, Reference Huber1991). Once the stressor is noticed, individuals and groups appraise and interpret it, or give meaning to it (Daft & Weick, Reference Daft and Weick1984; Atkins & Parker, Reference Atkins and Parker2012). Yet, both noticing and interpreting will be affected by what we know, or the organizational explicate order.
Maturana and Varela (Reference Maturana and Varela1980), based on their logic of self-reference, consider that the information we obtain from the environment is not gained impartially; we are self referential; we see and interpret what we know. We assign patterns of meaning and significance to the world in which we operate. Therefore, knowledge can be considered as a hindrance to learning (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010); in other words the organizational explicate order might condition how we see and interpret a stressor. However, both noticing and interpreting a stressor will be affected not only by what we know, but also by our level of consciousness.
CONSCIOUNESS LEVELS
Human consciousness is a long-established concept that has been analyzed and developed across a range of disciplines such as philosophy, psychology or medicine. According to Antony (Reference Antony2002), there are distinct views, kinds and meanings of consciousness. For the purposes of this paper, we take into account the definition given by Wilber (Reference Wilber2000), who views consciousness as the degree of ‘awareness of one’s inner and outer worlds, being mentally perceptive and feeling the undivided wholeness of existence.’ According to Kofman (Reference Kofman2006), high consciousness is the ability to experience reality, to be aware of our inner and outer worlds, to be awake, mindful. Senge, Scharmer, Jaworski, and Flowers (Reference Senge, Scharmer, Jaworski and Flowers2005) use the term ‘presence’ to represent being fully conscious and aware in the present moment, being open beyond one’s preconceptions and historical ways of making sense.
Boucouvalas (Reference Boucouvalas1993) argues that shifts in states of consciousness are essentially a major alteration in the way that the mind functions, as each state of consciousness has its unique configuration and pattern, and involves progressive transformations of the contextual foundation of the conscious. These transformations entail movement toward more complexity, greater awareness, and less egocentrism. Stressors will be then noticed and interpreted depending on the level of individuals and groups consciouness.
In this paper, we consider the existence of three levels of consciousness (Table 1). In order to do so, we draw on several authors and mainly two groups of literatures. Regarding the first one, Wilber (Reference Wilber2000, Reference Wilber2006), based on previous authors like Gebser (Reference Gebser1949), Graves (Reference Graves1970), Kohlberg (Reference Kohlberg1981) and Beck and Cowan (Reference Beck and Cowan1996), argues the existence of three levels, from the lowest to the highest degree of consciousness: egocentrism, sociocentrism and worldcentrism. These levels are related to the individuals’ breadth of focus: from the most centered in the individual to the broadest centered in everything and everyone. Other authors (Crocker & Canevello, Reference Crocker and Canevello2012; Rynes, Bartunek, Dutton, & Margolis, Reference Rynes, Bartunek, Dutton and Margolis2012) name them egosystem and ecosystem, respectively.
Table 1 Consciousness levels
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:92808:20160418074637905-0966:S1833367214000881_tab1.gif?pub-status=live)
Considering the second group of literature, the three levels of consciousness are related to three states of consciousness: mindlessness, flow and mindfulness. A high degree of consciousness is closely related to the term mindfulness, which has been much discussed in the literature of organizations (e.g., Brown & Ryan, Reference Brown and Ryan2003; Fiol & O’Connor, Reference Fiol and O’Connor2003; Levinthal & Rerup, Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006; Weick & Sutcliffe, Reference Weick and Sutcliffe2006), mainly based on the works by Langer (Reference Langer1989, Reference Langer1997). Mindfulness is a state of consciousness in which attention is focused on present-moment phenomena occurring both externally and internally and maintaining a wide attentional breadth (Dane, Reference Dane2011). This literature considers the opposite of mindfulness to be mindlessness (Langer, Reference Langer1997), which involves reliance on previously established categorizations of information, a reduced level of attention and vigilance, and the adherence to a rigid rule system governing behavior (Langer, Reference Langer1989; Ray, Baker, & Plowman, Reference Ray, Baker and Plowman2011) or ‘dead’ routines that are static and automatically repeated (Pentland & Feldman, Reference Pentland and Feldman2008). Furthermore, Dane (Reference Dane2011) understands that there is another narrower, more focused level of attention: flow (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi1990; Jackson, Reference Jackson1992), which involves directing attention to present-moment phenomena and entails a narrower attentional breadth.
Therefore, based on these two streams from the literature, we consider the existence of three levels of consciousness: low, medium and high. The higher the level of consciousness the more aware individuals and groups are of the stressor, its solutions, connections and relationships, and so, the higher the possibilities of learning.
Low level of consciousness
Concerning their state of consciousness, individuals and groups with low consciousness are mindless. Brown and Ryan (Reference Brown and Ryan2003) consider mindlessness as the relative absence of mindfulness; this takes place when individuals behave compulsively or automatically, without awareness of or attention to one’s behavior (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan1980). Fiol and O’Connor (Reference Fiol and O’Connor2003) suggest that mindlessness is characterized by relying on past categories, acting on automatic pilot or routine, precluding attention to new information and fixating on a single perspective (Langer, Reference Langer1997; Weick, Sutcliffe, & Obstfeld, Reference Weick, Sutcliffe and Obstfeld1999; Levinthal & Rerup, Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006).
Those who are mindless operate from a state of reduced attention that tends to lead to mechanically employing cognitively and emotionally rigid, rule-based behaviors. Trapped in previously created categories, these individuals easily confuse the stability of their assumptions with stability in the world, thus giving themselves a false reading on their surroundings (Langer, Reference Langer1989). Argote (Reference Argote2006) argues that less mindful behaviors involve fewer cognitive processes and greater reliance on familiar routines. Mindlessness might be linked to repetition, boredom, exploitation of past success, etc., which is closely related to emotions like apathy or anxiety. In fact, mindlessness implies having internalized norms, procedures and established action repertories – Bateson’s (Reference Bateson1972) zero learning – and using these routinized behaviors again and again. Mindlessness is marked by a rigid use of information during which the individual or the group are not aware of its potentially novel aspects or its possibility to introduce variations in the current routines. As a consequence, one deals with information as though it has a single meaning and can only be used in that way, which results in a lack of attention to details (Langer & Piper, Reference Langer and Piper1987).
Weick, Sutcliffe, and Obstfeld (Reference Weick, Sutcliffe and Obstfeld1999) state that when fewer cognitive processes are activated less often, the resulting state is one of mindlessness characterized by reliance on past categories, acting on ‘automatic pilot,’ and fixation on a single perspective without awareness that things could be otherwise.
Wilber (Reference Wilber2006) underlines that at the egocentric stage individuals remain selfish and narcissistic. This does not imply that individuals have no feelings for others or are amoral, but that compared with subsequent development, their feelings and morals are still centered on their own impulses, physiological needs and instinctual discharges.
Medium level of consciousness
Concerning the literature related to the state of consciousness, the medium level is associated to flow. Flow, like mindfulness, involves directing attention to present-moment phenomena; however, it may be distinguished from mindfulness in that it involves narrower attentional breadth (Dane, Reference Dane2011). Flow was a term first coined by Csikszentmihalyi (Reference Csikszentmihalyi1990) who defined it as the holistic sensation that people feel when they act with total involvement. Flow is a high level of engagement in an optimally challenging activity that produces intense concentration and a strong feeling of control (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi1990; Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Nakamura and Csikszentmihalyi2009; Dane, Reference Dane2011). Concentration is strongly related to flow (Quinn, Reference Quinn2005), and according to Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) strongly related to adaptive learning. Quinn (Reference Quinn2005) argued that flow means that one becomes so focused on the task that one no longer perceives oneself as being distinct from the activity. As such, an individual in a flow state is unlikely to attend to a range of internal stimuli and is unlikely to perceive external phenomena not centrally relevant to the task at hand (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi1990).
As the individual transcends the ego to support or sustain other things, tasks or other people, Wilber (Reference Wilber2006) terms the next stage ethnocentric and sociocentric, or focused on the group. Individuals move from ‘me’ to ‘us’ by rejecting egocentrism and engaging in the group or any activity. Identity extends beyond one’s own body to embrace cultural roles, collective identities and shared values or even objects, things or activities. Wilber (Reference Wilber2000) considers this stage to represent the mature ego: a person at this level has a highly differentiated, reflexive self-structure; identity issues need to be explored and the processes of philosophical contemplation and introspection can take place.
Amiot, Vallerand, and Blanchard (Reference Amiot, Vallerand and Blanchard2006) affirm that the concept of passion has some ties with other concepts, such as those of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi1990, Reference Csikszentmihalyi2000; Vallerand & Houlfort, Reference Vallerand and Houlfort2003) and intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan1985). Thus, passionate people should experience more flow than less passionate individuals (Amiot, Vallerand, & Blanchard, Reference Amiot, Vallerand and Blanchard2006).
High level of consciousness
Based on an integrative review of the literature, Dane (Reference Dane2011) defines mindfulness as a state of consciousness, in which attention is focused on present-moment phenomena occurring both externally and internally and maintaining a wide attentional breadth. As a state of consciousness (Rosch, Reference Brown and Eisenhardt2007), mindfulness is not a quality that some individuals possess and others lack (Dane, Reference Dane2011). Attaining a mindful state of consciousness is an inherent human capacity, an assertion implying that most people have been or at least can be mindful at one point or another. Nevertheless, research shows that, owing to dispositional tendencies, some people may be in a mindful state of consciousness more often than others (Dane, Reference Dane2011). Hülsheger, Alberts, Feinholdt, and Lang (Reference Hülsheger, Alberts, Feinholdt and Lang2013) consider mindfulness as an inherent human capacity that varies in strength, both across situations and persons.
The state of consciousness characterizing mindfulness is one in which attention focuses on the ‘here and now,’ the present moment (Weick & Putnam, Reference Weick and Putnam2006; Dane, Reference Dane2011), as opposed to preoccupation with thoughts about the past or the future (Brown & Ryan, Reference Brown and Ryan2003). Mindfulness involves attending to external and internal phenomena, because they are both in the present moment (Brown & Ryan, Reference Brown and Ryan2003). Therefore, the more conscious we are, the more connections we perceive around us, and the more connected we feel with everything and everyone. In this line, Brown, Ryan, and Creswell (Reference Brown, Ryan and Creswell2007) consider that the study of mindfulness presents challenges to popular Western cultural attitudes, and to some established paradigms that emphasize the primacy of the ego.
When individuals are or become more conscious they are more aware of their internal and external worlds, which makes them less egocentric and more altruistic (Boucouvalas, Reference Boucouvalas1993; Wilber, Reference Wilber2000). Boucouvalas (Reference Boucouvalas1993) considers that when conscious individuals perceive their commonality with all living creatures and thus protect the environment and the system, they are motivated from within, not just from a feeling of moral obligation emanating from externally given ‘shoulds’ and ‘oughts.’ In fact, she relates high consciousness with Bohm’s (Reference Bohm1980) implicate order of the universe, which is a world of interconnectedness, where new explicate orders arise and generative learning is developed.
According to Wilber (Reference Wilber2000, Reference Wilber2006), high consciousness imply not focusing on the ego, but on everything and everyone; the self is totally integrated with the whole world. Compassion and altruism become essential issues: unselfish caring for others, noticing another person’s suffering, empathically feeling that person’s pain and acting in a manner intended to ease that suffering (Dutton, Worline, Frost, & Lilius, Reference Dutton, Worline, Frost and Lilius2006; Lilius, Worline, Maitlis, Kanov, Dutton, & Frost, Reference Lilius, Worline, Maitlis, Kanov, Dutton and Frost2008). A high level of consciousness or mindfulness implies a certain transcendence of one’s own concepts and knowledge, and a greater importance of others, not only one’s own group or team, but the rest of the world.
Most of the literature on mindfulness understands that this state produces a void (Langer, Reference Langer1989; Weick & Putnam, Reference Weick and Putnam2006), which is a non-conceptual phase: one sees things deeply, down below the level of concepts and opinions, one lets concepts go, unlearns (Hedberg, Reference Hedberg1981) or changes in the routine (Feldman & Pentland, Reference Feldman and Pentland2003). There is a non-judgmental awareness, a greater sensitivity to one’s environment, more openness to new information, the creation of new categories for structuring perception, the emergence of improvisation and enhanced awareness of multiple perspectives and intuition appears (Weick & Putnam, Reference Weick and Putnam2006; Dane & Pratt, Reference Dane and Pratt2009). All of this implies approaching the implicate order, which is a holistic perception of the world, and consequently learning generatively (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010).
Compassion and dispassion are connected to a high level of consciousness – mindfulness (Langer, Reference Langer1989), transcendence (Maslow, Reference Maslow1971) or worldcentrism (Wilber, Reference Wilber2000) – as they imply a certain detachment from ideas and concepts and inquiry into them, and a search for a connection to everything, environment, others, etc. (implicate order).
EMOTIONS, STRESS AND COMPLEX BEHAVIORS
In this epigraph we will relate the three levels of consciousness, which are considered essential for the ZAG organizational learning framework, to other facets like emotions, stress and complex behaviors.
The term emotion comes from the French word emouvoir, which means to stir up, move; in fact it is understood as a driving force behind some actions. Fischer, Shaver, and Carnochan (Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990: 84) define emotion as a ‘discrete, innate, functional, biosocial action and expression system,’ and posit that emotions can be divided into two groups. The first group or positive emotions, like love and joy, imply goal advancement. The second group, negative emotions such as anger, sadness and fear, imply goal hindrance. In this framework, we suggest that positive emotions like passion, compassion and dispassion promote eustress, while negative emotions like apathy and anxiety bring about distress.
The specific qualities of an emotion are determined by noticing the event and also by interpreting it (appraisal) (Lazarus, Reference Lazarus1991; Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross and Thompson2007; Atkins & Parker, Reference Atkins and Parker2012). The higher the level of consciousness, the more positive the emotions will be. In this vein, Atkins and Parker (Reference Atkins and Parker2012) consider that a high level of consciousness, mindfulness, has a positive effect on compassion, a positive emotion. The more aware we are of the stressor (event, change, etc.) and ourselves, the more attentive and present we will be, the more interested, passionate, and the less egocentric, fearful, anxious and apathetic we will feel about it.
On the other hand, emotions affect how we manage stressors, or what form our stress will take: good or bad. Stress, considered by Selye (Reference Selye1987) as an inevitable consequence of living, is defined as the physical and psychological responses to adverse conditions or influences. However, Selye (Reference Selye1987) distinguished between two types of stress: eustress or good stress, and distress or bad stress, meaning that the former implies a positive response to stressors and the latter a negative response. According to Le Fevre, Matheny, and Kolt (Reference Le Fevre, Matheny and Kolt2003) and Selye (Reference Selye1987), positive emotions are determinant for maximizing eustress and negative emotions for maximizing distress.
Furthermore, individuals and groups within organizations try to respond to diverse and adverse conditions with either positive behaviors (eustress) that improve or create new conditions, that is, creative tension, edge of chaos (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996) or negative behaviors (distress), based on indolence, simply doing the same, that is, stability, or anger, provoking instability.
Several authors (Lazarus, Reference Lazarus1991; Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross and Thompson2007; Atkins & Parker, Reference Atkins and Parker2012) state that emotions are determined by noticing the event and also interpreting it, which we consider is moderated by the organizational explicate order and the level of consciousness. The higher the level of consciousness, the more aware we are about the stressor (event, change, etc.) and ourselves, the more attentive and present we are, avoiding thoughts about the past and the future, the more connections we will see between the situation and other aspects. Therefore, we will feel less frightened, worried and uninterested; thus, negative emotions like anxiety and apathy are moved aside, and positive emotions like passion, compassion or dispassion appear.
Internal and external organizational stressors provoke stress within organizations that, depending on individuals’ and groups’ consciousness and emotions, will fall into the categories of either eustress or distress (Selye, Reference Selye1987). In this paper, we consider that two negative emotions, apathy and anxiety, will bring about distress. Kaplan and Sadock (Reference Kaplan and Sadock1991) define apathy as a state of lacking inner feeling and emotion, a lack of interest and emotional involvement with one’s environment. The term apathy is synonymous with lack of participation, reluctance, lack of incentive and isolation (Ghadimi & Nir, Reference Ghadimi and Nir2011). On the other hand, anxiety is the displeasing feeling of fear and concern (Davison, Reference Davison2008), and consequently can create feelings of fear, worry, uneasiness and dread (Bouras & Holt, Reference Bouras and Holt2007). In fact, the two emotions are highly connected (Charlton & Birkett, Reference Charlton and Birkett1995) and cause goal hindrance (Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990), which implies difficulty in solving the stressor. According to Le Fevre, Matheny, and Kolt (Reference Le Fevre, Matheny and Kolt2003), negative emotions will provoke distress, or negative stress, that will impede solving the problem or incidence. Two main options will come out from this, either stability or instability.
Consequently, we consider that eustress (Selye, Reference Selye1987) or positive stress is fostered by positive emotions such as passion, compassion and dispassion, which give rise to goal advancement (Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990), or an active and positive attitude toward the stressor. Vallerand and Houlfort (Reference Vallerand and Houlfort2003) view passion as a strong inclination toward an activity that individuals like, that they value, and in which they invest time and energy. Dutton et al. (Reference Dutton, Worline, Frost and Lilius2006) define compassion as noticing, feeling and responding to another’s suffering. They also consider that compassion must involve some sort of response. On the other hand, dispassion implies detachment or not being dictated by passion; not proceeding from temper or bias; impartial, not warped, prejudiced, swerved or carried away by passion; judicial; calm; and composed. Both compassion and dispassion are associated with transpersonal development (Daniels, Reference Daniels2009), and can also be connected to emotional intelligence (Salovey & Mayer, Reference Salovey and Mayer1990; Davies, Stankov, & Roberts, Reference Davies, Stankov and Roberts1998).
Passion, compassion and dispassion are positive emotions with which to face stressors: either to concentrate on them, to detach from them, or to take into account others in order to present a new view. In any case, they are accompanied by eustress or positive stress. Distress and eustress represent the two options individuals and groups have when facing stressors. The appearance of one or the other will depend on whether negative or positive emotions are present.
Distress and eustress will determine individuals and groups behaviors on learning. As distress and negative emotions are related to goal hindrance (Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990), little learning is supposed to emerge. On the contrary, eustress and positive emotions, linked to goal advancement (Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, Reference Fischer, Shaver and Carnochan1990), might foster learning. Therefore, we consider two behaviors: one that hinders learning and one that fosters learning.
In similar terms, the complex adaptive systems theory (Gell-Mann, Reference Gell-Mann1994), one of the most dominant theoretical approaches of the complexity paradigm in Management Sciences, considers that complex systems, like organizations, can develop three types of behaviors: stability, instability and edge of chaos (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996). The edge of chaos is regarded as a phase of self-organization, representing the ‘highest effective complexity’ (Gell-Mann, Reference Gell-Mann1994) that allows the emergence of new organizational patterns. A complex adaptive system can learn and change only when effective complexity is sizeable, that is, in conditions that are intermediate between chaos and stability, between order and disorder (Gell-Mann, Reference Gell-Mann1994). Carlisle and McMillan (Reference Carlisle and McMillan2006: 4) state that ‘their ability to learn and adapt is underpinned by key self-organizing behaviours including exploration and experimentation.’ This is what Stacey (Reference Stacey1993, Reference Stacey1995) calls ‘creative tension,’ a state of moderated instability gives rise to the appearance of emergent processes like change, creativity and innovation. In the edge of chaos, organization moves away from equilibrium, accepts disorder and uncertainty (Stacey, Reference Stacey1995). The edge chaos involves a process of transition from an older state to a new state produced by fluid and informal processes as well as spontaneous interactions between self-managing teams (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996; Koch and Leitner, Reference Koch and Leitner2008).
Nevertheless, effective complexity could be viewed as low when there is a high level of chaos and the environment is random, and could be also perceived as low when a system operates in an environment that is highly stable, in the sense that its component systems behave in a perfectly regular manner (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996). In this situation very little happens and little learning or change is needed (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996). Therefore, we consider that there are two types of behaviors. First are the complex ones, between order and chaos, at the edge of chaos that promote learning and exploration (March, Reference March1991). Weick and Westley (Reference Weick and Westley1996) argue that a juxtaposition of order and chaos precedes organizational learning. Second, the non-complex ones, fully unstable or stable, which hinder learning (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996). Stability involves only reproduction or exploitation, using what has been learned, or continuing the same or past routines or models (potentially defective) and paradigms – Bateson’s (Reference Bateson1972) zero learning. In a similar vein, Miller (Reference Miller1993) uses the term simplicity to refer to organizational attitudes that highlight successful past approaches, which might eventually bring organizational decline. Conversely, instability entails chaos and conflicts, which impedes or blocks any learning, but might bring about dysfunctions that cause ineffectiveness and even the decline of the complex adaptive system (Stacey, Reference Stacey1996).
THE TYPES OF ORGANIZATIONAL LEARNING: ZERO, ADAPTIVE AND GENERATIVE
In this section we describe three different types of organizational learning, and mainly connect them with the three consciousness levels, emotions and behaviors explained above.
Based mainly on Bateson’s (Reference Bateson1972) categories, organizational learning has generally distinguished between two levels of learning (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010): adaptive or single loop learning (e.g., Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Senge, Reference Senge1990) related to Bateson’s level I, and generative or double loop learning (e.g., Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Senge, Reference Senge1990) related to Bateson’s level II and beyond.
However, altogether Bateson (Reference Bateson1972) posited five levels of learning, from zero learning to learning IV. Bateson (Reference Bateson1972) scarcely discussed learning IV, commenting that ‘it probably does not occur in any adult living organism on this earth,’ as it involves evolutionary change in a species. We therefore confine our framework to zero learning through to learning III. Each higher learning type includes the lower ones, and involves a change in the lower learning process. Therefore, the higher the level, the more transcendent the change involved in the learning, because the learner or learners are more conscious of themselves, the learning object and the situation (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972). Thus, there is a relationship between learning types and consciousness levels.
Zero learning
Zero learning is a conditioned response; it entails responding to stimuli but making no changes based on experience or information (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972), which implies no changes in the individual, group or organizational explicate order. For example, a group or an organization can maintain defective routines that are continually repeated without reflections or introduction of new actions by participants. Zero learning is a response to impulses, which can be related to associative learning, the process by which an association between two stimuli or a behavior and a stimulus is learned (Tsakanikos, Reference Tsakanikos2006). The two forms of associative learning are classical and operant conditioning (Bitterman, Menzel, Fietz, & Schafer, Reference Bitterman, Menzel, Fietz and Schafer1983). In classical conditioning (Pavlov, [Reference Pavlov1904] 1967, Reference Pavlov1926) two stimuli are repeatedly presented in close succession, until the response given to one becomes associated with the other. In the most famous example Pavlov repeatedly paired the neutral stimulus of a ringing bell with the positive unconditional stimulus of food, until the ringing bell caused the dog to salivate. This learned consistency is called the conditional response.
Classical conditioning learning refers to synchronic association between a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus, whereas operant or instrumental conditioning implies a diachronic association (contingency) between a behavior and an ulterior reinforcement (Skinner, Reference Skinner1938). In operant conditioning a certain behavior is either reinforced or punished, which results in an altered probability that the behavior will happen again. Operant conditioning could be the consequence of a simple reward or punishment depending on a wide range of reinforcement types. We all learn everyday in both ways by accustoming, performing typical routines or programming our acts by scheduling our time and complying with rules and existing norms. Consequently, this type of learning is very much related to mindlessness and brigs about stability: the same explicate order is used and internalized by members of the organization.
Adaptive learning
Learning I, which corresponds to single loop or adaptive learning (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Fiol & Lyles, Reference Fiol and Lyles1985; Senge, Reference Senge1990; Arthur & Aiman-Smith, Reference Arthur and Aiman-Smith2001; Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010), refers to changes in zero learning, by correcting errors of choices within a set of alternatives (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972). Cochrane (Reference Cochrane2004) defines this type of learning as obtaining information to correct or improve procedures. In that sense, it will imply an alteration or improvement in the explicate order: models, paradigms, processes, etc. Individuals or groups can modify their routines with introduction of variations in the way in which they are implemented. They change the performative aspect of routines in the view of Feldman and Pentland (Reference Feldman and Pentland2003). These are models of behavior at the organizational or collective level that involve sensemaking learning capacities.
According to Argyris and Schön (Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978), single loop learning allows an organization to maintain its present policies or achieve its present objectives by adjusting or adapting its behaviors. Single loop learning seems to be present when goals, values, frameworks or strategies are taken for granted. Cochrane (Reference Cochrane2004) understands this type of learning as highly instrumentalist as it seeks to obtain information or objective knowledge in order to correct or improve procedures.
In the organizational learning literature adaptive learning is considered as the refinement and improvement of existing competences, technologies and paradigms without necessarily examining or challenging our underlying beliefs and assumptions (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010). Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) relate adaptive learning to complex adaptive systems. Complexity literature understands that complex ‘adaptive’ systems have the capacity to adjust to changes in the environment without endangering their essential organization. These authors define adaptive learning as any improvement or development of the explicate order (Bohm, Reference Bohm1980). According to Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010), adaptive learning takes place when individuals and groups within organizations mainly exercise logic or deductive reasoning, concentrate, discuss and focus on improving any mental model, knowledge, process or routines, etc. Concentration is essential for adaptive learning, as it is for flow. Therefore, flow and adaptive learning can be related to each other. When individuals and groups are passionately focused on an activity or a situation they can improve it, modify it, but it is difficult to question it when there is such concentration or focused attention. Once the explicate order is improved by individuals and groups, a process of institutionalization (Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) takes place to become organizational explicate order.
Generative learning
Learning II, which corresponds to double loop, reflective or generative learning (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Fiol & Lyles, Reference Fiol and Lyles1985; Senge, Reference Senge1990; Arthur & Aiman-Smith, Reference Arthur and Aiman-Smith2001; Elkjaer, Reference Elkjaer2001, Reference Elkjaer2004; Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010), is described by Bateson (Reference Bateson1972) as a change in the process of learning I, which means a corrective change in the set of alternatives from which choice is made (Tosey & Mathison, Reference Tosey and Mathison2008). While learning I is concerned with improving a procedure or behavior, learning II involves questioning this procedure, behavior or assumption, and therefore implies that it is understood. According to Cochrane (Reference Cochrane2004), learning II means challenging one’s beliefs and assumptions by becoming aware of what Argyris and Schön (Reference Argyris and Schön1978) call defensive routines. Individuals or groups give up their established routines to create new institutionalized understandings and guidelines. They modify the constraints and the resources of their actions, the ostensive aspect of their routines in the view of Feldman and Pentland (Reference Feldman and Pentland2003).
Double loop or generative learning involves the modification of an organization’s underlying norms, policies and objectives (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Fiol & Lyles, Reference Fiol and Lyles1985; Senge, Reference Senge1990), and entails being able to see beyond the situation and questioning operating norms (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1974, Reference Argyris and Schön1978). Double loop learning occurs when error is detected and corrected in ways that involve the modification of an organization’s underlying norms, policies and objectives (Smith, 2001).
Senge (Reference Senge1990) affirms that generative learning, unlike adaptive learning, requires new ways of looking at the world, whether in understanding customers or understanding how to better manage a business. In order to look more deeply into generative learning, he introduces the concept of metanoia, a Greek word meaning a profound shift of mind, which he considers to be synonymous with generative learning. He explains that for the Greeks it meant a fundamental change, transcendence (meta) mind (noia). Furthermore, Senge (Reference Senge1990) considers systems thinking as an essential discipline for organizational learning, and defines it as the ability to comprehend and address the whole, and to examine the interrelationship between parts. Mindfulness and its approach to the implicate order seem to be essential in his perspective.
Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) consider generative learning as a process that involves searching for implicate order (Bohm, Reference Bohm1980), which is a holistic understanding of anything or anyone we interact with. Within the implicate order everything is connected and enfolded into everything else. According to Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010), generative learning might take place when individuals and groups within organizations mainly use intuition, attention, dialogue and aim to question any explicate order. Bohm (Reference Bohm1980) describes the implicate order as a kind of generative order, which is primarily concerned with a deep and inward order out of which the manifest form of things can emerge creatively. According to Bohm (Reference Bohm1980), to approach the implicate or generative order requires (creative) intelligence, which is an unconditioned act of perception (intuition) that must lie beyond any factors that can be included in any knowable law. This kind of intuition is similar to Crossan, Lane, and White’s (Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) entrepreneurial intuition that allows strategic renewal or innovation; and is the opposite of their expert intuition, which would be closely related to mindlessness and zero learning.
While learning II is concerned with the emergence of new understandings about contexts, learning III implies a deeper shift, a change in the set of contexts (Tosey & Mathison Reference Tosey and Mathison2008). Learning III involves a change in the process of learning II (Bateson, Reference Bateson1972), leading to a transformation of who we are. It is a corrective change in the system of sets of alternatives from which choice is made; it implies choosing between those sets of factors that influence the paradigm we are working from in learning II (Cochrane, Reference Cochrane2004). According to this author, an important aspect of learning III is the extent to which the self is no longer significant. Bateson (Reference Bateson1972) affirms that once someone achieves learning III, and learns to perceive in terms of the context of contexts, he or she goes beyond the context of personality, starts to look at the context that influences the shaping of personality traits, and his or her self becomes irrelevant. In doing so, the world is seen from a holistic point of view (Cochrane, Reference Cochrane2004).
In the organizational literature, several concepts have been proposed to go beyond double loop learning, such as triple loop learning, which is regarded as a profound or transformational change (Swieringa & Wierdsma, Reference Swieringa and Wierdsma1992; Romme & van Witteloostuijn, Reference Romme and Van Witteloostuijn1999; Wang & Ahmed, Reference Wang and Ahmed2003). However, and generally speaking, the organizational literature has considered generative learning as the ‘transformative’ type of learning (Argyris & Schön, Reference Argyris and Schön1978; Senge, Reference Senge1990) which would imply Bateson’s learning II and beyond.
Therefore, generative learning is developed through mindfulness to approach the implicate order. When this is done, a process of unfoldment (Bohm, Reference Bohm1980) or interpretation (Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) occurs. Once it is unfolded or interpreted, a new explicate order appears that might be integrated and institutionalized (Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) to become an organizational explicate order.
Over time organizations or teams become increasingly less mindful unless problems or interruptions arrive (stressors). A sort of edge of chaos (Chiva, Grandío, & Alegre, Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) activates mindfulness. Levinthal and Rerup (Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006) believe that interruptions or problems may trigger a sequential switch from less-mindful to mindful processes. This dichotomy portrays mindful and less-mindful learning as distinct categories between which organizations and their members sequentially alternate or simultaneously manage.
CONCLUSION
From a theoretical point of view, the contribution of this paper appears to lie in the enrichment of the organizational learning literature and the integration of different perspective in a systemic framework. In fact, in their reflections on the 4I framework, Crossan, Maurer, and White (Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011) remind us that there are different types of learning, which we consider should be taken into account for an organizational learning framework. The central contribution of this paper is the incorporation of different types of learning into an organizational learning framework: zero, adaptive and generative learning; and the inclusion of several facets in the framework, mainly consciousness and emotions. We hold that these concepts contribute to a broader understanding of systemic learning. Indeed, although the literature has previously underlined the importance of emotions for organizational learning (Vince, Reference Vince2001, Reference Vince2002), it has not been incorporated into any theory or framework. In fact, Crossan, Maurer, and White (Reference Crossan, Maurer and White2011: 452) also affirm that emotions should be taken into account for any eventual organizational learning theory.
In the ZAG framework, we suggest that the higher the degree of consciousness, the more positive the emotions; and the lower the degree of consciousness, the more negative the emotions. We also propose that negative emotions like anxiety and apathy lead to distress and zero learning. In contrast, positive emotions, like passion, compassion and dispassion, cause eustress and promote adaptive and generative learning. However, Vince (Reference Vince2002) reports how several authors have noted that learning and change are unlikely to occur without anxiety (Kofman & Senge, Reference Kofman and Senge1993; Schein, Reference Schein1993). Vince (Reference Vince2002) describes two directions to travel in from the starting point of anxiety. When anxiety is held in, individuals struggle, take risks, and learning is promoted. When anxiety is not held in or controlled, emotions are denied, resistance comes out and learning is blocked. In a similar vein, Quick, Gavin, and Schein (Reference Quick, Gavin and Schein2000) affirm that organizations experiencing anxiety can hinder organizational learning. Thus, our approach could be considered as relatively similar; positive emotions displace negative emotions like anxiety, thereby developing eustress.
The framework presented in this paper considers that organizational learning is initiated by stressors. Similarly, Cangelosi and Dill (Reference Cangelosi and Dill1965) view organizational learning to be a product of interactions of three types of stress: discomfort, performance and disjunctive stress. Discomfort stress is generated because the task demands more time and energy than is available; performance stress is generated because actual performance might fall short of aspirations and disjunctive stress is generated by divergence and conflict. However, in spite of the seminal work by Cangelosi and Dill (Reference Cangelosi and Dill1965), organizational learning has rarely been associated with stress (e.g., Mikkelsen, Saksvik, & Ursin, Reference Mikkelsen, Saksvik and Ursin1998).
Concerning first the three types of learning processes, the issue of their interrelations (or their sequence) can be informed through the dilemma between exploration and exploitation. According to March (Reference March1991), organizational learning reveals a balance between exploitation and exploration. However, Crossan, Lane, and White (Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) affirm that instead of a balance there is a tension between the two owing to the implicit competition for resources: organizations either follow a feed-forward or a feedback process. Similarly, the literature on ambidexterity, or the organization’s capability to explore and exploit, presents two models. The first one, structural ambidexterity, considers that organizations should promote two separate approaches, one for exploration and one for exploitation (Adler, Goldoftas, & Levine, Reference Adler, Goldoftas and Levine1999); therefore organizations solve the paradox by temporarily cycling through periods of exploitation and periods of exploration (Brown & Eisenhardt, 1998; Crossan, Lane, & White, Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999; Venkatraman, Lee, & Iyer, Reference Venkatraman, Lee and Iyer2007). In contrast, contextual ambidexterity understands that the two can be integrated (Gibson & Birkinshaw, Reference Gibson and Birkinshaw2004), and a certain simultaneity and synchronization might happen between them.
In the same line, but this time on the relationship between mindfulness and mindlessness, Weick and Sutcliffe (Reference Weick and Sutcliffe2006) question whether mindful and less mindful processes are best conceived as dual processes or as a continuum. This question is significant because the answer affects whether it is conceivable that the two processes can operate simultaneously, or whether they are mutually exclusive and can operate only sequentially. Levinthal and Rerup (Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006) consider that they are mutually exclusive and have to be conceived of as a continuum because they believe that there are important points of conflict between mindfulness and mindlessness. One stresses the importance of novelty to respond to changing, and possibly unique circumstances, while the other emphasizes the role of continuity as a mechanism to preserve accumulated experience. Levinthal and Rerup (Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006) also affirm that mindfulness and mindlessness are distinct categories but there is a strong complementarity and interdependence between the two perspectives or processes. Langer and Piper (Reference Langer and Piper1987) see mindlessness as a consequence of mindfulness. One mindfully creates categories and then is able to mindlessly use them. Organizational performance calls for a routinization of behaviors. The continuity of mindfulness is rare (Valorinta, Reference Valorinta2009) and costly (Levinthal & Rerup, Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006). According to Germer (Reference Germer2005), without unexpected stimuli, mindfulness can easily transform into mindlessness. Thus, this author suggests that mindfulness and mindlessness are not exclusive processes; most organizations are characterized by some level of both mindfulness and mindlessness. The question is rather their proportion and simultaneity in a particular organization (Weick, Reference Weick1998). In any event, Levinthal and Rerup (Reference Levinthal and Rerup2006) call for research on their relationship. We also consider that future research should focus on it.
The ZAG organizational learning framework presented in this paper considers that, depending on their level of consciousness, individuals and groups tend to either reach a higher proportion of exploitation (zero learning) or a higher proportion of exploration (adaptive/generative learning). Thus, there is a tension between exploitation and exploration (Langley et al., Reference Langley, Smallman, Tsoukas and Van de Ven2013) and a certain simultaneity and synchronization might happen between them. When individuals and groups possess mainly high levels of consciousness and positive emotions, adaptive and generative learning arise. Thus, there is a higher proportion of exploration, but exploitation might also happen. In fact, and following Bateson (Reference Bateson1972) who understands that each higher learning type includes the lower ones, adaptive and generative learning might also imply some degree of zero learning. However, in these terms, when negative emotions and low levels of consciousness are mostly present simplicity, distress, zero learning and exploitation arise, and rarely exploration arises. Exploitation and exploration depend strongly on consciousness and emotions, on how individuals face stressors. Our framework considers the resulting degree of exploration/exploitation to be an attentional and emotional consequence. Organizations tend toward exploitation when mindlessness, apathy and anxiety are generalized within the organization, mainly owing to the way organizations are managed. Hence, human resource practices, organizational structures or general strategies might affect attention and emotions, which in turn influence organizational learning. Organizations tend toward exploration when mindfulness, flow, passion, compassion and dispassion are present at different levels, which might also be a consequence of the way organizations are managed. Future research should examine these organizational and managerial characteristics.
Consciousness and emotions take individuals, groups and organizations either to the edge of chaos, a highly complex situation, which implies mostly exploration; or to stability, a situation with low complexity, which implies mainly exploitation. According to the complex system literature (Gell-Mann, Reference Gell-Mann1994; Stacey, Reference Stacey1996), another situation can appear that is related to low complexity: full instability or chaos. Based on the literature on the matter (e.g., Stacey, Reference Stacey1996), we understand that full instability is also related to mindlessness and no learning is developed.
Complexity literature has traditionally influenced organizational learning research (e.g., Stacey, Reference Stacey1996; Antonacopoulou & Chiva, Reference Antonacopoulou and Chiva2007). Following Chiva, Grandío, and Alegre’s (Reference Chiva, Grandío and Alegre2010) analysis of adaptive and generative learning based on complexity science, we consider the concepts of complex system behaviors (Gell-Mann, Reference Gell-Mann1994; Stacey, Reference Stacey1996) and Bohm’s (Reference Bohm1980) concepts of order. The concept of Bohm’s (Reference Bohm1980) implicate order is essential to understand generative learning. Organizational explicate order has been considered as the underlying phenomenon of interest, similar to Crossan, Lane, and White’s (Reference Crossan, Lane and White1999) strategic renewal. In fact, we consider that explicate order might be a broader concept; that it not only relates to strategic changes but to any change in organizational mental model, process or paradigm.
In spite of the recent importance of concepts like mindfulness, flow or mindlessness in the organizational learning literature (e.g., Brown & Ryan, Reference Brown and Ryan2003; Weick & Putnam, Reference Weick and Putnam2006; Weick & Sutcliffe, Reference Weick and Sutcliffe2006), they had not previously been introduced in the process of organizational learning. In this framework we link them to emotions and learning types. Furthermore, mindfulness and its connection to Bohm’s (Reference Bohm1980) implicate order improve our understanding of the process of generative learning.
As a conceptual paper, this framework has obvious and important limitations that should be overcome through qualitative and quantitative empirical research. This research should test relationships and processes, and propose new constructs for the model. Future research might extend this theoretical framework, for instance by analyzing how certain contingent factors like organizational structure, leadership or culture might affect the different facets of learning processes. It would also be interesting to explore with this new theoretical model the processes of emotional contagion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, Reference Hatfield, Cacioppo and Rapson1994; Barsade, Reference Barsade2002) and their effects on the dynamics of collective learning. Defined as the tendency for two individuals to emotionally converge, the analysis of the emotional contagion phenomenon in the work place could be useful to better interconnect the levels of learning (individual, collective and organizational) with the types of learning. In the same line, we could further develop our model and its different facets in understanding the factors able to influence positive emotions such as leadership (Johnson, Reference Johnson2008), organizational culture or other group characteristics.
Finally, this paper has also practical value for managers who want their organizations learn. They should focus their attention on individual’s and group’s consciousness and emotions and might have a pivotal role in the process of organizational learning. When consciousness is high and emotions are positive organizations are more able to attain adaptive and even generative learning; however, how can practitioners attain individuals and groups with these characteristics? New organizational models that promote trust, altruism, reflexive approach of rules and routines, servant leadership, individual and group responsibility and autonomy, direct democracy, creativity and innovation might have the answer.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (Ref. ECO2011-26780) and the University Jaume I (Ref. P11B2013-14) for their financial support for this research.