Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T06:38:27.829Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part IV - Current Trends

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Isabelle Roskam
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
James J. Gross
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Moïra Mikolajczak
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium

Summary

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

Chapter 10 Culture and the Challenges of Being a Good Parent

Sara Harkness , Charles M. Super , Marjolijn J. M. Blom , Ughetta Moscardino , Sabrina Bonichini , Moises Rios Bermudez , Jong-Hay Rha , Barbara Welles , and Olaf Zylicz

The young mother in Norman Rockwell’s famous portrayal sits bolt upright on a chair, hairbrush in her right hand, which also grasps the shirt of the little boy splayed across her lap, tummy down, waiting for his expected spanking. In her left hand, however, the mother is holding a book, Child Psychology, with an expression on her face that combines consternation with anger (Rockwell, Reference Rockwell1933). She has plenty of reason to think her son deserves a punishment, as the broken objects strewn on the floor show – a vase, a tennis racquet, and a clock, along with a hammer. Yet she hesitates to carry out the punishment that is most familiar to her for such a transgression. “Child psychology,” a new kind of way to think about children’s behavior, has just begun to take hold in this young mother’s world. And the book in her hand is probably telling her that the old forms of discipline need to be replaced with new, more “scientific” guidance. Clearly, this mother wants to do the right thing. But what actually is the right thing?

Ideas about how to be a good mother – or good father – have varied across historical time as well as across cultures, but they consistently play a major role in parents’ own sense of well-being and thus emotion regulation. Norman Rockwell’s picture was first published on the cover of The Saturday Evening Post in 1933, just 5 years after John B. Watson’s slim but extraordinarily influential book Psychological Care of Infant and Child, in which he prescribed a business-like approach to child-rearing and pressed his case for reward-and-punishment behaviorism (Watson, Reference Watson1928). In contrast to Watson’s views, however, other influential professionals such as Arnold Gesell (Gesell & Ilg, Reference Gesell and Ilg1943), saw child development as a natural, maturational process. According to family historian Ann Hulbert, the dueling opposites set up at that time have endured to the present (Hulbert, Reference Hulbert2004). In short, for present-day US parents, the question of how to be a good parent has yet to be answered satisfactorily.

10.1 Mothers as Cross-Cultural Pioneers: The Experiences of Expatriate Journalists

In recent years, however, American parents – as well as parents in other postindustrial societies – have begun to look beyond local sources of advice to what can be learned from other cultures, both within and beyond the middle-class Western world. Among the pioneers in this quest are mothers living in cultural places different from where they grew up, who have produced books written as participant observers of what they see as better ways to be a good parent.

Perhaps the most widely read example of this new genre is Pamela Druckerman’s account of French parenting (Druckerman, Reference Druckerman2014). In her book French Children Don’t Throw Food, first published in Great Britain and later republished in the United States under the title Bringing up Bebé, Druckerman describes her experiences as a new mother living in Paris, where (as noted in the book’s subtitle) she “discovers the wisdom of French parenting.” Druckerman presents herself as a confused American new mother, desperate for guidance but shocked by the parenting practices she observes when she goes home to visit. Her new discoveries include a range of topics, from establishing regular schedules of eating and sleeping in the opening months of the baby’s life, to teaching the baby to “wait” for a response from adults, to advantages of the infant crèche for full-day care whether the mother is employed or not. Two themes appear repeatedly in her French experience: one, that the task of parents and other caregivers is to “awaken” babies’ senses and encourage them to “discover” the world around them; the other, that babies need to understand from the beginning that they are not the center of the universe, their parents also have needs, and they must learn to be patient, entertain themselves, and not expect frequent expressions of praise. Notably absent are concerns about stimulating children’s school-related skills; for example, the infant crèche has no apparent curriculum, though they do serve the children four-course lunches (purėed or cut in small pieces depending on the age of the child).

Two other books – one about Danish parenting and the other about Dutch parents, both written by expatriate American or British mothers married to local citizens – offer further perspectives on how to raise children successfully with a lot less stress than they attribute to their home countries. The premise of both books is that residents of their respective countries have received top ratings from international organizations as being the “happiest” in the world, thus they must have something helpful to offer the rest of us. The main idea, or “parenting theory,” put forth by American author Jessica Alexander and her Danish coauthor Iben Sandahl in The Danish Way of Parenting (2016, p. 21) is straightforward: “Happy kids grow up to be happy adults who raise happy kids, and so on.” Invoking our work on “parental ethnotheories“ (Harkness & Super, Reference Harkness, Super, Harkness and Super1996), they urge readers to pay attention to their own “default settings,” that is, the implicit, taken-for-granted ideas that guide the way they raise their children. The “Danish way of parenting” is contrasted to what they describe as the “epidemic of stress” in the United States, where parents compete with each other to raise the highest-achieving children but where prescription drugs – Ritalin for children, antidepressants for both adults and increasingly young sufferers – are used at much higher rates than in Europe. In Denmark, on the other hand, they describe an approach to parenting that emphasizes letting children learn on their own as much as possible, through play rather than direct instruction. Humility is valued, and praise for children’s accomplishments is given out sparingly. Paradoxically, they note, Danish movies often have sad endings (unlike typical American movies) – but they see this as an aspect of “authenticity,” another key feature of Danish parenting that should produce children who can recognize and accept their own emotions – and relatedly (as they see it), know their own limits. Last but not least, they describe the importance of “cozy” (hygge) family times that help everyone feel connected, an idea that they contrast to American “individualism.”

Although Danish people have been ranked at the apex of happiness (according to a worldwide survey by the United Nations), Dutch children come out at the top in a UNICEF report (2012). These rankings may be taken with a grain of salt because they seem to vary a bit from one survey to the next (Norway edged out Denmark as the “happiest” country in 2017). Another caveat relates to the definition of “happiness” in such surveys, which includes measures of economic well-being, health, and educational achievement in addition to simply asking people how happy they feel. Not surprisingly, given their strong social benefit programs and low poverty rates, the northern European countries hover around the top of the scales.

But is there more to actual “happiness” in Denmark and the Netherlands than just rankings of overall socioeconomic well-being? In The Happiest Kids in the World, American writer Rina Mae Acosta and her English colleague Michele Hutchison set out to explain just why Dutch children are given such high grades for happiness (Acosta & Hutchinson, Reference Acosta and Hutchinson2017). Both authors are expatriates in the Netherlands, married to Dutch men and bringing up their children in a foreign environment that they find increasingly homelike. Some of the themes they invoke are familiar. In contrast to superstimulated American and British children, Dutch children have more time to play without an imposed educational agenda. Parenthood is not a competition about whose child achieves the most. In fact, a teacher urges one of the authors not to help her young son prepare for an important test, as it might create a “strain” on him. There’s more time for families to enjoy being together. The term gezellig (which the authors compare to the Danish hygge) captures the cozy, comfortable sociability that is the essence of a good life. In addition to these similarities, though, Acosta and Hutchison also discover a uniquely Dutch mainstay of parenting: the emphasis on rest and a regular schedule as key to children’s healthy development and – yes – happiness.

10.2 Academic Research on Parenting

Academic research on parenting across cultures is generally consistent with the more journalistic accounts by expatriate mothers (it should be acknowledged that they too relied on scholarly sources). In our own research with Dutch families in the 1990s, for example, we found that Dutch mothers tended to rate their days more favorably than did mothers in six other Western cultures, especially in relation to their child’s sleeping patterns, the time and energy they had available for their child, and the balance of parenting with other tasks. The importance of the Dutch Three R’s – rest (Rust), regularity (Regelmaat), and cleanliness (Reinheid) – became obvious in our interviews with parents and observations of infant behavior: compared to same-aged US children, the Dutch babies in our study were sleeping two hours more per 24-hour day at 6 months of age. This difference continued to our oldest measurement point when the children were 7 or 8 years old, although it diminished over time (Super et al., Reference Super, Harkness, van Tijen, van der Vlugt, Dykstra, Fintelman, Harkness and Super1996). A detailed analysis of how Dutch and US mothers of 2- and 6-month-old infants talked about regularity revealed a fundamental difference in ideas about “getting the baby on a schedule”: whereas the US mothers “hoped” that their baby would settle into a regular schedule for the sake of the whole family, the Dutch mothers were confident that regularity in sleep, eating, and other activities was essential for the baby’s healthy development. In addition, the Dutch mothers evidently felt more entitled to take their own needs and the needs of the rest of the family into consideration than did the US mothers, who seemed convinced that being a “good mother” necessarily entailed prioritizing the baby’s perceived needs almost to the exclusion of their own (van Schaik et al., Reference van Schaik, Mavridis, De Looze, Blom, Super and Harkness2020).

The emphasis on the importance of a regular schedule carried through to these Dutch mothers’ talk about their children’s daily routines at older ages. The mother of 7-year-old Willem described her son’s typical weekday, in the process incorporating her own day:

Now, he gets up in the morning around 7:00 am, and he sits in front of the TV watching “children’s TV.” At about 7:30 or quarter of 8:00, I get up, and he gets his orange juice and bread with butter and chocolate sprinkles, we drink coffee … then we go wash and get dressed, then he goes to school – nowadays, on his own. At 12:00 I pick him up at school, and we come home on our bikes, we eat something, he plays inside or outside. Then I bring him back to school, on our bikes. At 3:00 I usually pick him up. And then we come home, or he goes to swimming lessons, or tennis, or he plays outside or he brings a friend home, or he goes to a friend’s house. We eat dinner late – about 6:30 or 7:00. Then he watches TV, usually Sesame Street, or now, with good weather he might even go outside. At 7:30 we go upstairs again, pajamas, wash up, brush teeth … at about 8:00 he’s in bed. A normal day, right?

Weekends are different, according to Willem’s father, although Willem still gets up at the same time. “We lie in bed longer, if possible. Then when he’s tired of watching TV, he often comes upstairs to see if we’re still in bed, and sometimes he climbs in with us.” The father continues, “So we get up at around 9:30, have coffee and bread (orange juice for him). Then, if I don’t have to work, the three of us go grocery shopping in town.”

In the accounts of both Willem’s mother and father, the theme of regularity is expressed through frequent references to the times of day when certain activities take place, with eating and sleeping schedules, plus school, as guideposts for variability in other activities. Being a good parent, in this context, includes providing emotional security through a regular schedule that the child can rely on. As the father of a 5-year-old girl said, “Regularity is the most important … so much happens in her little world. It’s important that promised things are as promised, and that means some regularity. Go to bed on time, eat on time.” The mother agrees, “Yes, regularity and rest, I think.”

10.3 Parents’ Ideas in Action: The Power of Parental Ethnotheories

As the stories of family life that we heard from Dutch parents illustrate, parenting practices in various cultural places are shaped by shared ideas about parenting. Parents’ ideas exist at many levels, from the most general and abstract to the specific and concrete. They also vary in how available they are for identification and explanation by the parents who hold them. The Three Rs of Dutch parenting are an example of a well-known, historically grounded principle of child-rearing (Super et al., Reference Super, Blom, Harkness, Ranade and Londhe2021); parents in our research were able to explain them to us as well as illustrate them with actual stories about daily life for their children and themselves. In anthropological terms, such constellations of beliefs are often referred to as “cultural models” that have strong motivational properties. They are not just mental representations of the way that things are but also of the way things should be (D’Andrade & Strauss, Reference D’Andrade and Strauss1992). We refer to such cultural models, when related specifically to parenting, as “parental ethnotheories“ (Harkness & Super, Reference Harkness, Super, Harkness and Super1996).

Parental ethnotheories can be thought of as a cascade of ideas, constraints, practices, and outcomes (Harkness & Super, Reference Harkness, Super, Rubin and Chung2006, figure 1, p. 71). The cultural models at the top of the model are the most general, often implicit ideas about the nature of the child, parenting, and the family. Below this triad are ideas about specific domains, such as infant sleep or social development. These ideas are closely tied to ideas about appropriate practices (such as strict bedtimes), and further to imagined child or family outcomes. The ideas are translated into behavior but only as mediated by factors such as the child’s temperament characteristics, parents’ schedules, and competing cultural models and their related practices. The results can be seen in actual parental behaviors and ultimately in actual child and family outcomes. In the example of the Three Rs of Dutch child-rearing, the triad of rest, regularity, and cleanliness appears to be an implicit template for much of family – indeed, societal – life, and it flows easily into the second level, regarding specific beliefs, in this case about the importance of rest etc. The daily routines that these families described were instantiations of the Three Rs, and they were often accompanied by comments about their benefits. During our time living with and learning from these Dutch families, we were struck by the implicit emphasis on emotional regulation inherent in the Three Rs. For example, a typical comment from a parent in support of a regular and restful routine was, “If he doesn’t get enough sleep, he’s fussy the next day.” Likewise, parents alluded to their own need for adequate rest and some time for themselves after the children were in bed. For these families, it appeared, keeping daily practices consistent with the principles of the Three Rs did not seem to be a problem; but as any parent knows, one cannot assume that practices will always reflect principles. Thus the parental ethnotheories model includes space for things that interfere with parents’ ideas about being a good parent.

The parental ethnotheories model ties together culturally shared ideas, practices, and outcomes. Thus, learning about one part of the model can lead the observer to explore how it functions in relation to other parts, as well as how it may vary across cultures. For example, learning about the Three Rs from Dutch parents aroused our curiosity about what parents in other cultures – even within Western Europe – might think about the importance of a regular and restful schedule for children. When we asked an Italian mother what she thought about feeding her baby on a regular schedule, her response was emphatic:

No, absolutely not – no scheduling with this one. With the other one I did … Because with the bottle you give him 200 ml at two o’clock, so you can’t give him 100 ml at three… It’s completely different, in my opinion. When you breastfeed, you not only give your milk, but many more things so you can’t refuse it – it’s not that you don’t want to, it’s just unthinkable, it’s as if your child said, “Mummy, give me a kiss,” and you said, “Not now, Sweetie, at three o’clock!”

In contrast to the Dutch mothers, the majority of mothers in our Italian sample did not expect their babies to self-regulate their state of rest and arousal; rather, they tended to accommodate to the infant’s inborn sleeping and feeding schedules without imposing any rules, because “babies learn to regulate themselves.” This idea in turn related to a more implicit principle that our Italian colleague, Vanna Axia, explained to us, that is the importance of emotional closeness, especially within the family. As with the Three Rs for the Dutch parents, the cultural model of emotional closeness for the Italian families could be related to various aspects of family life. For example, when we asked the Italian parents about whether children should have a regular bedtime, the general response was “yes … ” but then with numerous exceptions. The mother of one 4-year-old girl stated firmly that “the rule is going to bed in the evening not after 9:00 – unless there are friends, parties, or we are out – then the rules are broken.” In this cultural scenario, emotion regulation for both children and their parents was a joint project, supported by a close and loving relationship.

10.4 The Importance of Context: Parenting across the Globe

The Dutch and Italian parents whom we met through our research lived in somewhat similar circumstances. Thus, the differences we observed in parenting might be seen as dictated more by cultural traditions than by what John Whiting (Reference Whiting, Leiderman, Tulkin and Rosenfeld1977) called “maintenance systems,” including economic structure, government, and the overall organization of society. A quick hop across the globe and across time to Kokwet, the rural African community where we carried out research with children and families in the 1970s, provides a contrasting example (Harkness & Super, Reference Harkness, Super, Lewis and Saarni1985; Super & Harkness, Reference Super and Harkness1986). Our images from those times include a photograph of Mrs Mitei (as our local research assistant always referred to her) seated on the ground outside her mud-and-wattle hut with the youngest three of her eight children as they work together peeling maize kernels off the dried cob. The photo catches her in an affectionate interchange with the toddler seated next to her, who is too young to participate but content to stay with his siblings and his mother. The maize kernels will be ground for making kimiet, a thick porridge that is the staple food in this community. Mrs Mitei’s day includes cooking kimiet, garnished with wild leafy vegetables that grow between the rows of maize on their plot of land. Kipsigis tea – a hearty beverage of milk and tea leaves boiled together and sweetened with a generous spoonful of sugar from the local store – accompanies every meal. Mrs Mitei and her children eat together, seated on the ground, while her husband, Arap (or “Mr” in our parlance) Mitei, eats off to the side, following Kipsigis etiquette. Mrs Mitei also goes down to the river to bring back a bucket of water, balanced on her head, for cooking and washing, and she spends several hours in her fields weeding, along with several neighborhood women who take turns helping in each other’s gardens, while older siblings take care of the younger ones at the edge of the field. At night, Mrs Mitei will sleep on the sculpted mud floor of her own hut, with her youngest child at her front and next-youngest at her back; the older children sleep in a separate hut across the compound, and Arap Mitei has his own. When the children are not with their mother, they are mostly busy helping out, for example, 4-year-old Kipkoech, captured in another photo, is posted by maize kernels spread out to dry on cow hides, where he drives away an errant calf from this tempting snack.

10.5 Cultural Typologies of Mothers

What Mrs Mitei does as a “good mother” is quite different from the European mothers mentioned previously, even though their fundamental goals of raising healthy and productive children are no doubt the same. Mrs Mitei and mothers like her are described by anthropologist Beatrice Whiting as “training mothers,” whose communications with their children often revolve around assigning tasks that contribute to the whole family’s sustenance (unlike picking up one’s toys or even setting the table for dinner). Based on observations of mothers’ behavior with their children in 12 cultural places, Whiting and her colleague Carolyn Pope Edwards proposed three maternal profiles (Whiting & Edwards, Reference Whiting and Edwards1988). The “training mother” profile was found in all their sub-Saharan research samples. In contrast to this profile was the “controlling mother,” whose interactions seemed more oriented to managing the child’s behavior. This profile was most prevalent in agricultural communities of the Philippines, Mexico, and north India, where – as Whiting theorized – children were not needed as much for household or farm work. The last profile, the “sociable mother,” was found only in the American community of “Orchard Town,” a western suburb of Boston. In fact, these mothers also used primarily “controlling” speech with their children; but unlike the other cultural places, sociable interactions were second most frequent in this place. Whiting and Edwards suggested that this maternal style was due in part to the social isolation of these mothers: in contrast to all the other cultural samples, the mothers of Orchard Town often had no one to talk with except their children.

To this triad of mothers’ parenting styles, we might add another, the “educating mother” or father. This approach to parenting has long been recognized as important in East Asian cultures, and it has become increasingly prevalent among middle-class families in post-industrial Western societies, especially the United States (Harkness et al., Reference Harkness, Super, Moscardino, Rha, Blom, Huitrón, Johnston, Sutherland, Hyun, Axia and Palacios2007). A Korean mother interviewed about her 2-month-old infant expressed the Asian version of this theme: “My baby looks at new things very intensively for a long time. I think he recognizes things and he is thinking. I like it. It is his brain development. I would like to show him lots of things to help and encourage his brain development …. I put some pictures on the wall to show him things” (ibid., p. 30). Another Korean mother recounted, “I play music to her or I play tapes of stories so she could listen to them. The stories are recorded in Korean and in English. The earlier she starts the better” (Harkness, Super, and Mavridis, Reference Harkness, Super, Mavridis, Chen and Rubin2011, p. 84).

The “educating mother” is well represented in studies of Asian parenting through adolescence and even into college. For example, Korean mothers of preschool children reported spending most of their time in educational activities with their children, even though in principle they claimed that raising children to have good socioemotional qualities was their highest priority. Many of these mothers expressed anxiety about the conflict between their beliefs and practices but explained that it was necessary to help their children meet academic expectations for children entering kindergarten (Park & Kwon, Reference Park and Kwon2009). A further explanation for Asian mothers’ focus on their children’s academic achievement is that, as the title of one report quotes, “My child is my report card” (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Pomerantz and Deng2014). As the authors suggest, the higher rate of controlling behavior that they found among Chinese mothers, in contrast to American mothers, may well be due to differences in the extent to which mothers’ feelings of self-worth are contingent on their children’s academic success, with Chinese mothers rating higher on this association.

A frequently noted aspect of the Asian “educating parent” style is the emphasis on criticism as a motivating force for both intellectual and moral development. Florrie Fei-Yin Ng, now a professor at the Chinese University of Hong Kong, for example, recounted a childhood memory of coming home with a score of 95 (out of 100) on a school assignment (Center for the Study of Culture Health and Human Development, 2021). Although 95 was in fact an excellent grade, her mother’s response was to focus on the missing five points, that is, what went wrong, and how could she have done better? Professor Ng’s own research aligns closely with her personal experience. She and colleagues Eva Pomerantz and Shui-fong Lam, for example, note that Asian parents tend to emphasize failure and deemphasize success in their children’s academic performance. Explanations of this cultural pattern include the traditional Confucian focus on self-improvement, a belief that effort (rather than innate ability) is the most powerful influence on success, and the economic importance of academic achievement in Chinese society (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Pomerantz and Lam2007).

Middle-class American mothers also exemplify the “educating mother” profile, but with a distinct focus on “stimulation” for growth rather than learning per se, and praise rather than criticism. In our research, American mothers of 2-month-old infants, like their Korean counterparts, talked about their babies in cognitive terms (Harkness et al., Reference Harkness, Super, Moscardino, Rha, Blom, Huitrón, Johnston, Sutherland, Hyun, Axia and Palacios2007). As one mother recounted:

Somebody got us a video. It’s Baby Einstein. It works a lot with colors and music and just stimulating, so we play that for him. Not every day, but almost every day. Just, there’s a whole different range of things. One of them is colors. One is language. The other one is just, you know, shapes and … It’s stimulating to him. We try to stimulate him in some way.

These US mothers’ attention to their babies’ cognitive development often seemed to refer to norms that they had learned about by reading books of advice for parents. One mother explained:

I definitely try and do some introducing her to the toys and having her like, just in the past week and a half I brought out the little gym that goes above her so she can start batting at some rattles and she is starting to kind of figure out, but you know her hands are doing that … um, so yeah, making sure, I try and read up on you know what a 2-month-old should be doing, what a 3-month-old should be doing, so I can make sure that I do some activities that are helping her develop those skills and things that she needs to do … some stimulation where she can start focusing on things, but not overstimulation ‘cause I can see that really, you know, makes her crazy.

(ibid., p. 19)

As these babies grow into early childhood, the American parents’ focus on teaching extends to getting their child ready for kindergarten or even preschool. In a comparative study of parents and preschools in the United States, Italy, Spain, and the Netherlands, for example, almost 70% of the US parents said they thought that parents should teach their preschool child school-related skills. In contrast, parents in Spain, Italy, and the Netherlands overwhelmingly rejected this idea. As one Spanish mother explained, “I think that is the job of teachers who have studied for this [teaching]. The teachers don’t know how to do my job, and I shouldn’t want to do theirs” (Harkness et al. Reference Harkness, Super, Bonichini, Ríos Bermúdez, Mavridis, van Schaik, Tomkunas and Palacios2020, p. 132). An Italian mother of a 4-year-old concurred, drawing a contrast with her perception of US ideas about parenting: “We teach him sometimes, but in a ‘soft’ way, and only if he asks for it. For example, I show him things, explain, but I do not insist, because children have their own maturation rhythms … yes, not like the Americans who want their children to grow up becoming little geniuses … I prefer to respect his developmental timing, he needs a time to play, because at primary school things get much more demanding” (ibid., p. 132). The Dutch parents were unanimous in their opposition to teaching their young children school-related skills, but they allowed for the possibility of doing educational activities with their children, as long as it was not (as one father put it), “with an eye to the future or his career.” Another Dutch father commented on the approach to learning taken both at preschool and at home: “So, I don’t think she has to learn anything. As long as she’s doing things she enjoys … she’s only 5” (ibid., p. 133).

The American parents’ focus on the importance of supporting the child’s self-esteem contrasts with not only the Asian parents but also the European parents in our research. We were prompted by the American parents’ frequent reference to this idea to explore it with parents in our five European samples. Interestingly, we found that the term “self-esteem” itself was almost impossible to translate; the closest that our European colleagues could suggest was more like “self-confidence.” A tabulation of themes in parent interviews across the six cultural samples showed a unique American emphasis on self-esteem, together with academic success and autonomy. These parents also talked about emotional closeness and a loving home, but less frequently than did all the European parents. The pairing of self-esteem with academic success in this analysis suggests why American parents seem to be so preoccupied with this theme. Namely, it is not easy to maintain a positive view of oneself in the highly competitive environment surrounding American children today. In this context, emotion regulation becomes a challenge of its own as parents attempt to maximize their child’s development while keeping some sense of their own selves as beings worthy of praise or at least appreciation. The title of Jennifer Senior’s (Reference Senior2014) popular book All Joy and No Fun: The Paradox of Modern Parenting captures the essence of this challenge.

10.6 Culture Change and the New Challenges of Being a Good Parent

Traditional ethnographic portraits of faraway places that were the focus of anthropologists in the early years of the field often included a last chapter about culture change, implying a contrast with the ostensibly stable social organization that had prevailed for generations. In reality, of course, cultures have always experienced change due to migrations, climate change, war, and population health events. Recent years, however, have brought more rapid change affecting families and communities. For example, Tsamaase et al. (Reference Tsamaase, Harkness and Super2020) describe traditional child-rearing patterns in rural Botswana as involving the entire extended family and community, in which maternal grandmothers played a special role as overseers of care for both their daughters and their grandchildren. Recent economic changes, however, including the growth of the diamond mining sector, have opened new employment options for women as well as men. Many young women have migrated to cities for salaried jobs, far from their families of origin and thus deprived of the communal care that their children would have received. This demographic change has required newly isolated urban nuclear families to find paid help by women unrelated to their families, and often from different cultural backgrounds. Grandmothers, in the meantime, find themselves increasingly burdened with primary care of children whose parents are not available to participate.

As we write the final pages of this chapter, we have just received news of the death of a dearly remembered man of the community where we lived and did research in the 1970s, and where our first child was born. Arap Mitei was born in the 1920s and lived through multiple culture changes that affected him directly, from serving as a housekeeper to British settlers during the colonial era, to becoming a farmer of his own property after Independence in 1963. Upon our arrival, he drew from his earlier training to take care of us, our house, our garden. Over the years, Arap Mitei became a respected boiyot, an elder in the community with a wife and many children and grandchildren. He raised his sons to become successful farmers, a manager of a safari company, a driver for a nongovernmental organization, and a security guard in a nearby town. His daughters are married and, like all mothers in Kokwet, farmers. In his last years, Arap Mitei suffered from dementia, but his family took loving and respectful care of him in his own home. His memory will be preserved through a new science lab for the local secondary school, a building originally constructed by the British settler who was given the land by the colonial government, and which became our home during our stay in the community before being changed into a school where none had been before, another bit of culture change in Arap Mitei’s community.

Changes in European and other postindustrial societies, although perhaps less visible than the changes in rural Africa, also shift the challenges of being a good parent. All the fathers in our seven-culture study described being more emotionally close to their children than what they had experienced with their own fathers. Economic changes have altered the predominance of the traditional single-earner family, and demographic changes include a wider variety of family forms and roles. A general although not universal trend is the increasing isolation of the nuclear family in both Western and non-Western settings. All of these entail changes in parental roles, with their accompanying demands on emotion regulation.

10.7 Culture and Emotion Regulation

The challenges of being a “good parent” are but one piece of the puzzle of culture (Harkness, Reference Harkness2023). Like a single jigsaw piece, they cannot be understood without some sense of the larger picture, for they necessarily reflect tradition and change, economics and psychology, and the experiences of individuals in families and community. Ideas about what makes a good parent are a core aspect of what it means to belong to a particular cultural place and time. Emotion regulation is a universal challenge of parenting (as well as successful functioning more generally), but its expression necessarily varies across cultures, even within the contemporary Western world, as captured by psychologist John Nash’s account of passengers disembarking from ships at an Australian port:

I had occasion twice in one week to meet passengers from ships at the ocean terminal in Sydney. One ship was the Southern Cross, from Southampton, and the other was the Galileo Galilei from Milan. In the one case the dockside was crowded with a throng of people, babies and grandparents, laughing, weeping, shouting. Men embraced and kissed; women shrieked and rushed into passionate greetings. There was tumultuous confusion. From the other ship the passengers passed sedately down the gang-plank, in orderly groups; there were waves of hands and smiles, polite handshakes, and impassive greetings such as “How nice to see you again.”

(Nash, Reference Nash1970, p. 428)

Based on the contrast between the Italian and British passengers in this scene, we might ask which group evidenced better emotion regulation. The answer, of course, is neither. Passengers in both groups were expressing their emotions in a culturally appropriate and thus well-regulated manner. In contrast to the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, Reference Gross2015; see also Chapter 2), the passengers disembarking in Sydney may not have experienced any of the four stages of emotion regulation described by this model; rather, they arrived at the Sydney ocean terminal equipped with cultural models that directed their expression of emotion (and probably their inner experience of it) without any felt effort. Critically, they were met by other members of the same cultural places who knew how to interpret their expressions, whether cries of joy or calm and pleasant greetings.

It is noteworthy that the contrasts observed by Nash were based on two cultural places that are rather similar in global perspective. Potthoff and colleagues (Reference Potthoff, Garnefski, Miklósi, Ubbiali, Domínguez-Sánchez, Martins, Witthöft and Kraaij2016) have pointed to other differences in emotional display rules within the limited European context. In northern Europe, they found Germans and Dutch making less use of cognitive strategies such as rumination and other-blame, in regulating their emotional responses, compared to participants from Spain, Portugal, Italy, and Hungary. More widely, Matsumoto et al. (Reference Matsumoto, Yoo and Fontaine2008) have demonstrated large cross-national variation in emotional expressivity in general, as well as in the open display of specific emotions such as happiness. Developmental studies demonstrate that at least some of these “rules” are learned within the opening months of life (e.g. Lavelli et al., Reference Lavelli, Carra, Rossi and Keller2019).

Observations of parents and children in various cultural contexts illustrate variation in the challenges of parenting, in contrast to simpler formulations based on the purported value of children in relation to economic development. Nevertheless, cross-cultural studies suggest some basic universals in parents’ ideas. In a recent survey of parents’ definitions of the “ideal parent” in 37 countries, the authors found that the theme of being “loving” was “the core of ideal-parent beliefs” in all the Western “culture zones,” whereas the African zone featured “responsibility” and “respect” and the Asian zone emphasized the themes of “family” plus “responsibility” (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023). Although the cultural variability evident in these themes is evident, they all support a positive view of what it takes to be a good parent. Successfully meeting those challenges in their local manifestation is in turn the basis of parents’ own sense of well-being and thus of their ability to regulate their own emotions and those of their children.

Chapter 11 Leveraging Parent–Youth Interactions to Measure and Analyze Emotion Regulation

Sarah A. Thomas and Lauren Micalizzi

Dyadic parent–child emotion regulation is a bidirectional developmental process that occurs over the span of more than a decade. Accordingly, the measurement of these interactions and subsequent analysis poses challenges for researchers to translate the theory of complex processes into study design and statistical models capable of inferring directionality and causality. In this chapter, we explore the interplay between parent and child emotion regulation to facilitate a better understanding of opportunities and challenges for improved measurement of these dynamic processes. We begin with a brief review that introduces the corpus of theory regarding interpersonal emotion regulation, directions of effects, and intergenerational transmission of emotion regulation. Then, we cover conceptual, methodological and analytic considerations while providing useful strategies for designing and implementing cutting edge studies on the interactive effects of parent and child emotion regulation.

11.1 Developing a Theoretical Foundation to Study Interactive Dyadic Emotion Regulation
11.1.1 Interpersonal Processes

The study of emotion regulation has historically focused on intrapersonal processes and strategies: how an individual influences what emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience or suppress them (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003; see also Chapter 3). Much less research attention has been paid to interpersonal processes, but it is no less relevant to the study of parental emotion regulation. Interpersonal regulation refers to the social contexts in which an individual’s emotion is regulated by another’s (Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2014). Emotion regulation is critical to human socialization and is a protracted developmental process that originates in early attachment relationships (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017, Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018). Thus, the parent–child dyad is among the most salient interpersonal contexts of emotion regulation.

11.1.2 Capturing the Developmental Nature of Emotion Regulation

Emotion regulation development begins in early infancy, continues through adolescence and beyond (Silvers et al., Reference Silvers, Shu, Hubbard, Weber and Ochsner2015; Thompson, Reference Thompson2011), and is entwined with parental processes (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007, Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018). Children learn to regulate emotions to navigate their world, from tolerating frustration to forming friendships (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Michel and Laureen O’Donnell1994), often with the aid of parents. In adolescence, emotion regulation tasks involve achieving greater independence, including managing increasingly complex interpersonal relationships. Parents remain a critical influence on adolescent behavior and adjustment during this developmental period (Silk, Reference Silk, Forbes, Whalen, Jakubcak, Thompson, Ryan, Axelson, Birmaher and Dahl2011, Reference Silk2019; Trucco et al., Reference Trucco, Cristello and Sutherland2021). Although parents may provide active guidance as adolescents navigate these developmental tasks, parental influence on adolescent emotion regulation originates from a foundation of lessons and interactions earlier in development. Consequently, translating the dynamic, developmental nature of emotion regulation to the study and analysis of interactive parent–child emotion regulation is complex and measurement of dyadic emotion regulation at a single point in time provides only a snapshot of an extended developmental process.

11.1.3 Dyadic Emotion Regulation

Zaki and Williams’ (Reference Zaki and Williams2013) interpersonal emotion regulation model refers to multiperson episodes that occur in social contexts and serve to pursue regulatory goals. In this framework, classes of regulation are distinguished by whether they are (1) internal (“intrinsic”) or external (“extrinsic”) and (2) influenced by another. Extrinsic regulation involves regulating other people’s emotions, whereas intrinsic regulation involves regulating one’s own emotion with the help of others. Regarding influence, these processes are either response dependent (i.e. reliant on a response by the another) or response independent (i.e. not reliant on a response of another). For example, a child shares good news with their parent; the child’s positive affect can be enhanced if the parent responds enthusiastically (i.e. intrinsic, response-dependent; this scenario could be modeled with sequential analysis techniques referenced later in the chapter). As another example, a parent’s prosocial act, such as providing social support to reduce their child’s negative affect, can produce a form of positive affect for the parent (extrinsic response-independent; Zaki & Williams, Reference Zaki and Williams2013).

11.1.4 Directions of Effects

Investigating the interactive nature of parent–child dyadic emotion regulation requires an understanding of the unidirectional contributions of each dyadic member to interactive emotion regulation, as well as to bidirectional effects.

11.1.4.1 Parent-Driven Effects

Most research on dyadic emotion regulation has focused on the effects of parental emotion regulation on child emotion regulation (i.e. parent-driven effects). The notion that parents can positively or negatively influence their children’s emotional responses and regulation dates back decades (Gottman et al., Reference Gottman, Katz and Hooven1997). As was covered in Part II and Part III of this book, regulating emotions well is an essential faculty of parenting that influences child development through several avenues. The pathways of the effects of family socialization on emotion regulation are outlined in the Tripartite Model (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007), which details influences that include observation (e.g. modeling), parenting practices (e.g. emotion coaching), and emotional climate of the family (e.g. marital relations). These pathways have been documented empirically since (for a review, see Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017).

Parent characteristics such as their own attachment styles, levels of stress and social support, and mental health influence familial socialization of emotion regulation. Most relevant to this chapter, parent-driven effects on child emotion regulation could involve the transmission of emotion dysregulation from parent to child (see more on this in Section 11.1.5). Leveraging this directionality, interventions target parenting skills to improve child behavior/emotion regulation (e.g. Rothenberg et al., Reference Rothenberg, Weinstein, Dandes and Jent2019). In sum, models with parent-driven effects view emotion dysregulation as originating from the parent and transmitting to the child through various mechanisms.

11.1.4.2 Child-Driven Effects

Less well characterized is the role that children play in the dyadic nature of emotion regulation (i.e. child-driven effects), though research indicates that children can also modulate the flow of parent emotion and corresponding regulatory strategies. Short term, within a parent–child exchange, a distressed child can evoke an emotionally dysregulated state within the parent. Long term, attributes of the child (e.g. difficult temperament) can negatively affect parenting (Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Wang and Saudino2017), perhaps through emotion dysregulation. Patterson’s Coercion Theory (Patterson, Reference Patterson, Dishion, Snyder and Patterson2016) captures early child emotion dysregulation as an evocative factor of negative parent behavior that cascades into a coercive dyadic cycle occurring over many years. Further, Sameroff’s transactional model (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff and Sameroff2009) outlines both bidirectional and recursive effects to relations between caregivers and children that adds to the complex, dynamic multilevel processes involved in emotion regulation development (Olson & Sameroff, Reference Olson and Sameroff2009).

11.1.4.3 Interactive Effects

Since Bell’s (Reference Bell1968) reinterpretation of directions of effects in socialization processes, an accumulating research base indicates that there are mutually interactive dyadic influences observed between parent and child (e.g. Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Wang and Saudino2017; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Deros, Jain, Jacobs and De Los Reyes2022). Parent–child interactions can evoke intense and complex emotions from both members of the dyad (Hajal et al., Reference Hajal, Teti, Cole and Ram2019) that can result in proximal, reciprocal exchanges of emotions and regulatory strategies as well as future implementation of these strategies. To illustrate, because a child’s emotion regulation abilities result from continuous and reciprocal interactions between the child and their caregiver over time (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff, Evans and Wachs2010), a caregiver’s response to their child’s anger can alter the child’s perception of if/how the expression of anger is acceptable and their subsequent expression of anger. At the same time, the caregiver receives information about if/how they must change their regulatory strategies to influence their child’s regulatory capacity (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Feng, Inboden, Hooper and Gerhardt2022). To this end, increased research attention has been paid to the conceptualization of emotion regulation as a dynamic, dyadic process in recent years (Gates & Liu, Reference Gates and Liu2016; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018; Silk, Reference Silk2019; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Mennies, Waller, Ladouceur, Forbes, Ryan, Dahl and Silk2019; Wright & Hopwood, Reference Wright and Hopwood2016).

11.1.5 Intergenerational Transmission of Emotion Regulation

Characterizing the origins of emotion regulation is critical to understand the interplay between parent and child emotion regulation. Emotion regulation is transmitted from parents to children through both genetic and environmental mechanisms (see Chapters 4, 9, and 10). Bridgett and colleagues (Reference Bridgett, Burt, Edwards and Deater-Deckard2015) proposed an intergenerational transmission model that examines the prenatal, social/contextual, and neurobiological mechanisms contributing to the intergenerational transmission of self-regulation (including emotion regulation). Genetic risk, for example, could emerge such that emotion dysregulation is a preexisting issue for the parent that is passed on to the child. Environmental transmission of risk may occur through parental modeling of emotion dysregulation. Although outside of the scope of this chapter, the study of dyadic processes of emotion regulation can be significantly enhanced by broadening the environmental focus to include siblings, co-parents, the family system (Paley & Hajal, Reference Paley and Hajal2022), peers, neighborhoods, and culture (Kiel & Kalomiris, Reference Kiel and Kalomiris2015).

11.2 Assessing Dyadic Interactions and Parent Emotion Regulation

The preceding review outlined some of the complexity in the dynamic processes of parent–child emotion regulation interactions. We turn now to assessment strategies. Investigating dyadic emotion regulation begins with study design. Prior to data collection, researchers must consider the research question and what process that reflects, feasibility of methods, and analytic techniques. There have been compelling arguments for the implementation of innovative paradigms to capture dyadic emotion regulation and for employing context-sensitive studies of emotion regulation influences (Dixon-Gordon et al., Reference Dixon-Gordon, Bernecker and Christensen2015; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018).

In this section, we review (1) assessment methods to measure parent–child dyadic interactions related to emotion, including surveys, interactive tasks, and physiological methods; (2) challenges researchers encounter when investigating these processes; and (3) suggestions for overcoming these challenges.

11.2.1 Assessment Methods
11.2.1.1 Questionnaires

The most accessible approach to studying interactive dyadic emotion regulation is to administer developmentally appropriate questionnaires to both parents and youth and to employ one of the analytic techniques outlined later in this chapter. If children are too young to self-report, researchers could obtain reports on child emotion regulation from teachers, parents, or researchers depending on the child’s age. Because methodological biases could be introduced if parents report on themselves and their children (Podsakoff et al., Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003), different raters for parent and child emotion regulation are preferable.

Measures that assess interpersonal emotion regulation include the Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Carpenter and Curtiss2016), the Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Morelli, Ong and Zaki2018), and the Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (Niven et al., Reference Niven, Totterdell, Stride and Holman2011). Notably, the psychometric properties of these measures were evaluated among adult samples; administration to children would require further psychometric evaluation. Questionnaires of intrapersonal regulation are more widely implemented and include the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003) and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004).

Generally, questionnaire methods for emotion regulation are poised to tap trait, rather than state, emotion regulation (Silk, Reference Silk2019). Assessing emotion regulation from parents and children over time would inform stability/change over time and/or how one member’s emotion regulation influences that of the other dyad member over time. Some limitations of self-report measures of emotion regulation include the ability of measures only to provide insight into explicit, conscious processes (i.e. processes for which the individual is aware), reliance on a high degree of the individual’s emotional awareness, retrospective recall, and global, rather than context-specific items. Next, we review other methods researchers use that attempt to assess parent–youth interactions and emotion regulation in vivo.

11.2.1.2 Ecological Momentary Assessment

Ecological momentary assessment (EMA), a form of ambulatory assessment and experience sampling, overcomes some of the limitations of questionnaires. EMA involves the repeated assessment of behaviors and experiences in-the-moment to assess a domain of interest when it occurs in real-time in the natural environment (Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022; Shiffman et al., Reference Shiffman, Stone and Hufford2008; Silk, Reference Silk2019). Using EMA, one can assess the fluctuations of a domain over time and across contexts to minimize recall bias and maximize external validity (Shiffman et al., Reference Shiffman, Stone and Hufford2008; Silk, Reference Silk2019). This enables researchers to make inferences into short-term dynamics of the parent–child dyad, yet it is an underused method for assessing parent–adolescent interactions (Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Boele and Bulow2022). With respect to emotion regulation and parenting, EMA can enable parents and/or youth to report on some aspect of their interactions with each other, and/or their emotion regulation throughout the day, with the ability to indicate context (e.g. with peers; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Mennies, Waller, Ladouceur, Forbes, Ryan, Dahl and Silk2019). Several studies using EMA demonstrate the role that parents play in supporting adolescents‘ emotion regulation, based on adolescents’ EMA reports (Silk, Reference Silk2019; Silk et al., Reference Silk, Forbes, Whalen, Jakubcak, Thompson, Ryan, Axelson, Birmaher and Dahl2011; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Silk, Stone and Dahl2014). For example, in adolescents with and without major depressive disorder, the use of EMA to measure daily social interactions after a negative event enabled researchers to determine that adolescents with depression co-ruminate with their parents, which adolescent participants without depression did not do (Waller et al., Reference Waller, Silk, Stone and Dahl2014). Limitations to EMA include potential burden on participants due to the repeated nature of assessments, reliance on self-report, and the potential for missing data (Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022). Several recent reviews provide further detail on this method and its relevance to emotion regulation (e.g. Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022; Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Boele and Bulow2022; Silk, Reference Silk2019).

11.2.1.3 Interaction Tasks

Some parent–child interactions may occur less frequently, for only a short period of time (Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Boele and Bulow2022), or are difficult to assess with a static survey report, and thus may be better suited to measurement while being induced and observed in a laboratory setting rather than via questionnaires or EMA (e.g. Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Wilson, Jain, Deros, Um, Hurwitz, Jacobs, Myerberg, Ehrlich, Dunn, Aldao, Stadnik and De Los Reyes2017, Reference Thomas, Jain, Wilson, Deros, Jacobs, Dunn, Aldao, Stadnik and De Los Reyes2019). Furthermore, if the process of how parents and children behave together is the focus, an interaction task is the optimal method to obtain such data. Moreover, behavior and psychophysiology are common facets of emotion regulation (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018) and such a task enables measurement of both for dyad members. Accordingly, observational procedures for assessing emotion regulation provide some advantages beyond self-report (Girard & Cohn, Reference Girard and Cohn2016). Methodologically, it is beneficial to have an objective source of data on emotion regulation, separate from survey reports. Survey reports and behavioral observations are not highly correlated, indicating they do not measure the same things (e.g. parent behavior; Hendriks et al., Reference Hendriks, Van der Giessen, Stams and Overbeek2018). Moreover, interaction tasks are often designed to be ecologically valid in order to observe behavior as it may naturally occur between dyads (rather than rely on retrospective recall or insight during survey reports).

Highlighting their importance, even some interventions for childhood behavior problems involve parent–youth interaction tasks. Their inclusion is supported by the role of emotion dysregulation in the context of impairing psychiatric conditions requiring early intervention (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Gee, De Los Reyes and Seager2016; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015), and the recognition that parents and youth evoke responses from one another that can become entrenched patterns (Patterson, Reference Patterson, Dishion, Snyder and Patterson2016). These tasks enable clinicians to observe patterns of interaction that may elicit emotional reactions that escalate/exacerbate problem behaviors as part of the assessment process, subsequently informing a treatment plan and skill development. One example is Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT), a treatment for disruptive child behavior in which clinicians coach parents in vivo how to respond to their children during activities. Child emotion dysregulation has been shown to decrease significantly from pre- to post-treatment (Rothenberg et al., Reference Rothenberg, Weinstein, Dandes and Jent2019).

In laboratory settings, many parent–youth interaction task designs salient to measuring emotion regulation and parenting involve a discussion task focused on problem-solving, planning, or conflict-resolution (Bodner et al., Reference Bodner, Kuppens, Allen, Sheeber and Ceulemans2018; Cui et al., Reference Cui, Morris, Harrist, Larzelere, Criss and Houltberg2015; Donenberg & Weisz, Reference Donenberg and Weisz1997). In the latter situation, topics about which parent–youth dyads have conflict are introduced (e.g. Issues Checklist; Prinz et al., Reference Prinz, Foster, Kent and O’Leary1979), and dyads are asked to come to a resolution within a brief period (e.g. 5–10 minutes). The theoretical underpinnings that inform how researchers observe and code parent and child behavior are diverse (e.g. stress, attachment). One commonality of these phenotypes is that many involve facets of emotion regulation, particularly if the objective of the task is to induce stress or replicate an emotionally intense interaction that is typical for the dyad.

In the context of the interaction task, researchers often measure emotional reaction or intensity, often along a spectrum of some variation of positive affect (warmth/validation) and negative affect (hostility/anger), the psychometric considerations of which are discussed thoroughly elsewhere (Girard & Cohn, Reference Girard and Cohn2016). Finally, interaction task measurement may occur from coding behavior such as body language, content of speech, and facial expressions; psychophysiological responding (e.g. heart rate variability during task; Cui et al., Reference Cui, Morris, Harrist, Larzelere, Criss and Houltberg2015; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Jain, Wilson, Deros, Jacobs, Dunn, Aldao, Stadnik and De Los Reyes2019); and/or post-task ratings of emotions experienced during the task (Chaplin et al., Reference Chaplin, Sinha, Simmons, Healy, Mayes, Hommer and Crowley2012; Turpyn et al., Reference Turpyn, Chaplin, Cook and Martelli2015).

In an example study including multiple domains to measure emotion, researchers assessed adolescents‘ psychophysiology (blood pressure, heart rate) during a 10-minute conflict discussion task with parents and asked them to report on their emotions pre- and post-task (Chaplin et al., Reference Chaplin, Sinha, Simmons, Healy, Mayes, Hommer and Crowley2012). Results indicated an inverse relationship between parenting behaviors during the task and adolescents’ emotions, such that lower parental involvement (e.g. offering solutions, setting/explaining rules) and parental support/warmth were associated with greater adolescent anger arousal and greater blood pressure. In another study using the same conflict discussion task, researchers created latent profiles of adolescent emotion regulation during the conflict discussion task, comprised of positive and negative emotional expressions during the conflict task (i.e. coded from observed behavior), self-reports of subjective experiences of anger and anxiety evoked from the task (completed immediately post task), and heart rate reactivity during the task (Turpyn et al., Reference Turpyn, Chaplin, Cook and Martelli2015). Parenting behaviors during the task were measured via an existing coding system by trained staff, with a focus on behaviors that were negative/critical (e.g. mocking, interrupting). Negative/critical parenting was associated with a greater likelihood of adolescents belonging to profiles indicative of less emotion regulation (Turpyn et al., Reference Turpyn, Chaplin, Cook and Martelli2015). It is important to note that although the two aforementioned studies are cross-sectional, the predominant theoretical rationale, which also underpins the analyses and results, reflect parent-driven effects; however, both articles acknowledge that adolescents’ behaviors stemming from emotional reactivity may evoke certain parental responses (i.e. child-driven effects). In sum, parent–youth interaction tasks are a flexible means of assessment that enable derivation of behavior consistent with emotion (dys)regulation, which can then be related to parenting domains and/or youth risk behaviors and psychopathology.

11.2.1.4 Physiological Indices

Although it is beyond the scope of what can be described in detail in this chapter, physiological measurements can be implemented during these interactions. Researchers use physiological measurements, such as of cardiovascular response and brain synchrony/connectivity, to index facets of emotion regulation (Cui et al., Reference Cui, Morris, Harrist, Larzelere, Criss and Houltberg2015; Ratliff et al., Reference Ratliff, Kerr, Misaki, Cosgrove, Moore, DeVille, Silk, Barch, Tapert, Simmons, Bodurka and Morris2021; Reindl et al., Reference Reindl, Gerloff, Scharke and Konrad2018; Turpyn et al., Reference Turpyn, Chaplin, Cook and Martelli2015). These assessments facilitate investigations of processes like synchrony between parents and youth (Abney et al., Reference Abney, daSilva and Bertenthal2021) that are theorized to be a mechanism by which parents help their offspring regulate their emotions (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018). As an example, one study found parent–child brain synchrony during a cooperative interaction task that was not present during a competitive interaction task or when completing tasks with a stranger (Reindl et al., Reference Reindl, Gerloff, Scharke and Konrad2018).

11.2.2 Challenges and Solutions to Assessing Dyadic Interactions and Parent Emotion Regulation

The methods described here advance the study of processes of emotion regulation in the context of parent–youth relationships. There are challenges to consider before planning an investigation of these dynamic processes, which we review next. With the proper study design, planning and training, it is possible to collect valid and informative data, the results of which can advance the field of emotion regulation.

11.2.2.1 Study Design

The study research question(s) and how the measures, means, and schedule of administration will fulfill that objective should guide the selection, design, or modification of the measurement instrument and subsequent analysis of data. As a priority, researchers must determine how to define and subsequently assess emotion regulation in the context of parent–youth interactions (discussed next). Then, researchers must decide how to measure interaction, either with a task or statistical inference. In the former case, researchers could implement an existing task (e.g. Chaplin et al., Reference Chaplin, Sinha, Simmons, Healy, Mayes, Hommer and Crowley2012). However, if the research question involves contextual variation of emotion regulation, a sufficient task design is necessary to elicit the behaviors of interest and perhaps include a “control” condition to contrast the conditions under which they expect to observe the target behavior (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Jain, Wilson, Deros, Jacobs, Dunn, Aldao, Stadnik and De Los Reyes2019). If the investigation involves ratings of observed behavior, sufficient numbers of research staff will be necessary, as well as a plan to monitor reliability of ratings (for a thorough review on considerations related to observational measurement, see Girard & Cohn, Reference Girard and Cohn2016). Carefully selecting study design can address the long-standing research question as to whether behaviors are stable across situations or depend on situational context (Donenberg & Weisz, Reference Donenberg and Weisz1997; Silk, Reference Silk2019); therefore, depending on the researcher’s interest, study design and task selection should be adjusted accordingly.

11.2.2.2 Defining Emotion Regulation and Its Level of Analysis

Operationally defining “emotion regulation” is crucial. In the case of a task designed to measure emotion regulation, a primary question is how one observes what may be an internal process. Because some adaptive emotion regulation skills are idiosyncratic to the individual and may be unobservable (e.g. deep breathing, reappraisal; Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003), researchers may instead focus on indicators of lack of emotion regulation or dysregulation. Thus, in many cases of interaction tasks, it has been the manifestation of an emotion that may be excessive (i.e. not adaptive), thereby interfering with the goal of the task (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015).

A related challenge is deciding what level of analysis emotion regulation will be recorded. Emotion dysregulation may be assessed at the level of behavior (e.g. yelling), or by measurement of physiological functioning, such as cardiovascular indices (e.g. heart rate) as a proxy for emotional reactivity/regulation that can be recorded continuously throughout the interaction and/or to measure synchrony between dyad members (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018; Reindl et al., Reference Reindl, Gerloff, Scharke and Konrad2018). Moreover, it is also possible to include a parent–youth interaction task to capture a parenting behavior of interest and associate those behaviors with survey reports of emotion regulation. Finally, an emotion regulation latent variable could be derived from different levels (self-report, behavior, physiology; e.g. Turpyn et al., Reference Turpyn, Chaplin, Cook and Martelli2015).

11.2.2.3 Selecting a Coding Scheme/Emotion Regulation Metric

Once emotion regulation has been defined, researchers must select an appropriate measurement instrument to facilitate inferences about interaction. Examples include survey, EMA, psychophysiology, or coding of observed behavior during an interaction task. Additional nontrivial considerations when coding behavior include having a laboratory audiovisual recording system so behaviors do not have to be coded live, and sufficient storage space and security for these recordings. For psychophysiological indices of emotion regulation, researchers could use wearables like heart rate watches or a BIOPAC system to record sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system response (Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022).

11.2.2.4 Informants

Even when assessing the same construct, parent and youth reports often do not agree, suggesting they may have different perspectives (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Thomas, Goodman and Kundey2013, Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). Although it may complicate the study design, ideally both parents and youth will be included as informants in a study of parent–youth interactive effects of emotion regulation, particularly when investigating questions pertaining to synchrony or coregulation. As discussed previously, shared method variance is of concern if parents report on both themselves and their child. Relatedly, objective measures like observed behavior by trained raters also address informant issues.

In some cases of prior research, adolescents have reported on facets of their own emotion regulation and reported on another domain that allowed inferences into parental role of emotion regulation (Silk, Reference Silk2019; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Mennies, Waller, Ladouceur, Forbes, Ryan, Dahl and Silk2019; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Silk, Stone and Dahl2014). These investigations are informative; however, when possible, including both informants eliminates bias in relying solely on one informant’s perspective. An additional consideration is whether both parent and youth are providing data on the same construct, or whether one dyad member reports on one construct (e.g. parenting), and the other dyad member reports on another construct (e.g. adolescent emotion regulation). To use certain interactive analyses (described in Section 11.3.2.1 Actor–Partner Interdependence Model), data from both members of the dyad on both constructs are required. Relatedly, there may be developmental issues to consider if parents and youth will report on the same emotion regulation domain using different measures. For example, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale Short Form (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Xia, Fosco, Yaptangco, Skidmore and Crowell2016) is appropriate for adolescents and adults, but if researchers wanted to measure the same emotion regulation domain in young children and their parents, determining which measures would enable inferences about the same domain across different developmental levels is key to avoiding invalid results due to measurement issues. Finally, studying family emotion regulation and interactive effects (i.e. including all members of a family unit) is exceptionally challenging due to translating the data into a consistent format needed for statistical models, and is therefore typically restricted to dyads (for example, parent and child; for designs integrating mother, father, and child emotion regulation, see, e.g. Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Rasmussen, Smiley, Buttitta and Borelli2021; Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Wu and Wang2019).

11.2.2.5 Timing

Dyadic emotion regulation transactions occur within interaction as well as across time (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Cui, Criss, Simmons and Cole2018). Within brief interactions, conventional approaches to the study of dyadic emotion regulation aggregate data over time (i.e. take the arithmetic average), which fails to capture the rich and dynamic moment-to-moment fluctuations. To illustrate, by aggregating data, nuance is lost such that one cannot evaluate how parent–child emotion regulation exchanges manifest over time. For example, the effect of a maternal regulation strategy on the child’s subsequent emotional experience or the influence of a child’s emotional experience on parent’s emotion coaching.

Furthermore, depending on the study design, researchers may prefer to sequentially code behavior on a micro level between parents and youth to infer how the sequence reveals the interaction process (e.g. sequential analysis; Bakeman & Gottman, Reference Bakeman and Gottman1997). For research questions involving synchrony or co-regulation for which psychophysiology serves as a proxy of emotion regulation indices, it is important to have accurate time stamps of the start and end of tasks, as well as any notable events to match dyadic processes for analysis. Finally, researchers should consider whether the interaction task will be used once with study participants or readministered at intervals (e.g. yearly). If the latter, it is important to consider youth developmental level and select a task that will enable multiple administrations over the time interval while still appropriate for the youth’s developmental level.

11.2.2.6 Causality

A brief but important note on causality is warranted, given that many theories are premised on the idea of whose emotion (dys)regulation impacts whom. To infer causality, research studies need sufficient temporal ordering, and an ability to measure change, either within-person over time, or before and after an intervention. Furthermore, experimental manipulation via random assignment to conditions helps eliminate alternate explanations (e.g. Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Jain, Wilson, Deros, Jacobs, Dunn, Aldao, Stadnik and De Los Reyes2019). Although the data may fit a particular analytic method, the use of that method is not what enables researchers to infer causality but rather their research design.

In sum, there are several design, staff, and implementation hurdles to overcome that explain why there are not as many studies on interactive effects of parent–youth emotion regulation as would be useful. Relatedly, it is often difficult to collect large sample sizes when interaction tasks are involved. By reducing complexity without compromising data quality (e.g. automated behavioral coding; Girard & Cohn, Reference Girard and Cohn2016) the likelihood is greater that researchers can scale up data collection and obtain a larger sample size.

11.3 Statistical Approaches for Analyzing Dyadic Interactions and Emotion Regulation in the Context of Parenting

In the preceding sections, we described methods of assessing dyadic interactions in the context of emotion regulation and parenting and the associated challenges and opportunities. A potential barrier to conducting a study on this topic is lack of knowledge regarding analytic methods and potential inferences. In this section, we describe some considerations when analyzing dyadic data and provide brief overviews and examples of analytic methods that are appropriate for addressing research questions on emotion regulation and parenting while pointing to further resources.

11.3.1 Violations of Nonindependence Due to Dyadic and Temporal Measurements

An important consideration when pursuing investigations of how parents’ and youth’s emotion regulation may be linked is the selection of data analytic strategy. One of the primary considerations when studying individuals who live intertwined lives is accounting for the inherent influence, or statistical nonindependence, that occurs due to their relationship. One of the assumptions of linear regression is that the observations are independent; thus, this assumption is violated when dyad members who are related genetically and/or share environments are studied. An additional nonindependence consideration is temporal (i.e. autocorrelation) for any data over time. Therefore, statistical techniques must be chosen that can account for this nonindependence; otherwise, regression estimates will not be accurate (Cook & Kenny, Reference Cook and Kenny2005).

11.3.2 Analytic Strategies
11.3.2.1 Actor–Partner Interdependence Model

The application of the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model (APIM; Kenny et al., Reference Kenny, Kashy and Cook2006) permits evaluation of bidirectional effects in the context of interpersonal relationships (Cook & Kenny, Reference Cook and Kenny2005). The APIM tests the effect of one individual’s predictor on their own outcome, as well as on the outcome of their dyadic partner (and vice versa), all within one model (Stas et al., Reference Stas, Kenny, Mayer and Loeys2018). To use the APIM, both dyad members must have data on a predictor and an outcome variable. APIM derives both actor and partner effects, enabling a test of interpersonal and intrapersonal processes. In other words, one can investigate the association between a characteristic (e.g. emotion regulation ability) and an outcome (e.g. depression symptoms) for both dyad members, controlling for the influence of the other. A significant actor effect indicates one’s own characteristics are related to one’s own outcome, whereas a significant partner effect indicates one dyad member’s characteristics are related to another’s outcome. If both partner effects are significant, it suggests bidirectional influence, but one significant partner effect indicates interdependence (Cook & Kenny, Reference Cook and Kenny2005).

APIM models can be implemented using structural equation modeling (SEM) or multilevel modeling (MLM). To assist with analysis and interpretation, several R Shiny apps have been made available by David Kenny and colleagues that allow the investigator to upload a data set and derive output of results, including tables, figures, text summarizing the analysis and results, and code (DyadR)Footnote 1. Briefly, APIM using SEM is recommended for dyads who are distinguishable, meaning dyad members differ according to an attribute like sex or gender, or family role (e.g. parent, child; Ledermann & Kenny, Reference Ledermann and Kenny2017). Should researchers select this method, there is an R Shiny app (APIM-SEM)Footnote 2 that executes the analysis, provides text interpretation of the findings, along with tables, figures, and R code, although it is not necessary that researchers know R programming to use this resource (Stas et al., Reference Stas, Kenny, Mayer and Loeys2018). MLM APIM is recommended when dyad members are not distinguishable (same-sex roommates; Ledermann & Kenny, Reference Ledermann and Kenny2017), which may be less relevant in the study of parent–youth emotion regulation. Best practices for APIM have been reviewed elsewhere (Ledermann & Kenny, Reference Ledermann and Kenny2017; Stas et al., Reference Stas, Kenny, Mayer and Loeys2018). Versions of this model have been adapted for longitudinal data as well (Bolger & Laurenceau, Reference Bolger, Laurenceau, Kenny and Little2013; Savord et al., Reference Savord, McNeish, Iida, Quiroz and Ha2022), one of which includes a Shiny app (L-APIMFootnote 3; Gistelinck & Loeys, Reference Gistelinck and Loeys2019). Longitudinal APIM has recently been applied to parent–child emotion regulation (Boeve et al., Reference Boeve, Beeghly, Stacks, Manning and Thomason2019). This approach stands to enhance our understanding of the unfolding of dyadic parent–child emotion regulation over time.

There are several examples of the APIM implemented to investigate parent–youth emotion regulation. First, given that psychopathology can be a manifestation of persistent emotional dysregulation (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Gee, De Los Reyes and Seager2016; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Suri and Gross2015), in a study of adolescents hospitalized for treatment of acute psychiatric symptoms and their parents, researchers used the APIM to probe the association between self-reports on one’s own difficulty with emotion regulation and depression symptoms (Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Thomas, Hood, Bettis, Rizzo and Liu2020). There were significant actor effects between difficulty accessing emotion regulation strategies and depression symptoms. Further, there was a significant negative partner effect for parental impulsive emotion regulation and adolescent depressive symptoms, demonstrating interdependence between parents and adolescents. Second, a study investigated the associations between parents’ and adolescents’ reports of sources of parental knowledge with observed parent and adolescent behavior during a conflict discussion task, based on the rationale that how parents and adolescents interact and manage their emotions when resolving conflict (i.e. a goal-directed activity) will be associated with processes of parental monitoring (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Deros, Jain, Jacobs and De Los Reyes2022). Behavioral codes came from attachment domains representing behavioral categories that either helps or hurts the dyad’s goal of resolving the conflict topic. Hostile behavior (e.g. mocking) and an attachment domain known as Secure Base Use (adolescent) or Secure Base Provision (parent) (e.g. validation, smiling) were coded by trained staff for each dyad member. Adolescent reports of greater adolescent disclosure about their activities and whereabouts were associated with more secure base behavior exhibited by both adolescents (actor effects) and parents (partner effect), as well as less hostile behavior exhibited by both adolescents (actor effects) and parents (partner effect; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Deros, Jain, Jacobs and De Los Reyes2022). These findings support the interdependence of sources of parental knowledge that are related to monitoring, and manifestations of emotion regulation processes during a conflict discussion task (see Figure 11.1, for example APIM figure).

CC BY 4.0 SARAH THOMAS

Figure 11.1 Illustration of cross-sectional Actor–Partner Interdependence Models

Note. (a) Unstandardized parameter estimates of actor and partner effects for adolescent- and parent-reports of Adolescent Disclosure in relation to Secure Base behavior, controlling for the effects of age, sex, and sample source (covariate parameters not included in figure). (b) Unstandardized parameter estimates of actor and partner effects for adolescent- and parent-reports of Adolescent Disclosure in relation to Hostile behavior, controlling for the effects of age, sex, and sample source (covariate parameters not included in the figure). Standard errors are presented in parentheses. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

11.3.2.2 Other Strategies

The APIM is not appropriate for all dyadic data, such as with only a youth outcome. In those situations, it is important to use an analytic strategy that will account for the nonindependent nature of the data. Options include generalized estimating equations (Hanley et al., Reference Hanley, Negassa, Edwardes and Forrester2003), MLM (Page-Gould, Reference Page-Gould, Cacioppo, Tassinary and Berntson2017), latent growth models for longitudinal data (Muniz-Terrera et al., Reference Muniz-Terrera, Robitaille, Kelly, Johansson, Hofer and Piccinin2017), and SEM. SEM permits evaluation of multivariate causal relations and these methods have significantly enhanced our understanding of the bidirectional nature of parent–child interactive effects (e.g. Micalizzi et al., Reference Micalizzi, Ronald and Saudino2016, Reference Micalizzi, Wang and Saudino2017). A specific model that is widely used is the cross-lagged panel model, a discrete-time structural equation model that can be used to analyze panel data where variables are assessed at least twice over time. The goal of these models is to examine the effect of one variable on the other variable (and vice versa) over time. Furthermore, SEM could be used if a researcher wished to analyze data from two parents and one youth (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Rasmussen, Smiley, Buttitta and Borelli2021; Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Wu and Wang2019). Detailed options for analytic strategies have been reviewed elsewhere (e.g. Gates & Liu, Reference Gates and Liu2016; Thorson et al., Reference Thorson, West and Mendes2018).

11.3.2.3 Latent Growth Curve

To measure a behavior over time, latent growth curve models enable assessment of slope and intercept, which can be used to assess the average rate of change in a construct, like emotion regulation. Further, by adding time-varying covariates, one can determine if another variable covaries with the behavior of interest. Consequently, it is also possible to evaluate parent–youth associations related to emotion regulation with growth curve models. To illustrate, researchers used growth curve analyses in a sample of mother–child dyads (half of the mothers were randomized to emotion regulation skills treatment) to investigate how change in maternal emotion regulation over 12 months was related to both the starting point and change in youth emotion regulation (Byrd et al., Reference Byrd, Lee, Frigoletto, Zalewski and Stepp2021). In another example, in the context of an intervention for comorbid adolescent psychiatric disorders and substance use, when assessing past 7-day cannabis use over time during and after an intervention, a time-varying covariate of parental frustration was used to determine how cannabis and parental frustration were related over time (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Brick, Micalizzi, Wolff, Frazier, Graves, Esposito-Smytherse and Spirito2020). This analytic technique has important implications for considering how the emotional facets of parent–adolescent relationships can be integral to the fluctuation of target behavior of an intervention over time. Despite the intervention, weekly cannabis use increased across the 1-year follow-up time, and parental ratings of frustration that were higher than their average were associated with greater adolescent cannabis use at baseline, 3-, and 6-month follow-up. This analytic strategy could be used to understand influence over time in parent–youth relationships and emotion regulation.

11.4 Future Directions

Future directions to advance the study of dynamic parent–youth interactions pertaining to emotion regulation involve thoughtfully selected study designs, the integration of complex technology, and collection of longitudinal data on both short- and long-term time scales. First, with the advent of wearable technology and passive sensing that can detect geo-location, study designs that prompt parents and youth to respond on EMA measures of emotion regulation when they are in the same location can also provide valuable insight into these interactions in the real world (Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022; Silk, Reference Silk2019). Second, incorporating parental role into design and measurement will enable inferences about maternal versus paternal or primary versus secondary caregiver role on emotion regulation dynamics, which has been undertaken by few researchers (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Rasmussen, Smiley, Buttitta and Borelli2021; Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Wu and Wang2019). Finally, to adequately capture the protracted, bidirectional development process of emotion regulation, studies are needed that can measure these dynamics from infancy through adulthood. This will be a large undertaking, and will benefit from support from funding agency stakeholders, as well as harnessing the potential of existing large, longitudinal studies (e.g. Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study [ABCD] and HEALthy Brain and Child Development Study). Although the ABCD Study includes measures of emotion regulation, data have not been released that capture continuous assessment of parent and child emotion regulation that would allow inference of individual’s abilities and how these covary over time. However, because the sample is so large (e.g. 11,875 youth at baseline) and assessments include numerous domains, it may be possible to derive a latent factor representing emotion regulation that could be investigated over time as youth develop. In conclusion, measuring the dynamic processes supporting parent and youth emotion regulation requires careful consideration and a range of skill, and so research teams that bring a variety of skill sets supporting the measurement and analysis of these processes will be well-equipped to address the challenges necessary to advance this field.

11.5 Conclusions

In this chapter, we reviewed the theoretical and empirical underpinnings of dynamic parent–youth emotion regulation processes, their measurement, and analysis. The very type of research designs and analyses that could advance the field of parent–youth dynamics of emotion regulation are still underused (Keijsers et al., Reference Keijsers, Boele and Bulow2022; Silk, Reference Silk2019). These research gaps represent promising opportunities for innovative study designs, and with technological advancements (Bettis et al., Reference Bettis, Burke, Nesi and Liu2022; Girard & Cohn, Reference Girard and Cohn2016; Silk, Reference Silk2019; Stas et al., Reference Stas, Kenny, Mayer and Loeys2018), conducting these types of studies may be more accessible to researchers than ever before.

Chapter 12 Neurobiology of Parenting and Implications for Emotion Regulation

Helena J. V. Rutherford

The transition to parenthood is a time of psychological change that serves to support parent and child development. While a breadth of research in animal studies of the neurobiology of maternal behavior exists (Pawluski et al., Reference Pawluski, Hoekzema, Leuner and Lonstein2021), efforts to identify the neurobiology of parenthood in humans is a more recent endeavor (Mayes et al., Reference Mayes, Rutherford, Suchman and Close2012). Investigating the neurobiology of parenting has theoretical value in understanding periods of adult development but has important clinical implications when considering contexts where parents may struggle in their caregiving role, with consequences for their own and their child’s well-being (Squire & Stein, Reference Squire and Stein2003). Critically, a common challenge for all new parents is the capacity to regulate their own and their child’s emotions, especially during infancy and early childhood. In particular, it has been hypothesized that one outcome of the neural and psychological reorganization during the transition to parenthood is to support the unique demands of parental emotion regulation (Rutherford, Wallace, et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015).

In this chapter, the neurobiology of human parenting is examined and implications for emotion regulation considered. Specifically, studies documenting maternal brain structure and function are reviewed. In advancing this area of inquiry, several studies will also be described that have examined maternal neural responses to negative stimuli (infant and noninfant) following distraction and cognitive reappraisal instructions to examine the neurobiological basis of parental emotion regulation more directly. Importantly, most parental brain research focuses on mothers, representing an inherent limitation to our understanding of this critical transitional period for nonbirthing parents, including fathers. While the studies discussed here focus on the maternal brain, where fathers are included this is noted, and the importance of understanding the paternal and nonbirthing parent brain is revisited when considering the next steps for this body of work.

12.1 Maternal Brain Structure

Only a handful of studies have examined the impact of pregnancy and the postpartum period on maternal brain structure, with a specific focus on gray matter (GM) volume measured using structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Specifically, these studies of GM volume indicate both growth and decline in GM during the perinatal period. In the first investigation of changing GM volume across the postpartum period, mothers completed an MRI scan at 2–4 weeks postpartum and again at 3–4 months postpartum (Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, et al., Reference Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Wang and Swain2010). GM volume increases over time were observed in the parietal lobes, prefrontal cortex, and the midbrain (including the hypothalamus, amygdala, and substantia nigra). Critically, GM growth in the midbrain areas was associated with mothers’ positive perceptions of their child, linking maternal brain structure to real-world parenting. Luders et al. (Reference Luders, Kurth, Gingnell, Engman, Yong, Poromaa and Gaser2020) also found widespread GM increases from 1–2 days post delivery to 4–6 weeks following delivery in recent mothers, including in the pre- and postcentral gyrus, middle and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), insula, parietal and temporal lobes, and the thalamus. Including a control group of nulliparous women, Lisofsky et al. (Reference Lisofsky, Gallinat, Lindenberger and Kühn2019) also showed maternal GM volume increases from 2 months to 4–5 months postpartum in numerous regions, including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, middle frontal gyrus, cerebellum, and nucleus accumbens. Taken together, these studies suggest significant growth of the maternal brain during the postpartum period as indexed by GM volume.

Although the majority of structural MRI studies have focused on the maternal brain postpartum, one study has examined GM volume in nulliparous women prior to conception and then again in the postpartum period to examine the impact of pregnancy on the maternal brain. In this study, Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) reported decreased GM volume in multiple brain areas from pregnancy to approximately 2 months postpartum, including across the anterior and posterior midline, and the lateral prefrontal and temporal cortex. Notably, fathers and a control group of men without children were scanned following the same timeline as the nulliparous women to examine whether the experience of becoming a parent, versus the biological experience of pregnancy, would lead to structural GM changes. Importantly, no difference in GM volume over time was observed between fathers and the men without children. Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) concluded that it was the biological experience of pregnancy underscoring the GM volume changes observed in nulliparous women, rather than the transition to parenthood alone.

These findings by Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) build on prior work evidencing reduced brain volume in women scanned during pregnancy and again in the postpartum period (Oatridge et al., Reference Oatridge, Holdcroft, Saeed, Hajnal, Puri, Fusi and Bydder2002). While the widespread reductions in GM volume were the primary focus of the Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) study, GM growth within the hippocampus from preconception to the postpartum period was also reported. Although GM volume decline may have negative connotations, decreased GM volume may reflect neural reorganization of the maternal brain conferring a benefit through “fine tuning” the brain to support mother and child well-being (Pawluski et al., Reference Pawluski, Hoekzema, Leuner and Lonstein2021).

In sum, the maternal brain undergoes significant structural reorganization during pregnancy and the postpartum period, evidenced by GM growth and decline. Of note, these structural brain changes in response to motherhood do not appear transitory; GM volume reductions reported by Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) were still apparent at 2 years postpartum, with a follow-up study evidencing the persistence of GM volume reductions 6 years later in a subset of women from the original sample (Martínez-García et al., Reference Martínez-García, Paternina-Die, Barba-Müller, Martín de Blas, Beumala, Cortizo, Pozzobon, Marcos-Vidal, Fernández-Pena, Picado, Belmonte-Padilla, Massó-Rodriguez, Ballesteros, Desco, Vilarroya, Hoekzema and Carmona2021). Critically, many of the brain regions identified in structural GM studies of the maternal brain overlap with those implicated in emotion regulation (Etkin et al., Reference Etkin, Büchel and Gross2015), suggesting the structural plasticity of the perinatal period (and beyond) may be important in our understanding of maternal emotion regulation, and parenting more generally.

12.2 Maternal Brain Function

The majority of maternal brain research has used functional MRI (fMRI) and event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine neural responses to infant stimuli (infant faces and vocalizations) as an index of maternal brain function (Maupin et al., Reference Maupin, Hayes, Mayes and Rutherford2015; Parsons et al., Reference Parsons, Young, Stein and Kringelbach2017; Swain, Reference Swain2011). Both these neuroimaging approaches provide insight into the detection and processing of salient infant signals (or cues) within and across samples of parents and nonparents. Importantly, neural responses to infant signals may reflect both reactivity and regulatory responding in the maternal brain. For instance, when presented with infant distress signals, neurobiological markers of reactivity to infant cry may be modulated by maternal regulatory function. Thus, reactivity and regulatory responding to infant signals may be interwoven, presenting a potential limitation when interpreting maternal neural responses to infant signals to inform the neurobiology of emotion regulation and parenting.

Nevertheless, converging lines of research have identified several “parental brain networks” responsive to infant cues that include brain areas implicated in mentalization, empathy, and emotion regulation (Feldman, Reference Feldman2015). This latter emotion regulation network includes the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), middle frontal gyrus (MFG), and the frontopolar cortex. The identification of these parental brain networks has been driven by research that presents infant face and cry stimuli, including stimuli from mother’s own child. Infant faces are hypothesized to be particularly salient cues motivating caregiving, activating brain regions implicated in reward processing in parents and nonparents (Glocker et al., Reference Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Gur and Sachser2009; Kringelbach et al., Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016; Lorenz, Reference Lorenz1943). Given the breadth of work in this area, an increasing number of meta-analyses have been conducted to refine understanding on maternal brain function. In meta-analytic fMRI maternal brain research, heightened reactivity to own as compared to unfamiliar infant faces (only positive and neutral expressions) is reported as prominent in the midbrain (substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area), amygdala, striatum, insula, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC; Rigo et al., Reference Rigo, Kim, Esposito, Putnick, Venuti and Bornstein2019; see also Paul et al., Reference Paul, Austin, Elliott, Ellison-Wright, Wan, Drake, Downey, Elmadih, Mukherjee, Heaney, Williams and Abel2019). Across parents and nonparents, meta-analytic maternal (and nonparent) ERP work has evidenced increased neural responding to infant distress as compared to positive and neutral infant faces (Kuzava et al., Reference Kuzava, Frost, Perrone, Kang, Lindhiem and Bernard2020).

Although maternal brain function can be studied in isolation by examining neural patterns of responding to infant cues, to understand the functional significance of these neural responses, an increasing number of studies are incorporating measures of both maternal brain and behavior. For instance, ERP responses to infant faces in mothers have been associated with sensitive and intrusive maternal behavior observed during interaction tasks (Bernard et al., Reference Bernard, Simons and Dozier2015; Endendijk et al., Reference Endendijk, Spencer, van Baar and Bos2018; Kuzava et al., Reference Kuzava, Nissim, Frost, Nelson and Bernard2019). Notably, one study measured ERP responses to infant faces in the third trimester of pregnancy and again at 3–5 months postpartum, finding that changing neural responses to infant faces was associated with postpartum maternal bonding (Dudek et al., Reference Dudek, Colasante, Zuffianò and Haley2020).

Understanding the sources of variability in maternal processing of infant faces has also been explored, recognizing that while parental brain networks exist, each person transitions to parenthood differently, reflected in the uniqueness of their current or previous life experiences. Indeed, maternal early experiences as indexed by attachment security has also been associated with maternal neural responses to infant faces in fMRI and ERP studies (e.g. Groh & Haydon, Reference Groh and Haydon2018; Lowell et al., Reference Lowell, Dell, Potenza, Strathearn, Mayes and Rutherford2021; Strathearn et al., Reference Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico and Montague2009). Concurrently, studies of maternal infant face processing have been informative in beginning to understand where challenges related to emotion regulation may emerge and affect caregiving. Specifically, a number of studies have examined how symptoms of emotion dysregulation may contribute to neural reactivity to infant face stimuli. Specifically, neural responses to infant faces may be modulated by depression (Bjertrup et al., Reference Bjertrup, Friis and Miskowiak2019), anxiety (Yatziv et al., Reference Yatziv, Vancor, Bunderson and Rutherford2021), and maternal substance use (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Kim, Yip, Potenza, Mayes and Strathearn2021). Taken together, these studies evidence that maternal brain function can be captured by neural responses to infant facial cues and may have important implications for maternal behavior, including emotion regulation and sensitive caregiving.

Infant cries have also been employed to probe maternal brain function. Behavioral and neuroimaging data suggest parental responding to infant cries may be consistent across cultures (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Putnick, Rigo, Esposito, Swain, Suwalsky, Su, Du, Zhang, Cote, De Pisapia and Venuti2017). Notably too, several neuroimaging studies indicate that parents (mothers and fathers) respond to infant signals differently to nonparents, particularly when infants express distress (e.g. Proverbio et al., Reference Proverbio, Brignone, Matarazzo, Del Zotto and Zani2006; Purhonen et al., Reference Purhonen, Kilpeläinen-Lees, Pääkkönen, Yppärilä, Lehtonen and Karhu2001; Seifritz et al., Reference Seifritz, Esposito, Neuhoff, Luthi, Mustovic, Dammann, von Bardeleben, Radue, Cirillo, Tedeschi and Di Salle2003). In particular, the latter studies suggest a heightened response to infant distress cues in parents as compared to nonparents. Heightened responding to infant distress may confer an adaptive advantage for the developing child in eliciting caregiving behavior. Consistent with this hypothesis, infant cries have been shown to activate midbrain dopaminergic regions implicated in reward neural circuity, presumably motivating approach to elicit caregiving behavior in some maternal brain studies (Rilling, Reference Rilling2013). However, infant cries may also be dysregulating for parents. Indeed, one of the earliest challenges many parents face is regulating their own emotional response to their crying child, while also trying to help their child become more regulated (Rutherford, Wallace, et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015). Although there is variability in mothers’ capacity to tolerate infant distress (Rutherford, Booth, et al., Reference Rutherford, Booth, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2015; Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Goldberg, Luyten, Bridgett and Mayes2013), inconsolable infant crying has been linked with harsh and abusive parenting during the postpartum period (Barr, Reference Barr2014). Given associations between increased reactivity to infant cries and negative parenting behaviors, interventions have been designed to help parents regulate during bouts of infant crying during the early postpartum months (e.g. Bechtel et al., Reference Bechtel, Gaither and Leventhal2020).

Given the importance of parental responding to infant cries, a number of studies have examined neural responses to varying types of infant cry stimuli in maternal samples (Witteman et al., Reference Witteman, Van IJzendoorn, Rilling, Bos, Schiller and Bakermans-Kranenburg2019). Converging evidence supports the notion that neural responding to infant cries is associated with caregiving behavior. Musser et al. (Reference Musser, Kaiser-Laurent and Ablow2012) found that maternal sensitive behavior measured at 18 months postpartum was associated with neural responses to infant cries, including in the IFG and right frontal pole. Similarly, neural responding to infant cries in the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and amygdala measured at 2–4 weeks postpartum was associated with maternal sensitivity measured at 3–4 months postpartum (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Feldman, Mayes, Eicher, Thompson, Leckman and Swain2011). Activity in the right frontal insula cortex, rolandic operculum, and subcortical regions (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus) in response to mothers listening to own infant cries has also been associated with maternal mental state talk during an interaction with their child (Hipwell et al., Reference Hipwell, Guo, Phillips, Swain and Moses-Kolko2015). While maternal sensitive behavior and use of mental state language are believed important antecedents for child development, Laurent and Ablow (Reference Laurent and Ablow2012) advanced this area of work by evidencing that mothers’ brain responses to own infant cry predicted their child’s attachment security – evidencing for the first time a link between maternal neurobiology and child developmental outcomes.

A number of other factors have been associated with maternal brain responses to infant cries. Clinically, maternal substance use has also been associated with decreased and delayed neural responses to unfamiliar infant cries (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Kim, Yip, Potenza, Mayes and Strathearn2021), whereas maternal depression has been associated with altered responding to own and unfamiliar infant cries (Bjertrup et al., Reference Bjertrup, Friis and Miskowiak2019). Poverty and maternal distress have been associated with decreased responses to infant cries, including in the medial prefrontal gyrus, middle prefrontal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus (STG; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Capistrano and Congleton2016). Building on this work, increased exposure to a variety of stressors has also been associated with decreased cry responses in the right insula/IFG and STG, activity which was also linked with maternal sensitivity (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Tribble, Olsavsky, Dufford, Erhart, Hansen, Grande and Gonzalez2020). Finally, it is worth noting that there are preliminary data to suggest that both mode of delivery (vaginal versus cesarean section; Swain et al., Reference Swain, Tasgin, Mayes, Feldman, Todd Constable and Leckman2008) and feeding (exclusive breastfeeding versus exclusive formula feeding; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Feldman, Mayes, Eicher, Thompson, Leckman and Swain2011) may also shape maternal brain responding to their own infant’s cries.

Taken together, employing infant face and cry stimuli in experimental tasks may be particularly valuable to probe reactivity and regulation in the maternal brain and how this may be associated with caregiving behavior. Notably, some studies have also linked maternal brain structure with functional brain responses to these salient infant stimuli. Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) reported that the regions evidencing GM volume reductions from pregnancy to postpartum were those regions that were activated when mothers viewed images of their infants during the postpartum MRI scan. Moreover, individual differences in perceived maternal care may shape both brain structure and function: mothers reporting higher level of maternal care in their own childhood, relative to those with lower levels of maternal care, evidenced greater GM volume and increased reactivity to infant cries in overlapping areas, including the MFG, STG, and fusiform gyrus (Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Newman, et al., Reference Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Wang and Swain2010). Therefore, it is important to consider both structural and functional brain changes during the transition to parenthood wherever possible to bridge these two methodological approaches, incorporating maternal characteristics too.

12.3 Empirical Studies of the Neurobiology of Maternal Emotion Regulation

In the research reviewed thus far, the focus has been on brain structure and functional response to infant cues in mothers. Although informative in understanding the neurobiology of parenting, as described earlier, reactivity and regulatory responding to these infant cues may be confounded and caution is warranted with interpreting these findings within an emotion regulation framework. Critically, an emerging body of research has begun to address this issue by focusing specifically on the neurobiological basis of regulatory responding to affective stimuli in maternal samples. Firk et al. (Reference Firk, Dahmen, Lehmann, Herpertz-Dahlmann and Konrad2018) investigated whether self-distraction would modulate neural responses to infant crying in primiparous mothers 5–8 months postpartum. In this context, self-distraction refers to the emotion regulation strategy of orienting attention away from an affective experience. Mothers in this sample evidenced a decreased response to infant cries in the amygdala, as well as the parahippocampal gyrus, insula, OFC, STG/MTG, precuneus, and cerebellum, when completing a counting task while infant cries were played, relative to when they were instructed only to listen to infant cries. Critically, this downregulation of the amygdala during self-distraction was associated with observed parenting behavior, such that higher levels of maternal sensitivity and nonhostility were correlated with less reactivity to infant cries during the self-distraction task. These findings evidenced for the first-time that an emotion regulation strategy can modulate the maternal brain but also that the neural correlates of maternal emotion regulation observed in this study may have downstream implications for caregiving behavior.

Two additional studies have examined cognitive reappraisal to negative affective (noninfant) stimuli in primiparous women at approximately 4 months postpartum. Grande et al. (Reference Grande, Olsavsky, Erhart, Dufford, Tribble, Phan and Kim2021) reported that mothers with higher levels of perceived stress evidenced greater activation to negative images in the DLPFC during cognitive reappraisal (as well as decreased activity in the caudate) relative to a condition where mothers were instructed to maintain their emotional response. The authors interpreted this heightened reactivity of the DLPFC to suggest that in highly stressed mothers, emotion regulation may be more effortful or inefficient, or that these mothers may be more reactive to negative emotional stimuli more generally. Interestingly, exploratory analyses in this sample showed that the heightened DLPFC activation in response to negative stimuli during the regulation (versus maintain) condition was also associated with self-reported perceived parenting-specific stress. Although exploratory, this latter finding suggests DLPFC reactivity during emotion regulation tasks may be associated with real-world experiences of parenting.

Building on this prior work, Capistrano et al. (Reference Capistrano, Grande, McRae, Phan and Kim2022) examined whether socioeconomic disadvantage (measured by income to needs ratio) would also be associated with cognitive reappraisal in recently postpartum primiparous women viewing negative affective stimuli. Consistent with their work on perceived stress, they found that mothers with greater socioeconomic disadvantage also evidenced decreased activity in prefrontal cortical regions, including in the SFG (including DLPFC), IFG (including VLPFC), precentral gyrus, MTG, as well as the caudate during the cognitive reappraisal condition. Consequently, heightened stress and socioeconomic disadvantage may shape the neural correlates of maternal emotion regulation by affecting prefrontal cortical function. It is also important to note that Capistrano et al. found lower behaviorally observed maternal sensitivity was associated with decreased responding in the precentral gyrus during cognitive reappraisal. Again, evidencing task-based regulatory responses may have implications for caregiving behavior beyond the MRI scanner.

12.4 Limitations and Future Directions

In this chapter, studies relevant to the neurobiology of parenting have been reviewed and their implications for emotion regulation considered. While informative, these studies should be considered in the context of their limitations and directions for future research. Indeed, it is important to note that the studies reviewed here focus on the maternal brain, with overlap as well as divergence reported in the few studies of the paternal brain and responding to infant cues (Feldman, Reference Feldman2015). Indeed, paternal brain changes may be driven more by the experience of caregiving following the arrival of a child (Abraham et al., Reference Abraham, Hendler, Shapira-Lichter, Kanat-Maymon, Zagoory-Sharon and Feldman2014). Nevertheless, a clear path forward requires greater consideration of paternal reactivity and regulation toward infant signals and extending this approach to all birthing and nonbirthing parents. In addition to understanding how different parenting experiences shape the brain, a critical advance in this area is recognizing the need for larger and more diverse samples of parents with respect to race and ethnicity in parental brain research (Penner et al., Reference Penner, Wall, Guan, Huang, Richardson, Dunbar, Groh and Rutherford2023).

Understanding the transition to parenthood inherently requires more longitudinal studies, ideally beginning before conception with repeat assessments during pregnancy (or an equivalent timeframe) and the postpartum period. Hoekzema et al. (Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Müller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, García-García, Soliva, Tobeña, Desco, Crone, Ballesteros, Carmona and Vilarroya2017) have evidenced the value of such a longitudinal approach but more work is needed in this area. Critically, this would be true for both structural and functional neuroimaging research. In particular, there may be value to understanding changes in the maternal brain unfolding prior to birth in pregnant people, which may prompt the identification of risk and protective factors during the transition to parenthood. Indeed the challenges related to maternal emotion regulation may unfold before birth (Penner & Rutherford, Reference Penner and Rutherford2022). For example, how maternal anxiety shapes neural processing of infant faces postpartum is comparable to how maternal anxiety is associated with processing infant faces during pregnancy (Rutherford, Byrne, et al., Reference Rutherford, Byrne, Austin, Lee, Crowley and Mayes2017).

The current chapter has focused on studies of maternal structure and function. As this work continues it will be important to incorporate our understanding of the changing levels of hormones during the transition to parenthood and their implications for the neurobiology of parenting. Oxytocin has been widely implicated in parenting behavior (Feldman & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference Feldman and Bakermans-Kranenburg2017), with peripheral levels of oxytocin increasing across the postpartum period in mothers and fathers (e.g. Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Zagoory-Sharon, Leckman and Feldman2010). A number of studies have examined how administration of oxytocin modulates neural responses to infant cues in parents and nonparents (e.g. Peltola et al., Reference Peltola, Strathearn and Puura2018; Riem et al., Reference Riem, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Pieper, Tops, Boksem, Vermeiren, van Ijzendoorn and Rombouts2011; Rutherford, Guo, et al., Reference Rutherford, Byrne, Austin, Lee, Crowley and Mayes2017) as well as how variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with neural responses to infant stimuli (Peltola et al., Reference Peltola, Yrttiaho, Puura, Proverbio, Mononen, Lehtimäki and Leppänen2014). Of course, oxytocin is not the only hormone that may shape maternal brain responding during the transition to parenthood (Brunton & Russell, Reference Brunton and Russell2008), requiring further extension of this approach to other hormones, including estrogen and progesterone.

Although the literature regarding the neural correlates of maternal emotion regulation is in its own infancy, the initial fMRI studies described here are promising in evidencing that explicit emotion regulation strategies modulate reactivity of the maternal brain and that this reactivity is associated with different aspects of caregiving. As this work continues, it will be important to consider the nature of the affective stimuli employed during emotion regulation tasks (i.e. infant versus noninfant stimuli), as well as the generalizability of these tasks to parenting experiences outside of the experimental setting. It may be valuable to include self-report assessments of how parents use different emotion regulation strategies (e.g. Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003), as well as adapting such measures to parenting-specific contexts. Understanding how regulatory functioning changes throughout the perinatal period would also be valuable, in particular in identifying periods of heightened risk and vulnerability for parents. This would allow a unique opportunity for parental brain researchers to partner with clinicians to both refine therapeutic approaches related to parental emotion regulation and to optimize the timing of these interventions for parents.

12.5 Conclusion

In the current chapter, structural and functional neuroimaging data has been presented that supports the notion that the transition to parenthood may be accompanied by neural reorganization, which may have important implications for caregiving. Meta-analytic work highlights that heightened reactivity to infant cues is particularly evident when parents view photographs of their own infant as well as when infants are expressing distress. Critically, increasing studies are evidencing important associations between maternal brain structure and function and different aspects of parenting to ensure the functional significance of this work is clear. An exciting development in this field are those studies specifically targeting maternal emotion regulation, moving beyond the combined reactivity and regulatory approach typically used. While there has been a strong foundation for studies of the neurobiological basis of the transition to parenthood, advancements are needed in relation to the extension of this work to birthing and nonbirthing parents, incorporating more longitudinal designs, and understanding the role of changing hormonal profiles to neural reorganization. Given only a paucity of research has been conducted to date in the neural correlates of maternal emotion regulation, there is significant space for the growth and development of this work, including partnerships with clinicians supporting parents during this transitional period.

Chapter 13 Emotional Labor in Parenting

Gao-Xian Lin and Dorota Szczygieł

In her groundbreaking book, The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feelings, Arlie Russell Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) described the phenomenon of an organization delineating a set of emotion display rules and pressuring employees to hide their identity in order to comply with them. As Goffman (Reference Goffman1959) had discussed earlier, people play their social roles just like in a drama; and employees, Hochschild noted, follow emotional display rules, showing “appropriate” emotions and refraining from expressing “inappropriate” emotions to control the impression they make on others, maintain relationships, and achieve personal goals (Thoits, Reference Thoits, Manstead, Frijda and Fischer2004; von Scheve, Reference von Scheve2012; Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993). Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) suggested that a similar phenomenon occurs in modern parenting, given the recent culture change in parenting (see Hays, Reference Hays1996): society defines a set of rules for emotion management in parenting and pressures parents to hide their identity in order to conform to these rules and expectations. In this chapter, our goal is to review the literature that supports Lin and colleagues’ study on emotional labor in parenting.

13.1 Modern Parenting
13.1.1 From Common Sense to Science

Parenting has undergone a major shift in recent decades. This can be traced back to the late twentieth century, when enormous economic and political changes occurred that were directly or indirectly caused by the two world wars (Overy, Reference Overy2009): the triumph of capitalism (Habib, Reference Habib1995), the generalization of the democratic spirit (Kettenacker & Riotte, Reference Kettenacker and Riotte2011), United Nations General Assembly, 1948), and the emergence of globalization (Barkawi, Reference Barkawi2006). These factors have intersected, eliminating authority and prompting people to doubt their own traditions (Giddens, Reference Giddens1999; Hobsbawm, Reference Hobsbawm1995). In parallel, science has acquired a higher status and it is to science (instead of traditions) that many people now turn to know how they should run their lives (Giddens, Reference Giddens, Beck, Giddens and Lash1994), including parenting. Science can check whether the evidence supports traditional behavior, for example, the “common sense” practices that parents used to feel obliged to follow in parenting. Based on its findings, science can suggest that a specific traditional practice be abolished or preserved. Alternatively, science can advocate innovative parenting strategies. In any case, recent decades have witnessed science playing a more critical role in instructing parents on child-rearing (Furedi, Reference Furedi2002; Pursell, Reference Pursell2007).

Science relating to the parenting domain has flourished for decades. Since the mid-nineteenth century, scientists have come to view children not only as unique and important, but also as fragile beings who require extra protective effort (Hoghughi, Reference Hoghughi, Hoghughi and Long2004). This ideology became more widespread after the Second World War. Many scholars, such as John Bowlby and Donald Winnicott, began to emphasize the importance of high-quality child-rearing and a sound mother–child relationship, contending that they create a supportive and warm family and thus ensure the harmonious development of children (Hendrick, Reference Hendrick2016). Such an emphasis – along with empirical research on the effects of parenting on children – has attributed to parenting a key, if not a decisive role in children’s development (Bornstein, Reference Bornstein, Bornstein and Leventhal2015). This ideology has been intensified to the point where some even believe that parents’ actions and choices with regard to their children irreversibly influence children’s development (for a critical discussion, see Furedi, Reference Furedi2002). Belief in the importance of parenting in child development has thus reached unprecedented levels and continues to grow (Lee, Reference Lee, Lee, Bristow, Faircloth and Macvarish2014b).

13.1.2 From Science to Prescribed Rules in Parenting

Given the importance of parenting, many people believe that parents should implement “correct parenting,” namely the practices approved by science and society (for related discussion; see Furedi, Reference Furedi2002; Lee, Reference Lee, Lee, Bristow, Faircloth and Macvarish2014a). Parenting has gradually become prescribed. The prescriptions concern what parents should do (e.g. provide their children with an emotionally secure environment, give them five helpings of fruit and vegetables a day); and what they should not do (e.g. use corporal or disproportionate punishment, put their very young children in front of screens). Along with these prescriptions there are expectations in terms of results: for instance, children should be physically healthy, emotionally secure, etc. If parents fail to meet some of these expectations, the authorities may punish them and remove their children from their custody. Some parents are terrified that they may be assessed as failing in their role (e.g. being judged as neglectful parents) and that they could receive a dire sanction: loss of custody of the child (Nomaguchi & Milkie, Reference Nomaguchi and Milkie2020, p. 200). The fulfilment of the parental role is now more or less controlled by implicit or explicit prescriptions.

13.1.3 Emotional Display Rules in Parenting

Among these prescriptions, particular attention is paid to parental emotions. This is not surprising if we consider the current scientific evidence concerning parental emotions. First, parenting occurs in a social context conducive to spontaneous emotional feelings. For example, research has shown that mothers report a greater variety of discrete emotions when they care for their children than when they do not (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Rasmussen, Buttitta, Smiley and Borelli2021). These emotions may be self-oriented, such as anger at themselves for not giving the child enough time and attention, or child oriented, such as anger at the child for not keeping their room tidy (Dix et al., Reference Dix, Gershoff, Meunier and Miller2004). Often, parenting activities evoke more negative emotions than other activities (Kahneman et al., Reference Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz and Stone2004), causing parents to experience more worry, stress, and anger than non-parents (Deaton & Stone, Reference Deaton and Stone2014). Parents’ negative emotions undermine parenting strategies, making them less supportive/positive and harsher (for a meta-analysis, see Rueger et al., Reference Rueger, Katz, Risser and Lovejoy2011), predicting poorer adjustment of children later on (e.g. more school aggression; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Schwartz, Dodge and McBride-Chang2003).

Second, how parents express and regulate emotions plays a crucial role in children’s social emotional development (Duncombe et al., Reference Duncombe, Havighurst, Holland and Frankling2012). On the one hand, how parents handle their own emotions acts as a model that provides children with important information about how to appropriately recognize, express, and regulate emotions in specific situations, which in turn socializes children’s emotion regulation (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017). Research has shown that parents’ capacity for adaptive emotion regulation is associated with better emotion regulation and social adjustment in children (for a meta-analysis, see Zimmer-Gembeck et al., Reference Zimmer-Gembeck, Rudolph, Kerin and Bohadana-Brown2022). On the other hand, how parents respond to their children’s emotions is also crucial. Research has demonstrated that a warm and supportive parental response to children’s emotions facilitates children’s acquisition of emotional knowledge and social competence, whereas harsh and unsupportive responses are detrimental to children’s social-emotional development (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes and Murphy1996, Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Guthrie, Murphy and Reiser1999; Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020; see also Chapter 10).

This scientific evidence converges and shapes emotional display rules that revolve around parental emotions. Parents are now expected to feel and express the “right” emotions, with the “right” intensity and in the “right” situations. To this end, parents are increasingly encouraged to manage their emotion expression during their interactions with their children. They should refrain from expressing too many negative emotions, such as fear (which can make their relationship with their children insecure; e.g. Manassis et al., Reference Manassis, Bradley, Goldberg, Hood and Swinson1994) or anger; and they should also express more positive emotions such as warmth and affection, to sustain their children’s emotional safety (e.g. Bai et al., Reference Bai, Repetti and Sperling2016). Such prescriptions have spread and prevailed to the point where they now constitute a crucial aspect of parenting culture. A survey in 37 countries of more than 10,000 parents showed that Western parents believe that to be an ideal parent, they should show positive emotions and control their negative emotions (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023).

Based on these observations, one may want to ask the following questions. What are the possible results of regulating parental emotion? Should parents simply follow these prescriptions and carry them out? Is compliance with them costly? Aiming to answer these questions, Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) came up with a pioneering adaptation to the parenting context of the emotional labor framework initially developed in the context of work (e.g. Grandey et al., Reference Grandey, Diefendorffand and Rupp2013). They borrowed this framework in order to describe the impact of display rules governing parents’ emotions and to summarize the consequences of complying with those rules. Before describing their adaptation, we first introduce the emotional labor framework in the work context below.

13.2 Emotional Labor Framework
13.2.1 The Origin of the Concept of Emotional Labor

The emotional labor concept dates from the 1980s, when Arlie Russell Hochschild, an American sociologist, published her foundational book, The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feelings (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). Hochschild described in detail the job requirements of Delta Airlines flight attendants. What drew her attention was that to ensure that passengers felt comfortable, the flight attendants were expected to express appropriate emotions regardless of how they felt and how the passengers behaved. They were expected to possess traits such as emotional stability, interpersonal warmth, concern for others, and a collective orientation. These qualities allowed them to employ what Hochschild termed “emotional management,” that is, cognitive, behavioral, and expressive strategies that enable them to align their emotional experiences and expressions with the feelings and rules of expression required by the organization. Based on this observation, Hochschild concluded that certain jobs are not only defined by the physical or cognitive work performed but also involve emotional management. Hochschild termed such emotional management – regulating and expressing the “right” emotions to satisfy the emotional requirements of the job in exchange for a wage – as “emotional labor.”

13.2.2 The Definition of Emotional Labor

Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983, p. 7) described emotional labor as “the management of feelings to create a publicly observable facial and bodily manifestation.” Three main characteristics of jobs involving emotional labor were also enumerated: (1) they involve a high level of direct contact with customers (“voice to voice” or “face to face”) in terms of duration, frequency, or intensity of interaction (Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1996); (2) they require the use of specific emotional displays (either explicitly or implicitly specified) to elicit the desired affective responses from customers; and (3) the organization directly or indirectly controls employees’ emotional displays.

13.2.3 The Emotional Labor Framework in the Work Context

Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) pioneering work has inspired a great deal of research and theoretical work, resulting in several detailed emotional labor models (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1997; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Tardino, Daus, Munz, Hartel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Totterdell & Holman, Reference Totterdell and Holman2003). Despite the differences in these theoretical models, researchers have concurred that emotional labor is a form of emotional management requiring effort from employees and should be positioned within a broader integrative framework. This framework includes (1) emotional demands of work (e.g. Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1996) as an antecedent to emotional dissonance experienced when perceived emotions do not match the demands of the job (e.g. Abraham, Reference Abraham1999; Zerbe, Reference Zerbe2000), causing (2) the employee to make an effort to engage in emotion regulation through deep and surface acting (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Scott & Barnes, Reference Scott and Barnes2011),Footnote 1 and to produce the desired emotional displays, ultimately leading to (3) consequences for the employee (see Figure 13.1).

Figure 13.1 Emotional labor framework in the job context

13.2.3.1 Emotional Demands of Work: Emotional Display Rules

The cornerstone of all emotional labor models is the view that organizations set emotional display rules that specify which emotions are appropriate and how and when they should be expressed during interactions with customers (Ashforth & Humphrey, Reference Ashforth and Humphrey1993; Cropanzano et al., Reference Cropanzano, Weiss, Elias, Perrewe and Ganster2003; Diefendorff et al., Reference Diefendorff, Richard and Croyle2006; Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Rafaeli & Sutton, Reference Rafaeli and Sutton1990). Emotional display rules are imposed on employees as early as the recruitment process and are later reinforced by activities such as training, performance appraisal, and supervision (Kruml & Geddes, Reference Kruml and Geddes2000; Sutton & Rafaeli, Reference Sutton and Rafaeli1988). These rules have both a positive aspect, requiring employees to show certain emotions (e.g. “Put a smile on your face!” or “Show interest and excitement!”) and a negative aspect, prohibiting employees from showing certain emotions (e.g. “Don’t raise your voice!” or “Don’t show boredom!”).

Such emotional display rules – requiring employees to conceal negative emotions and express positive emotions – is especially common in customer service work (Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993). The organization sets out these rules based on the assumption that employee behavior influences customer satisfaction and interest in the products or services offered (Grandey et al., Reference Grandey, Fisk, Mattila, Jansen and Sideman2005), and studies have in fact shown this to be true. When employees directly follow the rules and perform the emotional behavior expected by their customers such as greeting, thanking, speaking in a rhythmic voice, smiling, and maintaining eye contact (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003; Tsai, Reference Tsai2001), customers are in a better mood (Luong, Reference Luong2005), buy more, rate the service better, and are more loyal to the organization (Korczynski, Reference Korczynski2005; Pugh, Reference Pugh2001; Tsai, Reference Tsai2001).

13.2.3.2 Emotional Labor and Its Implication for Employee Well-Being

However, what happens when the emotions employees experience are not the same as those they are required to display? Researchers argue that incongruence between the emotions experienced and the expressions expected by the organization induces a negatively affect-laden state of emotional dissonance in employees (Holman et al., Reference Holman, Martinez-Iñigo, Totterdell, Ashkanasy and Cooper2008; Zapf & Holz, Reference Zapf and Holz2006). Jansz and Timmers (Reference Jansz and Timmers2002) point out that the concept of emotional dissonance underlines both the negative nature of this state and its motivational implications. When they experience a feeling of dissonance, employees are motivated to take action to reduce this feeling, which has implications for regulating the emotional process. Undertaking a regulatory effort is vital because emotional dissonance, if prolonged, threatens employees’ mental and physical well-being (Dijk & Brown, Reference Dijk and Brown2006; Hartel et al., Reference Hartel, Hsu, Boyle, Ashkanasy, Zerbe and Hartel2002; Schaubroeck & Jones, Reference Schaubroeck and Jones2000). However, the impact of this regulatory effort may depend on how it is made.

Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) followed Goffman’s (Reference Goffman1959) metaphor of different ways of acting in a drama, describing two ways in which employees work to regulate their emotions, that is, two emotional labor strategies: surface acting and deep acting. Surface acting refers to modifying emotional expressions without changing internal feelings. It involves suppressing the display of felt emotions and faking the emotion required by the organization. Deep acting, on the other hand, refers to modifying actual feelings so that they are consistent with the employer’s emotional display rules. It entails an effort to change the felt emotion in order to elicit the appropriate emotional display. As discussed later, the distinction between surface and depth acting is critical in demonstrating and explaining the varying impact of emotional labor on employees’ well-being (see also Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Gabriel, Reference Grandey and Gabriel2015; Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Scott & Barnes, Reference Scott and Barnes2011).

Following Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) description of emotional labor strategies, studies have accumulated evidence that the two forms differ in their effects on employees’ well-being. Most studies have found that surface acting reliably and consistently predicts unfavorable consequences such as worse job performance, less job satisfaction, more psychological stress, more psychosomatic complaints, and more burnout symptoms (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Grandey & Gabriel, Reference Grandey and Gabriel2015; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Huppertz et al., Reference Huppertz, Ute, Velozo and Schreurs2020; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013); however, the findings regarding the effect of deep acting are not as consistent across studies (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Sayre, Reference Grandey and Sayre2019). Although some studies have shown a damaging effect of deep acting (although it is still thought to be less harmful than surface acting; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Menil and Luminet2007), most studies have found that deep acting is neither beneficial nor detrimental (e.g. Brotheridge & Lee, Reference Brotheridge and Lee2002; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011).

The concept of “regulatory effort “ further explains the different consequences of surface and deep acting (Huppertz et al., Reference Huppertz, Ute, Velozo and Schreurs2020). Although surface and deep acting are both effortful, the amount of regulatory effort they require differs. In particular, if individuals rely more on surface acting to meet the organization’s rules, their effort and cognitive resources are more strained due to the need to monitor emotional expression constantly. The constant depletion of resources eventually causes tension and strain; if it becomes chronic, it can result in burnout (Brotheridge & Grandey, Reference Brotheridge and Grandey2002). Deep acting also requires cognitive resources to manage emotions, but these efforts are only needed at the beginning; this explains the weaker predictive relationship of deep acting with poor adaptation (see Grandey & Sayre, Reference Grandey and Sayre2019).

13.3 Extending the Emotional Labor Framework to Parenting
13.3.1 Rationale

Does emotional labor also occur in parenting? Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) had already foreseen this possibility in her seminal book. She suggested that emotional labor may occur in the professional context and in private life, such as in the family. Hochschild pointed out that of all relationships in the family, emotional labor may be most pronounced in the parent–child relationship, given the strong bond between parents and their children (therefore, more contacts). However, for decades the idea remained at the stage of Hochschild’s early reflections and some pioneering work (see Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993, Reference Wharton and Erickson1995 for how emotional labor may be performed within the family). Research on emotional labor has so far mainly focused on the professional sphere, and the assumption seems fairly widespread that emotional labor is only performed in return for salary, bonuses, or rewards (Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013; von Scheve, Reference von Scheve2012). However, with recent developments in parenting, this picture is changing (see Section 13.1, Modern Parenting); emotional labor in parenting may have been a long-standing phenomenon that has been ignored because it is a taboo subject. In a pioneering study, however, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) extended the emotional labor framework to the parenting context (see Figure 13.2).

Figure 13.2 Emotional labor framework in the parenting context

Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) point out that features of contemporary parenting present characteristics of emotional labor. First, the parent–child relationship is one of the closest social relationships (it includes “voice-to-voice,” “face-to-face,” and even “body-to-body” interaction; Bornstein, Reference Bornstein, Bornstein and Leventhal2015). Second, as also briefly summarized in Section 13.1, Modern Parenting, modern parents are now increasingly expected to regulate their emotions during interaction with children. This phenomenon is particularly evident in Western countries, where mothers and fathers are supposed to show positive emotions (e.g. being loving) and maintain patience to be considered ideal parents (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023). Third, such emotional display rules have been explicitly expressed in government policies such as the Council of Europe’s policy on positive parenting (Rodrigo, Reference Rodrigo2010) or the positive parenting tips recommended by the National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities (2020), etc. The implementation of these rules is therefore institutionally monitored and controlled. As we have seen, these characteristics are consistent with Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) observation on the characteristics/determinants of emotional labor in the work context, confirming that the concept is eligible to be applied in parenting.

Given such eligibility, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) proposed their framework model of “emotional labor in parenting.” As they describe in this framework, society, like an employer, sets explicit rules on emotional expression. And just as employees need to follow emotional display rules to meet organizational expectations and to be recognized as good employees, parents need to follow emotional display rules to meet societal and institutional expectations and to be recognized as parents who are at least good enough (and preferably ideal). As in the work context, these efforts may lead to an immediate beneficial outcome, that is, children may feel positive just as customers do when they observe positive emotional expressions from employees; yet these efforts may eventually jeopardize parental well-being, for example by leading to parental burnout. Together with other studies, the research by Lin and colleagues provides support for their proposal, as described next.

13.3.1.1 Emotional Demands of Parenting: Emotional Display Rules

To begin with, as we saw in Section 13.1, Modern Parenting, social institutions such as governments (e.g. Rodrigo, Reference Rodrigo2010) and media (see Douglas & Michaels, Reference Douglas and Michaels2004) have presented parents with a specific set of emotional display rules. As we might expect, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) have shown that some parents internalize these rules to the extent that, as shown in a later study, parents think they need to show positive emotions and control their negative emotions in order to be perceived as an ideal parent (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023). It is true that parents may make efforts to align their expressed emotions with the rules, and that this may indeed result in better parenting strategies (Minnotte et al., Reference Minnotte, Pedersen, Mannon and Kiger2010). Such strategies may eventually benefit children in terms of improving their emotional state (Olszanowski et al., Reference Olszanowski, Wróbel and Hess2020) and subsequent social functioning (e.g. Chen et al., Reference Chen, Haines, Charlton and VanderWeele2019). However, this is only one side of the coin. As discussed next, there is disturbing evidence that parents’ regulatory efforts can also be detrimental to their children’s well-being.

13.3.1.2 Emotional Labor and Its Implication for Parents’ Well-Being

Parental regulatory efforts may be costly for parents, just as emotional labor is costly for employees. Le and Impett’s (Reference Le and Impett2016) pioneering daily diary study found evidence that parents’ efforts to control emotional expression can have a negative impact on their emotional well-being and the parent–child relationship (see also Karnilowicz et al., Reference Karnilowicz, Waters and Mendes2019; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Karnilowicz, West and Mendes2020). Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) went a step further by demonstrating that such efforts (to satisfy emotional display rules) may be so demanding that they put parents at risk of parental burnout, although this depends on how parents make their efforts. Parents are at greater risk of burnout when they prefer surface acting (or expressive suppression; see Lin & Szczygieł, Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) but are at lower risk when they prefer deep acting (or cognitive reappraisal; Lin et al., Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022). Either way, research has shown that when parental burnout occurs, some parents may become neglectful or violent toward their children (Brianda et al., Reference Brianda, Roskam, Gross, Franssen, Kapala, Gérard and Mikolajczak2020; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Brianda, Avalosse and Roskam2018, Reference Mikolajczak, Gross and Roskam2019), and that this maltreatment may eventually compromise children’s development (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016). It is, therefore, plausible that well-intentioned parental regulatory efforts may have a paradoxical, undesirable effect and work against the child.

13.4 Future Research Directions

As discussed in this chapter, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021), together with other pioneering researchers, provided convincing evidence to support their proposal to adapt the emotional labor framework identified in the work context (see Figure 13.1) to the parenting context (see Figure 13.2). Parents perceive the existence of emotional display rules and put effort into aligning their emotions with those rules through different emotional labor strategies, which ultimately have different consequences (such as different vulnerability to parental burnout). The strengths of this framework are that it provides a backbone connecting society and individuals. It specifically delineates the interplay between society’s rules for emotional expression and parents’ regulatory efforts, emotional responses, and well-being. For this framework to be useful, it should not only contribute to theoretical understanding but also provide direction for future studies. As we will see next, this backbone framework has the potential to open many future research directions in the study of parenting, just as it has contributed to organizational literature.

13.4.1 Within the Emotional Labor Framework

Most obviously, parenting researchers can examine the components of this backbone framework in detail. First, Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983), in her conceptualization of emotional labor, emphasized the importance of culture in shaping emotional display rules. Following this reasoning, we can expect that rules on parental emotional display, such as which emotions can be expressed and which should be hidden, may differ across cultures (see Matsumoto & Hwang, Reference Matsumoto and Hwang2012). Cultural differences in emotional display rules may further influence how parents follow the rules and thus their effect on their well-being; however, future research is needed to verify and investigate this. Second, in this chapter we have followed Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) original approach, focusing on only two emotional labor strategies. In real-world parenting, parents use a broader range of emotion regulation strategies to comply with the emotional display rules (see Part II of this book), and these affect their well-being differently. In fact, researchers have already suggested that deep and surface acting are not the only ways to tune emotions to emotional display rules (see Diefendorff et al., Reference Diefendorff, Richard and Yang2008; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Tran, Brotheridge, Gross, Charmine, Neal and Wilfred2009). Pursuing the investigation of the impact of parents’ emotion regulation in their parental role and its impact on their well-being through a wider range of emotion regulation strategies will prove fruitful.

13.4.2 Beyond the Emotional Labor Framework

Researchers may also find it promising to include exogenous moderators in this backbone framework. First, there may be factors influencing parents’ propensity to choose specific emotional labor strategies, resulting in different consequences for parental well-being. In organizational literature, service employees’ personality traits have been shown to be a key variable affecting the type of emotional labor they perform (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Saklofske and Egan2005). For example, research findings indicate that individuals with high negative affectivity are more likely to use surface than deep acting (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013; Mesmer-Magnus et al., Reference Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch and Wax2012). This may be because, for people who have a dispositional tendency to experience negative emotions and are therefore inclined to process information in a way that directs them towards negative affective states (Larsen & Ketelaar, Reference Larsen and Ketelaar1991), using deep acting to evoke positive emotions can be a real challenge. Thus, feigning positive emotions becomes the only way to meet their role expectations. On this basis, we can expect that negative affectivity may moderate parents’ adoption of emotional labor strategies. Lin and Szczygieł (Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) provided preliminary evidence by showing that parents who place more emphasis on their mistakes in parenting have a higher propensity for expressive suppression.

In addition to employees’ personality traits, research on emotional labor also points to the importance of the circumstances – customer behavior such as mistreatment – in predicting the use of emotional labor strategies. These studies have found that employees who experience rudeness and mistreatment from customers find it easier and more convenient to use surface acting than deep acting and are therefore more likely to do so (Adams & Webster, Reference Adams and Webster2013; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; Szczygieł & Bazińska, Reference Szczygieł and Bazińska2021). In view of such evidence, we predict that in the parenting context, children’s challenging behaviors (like those of customers in the organizational context) may act as a crucial factor predisposing parents to use surface acting more often than deep acting and thus put them at risk of ill-being. In a recently published study, Zhang et al. (Reference Zhang, Gatzke-Kopp, Cole and Ram2023) demonstrated that mothers whose children exhibited more challenging behaviors experienced more negative emotions, which was associated with using multiple emotion regulation strategies. Although children’s challenging behavior was not directly significantly related to mothers’ emotion regulation strategies, the associations found between it and suppression and cognitive reappraisal support our reasoning here. Specifically, children’s challenging behavior was positively, albeit insignificantly, related to mothers’ expressive suppression and negatively, albeit insignificantly, related to cognitive reappraisal.

Finally, moderating factors may mitigate or exacerbate emotional labor’s consequences for parents’ well-being. When employees have no choice but to use emotional labor, their personality traits can mitigate the potential harm it causes. As a personality trait, emotional intelligence seems to be a promising moderator of this kind. Emotional intelligence (also known as emotional competence) refers to individuals’ ability to identify, express, understand, regulate and use their own and others’ emotions (Mayer & Salovey, Reference Mayer, Salovey, Salovey and Sluyter1997). Mikolajczak et al. (Reference Mikolajczak, Menil and Luminet2007) showed that when employees have higher emotional intelligence, their regulatory efforts to perform emotional labor are reduced, which in turn predicts a lower risk of job burnout. Based on this line of research, it can be expected that parents’ high emotional intelligence can reduce the regulatory efforts inherent to emotional labor and, consequently, its negative impact on their well-being. Lin and Szczygieł (Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) provided initial evidence for this. They showed that parents’ emotional intelligence moderates the effects of expressive suppression on parental burnout such that the effects are reduced (although they remain significant) when parents have higher emotional intelligence.

To sum up, after our introduction to the emotional labor framework in the work context, in this section we have summarized the reasons for extending the emotional labor framework to the parenting context, described current evidence in favor of this proposal, and finally suggested a few fascinating areas that future studies may find it fruitful to explore further. Taken together, this demonstrates what a promising approach this is.

13.5 Conclusion

The goal of this chapter was to introduce Lin and colleagues’ (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) attempt to adapt emotional labor, originally intended for work, to the parenting context. As they themselves also noted, equating parenting with labor activity is controversial, as parenting does not meet the fundamental characteristic of “labor”: being financially rewarded. With this in mind, Lin and colleagues (p. 2703) pointed out that their import of emotional labor to the parenting context “does not amount to reducing parenting to a job”; rather, it should be seen as a metaphor to describe the situation faced by today’s parents, who are expected to adhere to rules regarding emotions when raising their children.

As we have seen in this chapter, Lin and colleagues’ (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) emotional labor framework offers a compelling theoretical lens to explain the mechanism by which desirable goals – to show more positive and demonstrate fewer negative emotions while interacting with children – can be so demanding in terms of emotional labor that they lead to poorer parent–child relationships or even severe costs such as burnout. The emotional labor framework emerged from the need to understand the service industry’s new labor form (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). It has subsequently generated hundreds of studies about the cost of emotion management in service jobs. Although a handful of studies exploring emotional labor in a family context have already emerged (e.g. Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993), there has been little investigation of parents’ regulatory efforts (the main exceptions being Le & Impett, Reference Le and Impett2016 and Lin et al., Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021). Yet these regulatory efforts probably resemble the emotional labor concept very closely in that modern parenting culture places external pressure on parents by prescribing emotional display rules (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021). In this sense, this framework contributes to a better understanding of the emotional experience of modern parents.

Chapter 14 Enhancing Parental Emotion Regulation Skills

Sophie Havighurst and Christiane Kehoe

Managing one’s own emotional reactions when parenting can be one of the most challenging aspects of being a parent. Many parents are depleted, overwhelmed, and triggered by their children’s emotions and may have limited patience and capacity to regulate their reactions in these interactions. The way parents regulate their own emotions influences the emotional climate of the home and determines whether (or not) they will be able to meet their child’s emotional needs in a supportive way. Evidence shows that when parents are not able to regulate their own emotions, it can hinder healthy emotional development in their children (Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020).

Assisting parents to understand and manage their own emotions has been a central focus of our Tuning in to Kids® (TIK) parenting programs. TIK recognizes that children’s healthy emotional development is partly shaped by how parents manage and express their own emotions (i.e. role modeling, impact on the family emotional climate), how parents react to children’s emotions (validation/invalidation, approval/disapproval and the messages that such reactions provide about emotion expression), and whether they discuss and teach their children about emotions (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). When parents engage in practices of self-care that reduce their own stress and can use emotion regulation (ER) strategies to help remain calm when faced with their children’s emotions, they can more accurately identify children’s emotions and respond in ways that validate the child’s emotional experience. This in turn has been linked to skills in children’s emotion knowledge and regulation. Our studies (e.g. Havighurst et al., Reference Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Prior and Kehoe2010, Reference Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, Kehoe, Efron and Prior2013, Reference Havighurst, Duncombe, Frankling, Holland, Kehoe and Stargatt2015; Kehoe et al., Reference Kehoe, Havighurst and Harley2020) show that the program improves parent emotion socialization, parent ER and children’s emotional and behavioral functioning in families from community and clinical populations with children ranging in age from toddlers through to teens (for reviews, see Havighurst & Kehoe, Reference Havighurst, Kehoe, Deater-Deckard and Panneton2017; Havighurst et al., Reference Havighurst, Radovini, Hao and Kehoe2020).

In this chapter we begin with relevant definitions and theoretical explanations of ER and briefly describe how adult therapeutic interventions generally target it. We then review parenting interventions and how these enhance parent ER. We unpack different ER strategies and examine how these work, with a focus on comparing “top-down ER” strategies (i.e. cognitive strategies that require a parent to be calmer) with bottom-up strategies (i.e. using the body or senses to regulate emotions that may be more effective when a parent is emotionally activated and has less access to executive functions). We also highlight the importance of self-care in facilitating “proactive ER” and healthy processing of emotions. Following this, we outline how our TIK parenting programs target parent ER.

14.1 Top-Down versus Bottom-Up Emotion Generation and Impact on Emotion Regulation

A variety of interventions exist that promote ER and are usually underpinned by theoretical models about how emotions and ER are conceptualized. We use a definition of emotions as “episodic, relatively short-term, biologically based patterns of perception, experience, physiology, action, and communication that occur in response to specific physical and social challenges and opportunities” (Keltner & Gross, Reference Keltner and Gross1999, p. 467). We distinguish emotions from “mood,” which comprises more prolonged emotional states. We align with a functionalist perspective in which emotions and behaviors are seen to serve a function to promote survival, including social affiliative purposes (e.g. Campos et al., Reference Campos, Mumme, Kermoian and Campos1994; Frijda, Reference Frijda1986).

When emotions occur, they involve changes in body state (e.g. facial expression, posture, muscle tension, heart rate, respiration, etc) and changes in cognitive functioning (e.g. motivation, attention, memory, perception, decision making; Kahneman, Reference Kahneman2011; Smith & Lane, Reference Smith and Lane2015, Reference Smith and Lane2016). Emotions can be elicited in a way that is fast, automatic, and experienced in a very physical way engaging “bottom-up” processes in the brain, like those ranging from unexpected encounters with threat or pain (e.g. seeing a child run toward the road or being kicked by one’s toddler; Kahnemann, Reference Kahneman2011; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux and Aggleton1992). Bottom-up generated emotions are important as they help us to respond quickly and accurately to emotion-relevant aspects of our environment. Top-down elicited emotions can be slower, occurring as a result of cognitive appraisal in a social interaction (e.g. children’s whining can elicit irritation when it is seen as attention seeking or it can elicit curiosity or care when it is seen as connection or help seeking). Whether emotions are elicited via bottom-up or top-down processes has important implications for parenting as well as for determining effective interventions that will appropriately assist parents to regulate their emotions when parenting.

The most common definition of ER is “processes individuals use to influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express emotions” (Gross, Reference Gross1998). ER can “involve generating, maintaining, increasing, or decreasing either positive or negative emotions” (p. 275). Gross suggests a “situation-attention-appraisal-response” sequence of emotion experience, whereby emotions may be generated and regulated at various stages along a timeline of unfolding emotional responding. The model distinguishes between antecedent-focused ER strategies (which include reappraisal, acceptance, problem-solving) and response-focused ER strategies (which include suppression, distraction, rumination, avoidance) (Gross, Reference Gross1998). With the exception of distraction and avoidance, these ER strategies involve cognition, and can therefore be viewed as top-down ER strategies (Kehoe & Havighurst, Reference Kehoe, Havighurst, Beauchaine and Crowell2018). Among these strategies, each can be adaptive or maladaptive depending on the context (English et al., Reference English, Lee, John and Gross2017), the goals people have (e.g. to self-regulate, help others, get on with work, keep up appearances) and who is present (e.g. friend or a stranger; English et al., Reference English, Lee, John and Gross2017).

Typically, parent ER requires a combination of bottom-up processing of encounters with emotional experience (e.g. child pushes their sibling or shouts at a parent) along with top-down conceptual knowledge, memories and linguistic representations that guide regulation and behavior (see also Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020). In any given moment, emotional encounters can be characterized by relatively stronger bottom-up (reactive) or top-down (considered) emotions, which influence both the speed and the intensity of the emotion. This in turn, influences the type of ER strategies (either top-down or bottom-up) that may successfully help parents in such emotional moments. Often, especially at high emotion intensity, ER is automatic and habitual, which means that more practiced strategies (adaptive or maladaptive) are more likely to be used (e.g. Campbell-Sills & Barlow, Reference Campbell-Sills, Barlow and Gross2007; Porges, Reference Porges, Fosha, Siegel and Solomon2009; Thompson, Reference Thompson and Thompson1990; Wylie et al., Reference Wylie, Colasante, De France, Lin and Hollenstein2022). For emotions that were generated in a faster, more bottom-up way, strategies that require breathing or sensory regulation may be more effective than cognitive strategies at lowering arousal (Sheppes & Meiran, Reference Sheppes and Meiran2007; Sheppes et al., Reference Sheppes, Scheibe, Suri and Gross2011; Wylie et al., Reference Wylie, Colasante, De France, Lin and Hollenstein2022). We discuss these strategies in more detail later in this chapter.

14.2 Emotion Regulation and Parenting: Under- and Overregulation Problems

Parenting evokes a variety of emotions, which may be regulated (e.g. healthy emotion expression or masked/unobservable affect) or dysregulated (e.g. excessive emotion expression), and affect parenting behavior (Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020). Additionally, although parental positive affect is associated with sensitive, supportive parenting (Rueger et al., Reference Rueger, Katz, Risser and Lovejoy2011), parental anger places parents at greater risk of harsh, hostile, or reactive responses to children’s emotions (Ateah & Durrant, Reference Ateah and Durrant2005; Leung & Slep, Reference Leung and Slep2006); parents’ anxiety and worry has been found to be related to more controlling, overprotective, and restrictive parenting (Dix et al., Reference Dix, Gershoff, Meunier and Miller2004; Kaitz & Maytal, Reference Kaitz and Maytal2005); and parental sadness, especially when experienced for prolonged periods of time, can result in parents being more self-focused and detached from children’s goals and needs (Dix et al., Reference Dix, Gershoff, Meunier and Miller2004).

When a parent experiences high intensity emotions such as anger, anxiety, fear, or sadness, these emotional states can be consuming and overwhelming. Although some of these emotions can be adaptive in times of danger or adversity so that parents can act quickly to keep themselves or their children safe, for many these emotions are strongly activated even in more minor situations (e.g. toddler throwing food; child not doing as asked). In part, the fast bottom-up emotion generation may occur due to appraisal of a situation (e.g. “toddler is throwing food to annoy me and won’t listen” versus “throwing food is something most toddlers will do and s/he is still learning”), highlighting the adaptive function that antecedent ER strategies, such as reappraisal hold for parenting (Kehoe & Havighurst, Reference Kehoe, Havighurst, Beauchaine and Crowell2018). When emotions are very intense, however, the resulting physiological aspects of emotion dysregulation can lead to a sense of being flooded with emotions and make it difficult to process information and inhibit maladaptive behaviors and actions (Farb et al., Reference Farb, Anderson, Irving, Segal and Gross2015; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux1998). Thus for many parents emotions come on so quickly that it compromises their ability to use cognitive strategies, emotion acceptance or coping statements (e.g. McRae et al., Reference McRae, Misra, Prasad, Pereira and Gross2012), and increases the likelihood of emotional dysregulation, harsh and ineffective parenting. In these situations, parents require strategies that do not require cognitive regulatory processes but rather employ ER strategies such as breathing, walking, or sensory regulation “in the moment” to prevent them from responding in an unsupportive way (Porges, Reference Porges1995).

Problems with ER are not just about being underregulated with emotions. For parents to be able to respond in a sensitive and attuned way to their children’s emotions, being overregulated or disconnected from emotions can also impair their ability to respond to children in a sensitive way (Baylin, Reference Baylin2017; Hughes & Baylin, Reference Hughes and Baylin2012). There are a variety of reasons why this might occur, including temperament (high inhibition); neurodiversity (e.g. autistic spectrum); that their socialization, culture, and family background has accentuated high levels of control or suppression; or that life experiences have included prolonged periods of stress or trauma where emotional detachment has been a learned pattern of coping (Hughes & Baylin, Reference Hughes and Baylin2012). Thus, overregulation or detachment from emotions affects both parents’ ability to be in touch with their own emotions and also their ability to reflect on their child’s internal world and successfully identify and empathize with child’s emotions.

There are, however, times when suppression (if used momentarily and not exclusively) is necessary and adaptive when parenting (Wylie et al., Reference Wylie, Colasante, De France, Lin and Hollenstein2022). Managing anger when one’s children are fighting by breathing slowly or use of self-talk to inhibit sharing one’s fears when a child is giving a musical performance can suppress expression in order to achieve one’s goals (help the children resolve conflict or support the child to manage performance anxiety). Parenting interventions that target ER may be most effective when they incorporate a range of different strategies that address parents unique challenges with ER. We now review some of the dominant therapy models that target adult ER.

14.3 Therapeutic Approaches that Target Emotion Regulation: Top-Down and Bottom-Up Strategies

Many therapies targeting ER or dysregulation work from a “top-down” model, where cognitions are used to change emotions and behaviors. Although some incorporate breathing and relaxation as central components (considered a more “bottom-up strategy”), primarily top-down models use cognitive strategies for ER such as reappraisal, reframing, distancing, and distraction, or behavioral strategies such as exposure with response prevention to achieve extinction. Here we briefly review well known therapies and how these work.

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), one of the most commonly used therapies, focuses on teaching skills to reduce aversive emotional states through behavioral techniques (e.g. breathing and relaxation strategies, scheduling pleasant events, encouraging use of behavioral hierarchies to face feared situations, and use of breathing/relaxation), and cognitive strategies (e.g. self-monitoring, cognitive restructuring, reframing, reattribution, coping skills). Mindfulness interventions also target ER using both top-down (targeting thinking) and bottom-up (focusing on breathing or the senses) strategies. A core assumption of mindfulness therapies is that psychological problems occur when people become overly caught up in thinking about the past or the future, both of which interfere with effective responding in the present (Kabat-Zinn, Reference Kabat-Zinn2003; Segal et al., Reference Segal, Williams and Teasdale2002). Shifting attention to the present and remaining nonjudgmental through use of top-down and bottom-up strategies improves insight and adaptive ER (Chambers et al., Reference Chambers, Gullone and Allen2009; Kabat-Zinn, Reference Kabat-Zinn1982; Segal et al., Reference Segal, Williams and Teasdale2002).

More recent “third-wave” behavioral and cognitive therapies also target ER. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT; Hayes & Wilson, Reference Hayes and Wilson1994), combines mindfulness techniques with CBT skills to alter patterns of dysfunctional behavior. ACT helps clients accept and tolerate negative emotions rather than avoid or extinguish them as in traditional CBT. Emotion-focused therapy (EFT; Paivio & Greenberg, Reference Paivio and Greenberg2001) specifically addresses aspects of emotional processing in therapy, namely, to be aware of, accept, express, and regulate emotions and to differentiate adaptive from maladaptive emotional reactions. Therapists guide clients toward emotions, exploring early experience, memories, and images that shape “emotional schemes.” Emotional schemes contribute to automatic emotional responses in a similar way that core maladaptive schemas determine dysfunctional patterns of thinking or behaving. Additionally, EFT recognizes two different pathways for producing emotions, one fast and automated, involving brainstem and gut responses, and the other slow, involving the neocortex in appraisal (Cozolino, Reference Cozolino2010; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux1998). Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT; Linehan, Reference Linehan1993) views dysregulated emotional responses as driven by distorted perception and dysfunctional thinking and also by automatic unconscious responses, such as biochemical changes, physiological changes, and action tendencies that can occur prior to distorted cognitions (see Neacsiu et al., Reference Neacsiu, Bohus, Linehan and Gross2015). Four primary skill sets are taught in DBT that assist in reducing emotion dysregulation: mindfulness, interpersonal effectiveness, ER, and distress tolerance. DBT focuses on proactive ER whereby clients learn skills in how to engage in self-care and actions that are self-soothing (such as paying attention to the senses). Distress tolerance skills are taught to assist in high-risk emotional situations which include sitting with emotions until they lessen, using mindfulness techniques, using STOP skills (Stop, Take a step back, Observe the situation, and Proceed effectively), and using bottom-up ER TIP skills (change body temperature, engage in intense physical exercise, and progressive muscle relaxation). Learning to tolerate emotions using these skills also assists the individual in working through their emotions rather than suppression, which often has less adaptive outcomes.

Bottom-up therapies work more specifically with somatic and emotional processes to change emotions, behaviors, and cognitions. Sensorimotor psychotherapy (Ogden et al., Reference Ogden, Minton and Pain2006) is a body-oriented therapy based on attachment theory, using techniques drawn from CBT and psychodynamic psychotherapy to assist (1) becoming aware of automatic maladaptive action tendencies, (2) learning to inhibit initial impulses, (3) experimenting with alternative actions in order to complete the “frozen actions” (e.g. fight or flight response) that were unable to be used during the trauma experience, and (4) practicing alternative and effective actions (Ogden et al., Reference Ogden, Minton and Pain2006). Treatment integrates top-down strategies including psychoeducation about trauma, mindfulness, and concentration practice with bottom-up therapies including development of somatic resources for regulating arousal. Yoga therapy is another body-oriented approach to help with physiological self-regulation that may alleviate distress through an integration of top-down and bottom-up processes that facilitate bidirectional communication between the mind and the body (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Erb, Schmalzl, Moonaz, Noggle Taylor and Porges2018). Reviews of studies find that yoga breathing, physical postures that require building distress tolerance, and meditation, lower posttraumatic stress disorder and generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in clinical participants including survivors of child abuse and interpersonal violence (Longacre et al., Reference Longacre, Silver-Highfield, Lama and Grodin2012; Telles et al., Reference Telles, Singh and Balkrishna2012). Yoga may be used in combination with exposure therapy, mindfulness, medication, or psychotherapy to achieve greater symptom reduction than those therapies alone (da Silva et al., Reference da Silva, Ravindran and Ravindran2009; Telles et al., Reference Telles, Singh and Balkrishna2012). Yoga adds a bottom-up approach to managing emotions that is often missing in other therapies, perhaps because body-oriented therapies are not a part of the professional training or clinical expertise of psychologists.

14.4 Parent Emotion Regulation Interventions

Many of the therapies described here and their underpinning theories have influenced parenting programs that help parents to understand and manage emotions and respond sensitively and supportively with their children. Increasingly there is a focus on parenting interventions underpinned by emotion socialization, attachment, reflective functioning, and mindfulness theories that also include a focus on parent ER. Recent reviews of emotion-focused parenting interventions have highlighted the efficacy of such programs in improving parent emotional functioning, parenting, parent–child relationships, and child outcomes (Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020; Havighurst et al., Reference Havighurst, Radovini, Hao and Kehoe2020; Jugovac et al., Reference Jugovac, O’Kearney, Hawes and Pasalich2022). Research has highlighted that emotion-focused programs may be especially important for families in which parents or children struggle with impulse control, mental health difficulties, or attachment problems (see Maliken & Katz, Reference Maliken and Katz2013; Scott & Dadds, Reference Scott and Dadds2009). Additionally, emotion dysregulation has been related to lower attendance and higher dropout in evaluation trials of parenting programs and to interfere with skill acquisition as well as skill implementation (Maliken & Katz, Reference Maliken and Katz2013; Zubrick et al., Reference Zubrick, Ward, Silburn, Lawrence, Williams, Blair, Robertson and Sanders2005). We briefly describe some of the key parenting frameworks here that focus on parent ER, before describing our work with TIK.

There are now a number of programs that focus specifically on helping parents manage emotions in the context of parenting. Katz and colleagues (Reference Katz, Gurtovenko, Maliken, Stettler, Kawamura and Fladeboe2020) designed a parenting intervention for mothers who had experienced intimate partner violence. A considerable portion of the early part of their 12-session program focused on building mothers’ emotion awareness and ER skills. This was viewed as essential for learning emotion coaching and to work through the memories and sequala of trauma. Similarly, the Let’s Connect program teaches caregivers’ social and emotional skills as a way of improving connection with the child, in a program working with families who have experienced trauma (Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Fitzgerald, Shipman and Torres2019). Tuning Relationships with Music (TRM; Colegrove et al., Reference Colegrove, Havighurst, Kehoe and Jacobsen2018), a music therapy program delivered individually to parent–adolescent dyads, was designed to address the cognitive, emotional, neurobiological (autonomic), and relational challenges for parents with a childhood interpersonal trauma history and their adolescent children. TRM provides resources and teaches skills to improve cognitive functioning (e.g. psychoeducation about beliefs and attributions that support emotional competence), ER (e.g. musical exercises to support emotion awareness and responsiveness for parent and adolescent; emotion coaching to assist the parent in helping their adolescent regulate emotions), autonomic regulation (e.g. music that supports diaphragmatic breathing) and relational functioning (e.g. musical exercises that teach parents to turn toward and connect with their adolescent) in order to support responsive and non-reactive communication during conflict.

Programs based on attachment theory often include helping a parent to understand their own emotional reactions to their child to facilitate sensitive and responsive caregiving. Circle of Security, a widely disseminated parenting program, uses the analogy of “shark music” to help parents recognize uncomfortable feelings about their past that influence the way they respond to their child (Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper and Powell2006). This awareness results in providing parents with the capacity to alter their emotional reactions, which is an important prerequisite for being able to regulate emotions. Although the program does not overtly teach ER skills, it promotes top-down ER strategies, because it results in greater insight, emotion acceptance and self-awareness that results in lowered emotion reactivity.

Reflective functioning and mentalization are also increasingly used in working with parents where there are attachment difficulties with the child. Reflective functioning refers to a parent’s ability to reflect on their own as well as their child’s internal states and is regarded as a central component contributing to secure attachment in children (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Maheux, Normandin, Sabourin, Diguer, Berthelot and Parent2013; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002). Interventions that focus on enhancing parental reflective functioning teach parents to understand their child’s motivations and needs rather than just responding to their behavior (Slade, Reference Slade2007). They help parents to become aware of their own internal motivations, thoughts, and feelings and how these are separate from their child’s. This awareness (using different therapeutic approaches) helps parents to regulate their reactions and learn new ways of responding to their child. A range of programs exist that focus on reflective functioning and mentalization (for a review, see Havighurst et al., Reference Havighurst, Radovini, Hao and Kehoe2020).

Finally, mindfulness parenting interventions have been found to be successful in improving parent ER and facilitating more sensitive caregiving (e.g. Bögels et al., Reference Bögels, Lehtonen and Restifo2010; Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Coatsworth and Greenberg2009; Lippold et al., Reference Lippold, Duncan, Coatsworth, Nix and Greenberg2015). Duncan et al. (Reference Duncan, Coatsworth and Greenberg2009) proposed a number of key components of mindfulness parenting programs that benefit parenting via how they affect parent’s own ER. These include nonjudgmental acceptance of oneself and the child, emotion awareness of self and child, self-regulation to reduce over-reactiveness, and compassion. Decentering includes pausing before reacting and shifting judgment by using self-talk such as “feelings are just feelings.” Mindfulness parenting programs use bottom-up strategies that include paying attention to the breath and bodily sensations, as well as top-down cognitive processes where beliefs are shifted to reduce reactiveness.

14.5 How Does Tuning in to Kids Focus on Emotion Regulation with Parents

In this section we outline the ways in which the TIK programs (including a version for parents of toddlers, parents of teens, and fathers) target parent ER. Although the primary focus of the programs is on developing parents’ capacity to emotion coach their children (with helping the child usually the main motivation for program attendance), TIK also focuses on parent ER because it is so central to parents’ ability to use emotion coaching and because it affects many other aspects of parent, family, and child functioning. We use four main approaches for improving parents’ abilities to regulate emotions: (1) teaching skills in emotion awareness and understanding; (2) exploring factors that contribute to parent emotion competence (including social and cultural influences, family-of-origin effects on emotion competence and meta-emotion philosophy, that is, understanding one’s own beliefs and automatic reactions to emotions); (3) teaching skills in ER, including proactive strategies to reduce stress and emotional arousal, bottom-up strategies for “in the moment” activation, and top-down strategies that include shifting beliefs about emotions; and (4) teaching parents emotion coaching to reduce parent and child emotion dysregulation.

14.6 Teaching Skills in Emotion Awareness and Understanding

Emotion awareness includes the capacity to notice and accurately identify one’s own and other’s emotions (Saarni, Reference Saarni1999). From early in life, children are primed to notice emotions and develop emotional literacy, being able to recognize, name, and understand their own and other’s emotions. Depending on the child’s own capacities and their experiences as they grow, children will vary in the development of these skills (Saarni, Reference Saarni1999). Emotional understanding plays an important role in social-emotional well-being and influences how emotions are regulated. In parents, there are substantial differences in these skills because of their childhood socialization experiences with emotions as well as their own temperament. Many parents have limited emotion awareness or emotion literacy and this can make it more difficult for them to teach their children about emotions and use emotion coaching (Havighurst & Kehoe, Reference Havighurst, Kehoe, Deater-Deckard and Panneton2017).

Learning skills in emotion awareness are threaded throughout the TIK program, beginning with helping parents with awareness of their own emotions, while also helping parents to recognize emotions underpinning children’s verbal statements, behaviors, and reactions to different situations. Specifically, TIK helps parents identify their children’s emotions by attending to facial expressions, body language, and tone of voice or by thinking of similar adult equivalent situations and reflecting on how they might feel (e.g. starting preschool may be like starting a new job). At the end of the first TIK session, parents are encouraged to begin noticing emotions more in their lives, including in themselves, their child and others. In session 2, parents begin the session with an activity about emotion awareness (the Bears exercise) where they choose a picture from a set of bears showing different emotional expressions that represent an emotion the parent has experienced during the last week. This is followed by answering a set of questions (i.e. What is the emotion? What was the situation? Where did you feel this emotion in your body? What thoughts went through your head? How did you feel about having this emotion?). These questions are designed to provide parents with ways to identify, name, and express how they felt, including linking an emotion experience to bodily sensations and thoughts as well as becoming aware of how one feels about having this feeling (i.e. identifying a meta-emotion belief about that emotion). Parents are encouraged to use these questions throughout day-to-day life when they experience emotions.

There are a number of other ways that TIK builds parents’ skills in emotion awareness. For example, through discussions after watching video examples or by engaging in role plays of parent–child interactions where parents are encouraged to identify and name the emotions of the parent and child. Parents are also taught to recognize feelings behind their child’s emotions and behavior using the “iceberg” metaphor to differentiate surface (irritability or withdrawal) versus deeper levels of emotional experience (e.g. disappointment, rejection, powerlessness, fear). Emotion word lists help parents to build greater complexity in language to differentiate and express emotions and feeling faces posters (that are ostensibly for children but often useful for parents) assist them to link emotion identification, facial expressions, and experiences to facilitate emotional competence. This process connects cognition and language to help with processing emotions, consistent with the mechanisms by which emotion-focused therapy works where putting words to felt experiences allows emotion to be processed rather than remaining unresolved (Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, Reference Greenberg and Pascual-Leone2006; Johnson & Lee, Reference Johnson, Lee and Bailey2000).

In TIK we also recognize that helping parents to become aware of and name children’s emotions (Steps 1 and 4 of emotion coaching) provides them with an anchor for present-moment-awareness, not unlike focusing the breath in mindfulness (Hill & Updegraff, Reference Hill and Updegraff2012). This enables parents to modify dysfunctional processes of suppressing emotions or becoming emotionally reactive and increases awareness of how they and their child are feeling. Parents’ report being calmer when attempting to identify emotions, being more present with their children when they are emotional and feeling more empathy for their children. This also assists parents to manage their automatic unsupportive reactions to children’s emotions thereby enabling alternative responses to be enacted (Bargh & Ferguson, Reference Bargh and Ferguson2000; Dumas, Reference Dumas2005).

14.7 Exploring Factors that Contribute to Parent Emotion Regulation

Gottman et al. (Reference Gottman, Katz and Hooven1996) outlined a theory of meta-emotion philosophy (MEP; thoughts and feelings about emotions) that determined parents’ responses to their children’s emotions. MEP is influenced by a range of factors including social, cultural, and family of origin experiences surrounding emotions (the messages received, verbally and nonverbally about expression) that shape socially proscribed ways of reacting to emotions (such as how grief is expressed at a funeral; whether anger is expressed overtly when another person has infringed an individual’s rights). In TIK we focus on shifting parents’ attitudes about emotions toward acceptance, noting that all emotions are acceptable and serve important functions of survival. This is differentiated from behavior, where not all behaviors (i.e. yelling at and hitting a child when angry) are acceptable or constructive in relationships and parenting. This distinction is also an example of the third wave of CBT, where emotions are accepted and validated as a mechanism for change, rather than altering thinking or behavior to alter emotions. This is often a challenging idea for parents who may hold strong negative attitudes about emotions and their expression (e.g. it is not okay to hate; jealousy is bad; anxiety is debilitating).

Emotion acceptance and learning ways to manage the discomfort that intense emotional arousal brings involves developing awareness of meta-emotions and automatic reactions. Parental automatic reactions occur often seemingly instantaneously, without conscious awareness in response to triggers (either memories, experiences or events), a bottom-up emotion generation. These might be when the individual is faced with an immediate threat to safety (or the safety of others), in specific situations (e.g. bedtimes or mealtimes if these were stressful when the parent was a child), with specific emotions (e.g. child or friend is sobbing unconsolably), or when their child engages in behaviors that the parent may have history with (e.g. lashes out at their sibling hurting them, or when their child reports they were not picked for a team at school). Automatic reactions are linked to the situational context (including memories of these from childhood), the type of emotion and their valence and often result in emotionally dysregulated responses that may have been adaptive during childhood but do not match or are no longer appropriate in the current context. For parents, for example, automatic reactions can show up as excessive anger in response to a child’s misbehavior, defensive retaliation in response to a child’s accusations of being unfair, disapproval and withdrawal in response to a child being jealous and harsh with a sibling, or embarrassment and criticalness when the child has a tantrum in front of grandparents. Automatic reactions can also include automatically dismissing the child’s emotions with the aim of regaining control or wanting the emotion to go away (e.g. use of disapproving or minimising, or laisse faire parental responses to the child). In this context, teaching parents’ ER skills is often ineffective because when activated in this way, they do not have access to optimal executive functioning and ER responses are less likely to occur.

Helping parents develop an awareness of their MEP, the influence of their family of origin experiences, and understanding their automatic reactions to emotions are critical in altering habitual, dysfunctional patterns of responding to emotions. This process is similar to schema-focused or emotion-focused psychotherapy in which changes occur by accessing past experiences and evoking emotions consistent with these memories in order to work through and alter automatic dysfunctional patterns of thinking, feeling, behaving, and interacting (Greenberg & Safran, Reference Greenberg and Safran1989). Intense automatic reactions are reduced because emotions no longer activate (often unconsciously) remembered past emotional experiences (Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, Reference Greenberg and Pascual-Leone2006; Lane et al., Reference Lane, Ryan, Nadel and Greenberg2015). Further, reflecting on the effect of dysfunctional family of origin experiences, while also learning new skills in parenting, has been found necessary for altering intergenerational patterns of neglectful parenting (Leerkes & Crockenberg, Reference Leerkes and Crockenberg2006).

Across the program, automatic reactions and family of origin (FOO) experiences are progressively explored so that defenses, retraumatizing or withdrawal responses are less likely to occur. Session 1 introduces parents to the basic premise of meta-emotion and that it is shaped by FOO, culture, and experiences. Session 2 builds on this by exploring the messages about emotion expression received from the wider culture. Session 3 explores family of origin experiences with sadness (how did your mother/father respond to you when you were sad; how did they show sadness; what were the messages that others gave you about sadness and expressing it, etc.). Session 4 explores FOO with fears and worries. Session 5 focuses on anger and session 6, on how conflict was responded to or is used as an opportunity for each parent to summarize what they have learned about their automatic reactions. In clinical settings the program is delivered over a longer duration enabling these issues to be explored gradually and sensitively, depending on parents’ capacity for reflection and self-disclosure, and accounting for difficult FOO experiences, past trauma, and current mental health. The graduated exploration of FOO and automatic reactions has resulted in many parents reporting shifts when they made the “light-bulb”-like connections between how their own mother or father responded with anger to them as a child and their automatic reactions as an adult to anger in close relationships or parenting. In turn, they will often report that they can now identify their own emotion dysregulation patterns, and this enables them to engage in and use skills taught in emotion regulation.

14.8 Teaching Emotion Regulation Skills: Proactive, Top-Down, and Bottom-Up Strategies

In TIK we teach three ways of regulating emotions: pausing, calming, and releasing. We also teach strategies parents can use proactively (i.e. self-care) and “in the moment,” with a specific focus on regulating anger, anxiety, and stress. We highlight that bottom-up ER strategies may work better in the moment, when emotions are generated automatically, fast and are high in intensity with the most frequently used skill to “build in a pause.” Building in a pause includes slow deep breaths, a cold drink of water, stepping outside or going to the bathroom, stretching, shifting the weight from one foot to the other, holding a necklace or keys to connect or ground to the senses and the present moment, counting to 10 in one’s head, or moving around. These activities require 10–30 seconds to reduce a parent’s arousal and inhibit their automatic reactions, instead enabling selection of more optional ER and parenting responses. Bottom-up strategies do not require cognitive ER skills such as attention-shifting or reframing that are often inaccessible to parents at times of heightened emotion. Strategies to build in a pause that are not cognitive are also more effective for parents who can be abusive when they are emotionally dysregulated.

Building in a pause allows parents to widen the gap between the emotionally activating situation and their response, reducing the likelihood of dysfunctional, unsupportive parenting. This also enables the parent to be more child centered, to engage their capacity for reflective functioning by considering the child’s internal world (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002), and then to use emotion coaching skills. Although in some respects building in a pause may entail momentary emotion suppression (e.g. telling yourself to stop) or distraction (e.g. counting, or focusing on one’s senses) we believe that this remains adaptive in the context of parenting, especially if followed by other strategies.

For regulating emotion in advance, we encourage proactive “emotional refueling” using both top-down (e.g. reflection) as well as bottom-up strategies to process emotions and prepare for future emotional moments. In contrast to avoidance or distraction behaviors, proactive ER includes “things one does before the emotion response tendencies have become fully activated and have changed one’s behaviour and one’s peripheral physiological responding” (Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007, p. 15). Proactive skills assist with emotion arousal reduction and include teaching breathing techniques (a range of options are taught or links to different resources that enable parents to find preferable methods), meditation, self-care activities that are calming (a hot bath, a walk in nature, taking off one’s shoes to walk on the grass and feel the texture under one’s feet) as well as physical activity to “let off steam” and reduce the physiological arousal that comes with emotion activation and stress (e.g. going for a run, walk or bike ride, kicking a ball, or weeding the garden). Parents are taught about the flight, flight, and freeze response and encouraged to find ways to manage these reactions and the activation that often ensues. Stress-reduction activities such as tense and release are also an effective component of CBT (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979; Hawton et al., Reference Hawton, Salkovskis, Kirk and Clark1989).

TIK does not directly teach cognitive or top-down strategies that are often the main components of adult-focused ER programs; however, many of the exercises, discussions and skills result in new experiences that can shift parents’ beliefs about emotions. For example, through exploring the function of emotions and meta-emotion, and gaining a more accurate understanding of children’s emotional development, change can occur in parents’ beliefs about emotions and help them to be less judgmental. Parents learn to reappraise emotional situations as emotion coaching opportunities, times for connection and teaching, which often reflects a change in beliefs about emotions.

Individual tailoring of program delivery based on parents’ challenges enables a targeted focus on areas of parent ER difficulties. Sometimes there is the need to introduce self-care earlier for very stressed or overwhelmed parents; sometimes parents dislike the focus on meditation and alternative exercises are needed; at other times when parents are more reactive, techniques to build in a pause are required earlier; or FOO is playing a very dominant role in preventing a parent from being able to take their child’s perspective, and so more work on this is needed in the later sessions. Lastly, the program normalizes ER difficulties, with the message that parenting is often a very challenging experience for many parents. Facilitators often share in a planned and limited way, personal experiences that illustrate their own challenges and learning surrounding ER. This process reduces again the “power” of expert/learner that can often result in parents feeling deskilled and devalued as a person and as a parent.

14.9 Teaching Parents Emotion Coaching to Assist with Emotion Regulation

The parenting skills taught in TIK for parents to use with their children also often assist parents in ER. Although emotion coaching is a way of responding that allows parents to coregulate their child, teaching the five steps of emotion coaching can act as an ER strategy for parents because it provides them with a model of how to work through emotional experiences. This alters their automatic reactions and shifts the attributions parents make when their children are emotional. Emotion coaching involves five main steps: (1) noticing the child’s emotion (which assists parents to be present in the moment); (2) seeing this as an opportunity to connect and guide/teach the child (a reappraisal of the situation); (3) communicating understanding and empathy; (4) helping the child to understand and name the emotion; and (5) if necessary, assist the child with problem-solving and/or setting limits around their behavior. By increasing parents’ awareness of their emotionally dismissive responses that create less optimal emotional competence, parents instead learn skills in how to use the emotion coaching steps with their children when emotions occur. For example, rather than viewing the child as being attention seeking, manipulative, or weak, use of the five steps helps the parent see their child’s emotions as an opportunity to connect with them and support them with emotions. Where the former reactions might activate a parent to irritability, anger, and an emotionally dismissive parenting response, the latter (where emotion coaching is used) results in increased empathy and less emotional reactivity in the parent.

Further, using the analogy of “the iceberg” parents are encouraged to notice (step 1) and explore emotions that underlie surface emotions (e.g. jealousy or fear behind anger) and behaviors (sadness, loneliness, or fear behind whining or oppositionality) and rather than withdrawing attention at these times, connection is encouraged and guidance is provided to the child to understand and regulate their emotions (step 2). Exploring deeper emotion (such as fears of separation, abandonment, rejection, failure, loss) that often drive surface level emotions and behaviors, significantly assists the child but also acts as a regulation strategy for the parent. Some parents will report that the act of enquiry when using the five steps shifts their focus and helps them to calm their own arousal levels as well.

Building parents capacity to communicate empathy (step 3) with their children is a key component of TIK. Empathy requires a parent to be regulated. In order to take the perspective of another (cognitive aspects of empathy) and “feel with” that person (the emotional aspects of empathy), a parent needs to shift their focus to their child’s mental and emotional state. We teach a stepped strategy called “The Emotion Detective” for helping parents to empathize, where parents ask themselves some reflective questions when their child experiences emotions (such as not being picked for a team; the birth of a sibling; being made to eat a least favorite food) and to (1) consider an adult equivalent situation (e.g. not getting a desired job; one’s partner bringing home a new girl/boyfriend to be part of the family; being made to do something at work that one disagrees with); then (2) imagine the emotions the parent might feel in that “adult equivalent” situation; and lastly (3) communicating understanding and acceptance by practicing an empathic response (e.g. how disappointing; I’d feel jealous and left out too; that’s annoying when you are made to do things you don’t like). The skills learned in this exercise are then promoted for use by the parent whenever their child becomes emotional. This gives parents a strategy that also helps them regulate their reactions and shifts their perspective from being adult centered to child centered. The theory of mentalization and reflective functioning is useful to understand this process where the parent is able to consider their child’s internal emotional world (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002). This functions as a regulation strategy for the parent and is usually helpful in calming the child. It also builds connection and closeness that reinforces the parents’ efforts, validating their actions and strengthening their confidence that they are able to support and guide their child. This results in shifts in their beliefs from being overwhelmed (I can’t cope with my child’s emotions; this child is so difficult) to having greater competence in parenting about emotions (I can manage my reactions; I can guide my child), and in turn contributes to parents being better able to regulate their own emotions.

The fourth step of naming emotions with children (step 4) also assists parents in their emotion awareness and regulation. Finally, a number of strategies are taught that provide parents with ways of assisting their child to regulate emotions (e.g. slow breathing, tense and release exercises, bringing attention to the senses, use of a special object to hold and feel the texture). These same skills are often useful for parents to assist them in their own ER.

14.10 Conclusion

Assisting parents to develop skills in regulating their emotions in the context of parenting, especially in ways that are supportive of the child’s emotion socialization, is central to emotion-focused parent work. Parenting has a significant emotional load and parents are often limited in what they have left for managing their own emotions. Part of parent work is finding ways to overcome the barriers to parents investing time and energy into efforts to address their own wellbeing. From our experiences with TIK programs and over 20 years of delivery of this suite of programs, we have refined ways of supporting parents to learn ER strategies as interwoven components in the parenting program so that defensiveness and barriers can be overcome. We combine a focus on increasing parent emotion awareness, exploration of factors that contribute to parent meta-emotion philosophy, proactive, bottom-up and top-down ER strategies and teaching emotion coaching skills for parents to use with their children that anchor them to a more child-centered approach to parenting and have benefits for parents as well. What works for parents depends on their own temperament, their family history with emotions and past experiences, as well as the context in which emotions occur and the intensity of the emotion. Parents also vary in what they find useful and effective, and a range of strategies enables individual preference and choice, ensuring parents feel more empowered to determine how they will parent and the choices they make. Many of the methods that we use are consistent with other theoretical approaches to adult ER interventions as well as parenting programs that have an emotion-focused or mindfulness approach. Although TIK programs often result in shifts in parent cognitive processes that aid in ER, this is not the primary approach used. Greater integration of bottom-up or body-focused strategies that assist in regulating emotions have been effective for many parents when responding to emotions in themselves and their children and illustrate the shifting theoretical and empirical state of knowledge about how to promote ER in the context of parenting.

Conclusions and Perspectives

Isabelle Roskam , James J. Gross , and Moïra Mikolajczak

Our goal in this book has been to highlight the importance of emotion regulation in the context of parenting. This book, the first on emotion regulation and parenting, both highlights the importance of emotion regulation in the specific context of parenting and shows how promising research at the intersection of these two fields is. With the help of the many wonderful experts who contributed to the chapters, this book allows us to (1) take stock of findings and trends in the field, (2) identify the main challenges to be addressed, and (3) pinpoint exciting directions and methods for future research. We address each of these in turn.

C.1 Emotion Regulation and Parenting

Research at the intersection of emotion regulation and parenting often seems to be studying different facets of this complex phenomenon. Some researchers focus on the parent’s emotion regulation, others on the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions, and still others on the role played by the parent’s and/or the child’s emotion regulation in the child’s development. One of the first challenges is to bring these different perspectives together.

At the outset of the book, the author of Chapter 1 offers an overview of the complexity of the field of research devoted to parenting cognitions and behaviors in relation to child development. Then, the authors of Chapter 2 point out the different facets of emotion regulation, the complexity of the process, and especially the fact that all its facets are worth studying in the field of parenting. It is clear from these two chapters that these two fields have developed independently, with their own complexity and issues. It will be of great interest and value to link them together. So how can we summarize what emotion regulation in parenting is?

First, emotion regulation in parenting is concerned with how parents regulate their own emotions as individuals. As highlighted in several chapters of the book, there is considerable interindividual variability in this respect due to genetic, hormonal, and neural factors (see Chapter 12), sociodemographic factors such as age or gender, primiparity, the developmental history of the individual including history of maltreatment (see Chapter 4), internal working models, personality, etc. The way in which parents regulate their own emotions as individuals is a key factor in parents’ well-being, stress, and behavior in general. In part, this is because the way in which parents regulate their emotions as individuals is an explanatory factor in the child’s emotional development via modeling (i.e. observation and imitation) (see Chapters 2, 7, and 9).

Second, emotion regulation in parenting concerns how parents regulate their own emotions within the specific context of parenting. On the one hand, parents must regulate the emotions that parenting brings in general. For example, they may feel fulfillment or pride in the role of mother or father or disappointment and disinterest. On the other hand, parents must also regulate the emotions they feel when interacting with the child. These emotions are specific to each interaction and fluctuate from moment to moment and from one context to another. For example, the parent may want to reduce the expression of anxiety on the teen’s first night out. The way in which the parent regulates their own emotions in the specific context of parenting is an explanatory factor in the parent’s well-being, stress, and burnout (see Chapter 6) and in their behavior as a parent (see Chapters 3 and 5).

Third, emotion regulation in parenting concerns how parents regulate the child’s emotions (i.e. reactions to child emotions, conversations and teaching about emotions) during parent–child interactions (see Chapters 7 and 8). Here too there is considerable interindividual variability. The way in which parents regulate the child’s emotions is the result of the two previous points, that is, the parent’s regulation of their own emotions in general, and the parent’s regulation of their own emotions in the specific context of parenting. It also results from the parent’s meta-emotion philosophy (see Chapter 8). This is the parent effect. To this effect is added the child effect. The way the parent regulates the child’s emotions is influenced by the child’s temperament, behavior in specific situations, age, and other characteristics such as a disability. There also may be interaction effects, meaning that the impact of the parent effects depends on the characteristics of the child. As an additional complexity, there is also an effect of the context in which the interaction occurs. For example, the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions may differ depending on whether regulation occurs in the family or in a public setting (see Chapter 1). A parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions also differs according to culture (see Chapter 10). The way in which the parent regulates the child’s emotions through their practices and reactions matters, as it influences the child’s social and emotional development (see Chapters 2, 7, and 9).

C.2 Challenges to Be Addressed

Without diminishing the work already accomplished by researchers in the field, it is clear that there are still many unexplored areas and that the field is struggling to progress in a coherent manner in all three directions (i.e. the parent’s regulation of their own emotions in general, the parent’s regulation of their own emotions in the specific context of parenting, and the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions). Taken together, the chapters that make up this book suggest that our field needs to address several main challenges.

One challenge is that there is a great imbalance between the three elements that the field of emotion regulation in parenting entails. Of the three, the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions, and more specifically its effect on the child’s development, has been documented the most (see Part III). There are proportionately very few studies in which the parent’s emotion regulation is studied for its own sake, without the objective of understanding its effects on the child’s development. In other words, researchers are interested in parents because they influence child development, not because the emotion regulation of the parent as a person is of interest in itself or because there is a concern to increase parental well-being and mental health (see Chapters 6 and 11).

It follows from this that parent-driven effects are much more widely considered than child-driven effects. Yet, the parent’s regulation of their own emotions and the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions occur in a dyadic context of mutual adaptation, based on reciprocal and transactional effects (see Chapters 9 and 11). Despite the bidirectional nature of emotion regulation in parenting, the vast majority of studies document how the parent’s emotion regulation influences the child’s emotion regulation. Few focus on how the child’s emotion regulation influences the parent’s emotion regulation. Most of these studies are based on correlational analyses that provide no indication of the direction of the effects, but their results are repeatedly interpreted in the direction of parent to child and rarely in the opposite direction. The child’s evocative effect, however, may be a key factor in explaining both interindividual (i.e. from one parent to another) and intraindividual (i.e. from moment to moment or from one context to another) variation in emotion regulation and parenting.

Another challenge stemming from the predominance of correlational studies is the urgent need to go beyond a linear and homogeneous view of the relationships between emotion regulation, parenting, and child development (see Chapter 1). According to this view, the better parents regulate their own emotions, the better they regulate their emotions in the specific context of parenting; the better parents regulate their emotions in the specific context of parenting, the better they regulate the child’s emotions; and the better parents regulate the child’s emotions, the more optimally the child develops (e.g. fewer behavioral symptoms and better peer relationships), and the better the child regulates their own emotions.

This linear and homogeneous view of the relationships between emotion regulation, parenting, and child development may be overly simple. In this book, the authors have drawn attention to the fact that (1) the size of the correlations between these variables is only small to modest at best (see Chapters 1 and 9); (2) the correlations were mostly obtained in samples of normative nuclear families from WEIRD (i.e. Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) countries (see Chapters 1 and 10); (3) there are shared third factors such as genetic factors or extrafamilial factors like shared ethnicity (see Chapter 10) explaining both the parent’s and the child’s emotion regulation (see Chapter 6); and (4) the relationships between emotion regulation, parenting, and child development are not limited to bivariate relationships but involve processes of mediation (see Chapters 1 and 6) and moderation by the child’s age, temperament, and behavior (see Chapter 1), and the parent’s gender (see Chapter 5) and culture (see Chapters 3, 9, and 10).

Beyond being overly simple, a linear and homogeneous view of relationships between emotion regulation, parenting, and child development reinforces the belief in parental determinism, that is, the belief that child development in general, and child emotion regulation in particular, are largely or exclusively the result of parenting. Although the influence of parenting on child development is not negligible, it is clear that child development is the result of a complex equation which, in addition to parental factors, includes factors beyond the control of the parent (e.g. the child’s own agency, genetic, physiological and contextual factors). The belief in parental determinism is all the more problematic as it seems to contribute to increasing the cultural pressure to be a good parent (see Chapter 10), parental stress, and burnout (see Chapters 6 and 13). And, ironically, by increasing parental stress and burnout, it also potentially increases the risk of parental neglect and violence (see Chapters 5 and 6).

Another oversimplification is the binary vision according to which there are “good” and “bad” strategies in emotion regulation, “good” and “bad” practices in parenting, and children with “good” and “bad” development. Despite the authors’ desire to use less prescriptive terms, our book exemplifies this simplifying binary vision: functional, adaptive, well-suited, better, correct, right, sensitive, optimal, supportive, against dysfunctional, maladaptive, ill-suited, negative, at risk, nonoptimal, and unsupportive. This is entirely understandable. However, the reality is much more complex. As we saw for example in Chapter 3, so-called positive emotions do not always have positive consequences, and negative emotions should not always be minimized. We saw in Chapter 6 that the best is sometimes the enemy of the good, because too much parental emotion regulation increases stress and burnout. And we know that in the context of moderations, the effect of a variable (e.g. parental emotion regulation) on another variable (e.g. child emotion regulation) is not true for all parents, for all children, and/or in all contexts (see Chapters 1, 3, 5, 6, 9, and 10).

The result of all this is the risk of delivering unhelpful take-home messages to researchers, professionals, and parents themselves. One example of such a take-home message is the promotion of emotion regulation in general and of this or that specific strategy (for instance, always encouraging reappraisal and always discouraging suppression) to ensure optimal child development. However, it has been shown in other domains that it is flexibility in the use of emotion regulation strategies that seems to be most predictive of good outcomes (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015; Bonanno & Burton, Reference Bonanno and Burton2013; Bonanno et al., Reference Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Westphal and Coifman2004). Another example of a potentially unhelpful take-home message is the requirement that parents achieve the desired affective state that corresponds to the model of good parenting relevant in the WEIRD countries (see Chapters 10 and 13).

Yet another challenge pertains to the need for a common language. Authors frequently use different terms to talk about the same things. In particular, we noted the following equivalences or proxies: (1) self-focused and intrinsic versus other-focused and extrinsic emotion regulation (see Chapters 2 and 6); (2) parent’s parenting of children’s emotions, children’s emotional socialization (practices), and parental socialization of child emotions (see Chapters 1, 2, and 9); (3) supportive socialization practices and emotion coaching (see Chapter 9); (4) supportive practices, coaching practices, and adaptive parental extrinsic emotion regulation versus unsupportive, dismissing practices, and maladaptive parental extrinsic emotion regulation (see Chapter 6); (5) “coregulation of and then by child’s emotion” and extrinsic emotion regulation (see Chapter 7); and (6) top-down emotion regulation and emotional labor (see Chapters 12 and 13). A dialogue between experts would undoubtedly contribute to greater conceptual clarity. Such a dialogue would make it possible to know whether the proxies are really synonyms, in which case a common language should be favored, or whether these proxies do not refer to exactly the same thing, in which case their specificity and the limits of their overlap should be more explicitly fixed.

C.3 Future Directions

In addition to these challenges, each of the contributors of this book identified in the conclusions of their chapters one or more directions for future research. We summarize and organize these future directions next, distinguishing content from methods.

C.3.1 Content Issues

One future direction is to document the dimensions of emotion regulation in parenting that have received the least attention, that is, the parent’s regulation of their own emotions vis-à-vis parenting in general, and the parent’s regulation of their own emotions in the specific context of parenting. If we want to understand emotion regulation in parenting, we can no longer focus in most studies on the parent’s regulation of the child’s emotions and its effects on the child’s development (see Chapter 3). Rather, we must accord the same importance to the parent as we give to the child, as has been emphasized by the authors of several chapters (see Chapters 6, 11, and 12). Greater attention to the parent should lead us to develop and promote interventions that emotionally care for the child and the parent as well. Currently, interventions are very often driven by a strong focus on the best interests of the child and a strong belief in parental determinism. Interventions can therefore increase the demands for emotion regulation on the part of the parent, and thus emotional labor (see Chapters 13 and 14). It is necessary to rethink these interventions with the benefit of both the child and the parent in mind, as the two form an inseparable dyad (i.e. the emotional well-being of one depends on the emotional well-being of the other), and to systematically test the effects of the interventions not only on the child’s development, well-being, and emotion regulation but also on the parent’s well-being, emotions, and emotion regulation.

Second, given the specificity of the context (i.e. parenting), the contributors to this book draw our attention to four important elements. First, we need to move from the regulation of emotions in general to the regulation of specific emotions (see Chapter 3). Second, we need to focus on the emotion regulation strategies that are relevant to the specific context of parenting, as not all strategies necessarily apply to parent–child interactions (see Chapter 3). In so doing, we need to study all relevant emotion regulation strategies in the context of the parent–child relationship, not just reappraisal or suppression (for a list of relevant emotion regulation strategies, see Chapter 8). Third, we must recognize that parents do not use emotion regulation strategies in isolation. Rather, they use several of them during the same interaction with the child and combine them sequentially. Cluster analysis (see Chapter 7) or ecological momentary assessment (see Chapters 8 and 11) are particularly interesting methods for studying how interpersonal regulatory interactions vary across and within interactions. Fourth, we would benefit from complementing the study of the regulation of negative emotions with studies on the regulation of the parent’s and child’s positive emotions (see Chapters 3 and 9).

C.3.2 Methodological Issues

This book has highlighted the complexity of emotion regulation in parenting. In order to account for this complexity, we must rely on methods other than correlational analyses. Although correlational approaches constitute an important and entirely legitimate first step, it is essential to go further in order to (1) disentangle the directions of causality between the variables (see Chapters 7, 8, and 11); (2) integrate into the models the processes of mediation and/or moderation (see Chapters 5, 6, 7, and 8); (3) take into account the normative and nonnormative developmental dimension (see Chapters 2, 7, and 11); (4) integrate the specificity of the context and be ecologically valid (see Chapters 10 and 11); (5) consider that the relations between variables may not be linear but curvilinear (see Chapter 6); and (6) take into account the dependence between child and parent data (see Chapter 11). We also need to remember that there is no single gold-standard method (for an excellent synthesis of recommendations for future research, see Chapter 11). The different methods must be seen as complementary because none of them, however sophisticated, can alone capture the whole complexity of emotion regulation in parenting.

The methods employed must also allow us to model the dyadic and bidirectional character and the process of mutual adaptation in the parent–child relationship (see Chapters 1 and 11). Moreover, they should allow us to integrate the asymmetric character of this relationship (i.e. the contribution of the child and the parent in terms of emotion regulation is different because of the different levels of maturity, see Chapters 1 and 7). Finally, because of the large interindividual differences in emotion regulation and parenting, these methods must be able to integrate the singularity of the parent and the child, as well as the singularity of each dyad (see Chapters 1 and 8). The dyadic and bidirectional nature of the parent–child relationship makes the systemic approach particularly well suited to study emotion regulation in the parenting context. This type of approach has the additional advantage of making it possible to consider more than two interacting partners (see Chapters 2 and 11). It also draws our attention to the importance of going beyond the exclusive focus on the mother-child dyad to include fathers in the system, as well as other important socializing agents such as grandparents, siblings, peers, or teachers (see Chapter 9).

C.4 Concluding Comment

Much important work has been done at the intersection of emotion regulation and parenting. However, it is clear that there is still much to do and to discover. It is our hope that this book will stimulate research in this area, benefiting both parents and children.

Footnotes

Chapter 10 Culture and the Challenges of Being a Good Parent

Chapter 11 Leveraging Parent–Youth Interactions to Measure and Analyze Emotion Regulation

Chapter 12 Neurobiology of Parenting and Implications for Emotion Regulation

Chapter 13 Emotional Labor in Parenting

1 Deep acting refers to changing felt emotions to achieve the desired emotional display. Surface acting refers to changing emotional displays without changing internal feelings. Both concepts are discussed in detail later in this chapter.

Chapter 14 Enhancing Parental Emotion Regulation Skills

Conclusions and Perspectives

References

References

Acosta, R. M., & Hutchinson, M. (2017 ). The happiest kids in the world: How Dutch parents help their kids (and themselves) by doing less. The Experiment.Google Scholar
Alexander, J. J., & Sandahl, I. (2016). The Danish way of parenting: What the happiest people in the world know about raising confident, capable kids. Tarcher Perigee.Google Scholar
Center for the Study of Culture Health and Human Development (Producer). (2021). Avoiding ethnocentrism in behavioral science through studying culture and human development [Conversation round-table]. 2021 biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Botswana, Brazil, Ghana, and Hong Kong. The UConn Center for the Study of Culture Health and Human Development. https://chhd.uconn.edu/avoiding-ethnocentrismGoogle Scholar
D’Andrade, R. G., & Strauss, C. (1992). Human motives and cultural models. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Druckerman, P. (2014). Bringing up bébé: One American mother discovers the wisdom of French parenting. Penguin.Google Scholar
Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. L. (1943). Infant and child in the culture of today: The guidance of development in home and nursery school. Hamish Hamilton.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1-26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkness, S. (2023). Commentary for “Parenting culture (s): Ideal parent beliefs across 37 countries.” Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 54(1), 2529.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1985). Child-environment transactions in the socialization of affect. In Lewis, M. & Saarni, C. (Eds.), The socialization of emotions (pp. 2136). Plenum.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1996). Introduction. In Harkness, S. & Super, C. M. (Eds.), Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences (pp. 123). Guilford.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2006). Themes and variations: Parental ethnotheories in western cultures. In Rubin, K. H. & Chung, O.-B. (Eds.), Parental beliefs, parenting, and child development in cross-cultural perspective (pp. 61–79). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Bonichini, S., Ríos Bermúdez, M., Mavridis, C., van Schaik, S. D. M., Tomkunas, A., & Palacios, J. (2020). Parents, preschools, and the developmental niches of young children: A study in four western cultures. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 170, 113–142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cad.20343Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., & Mavridis, C. J. (2011). Parental ethnotheories about children’s socioemotional development. In Chen, X. & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.), Socioemotional development in cultural context, pp. 7398. New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Moscardino, U., Rha, J.-H., Blom, M. J. M., Huitrón, B., Johnston, C., Sutherland, M., Hyun, O.-K., Axia, G., & Palacios, J. (2007). Cultural models and developmental agendas: Implications for arousal and self-regulation in early infancy. Journal of Developmental Processes, 2(1), 539.Google Scholar
Hulbert, A. (2004). Raising America: Experts, parents, and a century of advice about children. Vintage.Google Scholar
Lavelli, M., Carra, C., Rossi, G., & Keller, H. (2019). Culture-specific development of early mother–infant emotional co-regulation: Italian, Cameroonian, and West African immigrant dyads. Developmental Psychology, 55(9), 18501867. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000696Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Mikolajczak, M., Keller, H., Akgun, E., Arikan, G., Aunola, K., Barham, E., Besson, E., Blanchard, M. A., Boujut, E., Brianda, M. E., Brytek-Matera, A., César, F., Chen, B.-B., Dorard, G., dos Santos Elias, L. C., Dunsmuir, S., Egorova, N., Escobar, M. J., … Roskam, I. (2023). Parenting culture(s): Ideal-Parent beliefs across 37 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 54(1), 424.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Fontaine, J. (2008). Mapping expressive differences around the world: The relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(1), 5574. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107311854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, J. (1970). Developmental psychology: A psychobiological approach. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ng, F. F.-Y., Pomerantz, E. M., & Deng, C. (2014). Why are Chinese mothers more controlling than American mothers? “My child is my report card.” Child Development, 85(1), 355369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, F. F.-Y., Pomerantz, E. M., & Lam, S.-F. (2007). European American and Chinese parents’ responses to children’s success and failure: Implications for children’s responses. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 12391255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J.-H., & Kwon, Y. I. (2009). Parental goals and parenting practices of upper-middle-class Korean mothers with preschool children. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 7(1), 5875.Google Scholar
Potthoff, S., Garnefski, N., Miklósi, M., Ubbiali, A., Domínguez-Sánchez, F. J., Martins, E. C., Witthöft, M., & Kraaij, V. (2016). Cognitive emotion regulation and psychopathology across cultures: A comparison between six European countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 218224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rockwell, N. (1933, November 5). Child psychology. Saturday Evening Post, cover.Google Scholar
Senior, J. (2014). All joy and no fun: The paradox of modern parenthood. Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Super, C. M., Blom, M. J. M., Harkness, S., Ranade, N., & Londhe, R. (2021). Culture and the organization of infant sleep: A study in the Netherlands and the USA. Infant Behavior and Development, 64, 101620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101620Google Scholar
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 545569.Google Scholar
Super, C. M., Harkness, S., van Tijen, N., van der Vlugt, E., Dykstra, J., & Fintelman, M. (1996). The three R’s of Dutch child rearing and the socialization of infant arousal. In Harkness, S. & Super, C. M. (Eds.), Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences (pp. 447466). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tsamaase, M., Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2020). Grandmothers’ developmental expectations for early childhood in Botswana. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 170, 93112.Google Scholar
UNICEF. (2012). State of the world’s children 2012: Children in an urban world. United Nations Children’s Fund.Google Scholar
van Schaik, S., Mavridis, C. J., De Looze, M., Blom, M. J. M., Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (2020). Getting the baby on a schedule: Dutch and American mothers’ ethnotheories and the establishment of diurnal rhythms in early infancy. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, (170), 1341. https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20336Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1928). Psychological care of infant and child. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M. (1977). A model for psychocultural research. In Leiderman, P. H., Tulkin, S. R., & Rosenfeld, A. (Eds.), Culture and infancy: Variations in the human experience (pp. 2948). Academic Press.Google Scholar

References

Abney, D. H., daSilva, E. B., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2021). Associations between infant–mother physiological synchrony and 4- and 6-month-old infants’ emotion regulation. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(6), e22161. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22161Google Scholar
Aldao, A., Gee, D. G., De Los Reyes, A., & Seager, I. (2016). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 927946. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000638Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 875896. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827Google Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 8195. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025583Google Scholar
Bettis, A. H., Burke, T. A., Nesi, J., & Liu, R. T. (2022). Digital technologies for emotion-regulation assessment and intervention: A conceptual review. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(1), 326. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026211011982Google Scholar
Bodner, N., Kuppens, P., Allen, N. B., Sheeber, L. B., & Ceulemans, E. (2018). Affective family interactions and their associations with adolescent depression: A dynamic network approach. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 14591473. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001699Google Scholar
Boeve, J. L., Beeghly, M., Stacks, A. M., Manning, J. H., & Thomason, M. E. (2019). Using the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model to assess maternal and infant contributions to mother-infant affective exchanges during the Still-Face Paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development, 57, 101351. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101351Google Scholar
Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. (2013). Design and analysis of intensive longitudinal studies of distinguishable dyads. In Kenny, D. A. & Little, T. D. (Eds.), Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary and experience sampling research (pp. 143171). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 602654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038662Google Scholar
Byrd, A. L., Lee, A. H., Frigoletto, O. A., Zalewski, M., & Stepp, S. D. (2021). Applying new RDoC dimensions to the development of emotion regulation: Examining the influence of maternal emotion regulation on within-individual change in child emotion regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 33(5), 18211836. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000948CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, M. H.-M., Feng, X., Inboden, K., Hooper, E. G., & Gerhardt, M. (2022). Dynamic, bidirectional influences of children’s emotions and maternal regulatory strategies. Emotion, 22(8), 18411855. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001005Google Scholar
Chaplin, T. M., Sinha, R., Simmons, J. A., Healy, S. M., Mayes, L. C., Hommer, R. E., & Crowley, M. J. (2012). Parent–adolescent conflict interactions and adolescent alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 37(5), 605612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.01.004Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Laureen O’Donnell, T. (1994). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 73100. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166139Google Scholar
Cook, W. L., & Kenny, D. A. (2005). The Actor–Partner Interdependence Model: A model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(2), 101109. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250444000405Google Scholar
Cui, L., Morris, A. S., Harrist, A. W., Larzelere, R. E., Criss, M. M., & Houltberg, B. J. (2015). Adolescent RSA responses during an anger discussion task: Relations to emotion regulation and adjustment. Emotion, 15(3), 360372. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Los Reyes, A., Augenstein, T. M., Wang, M., Thomas, S. A., Drabick, D. A., Burgers, D. E., & Rabinowitz, J. (2015). The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 141(4), 858-–900. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038498Google Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., Thomas, S. A., Goodman, K. L., & Kundey, S. M. (2013). Principles underlying the use of multiple informants’ reports. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 123149. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185617Google Scholar
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Bernecker, S. L., & Christensen, K. (2015). Recent innovations in the field of interpersonal emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 3642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donenberg, G. R., & Weisz, J. R. (1997). Experimental task and speaker effects on parent-child interactions of aggressive and depressed/anxious children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(5), 367387. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025733023979Google Scholar
Gates, K. M., & Liu, S. (2016). Methods for quantifying patterns of dynamic interactions in dyads. Assessment, 23(4), 459471. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191116641508Google Scholar
Girard, J. M., & Cohn, J. F. (2016). A primer on observational measurement. Assessment, 23(4), 404413. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191116635807Google Scholar
Gistelinck, F., & Loeys, T. (2019). The Actor–Partner Interdependence Model for longitudinal dyadic data: An implementation in the SEM framework. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 26(3), 329347. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2018.1527223Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 4154. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348Google Scholar
Hajal, N. J., Teti, D. M., Cole, P. M., & Ram, N. (2019). Maternal emotion, motivation, and regulation during real-world parenting challenges. Journal of Family Psychology, 33(1), 109120. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000475Google Scholar
Hanley, J. A., Negassa, A., Edwardes, M. D. D., & Forrester, J. E. (2003). Statistical analysis of correlated data using generalized estimating equations: An orientation. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(4), 364375. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwf215Google Scholar
Hendriks, A. M., Van der Giessen, D., Stams, G., & Overbeek, G. (2018). The association between parent-reported and observed parenting: A multi-level meta-analysis. Psychological Assessment, 30(5), 621633. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000500Google Scholar
Hofmann, S. G. (2014). Interpersonal emotion regulation model of mood and anxiety disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(5), 483492. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9620-1Google Scholar
Hofmann, S. G., Carpenter, J. K., & Curtiss, J. (2016). Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (IERQ): Scale development and psychometric characteristics. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(3), 341356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9756-2Google Scholar
Kaufman, E. A., Xia, M., Fosco, G., Yaptangco, M., Skidmore, C. R., & Crowell, S. E. (2016). The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale Short Form (DERS-SF): Validation and replication in adolescent and adult samples. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38(3), 443455. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-015-9529-3Google Scholar
Keijsers, L., Boele, S., & Bulow, A. (2022). Measuring parent-adolescent interactions in natural habitats. The potential, status, and challenges of ecological momentary assessment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 264269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.10.002Google Scholar
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. Guilford. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2006-12719-000&site=ehost-liveGoogle Scholar
Kerr, M. L., Rasmussen, H. F., Smiley, P. A., Buttitta, K. V., & Borelli, J. L. (2021). The development of toddlers’ emotion regulation within the family system: Associations with observed parent-child synchrony and interparental relationship satisfaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 57, 215227. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.06.004Google Scholar
Kiel, E. J., & Kalomiris, A. E. (2015). Current themes in understanding children’s emotion regulation as developing from within the parent–child relationship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 1116. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.006Google Scholar
Ledermann, T., & Kenny, D. A. (2017). Analyzing dyadic data with multilevel modeling versus structural equation modeling: A tale of two methods. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(4), 442452. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000290Google Scholar
Li, D., Li, D., Wu, N., & Wang, Z. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion regulation through parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Tests of unique, actor, partner, and mediating effects. Children and Youth Services Review, 101, 113122.Google Scholar
Micalizzi, L., Ronald, A., & Saudino, K. J. (2016). A genetically informed cross-lagged analysis of autistic-like traits and affective problems in early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(5), 937947. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0088-6Google Scholar
Micalizzi, L., Wang, M., & Saudino, K. J. (2017). Difficult temperament and negative parenting in early childhood: A genetically informed cross-lagged analysis. Developmental Science, 20(2), e12355. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12355Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., & Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 233238. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12238Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Cui, L., Criss, M. M., & Simmons, W. K. (2018). Emotion regulation dynamics during parent–child interactions: Implications for research and practice. In Cole, P. M. H., T. (Ed.), Emotion regulation (pp. 7090). Routledge.Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.xGoogle Scholar
Muniz-Terrera, G., Robitaille, A., Kelly, A., Johansson, B., Hofer, S., & Piccinin, A. (2017). Latent growth models matched to research questions to answer questions about dynamics of change in multiple processes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 82, 158166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.09.001Google Scholar
Niven, K., Totterdell, P., Stride, C. B., & Holman, D. (2011). Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS): The development and validation of a new individual difference measure. Current Psychology, 30(1), 5373. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9099-9Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (Eds.). (2009). Biopsychosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavioral problems. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Page-Gould, E. (2017). Multilevel modeling. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 662678). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paley, B., & Hajal, N. J. (2022). Conceptualizing emotion regulation and coregulation as family-level phenomena. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 25(1), 1943. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-022-00378-4Google Scholar
Patterson, G. R. (2016). Coercion theory: The study of change. In Dishion, T. J., Snyder, J., & Patterson, G. R. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of coercive relationship dynamics (pp. 722). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324552.013.2Google Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879Google Scholar
Prinz, R. J., Foster, S. L., Kent, R. N., & O’Leary, K. D. (1979). Multivariate assessment of conflict in distressed and nondistressed mother–adolescent dyads. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12(4), 691700. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1979.12-691Google Scholar
Ratliff, E. L., Kerr, K. L., Misaki, M., Cosgrove, K. T., Moore, A. J., DeVille, D. C., Silk, J. S., Barch, D. M., Tapert, S. F., Simmons, W. K., Bodurka, J., & Morris, A. S. (2021). Into the unknown: Examining neural representations of parent–adolescent interactions. Child Development, 92(6), e1361e1376. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13635Google Scholar
Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Scharke, W., & Konrad, K. (2018). Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. Neuroimage, 178, 493502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.060Google Scholar
Rothenberg, W. A., Weinstein, A., Dandes, E. A., & Jent, J. F. (2019). Improving child emotion regulation: Effects of parent–child interaction-therapy and emotion socialization strategies. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(3), 720731. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-018-1302-2Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. (2009). The transactional model. In Sameroff, A. (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 321). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11877-001Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. (2010). Dynamic developmental systems: Chaos and order. In Evans, In G. W. & Wachs, T. D. (Eds.), Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective (pp. 255264). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12057-016Google Scholar
Savord, A., McNeish, D., Iida, M., Quiroz, S., & Ha, T. (2022). Fitting the longitudinal Actor-Partner Interdependence Model as a dynamic structural equation model in Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2022.2065279Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 379405. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739Google Scholar
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 132. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415Google Scholar
Silk, J. S. (2019). Context and dynamics: The new frontier for developmental research on emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 55(9), 20092014. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000768Google Scholar
Silk, J. S., Forbes, E. E., Whalen, D. J., Jakubcak, J. L., Thompson, W. K., Ryan, N. D., Axelson, D. A., Birmaher, B., & Dahl, R. E. (2011). Daily emotional dynamics in depressed youth: A cell phone ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(2), 241257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.007Google Scholar
Silvers, J. A., Shu, J., Hubbard, A. D., Weber, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2015). Concurrent and lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response in adolescence and young adulthood. Developmental Science, 18(5), 771784. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12260Google Scholar
Stas, L., Kenny, D. A., Mayer, A., & Loeys, T. (2018). Giving dyadic data analysis away: A user-friendly app for actor-partner interdependence models. Personal Relationships, 25(1), 103119. https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12230Google Scholar
Stone, L. B., Mennies, R. J., Waller, J. M., Ladouceur, C. D., Forbes, E. E., Ryan, N. D., Dahl, R. E., & Silk, J. S. (2019). Help me feel better! Ecological momentary assessment of anxious youths’ emotion regulation with parents and peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(2), 313324. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0454-2Google Scholar
Thomas, S. A., Brick, L. A., Micalizzi, L., Wolff, J. C., Frazier, E. A., Graves, H., Esposito-Smytherse, C., & Spirito, A. (2020). Parent–adolescent relationship characteristics and adolescent cannabis use: A growth curve analysis. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 46(5), 659669. https://doi.org/10.1080/00952990.2020.1789159Google Scholar
Thomas, S. A., Deros, D. E., Jain, A., Jacobs, I., & De Los Reyes, A. (2022, December 2). Links between parental monitoring and parent-adolescent conflict: A multi-modal test of bidirectional relations. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/xfj48Google Scholar
Thomas, S. A., Jain, A., Wilson, T., Deros, D. E., Jacobs, I., Dunn, E. J., Aldao, A., Stadnik, R., & De Los Reyes, A. (2019). Moderated mediation of the link between parent-adolescent conflict and adolescent risk-taking: The role of physiological regulation and hostile behavior in an experimentally controlled investigation. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 41, 699715. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-019-09747-wGoogle Scholar
Thomas, S. A., Wilson, T., Jain, A., Deros, D. E., Um, M., Hurwitz, J., Jacobs, I., Myerberg, L., Ehrlich, K. B., Dunn, E. J., Aldao, A., Stadnik, R., & De Los Reyes, A. (2017). Toward developing laboratory-based parent-adolescent conflict discussion tasks that consistently elicit adolescent conflict-related stress responses: Support from physiology and observed behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(12), 32883302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0844-zGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3(1), 5361.Google Scholar
Thorson, K. R., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2018). Measuring physiological influence in dyads: A guide to designing, implementing, and analyzing dyadic physiological studies. Psychological Methods, 23(4), 595616. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000166Google Scholar
Trucco, E. M., Cristello, J. V., & Sutherland, M. T. (2021). Do parents still matter? The impact of parents and peers on adolescent electronic cigarette use. Journal of Adolescent Health, 68(4), 780786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.12.002Google Scholar
Turpyn, C. C., Chaplin, T. M., Cook, E. C., & Martelli, A. M. (2015). A person-centered approach to adolescent emotion regulation: Associations with psychopathology and parenting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.009Google Scholar
Waller, J. M., Silk, J. S., Stone, L. B., & Dahl, R. E. (2014). Co-rumination and co-problem solving in the daily lives of adolescents with major depressive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(8), 869878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.004Google Scholar
Williams, W. C., Morelli, S. A., Ong, D. C., & Zaki, J. (2018). Interpersonal emotion regulation: Implications for affiliation, perceived support, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(2), 224254. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000132Google Scholar
Wolff, J. C., Thomas, S. A., Hood, E., Bettis, A. H., Rizzo, C. J., & Liu, R. T. (2020). Application of the actor-partner interdependence model in parent-adolescent emotion regulation and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 733741. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.075Google Scholar
Wright, A. G., & Hopwood, C. J. (2016). Advancing the assessment of dynamic psychological processes. Assessment, 23(4), 399403. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191116654760Google Scholar
Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion, 13(5), 803810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033839Google Scholar

References

Abraham, E., Hendler, T., Shapira-Lichter, I., Kanat-Maymon, Y., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2014). Father’s brain is sensitive to childcare experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(27), 97929797.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G. (2014). Crying as a trigger for abusive head trauma: A key to prevention. Pediatric Radiology, 44(4), 559564.Google Scholar
Bechtel, K., Gaither, J. R., & Leventhal, J. M. (2020). Impact of the Take 5 Safety Plan for Crying on the occurrence of abusive head trauma in infants. Child abuse review, 29(3), 282290.Google Scholar
Bernard, K., Simons, R., & Dozier, M. (2015). Effects of an attachment‐based intervention on Child Protective Services–referred mothers’ event‐related potentials to children’s emotions. Child Development, 86(6), 16731684.Google Scholar
Bjertrup, A. J., Friis, N. K., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2019). The maternal brain: neural responses to infants in mothers with and without mood disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 107, 196207.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Rigo, P., Esposito, G., Swain, J. E., Suwalsky, J. T., Su, X., Du, X., Zhang, K., Cote, L. R., De Pisapia, N., & Venuti, P. (2017). Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(45), E9465E9473.Google Scholar
Brunton, P. J., & Russell, J. A. (2008). The expectant brain: adapting for motherhood. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 1125.Google Scholar
Capistrano, C. G., Grande, L. A., McRae, K., Phan, K. L., & Kim, P. (2022). Maternal socioeconomic disadvantage, neural function during volitional emotion regulation, and parenting. Social Neuroscience, 17(3) 276292. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2022.2082521.Google Scholar
Dudek, J., Colasante, T., Zuffianò, A., & Haley, D. W. (2020). Changes in cortical sensitivity to infant facial cues from pregnancy to motherhood predict mother–infant bonding. Child Development, 91(1), e198e217. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13182Google Scholar
Endendijk, J. J., Spencer, H., van Baar, A. L., & Bos, P. A. (2018). Mothers’ neural responses to infant faces are associated with activation of the maternal care system and observed intrusiveness with their own child. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 609621.Google Scholar
Etkin, A., Büchel, C., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693700.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2015). The adaptive human parental brain: Implications for children’s social development. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(6), 387399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.004Google Scholar
Feldman, R., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2017). Oxytocin: A parenting hormone. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 1318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firk, C., Dahmen, B., Lehmann, C., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2018). Down-regulation of amygdala response to infant crying: A role for distraction in maternal emotion regulation. Emotion, 18(3), 412423.Google Scholar
Glocker, M., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115(3), 257263.Google Scholar
Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010). Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 68(4), 377382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005Google Scholar
Grande, L. A., Olsavsky, A. K., Erhart, A., Dufford, A. J., Tribble, R., Phan, K. L., & Kim, P. (2021). Postpartum stress and neural regulation of emotion among first-time mothers. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(5), 10661082.Google Scholar
Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2018). Mothers’ neural and behavioral responses to their infants’ distress cues: The role of secure base script knowledge. Psychological Science, 29(2), 242253.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362.Google Scholar
Hipwell, A. E., Guo, C., Phillips, M. L., Swain, J. E., & Moses-Kolko, E. L. (2015). Right frontoinsular cortex and subcortical activity to infant cry is associated with maternal mental state talk. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(37), 1272512732.Google Scholar
Hoekzema, E., Barba-Müller, E., Pozzobon, C., Picado, M., Lucco, F., García-García, D., Soliva, J. C., Tobeña, A., Desco, M., Crone, E. A., Ballesteros, A., Carmona, S., & Vilarroya, O. (2017). Pregnancy leads to long-lasting changes in human brain structure. Nature Neuroscience, 20(2), 287296. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4458Google Scholar
Kim, P., Capistrano, C., & Congleton, C. (2016). Socioeconomic disadvantages and neural sensitivity to infant cry: Role of maternal distress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 15971607.Google Scholar
Kim, P., Feldman, R., Mayes, L. C., Eicher, V., Thompson, N., Leckman, J. F., & Swain, J. E. (2011). Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(8), 907915. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02406.xGoogle Scholar
Kim, P., Leckman, J. F., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Wang, X., & Swain, J. E. (2010). The plasticity of human maternal brain: Longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124(5), 695700. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020884Google Scholar
Kim, P., Leckman, J. F., Mayes, L. C., Newman, M. A., Feldman, R., & Swain, J. E. (2010). Perceived quality of maternal care in childhood and structure and function of mothers’ brain. Developmental Science, 13(4), 662673.Google Scholar
Kim, P., Tribble, R., Olsavsky, A. K., Dufford, A. J., Erhart, A., Hansen, M.,Grande, L., & Gonzalez, D. M. (2020). Associations between stress exposure and new mothers’ brain responses to infant cry sounds. Neuroimage, 223, 117360.Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2016). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(7), 545558.Google Scholar
Kuzava, S., Frost, A., Perrone, L., Kang, E., Lindhiem, O., & Bernard, K. (2020). Adult processing of child emotional expressions: A meta-analysis of ERP studies. Developmental Psychology, 56(6), 11701190.Google Scholar
Kuzava, S., Nissim, G., Frost, A., Nelson, B., & Bernard, K. (2019). Latent profiles of maternal neural response to infant emotional stimuli: Associations with maternal sensitivity. Biological Psychology, 143, 113120.Google Scholar
Laurent, H. K., & Ablow, J. C. (2012). The missing link: Mothers’ neural response to infant cry related to infant attachment behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 761772.Google Scholar
Lisofsky, N., Gallinat, J., Lindenberger, U., & Kühn, S. (2019). Postpartal neural plasticity of the maternal brain: Early renormalization of pregnancy-related decreases? Neurosignals, 27, 1224.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung [The innate forms of potential experience]. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 5, 233519.Google Scholar
Lowell, A. F., Dell, J., Potenza, M. N., Strathearn, L., Mayes, L. C., & Rutherford, H. J. (2021). Adult attachment is related to maternal neural response to infant cues: An ERP study. Attachment & human development, 1–18.Google Scholar
Luders, E., Kurth, F., Gingnell, M., Engman, J., Yong, E.-L., Poromaa, I. S., & Gaser, C. (2020). From baby brain to mommy brain: Widespread gray matter gain after giving birth. Cortex, 126, 334342.Google Scholar
Martínez-García, M., Paternina-Die, M., Barba-Müller, E., Martín de Blas, D., Beumala, L., Cortizo, R., Pozzobon, C., Marcos-Vidal, L., Fernández-Pena, A., Picado, M., Belmonte-Padilla, E., Massó-Rodriguez, A., Ballesteros, A., Desco, M., Vilarroya, Ó., Hoekzema, E., & Carmona, S. (2021). Do pregnancy-induced brain changes reverse? The brain of a mother six years after parturition. Brain sciences, 11(2), 168.Google Scholar
Maupin, A. N., Hayes, N., Mayes, L., & Rutherford, H. J. V. (2015). The application of electroencephalography to investigate the neural basis of parenting. Parenting: Science and Practice, 15(1), 923. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2015.992735Google Scholar
Mayes, L., Rutherford, H. J. V., Suchman, N., & Close, N. (2012). The neural and psychological dynamics of adults’ transition to parenthood. Zero to Three, 33(2), 8384.Google Scholar
Musser, E. D., Kaiser-Laurent, H., & Ablow, J. C. (2012). The neural correlates of maternal sensitivity: An fMRI study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(4), 428436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.04.003Google Scholar
Oatridge, A., Holdcroft, A., Saeed, N., Hajnal, J. V., Puri, B. K., Fusi, L., & Bydder, G. M. (2002). Change in brain size during and after pregnancy: Study in healthy women and women with preeclampsia. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 23(1), 1926.Google Scholar
Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2017). Intuitive parenting: understanding the neural mechanisms of parents’ adaptive responses to infants. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 4044.Google Scholar
Paul, S., Austin, J., Elliott, R., Ellison-Wright, I., Wan, M. W., Drake, R., Downey, D., Elmadih, A., Mukherjee, I., Heaney, L., Williams, S., & Abel, K. M. (2019). Neural pathways of maternal responding: systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 22(2), 179187.Google Scholar
Pawluski, J. L., Hoekzema, E., Leuner, B., & Lonstein, J. S. (2021). Less can be more: Fine tuning the maternal brain. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.045Google Scholar
Peltola, M. J., Strathearn, L., & Puura, K. (2018). Oxytocin promotes face-sensitive neural responses to infant and adult faces in mothers. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 91, 261270.Google Scholar
Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., Puura, K., Proverbio, A. M., Mononen, N., Lehtimäki, T., & Leppänen, J. M. (2014). Motherhood and oxytocin receptor genetic variation are associated with selective changes in electrocortical responses to infant facial expressions. Emotion, 14(3), 469477.Google Scholar
Penner, F., & Rutherford, H. J. (2022). Emotion regulation during pregnancy: A call to action for increased research, screening, and intervention. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 25(2), 527531.Google Scholar
Penner, F., Wall, K., Guan, K., Huang, H., Richardson, L., Dunbar, A., Groh, A. M., & Rutherford, H. (2023). Racial disparities in EEG research and their implications for our understanding of the maternal brain. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 23, 116. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-022-01040-wGoogle Scholar
Proverbio, A. M., Brignone, V., Matarazzo, S., Del Zotto, M., & Zani, A. (2006). Gender and parental status affect the visual cortical response to infant facial expression. Neuropsychologia, 44(14), 29872999. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.015Google Scholar
Purhonen, M., Kilpeläinen-Lees, R., Pääkkönen, A., Yppärilä, H., Lehtonen, J., & Karhu, J. (2001). Effects of maternity on auditory event-related potentials to human sound. Neuroreport, 12(13), 29752979. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200109170-00044Google Scholar
Riem, M. M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Pieper, S., Tops, M., Boksem, M. A., Vermeiren, R. R., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Rombouts, S. A. (2011). Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: A randomized controlled trial. Biological Psychiatry, 70(3), 291297.Google Scholar
Rigo, P., Kim, P., Esposito, G., Putnick, D. L., Venuti, P., & Bornstein, M. H. (2019). Specific maternal brain responses to their own child’s face: An fMRI meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 51, 5869.Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K. (2013). The neural and hormonal bases of human parental care. Neuropsychologia, 51(4), 731747.Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Booth, C. R., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2015). Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 5463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.04.005Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Byrne, S. P., Austin, G. M., Lee, J. D., Crowley, M. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Anxiety and neural responses to infant and adult faces during pregnancy. Biological Psychology, 125, 115120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.03.002Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Goldberg, B., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2013). Parental reflective functioning is associated with tolerance of infant distress but not general distress: Evidence for a specific relationship using a simulated baby paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 635641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.008Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Guo, X. M., Graber, K. M., Hayes, N. J., Pelphrey, K. A., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Intranasal oxytocin and the neural correlates of infant face processing in non-parent women. Biological Psychology, 129, 4548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.002Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Kim, S., Yip, S. W., Potenza, M. N., Mayes, L. C., & Strathearn, L. (2021). Parenting and addictions: Current insights from human neuroscience. Current addiction reports, 8(3), 380388.Google Scholar
Rutherford, H., Wallace, N. S., Laurent, H. K., & Mayes, L. C. (2015). Emotion regulation in parenthood. Developmental Review, 36, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.008Google Scholar
Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J. G., Luthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, G., von Bardeleben, U., Radue, E. W., Cirillo, S., Tedeschi, G., & Di Salle, F. (2003). Differential sex-independent amygdala response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological Psychiatry, 54(12), 13671375.Google Scholar
Squire, S., & Stein, A. (2003). Functional MRI and parental responsiveness: a new avenue into parental psychopathology and early parent–child interactions? The British Journal of Psychiatry, 183(6), 481483. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.183.6.481Google Scholar
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(13), 26552666. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.103Google Scholar
Swain, J. E. (2011). The human parental brain: In vivo neuroimaging. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35(5), 12421254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.017Google Scholar
Swain, J. E., Tasgin, E., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Todd Constable, R., & Leckman, J. F. (2008). Maternal brain response to own baby-cry is affected by cesarean section delivery. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(10), 10421052. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01963.xGoogle Scholar
Witteman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M., Rilling, J., Bos, P., Schiller, N., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. (2019). Towards a neural model of infant cry perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 99, 2332.Google Scholar
Yatziv, T., Vancor, E. A., Bunderson, M., & Rutherford, H. J. (2021). Maternal perinatal anxiety and neural responding to infant affective signals: Insights, challenges, and a road map for neuroimaging research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 131, 387399.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, R. (1999). Emotional dissonance in organizations: Conceptualizing the roles of self‐esteem and job‐induced tension. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 20(1), 1825. https://doi.org/10.1108/01437739910251152Google Scholar
Adams, G. A., & Webster, J. R. (2013). Emotional regulation as a mediator between interpersonal mistreatment and distress. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(6), 697710. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2012.698057Google Scholar
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18(1), 88115. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1993.3997508Google Scholar
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2005). Personality, well-being and health correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(3), 547558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.05.009Google Scholar
Bai, S., Repetti, R. L., & Sperling, J. B. (2016). Children’s expressions of positive emotion are sustained by smiling, touching, and playing with parents and siblings: A naturalistic observational study of family life. Developmental Psychology, 52(1), 88101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039854Google Scholar
Barkawi, T. (2006). Globalization and war. Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Bono, J. E., & Vey, M. A. (2005). Toward understanding emotional management at work: A quantitative review of emotional labor research. In Härtel, C. E., Zerbe, W. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (Eds.), Emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 213233). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 4. Ecological settings and processes (7th ed., pp. 55132). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Brianda, M. E., Roskam, I., Gross, J. J., Franssen, A., Kapala, F., Gérard, F., & Mikolajczak, M. (2020). Treating parental burnout: Impact of two treatment modalities on burnout symptoms, emotions, hair cortisol, and parental neglect and violence. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 330–332. https://doi.org/10.1159/000506354Google Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A. (2002). Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives of “people work.” Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60(1), 1739. https://doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001.1815Google Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7(1), 5767. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.7.1.57Google Scholar
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598Google Scholar
Chen, Y., Haines, J., Charlton, B. M., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2019). Positive parenting improves multiple aspects of health and well-being in young adulthood. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 684691. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0602-xGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 187211. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259Google Scholar
Cropanzano, R., Weiss, H. M., & Elias, S. M. (2003). The impact of display rules and emotional labor on psychological well-being at work. In Perrewe, P. L. & Ganster, D. C. (Eds.), Emotional and physiological processes and positive intervention strategies (Vol. 3, pp. 4589). Emerald Group Publishing Limited. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1479-3555(03)03002-6Google Scholar
Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2014). Evaluative and hedonic wellbeing among those with and without children at home. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(4), 13281333. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1311600111Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Croyle, M. H. (2006). Are emotional display rules formal job requirements? Examination of employee and supervisor perceptions. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79(2), 273298. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317905X68484Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Yang, J. (2008). Linking emotion regulation strategies to affective events and negative emotions at work. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 73(3), 498508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2008.09.006Google Scholar
Dijk, P. A. V., & Brown, A. K. (2006). Emotional labour and negative job outcomes: An evaluation of the mediating role of emotional dissonance. Journal of Management & Organization, 12(2), 101115. https://doi.org/10.5172/jmo.2006.12.2.101Google Scholar
Dix, T., Gershoff, E. T., Meunier, L. N., & Miller, P. C. (2004). The affective structure of supportive parenting: Depressive symptoms, immediate emotions, and child-oriented motivation. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 12121227. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1212Google Scholar
Douglas, S. J., & Michaels, M. W. (2004). The mommy myth: The idealization of motherhood and how it has undermined all women. Free Press.Google Scholar
Duncombe, M. E., Havighurst, S. S., Holland, K. A., & Frankling, E. J. (2012). The contribution of parenting practices and parent emotion factors in children at risk for disruptive behavior disorders. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43(5), 715733. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0290-5Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 22272247.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child Development, 70(2), 513534.Google Scholar
Furedi, F. (2002). Paranoid parenting: Why ignoring the experts may be best for your child. Chicago Review Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1994). Living in a post-traditional society. In Beck, U., Giddens, A., & Lash, S., Reflexive modernization: Politics, tradition and aesthetics in the modern social order (pp. 56109). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1999). Risk and responsibility. Modern Law Review, 62(1), 110.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Doubleday.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 95110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: Surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46(1), 8696. https://doi.org/10.5465/30040678Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads: Where do we go from here? Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2(1), 323349. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Melloy, R. C. (2017). The state of the heart: Emotional labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22(3), 407422. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000067Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Sayre, G. M. (2019). Emotional labor: Regulating emotions for a wage. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(2), 131137. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418812771Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Diefendorffand, J. M., & Rupp, D. E. (Eds.). (2013). Emotional labor in the 21st century: Diverse perspectives on the psychology of emotion regulation at work. Routledge.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., Mattila, A. S., Jansen, K. J., & Sideman, L. A. (2005). Is “service with a smile” enough? Authenticity of positive displays during service encounters. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96(1), 3855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.08.002Google Scholar
Habib, I. (1995). Capitalism in history. Social Scientist, 23(7–9), 1531. https://doi.org/10.2307/3517858Google Scholar
Hajal, N. J., & Paley, B. (2020). Parental emotion and emotion regulation: A critical target of study for research and intervention to promote child emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 403417. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000864Google Scholar
Hartel, C. E. J., Hsu, A. C. F., & Boyle, M. V. (2002). A conceptual examination of the causal sequences of emotional labor, emotional dissonance, and emotional exhaustion: The argument for the role of contextual and provider characteristics. In Ashkanasy, N. M., Zerbe, W. J., & Hartel, C. E. J. (Eds.), Managing emotions in the workplace (pp. 251275). M. E. Sharpe.Google Scholar
Hays, S. (1996). The cultural contradictions of motherhood. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hendrick, H. (2016). Narcissistic parenting in an insecure world: A history of parenting culture 1920s to present. Policy Press. https://doi.org/10.1332/policypress/9781447322559.001.0001Google Scholar
Hobsbawm, E. (1995). The age of extremes: A history of the world, 1914–1991. Abacus.Google Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hoghughi, M. (2004). Parenting – an introduction. In Hoghughi, M. & Long, N. (Eds.), Handbook of parenting: Theory and research for practice (pp. 118). Sage.Google Scholar
Holman, D., Martinez-Iñigo, D., & Totterdell, P. (2008). Emotional labour and employee well-being: An integrative review. In Ashkanasy, N. M. & Cooper, C. L. (Eds.), Research companion to emotion in organizations (pp. 315340). Edward Elgar Publishing. https://www.elgaronline.com/view/9781845426378.00029.xmlGoogle Scholar
Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16(3), 361389. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022876Google Scholar
Huppertz, A. V., Ute, R. H., Velozo, J. D. C., & Schreurs, B. H. (2020). Why do emotional labor strategies differentially predict exhaustion?: Comparing psychological effort, authenticity, and relational mechanisms. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 25(3), 214226. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000179Google Scholar
Jansz, J., & Timmers, M. (2002). Emotional dissonance: When the experience of an emotion jeopardizes an individual’s identity. Theory & Psychology, 12(1), 7995. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354302121005Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 17761780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572Google Scholar
Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Rubenstein, A. L., Long, D. M., Odio, M. A., Buckman, B. R., Zhang, Y., & Halvorsen-Ganepola, M. D. K. (2013). A Meta-analytic structural model of dispositional affectivity and emotional labor. Personnel Psychology, 66(1), 4790. https://doi.org/10.1111/peps.12009Google Scholar
Karnilowicz, H. R., Waters, S. F., & Mendes, W. B. (2019). Not in front of the kids: Effects of parental suppression on socialization behaviors during cooperative parent–child interactions. Emotion, 19(7), 11831191. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000527Google Scholar
Kerr, M. L., Rasmussen, H. F., Buttitta, K. V., Smiley, P. A., & Borelli, J. L. (2021). Exploring the complexity of mothers’ real-time emotions while caregiving. Emotion, 21(3), 545556. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000719Google Scholar
Kettenacker, L., & Riotte, T. (2011). The legacies of two world wars: European societies in the twentieth century. Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Korczynski, M. (2005). Skills in service work: An overview. Human Resource Management Journal, 15(2), 314.Google Scholar
Kruml, S. M., & Geddes, D. (2000). Exploring the dimensions of emotional labor: The heart of Hochschild’s work. Management Communication Quarterly, 14(1), 849. https://doi.org/10.1177/0893318900141002Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132Google Scholar
Le, B. M., & Impett, E. A. (2016). The costs of suppressing negative emotions and amplifying positive emotions during parental caregiving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(3), 323336. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216629122Google Scholar
Lee, E. (2014a). Experts and parenting culture. In Lee, E., Bristow, J., Faircloth, C., & Macvarish, J. (Eds.), Parenting culture studies (pp. 5175). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137304612_3Google Scholar
Lee, E. (2014b). Introduction. In Lee, E., Bristow, J., Faircloth, C., & Macvarish, J. (Eds.), Parenting culture studies (pp. 122). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137304612_1Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., & Szczygieł, D. (2022). Perfectionistic parents are burnt out by hiding emotions from their children, but this effect is attenuated by emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 184, 111187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111187Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Goldenberg, A., Arikan, G., Brytek-Matera, A., Czepczor-Bernat, K., Manrique-Millones, D., Mikolajczak, M., Overbye, H., Roskam, I., Szczygieł, D., Ustundag-Budak, A. M., & Gross, J. J. (2022). Reappraisal, social support, and parental burnout. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(4), 10891102. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12380Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Hansotte, L., Szczygieł, D., Meeussen, L., Roskam, I., & Mikolajczak, M. (2021). Parenting with a smile: Display rules, regulatory effort, and parental burnout. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(9), 27012721. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075211019124Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Mikolajczak, M., Keller, H., Akgun, E., Arikan, G., Aunola, K., Barham, E., Besson, E., Blanchard, M. A., Boujut, E., Brianda, M. E., Brytek-Matera, A., César, F., Chen, B.-B., Dorard, G., dos Santos Elias, L. C., Dunsmuir, S., Egorova, N., Escobar, M. J., … Roskam, I. (2023). Parenting culture(s): Ideal-parent beliefs across 37 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 54(1), 424. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221123043Google Scholar
Luong, A. (2005). Affective service display and customer mood. Journal of Service Research, 8(2), 117130. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094670505279341Google Scholar
Manassis, K., Bradley, S., Goldberg, S., Hood, J., & Swinson, R. P. (1994). Attachment in mothers with anxiety disorders and their children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(8), 11061113. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199410000-00006Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2012). Culture and emotion: The integration of biological and cultural contributions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 91118. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111420147Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In Salovey, P. & Sluyter, D. J. (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 331). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., & Wax, A. (2012). Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting: A discordance–congruence perspective. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(1), 653. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386611417746Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Brianda, M. E., Avalosse, H., & Roskam, I. (2018). Consequences of parental burnout: Its specific effect on child neglect and violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 80, 134145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.025Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., & Roskam, I. (2019). Parental burnout: What is it and why does it matter? Clinical Psychological Science, 7(6), 13191329.Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Menil, C., & Luminet, O. (2007). Explaining the protective effect of trait emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional labour processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), 11071117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.01.003Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Tran, V., Brotheridge, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Chapter 11: Using an emotion regulation framework to predict the outcomes of emotional labor. In Charmine, E. J. H, Neal, M. A, & Wilfred, J. Z (Eds.), Emotions in groups, organizations and cultures (Vol. 5, pp. 245273). Emerald Group Publishing Limited. https://doi.org/10.1108/S1746-9791(2009)0000005013Google Scholar
Minnotte, K. L., Pedersen, D. E., Mannon, S. E., & Kiger, G. (2010). Tending to the emotions of children: Predicting parental performance of emotion work with children. Marriage & Family Review, 46(3), 224241. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2010.490199Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., & Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 233238. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12238Google Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 9861010. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1996.9704071861Google Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1997). Managing emotions in the workplace. Journal of Managerial Issues, 9(3), 257274.Google Scholar
National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities. (2021). Positive parenting tips. National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/childdevelopment/positiveparenting/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Nomaguchi, K., & Milkie, M. A. (2020). Parenthood and well-being: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and Family, 82(1), 198223. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12646Google Scholar
Olszanowski, M., Wróbel, M., & Hess, U. (2020). Mimicking and sharing emotions: A re-examination of the link between facial mimicry and emotional contagion. Cognition and Emotion, 34(2), 367376. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1611543Google Scholar
Overy, R. (2009). The morbid age: Britain and the crisis of civilisation, 1919–1939. Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44(5), 10181027. https://doi.org/10.5465/3069445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pursell, C. (2007). Technology in postwar America: A history. Columbia University Press. https://doi.org/10.7312/purs12304Google Scholar
Rafaeli, A., & Sutton, R. I. (1990). Busy stores and demanding customers: How do they affect the display of positive emotion? Academy of Management Journal, 33(3), 623637. https://doi.org/10.5465/256584Google Scholar
Rodrigo, M. J. (2010). Promoting positive parenting in Europe: New challenges for the European society for developmental psychology. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(3), 281294. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405621003780200Google Scholar
Rubin, R. S., Tardino, V. M. S., Daus, C. S., & Munz, D. C. (2005). A reconceptualization of the emotional labor construct: On the development of an integrated theory of perceived emotional dissonance and emotional labor. In Hartel, C. E. J., Zerbe, W. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (Eds.), Emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 189211). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rueger, S. Y., Katz, R. L., Risser, H. J., & Lovejoy, M. C. (2011). Relations between parental affect and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Parenting, 11(1), 133. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2011.539503Google Scholar
Schaubroeck, J., & Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emotional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(2), 163183. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<163::AID-JOB37>3.0.CO;2-LGoogle Scholar
Scott, B. A., & Barnes, C. M. (2011). A multilevel field investigation of emotional labor, affect, work withdrawal, and gender. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 116136. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.59215086Google Scholar
Sliter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). How rude! Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15(4), 468481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020723Google Scholar
Sutton, R. I., & Rafaeli, A. (1988). Untangling the relationship between displayed emotions and organizational sales: The case of convenience stores. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3), 461487. https://doi.org/10.5465/256456Google Scholar
Szczygieł, D., & Bazińska, R. (2021). Emotional intelligence mitigates the effects of customer incivility on surface acting and exhaustion in service occupations: A moderated mediation model. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.506085Google Scholar
Thoits, P. A. (2004). Emotion norms, emotion work, and social order. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium (pp. 359378). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2003). Emotion regulation in customer service roles: Testing a model of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(1), 5573. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.8.1.55Google Scholar
Tsai, W.-C. (2001). Determinants and consequences of employee displayed positive emotions. Journal of Management, 27(4), 497512. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920630102700406Google Scholar
United Nations General Assembly. (1948). Universal declaration of human rights. http://www.ohchr.org/EN/UDHR/Documents/UDHR_Translations/eng.pdfGoogle Scholar
von Scheve, C. (2012). Emotion regulation and emotion work: Two sides of the same coin? Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00496Google Scholar
Waters, S. F., Karnilowicz, H. R., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2020). Keep it to yourself? Parent emotion suppression influences physiological linkage and interaction behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 34(7), 784793. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000664Google Scholar
Wharton, A. S., & Erickson, R. J. (1993). Managing emotions on the job and at home: Understanding the consequences of multiple emotional roles. The Academy of Management Review, 18(3), 457486. https://doi.org/10.2307/258905Google Scholar
Wharton, A. S., & Erickson, R. J. (1995). The consequences of caring: Exploring the links between women’s job and family emotion work. The Sociological Quarterly, 36(2), 273296.Google Scholar
Zapf, D., & Holz, M. (2006). On the positive and negative effects of emotion work in organizations. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1080/13594320500412199Google Scholar
Zerbe, W. J. (2000). Emotional dissonance and employee well-being. In Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice (pp. 189214). Quorum Books/Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Zhang, X., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Cole, P. M., & Ram, N. (2023). The role of strategy-use and parasympathetic functioning in maternal emotion regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 37(1), 110120. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0001017Google Scholar
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Rudolph, J., Kerin, J., & Bohadana-Brown, G. (2022). Parent emotional regulation: A meta-analytic review of its association with parenting and child adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 46(1), 6382. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254211051086Google Scholar

References

Ateah, C. A., & Durrant, J. E. (2005). Maternal use of physical punishment in response to child misbehavior: Implications for child abuse prevention. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(2), 169185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.10.010Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviourism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 925945. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.126.6.925Google Scholar
Baylin, J. (2017). Social buffering and compassionate stories: The neuroscience of trust building with children in care. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy, 38, 606612. https://doi.org/10.1002/anzf.1272Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bögels, S., Lehtonen, A., & Restifo, K. (2010). Mindful parenting in mental health care. Mindfulness, 1(2), 107120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0014-5Google Scholar
Campbell-Sills, L., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In Gross, J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542559). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Mumme, D. L., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. G. (1994). A functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 284303.Google Scholar
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 560572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005Google Scholar
Colegrove, V. M., Havighurst, S. S., Kehoe, C. E., & Jacobsen, S. L. (2018). Pilot randomized controlled trial of Tuning Relationships with Music: Intervention for parents with a trauma history and their adolescent. Child Abuse & Neglect, 79, 259268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.02.017Google Scholar
Cozolino, L. (2010). The neuroscience of psychotherapy: Healing the social brain. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
da Silva, T. L., Ravindran, L. N., & Ravindran, A. V. (2009). Yoga in the treatment of mood and anxiety disorders: A review. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 2(1), 616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2008.12.002Google Scholar
Dix, T., Gershoff, E. T., Meunier, L. N., & Miller, P. C. (2004). The affective structure of supportive parenting: Depressive symptoms, immediate emotions, and child-oriented motivation. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 12121227.Google Scholar
Dumas, J. E. (2005). Mindfulness-based parent training: Strategies to lessen the grip of automaticity in families with disruptive children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 34(4), 779791. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3404_20Google Scholar
Duncan, L. G., Coatsworth, J. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2009). A model of mindful parenting: Implications for parent-child relationships and prevention research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 12(3), 255270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-009-0046-3Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1Google Scholar
English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in daily life: The role of social context and goals. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 230242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9597-zGoogle Scholar
Ensink, K., Maheux, J., Normandin, L., Sabourin, S., Diguer, L., Berthelot, N., & Parent, K. (2013). The impact of mentalization training on the reflective function of novice therapists: A randomized controlled trial. Psychotherapy Research: Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, 23(5), 526538. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2013.800950Google Scholar
Farb, N. A. S., Anderson, A. K., Irving, J. A., & Segal, Z. V. (2015). Mindfulness interventions and emotion regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 548568). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. Other Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(3), 243268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243Google Scholar
Greenberg, L. S., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emotion in psychotherapy: A practice-friendly research review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 611630. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20252Google Scholar
Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1989). Emotion in psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 44(1), 1929. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.1.19Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Journal of General Psychology, 2, 271299.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hajal, N. J., & Paley, B. (2020). Parental emotion and emotion regulation: A critical target of study for research and intervention to promote child emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 403417. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000864Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Duncombe, M. E., Frankling, E. J., Holland, K. A., Kehoe, C. E., & Stargatt, R. (2015). An emotion-focused early intervention for children with emerging conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(4), 749760. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9944-zGoogle Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., & Kehoe, C. E. (2017). The role of parental emotion regulation in parent emotion socialization: Implications for intervention. In Deater-Deckard, K. & Panneton, R. (Eds.), Parental stress and early child development: Adaptive and maladaptive outcomes (pp. 285307). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55376-4_12Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Radovini, A., Hao, B., & Kehoe, C. E. (2020). Emotion-focused parenting interventions for prevention and treatment of child and adolescent mental health problems: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 33(6), 586601. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000647Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Kehoe, C. E., Efron, D., & Prior, M. R. (2013). “Tuning in to Kids”: Reducing young children’s behavior problems using an emotion coaching parenting program. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44(2), 247264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0322-1Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Prior, M. R., & Kehoe, C. E. (2010). Tuning in to Kids: Improving emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children – findings from a community trial. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 51, 13421350. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02303.xGoogle Scholar
Hawton, K., Salkovskis, P. M., Kirk, J., & Clark, D. M. (1989). Cognitive behaviour therapy for psychiatric problems: A practical guide. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1994). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Altering the verbal support for experiential avoidance. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 289303.Google Scholar
Hill, C. L. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion, 12(1), 8190. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026355Google Scholar
Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G., & Powell, B. (2006). Changing toddlers’ and preschoolers’ attachment classifications: The Circle of Security Intervention. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 74, 10171026. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1017Google Scholar
Hughes, D. A., & Baylin, J. (2012). Brain-based parenting: The neuroscience of caregiving for healthy attachment. Norton.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. M., & Lee, A. C. (2000). Emotionally focused family therapy: Restructuring attachment In Bailey, C. E. (Ed.), Children in therapy: Using the family as a resource (pp. 112136). Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Jugovac, S., O’Kearney, R., Hawes, D. J., & Pasalich, D. S. (2022). Attachment- and emotion-focused parenting interventions for child and adolescent externalizing and internalizing behaviors: A meta-analysis. Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review, 25, 754773. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-022-00401-8Google Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An out-patient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 3347.Google Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144156. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg016Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kaitz, M., & Maytal, H. (2005). Interactions between anxious mothers and their infants: An integration of theory and research findings. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26(6), 570597. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20069Google Scholar
Katz, L. F., Gurtovenko, K., Maliken, A., Stettler, N., Kawamura, J., & Fladeboe, K. (2020). An emotion coaching parenting intervention for families exposed to intimate partner violence. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 638651. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000800Google Scholar
Kehoe, C. E., & Havighurst, S. S. (2018). Treating emotion dysregulation in internalizing disorders. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Crowell, S. E. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 443462). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190689285.013.31Google Scholar
Kehoe, C. E., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2020). Tuning in to Teens: Investigating moderators of program effects and mechanisms of change of an emotion focused group parenting program. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 623637. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000875Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 467480.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L. (2015). Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal, and the process of change in psychotherapy: New insights from brain science. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 38, E1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000041Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Emotion and the amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction (pp. 339351). Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1998). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Leerkes, E. M., & Crockenberg, S. C. (2006). Antecedents of mothers’ emotional and cognitive responses to infant distress: The role of family, mother, and infant characteristics. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27(4), 405428. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20099Google Scholar
Leung, D. W., & Slep, A. M. S. (2006). Predicting inept discipline: The role of parental depressive symptoms, anger, and attributions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(3), 524534. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.524Google Scholar
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lippold, M. A., Duncan, L. G., Coatsworth, J. D., Nix, R. L., & Greenberg, M. T. (2015). Understanding how mindful parenting may be linked to mother–adolescent communication. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(9), 16631673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0325-xGoogle Scholar
Longacre, M., Silver-Highfield, E., Lama, P., & Grodin, M. (2012). Complementary and alternative medicine in the treatment of refugees and survivors of torture: A review and proposal for action. Torture, 22(1), 3857.Google Scholar
Maliken, A. C., & Katz, L. F. (2013). Exploring the impact of parental psychopathology and emotion regulation on evidence-based parenting interventions: A transdiagnostic approach to improving treatment effectiveness. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 16(2), 173186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0132-4Google Scholar
McRae, K., Misra, S., Prasad, A. K., Pereira, S. C., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Bottom-up and top-down emotion generation: Implications for emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(3), 253262. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq103Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.xGoogle Scholar
Neacsiu, A. D., Bohus, M., & Linehan, M. M. (2015). Dialectic behavior therapy: An intervention for emotion dysregulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 491507). Guilford.Google Scholar
Ogden, P., Minton, K., & Pain, C. (2006). Trauma and the body: A sensorimotor approach to psychotherapy. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Paivio, S. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (2001). Introduction: Treating emotion regulation problems. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(2), 153155. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(200102)57:2<153::AID-JCLP2>3.0.CO;2-FGoogle Scholar
Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. Psychophysiology, 32, 301318.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (2009). Reciprocal influences between body and brain in the perception and expression of affect: A polyvagal perspective. In Fosha, D., Siegel, D. J., & Solomon, M. (Eds.), The healing power of emotion: Affective neuroscience, development, and clinical practice (pp. 2754). Norton.Google Scholar
Rueger, S. Y., Katz, R. L., Risser, H. J., & Lovejoy, M. C. (2011). Relations between parental affect and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Parenting: Science and Practice, 11(1), 133. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2011.539503Google Scholar
Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Scott, S., & Dadds, M. R. (2009). Practitioner review: When parent training doesn’t work: theory-driven clinical strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50(12), 14411450. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02161.xGoogle Scholar
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, T. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shaffer, A., Fitzgerald, M. M., Shipman, K., & Torres, M. (2019). Let’s Connect: A developmentally-driven emotion-focused parenting intervention. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 63, 3341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2019.05.007Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2007). Better late than never? On the dynamics of online regulation of sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(11), 15181532. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207305537Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22(11), 13911396. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611418350Google Scholar
Slade, A. (2007). Reflective parenting programs: Theory and development. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 26(4), 640657. https://doi.org/10.1080/07351690701310698Google Scholar
Smith, R., & Lane, R. D. (2015). The neural basis of one’s own conscious and unconscious emotional states. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 57, 129. https://doi.org/10.1037/EMO0000376Google Scholar
Smith, R., & Lane, R. D. (2016). Unconscious emotions: A cognitive neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 69, 216238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j/neubiorev.2016/08.013Google Scholar
Sullivan, M. B., Erb, M., Schmalzl, L., Moonaz, S., Noggle Taylor, J., & Porges, S. W. (2018). Yoga therapy and polyvagal theory: The convergence of traditional wisdom and contemporary neuroscience for self-regulation and resilience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12(67). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00067Google Scholar
Telles, S., Singh, N., & Balkrishna, A. (2012). Managing mental health disorders resulting from trauma through yoga: A review. Depression Research and Treatment, Article ID 401513. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/401513Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In Thompson, R. (Ed.), Socioemotional development (pp. 367467). University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Wylie, M. S., Colasante, T., De France, K., Lin, L., & Hollenstein, T. (2022). Momentary emotion regulation strategy use and success: Testing the influences of emotion intensity and habitual strategy use. Emotion, 23(2), 375386. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001074Google Scholar
Zubrick, S. R., Ward, K. A., Silburn, S. R., Lawrence, D., Williams, A. A., Blair, E., Robertson, D., & Sanders, M. R. (2005). Prevention of child behaviour problems through universal implementation of a group behavioural family intervention. Prevention Science, 6(4), 287304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-005-0013-2Google Scholar

References

Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39, 263278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4Google Scholar
Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 591612. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613504116Google Scholar
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15(7), 482487. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.xGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 11.1

Figure 1

Figure 11.1

CC BY 4.0 SARAH THOMAS
Figure 2

Figure 13.1 Emotional labor framework in the job context

Figure 3

Figure 13.2 Emotional labor framework in the parenting context

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Current Trends
  • Edited by Isabelle Roskam, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium, James J. Gross, University of California, Berkeley, Moïra Mikolajczak, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
  • Book: Emotion Regulation and Parenting
  • Online publication: 05 January 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304368.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Current Trends
  • Edited by Isabelle Roskam, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium, James J. Gross, University of California, Berkeley, Moïra Mikolajczak, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
  • Book: Emotion Regulation and Parenting
  • Online publication: 05 January 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304368.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Current Trends
  • Edited by Isabelle Roskam, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium, James J. Gross, University of California, Berkeley, Moïra Mikolajczak, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
  • Book: Emotion Regulation and Parenting
  • Online publication: 05 January 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304368.014
Available formats
×