Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T09:11:26.384Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Concurrent Impairments in Sleep and Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2012

Carmen E. Westerberg*
Affiliation:
Psychology Department and Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois Psychology Department, Texas State University, San Marcos, Texas
Bryce A. Mander
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, University of California, Berkeley, California
Susan M. Florczak
Affiliation:
Psychology Department and Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
Sandra Weintraub
Affiliation:
Psychology Department and Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Center, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois Neurology Department, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois
M.-Marsel Mesulam
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Center, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois Neurology Department, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois
Phyllis C. Zee
Affiliation:
Neurology Department, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois
Ken A. Paller
Affiliation:
Psychology Department and Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Carmen E. Westerberg, Department of Psychology, Texas State University, San Marcos, 601 University Drive, San Marcos, TX 78666. E-mail: cw54@txstate.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Whereas patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) experience difficulties forming and retrieving memories, their memory impairments may also partially reflect an unrecognized dysfunction in sleep-dependent consolidation that normally stabilizes declarative memory storage across cortical areas. Patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) exhibit circumscribed declarative memory deficits, and many eventually progress to an AD diagnosis. Whether sleep is disrupted in aMCI and whether sleep disruptions contribute to memory impairment is unknown. We measured sleep physiology and memory for two nights and found that aMCI patients had fewer stage-2 spindles than age-matched healthy adults. Furthermore, aMCI patients spent less time in slow-wave sleep and showed lower delta and theta power during sleep compared to controls. Slow-wave and theta activity during sleep appear to reflect important aspects of memory processing, as evening-to-morning change in declarative memory correlated with delta and theta power during intervening sleep in both groups. These results suggest that sleep changes in aMCI patients contribute to memory impairments by interfering with sleep-dependent memory consolidation. (JINS, 2012, 18, 490–500)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Introduction

Declarative memories concern knowledge of episodes and facts, and generally depend on hippocampal-mediated binding that links memory fragments stored across multiple neocortical zones (Eichenbaum & Cohen, Reference Eichenbaum and Cohen2001). Declarative memory impairments can result from deficient encoding, storage, retrieval, or some combination. In Alzheimer's disease (AD) and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), hippocampal dysfunction and decreased cholinergic innervation substantially contribute to declarative memory impairment (Braak & Braak, Reference Braak and Braak1991; Mesulam, Reference Mesulam2004; Mesulam, Shaw, Mash, & Weintraub, Reference Mesulam, Shaw, Mash and Weintraub2004; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Parisi, Dickson, Johnson, Knopman, Boeve and Kokmen2006).

Declarative memories are stabilized for long-term storage through consolidation, wherein neocortical connections become strengthened and hippocampal dependence decreases (Paller, Reference Paller2009; for alternative views concerning episodic memories see Moscovitch, Nadel, Winocur, Gilboa, & Rosenbaum, Reference Moscovitch, Nadel, Winocur, Gilboa and Rosenbaum2006). Given the hypothesis that sleep facilitates consolidation (Maquet, Reference Maquet2001; Marshall & Born, Reference Marshall and Born2007; Paller, Reference Paller1997; Stickgold, Reference Stickgold2005; Sutherland & Lehmann, Reference Sutherland and Lehmann2011), it is plausible that sleep deficiencies in AD and aMCI patients could disrupt consolidation and contribute to patients’ memory problems.

Slow-wave sleep (SWS) may be especially pertinent for declarative memory consolidation, owing in part to the low-frequency neuronal oscillations [measured as delta power in electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings] that predominate during this sleep stage and are thought to induce widespread neuronal synchrony that facilitates hippocampal-neocortical interaction (Diekelmann & Born, Reference Diekelmann and Born2010). SWS-rich retention intervals benefit subsequent declarative memory (Drosopoulos, Wagner, & Born, Reference Drosopoulos, Wagner and Born2005), as does oscillating transcranial electrical stimulation at slow-wave frequencies (Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle, & Born, Reference Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle and Born2006; Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid, & Born, Reference Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid and Born2004). Coherence between hippocampal and neocortical networks increases during SWS (Ji & Wilson, Reference Ji and Wilson2007; Sirota, Csicsvari, Buhl, & Buzsaki, Reference Sirota, Csicsvari, Buhl and Buzsaki2003), and neuroimaging studies reveal relationships between brain activity during SWS and declarative memory (Chee & Chuah, Reference Chee and Chuah2008). Furthermore, observations of EEG power in the delta band during sleep are related to declarative memory assessed before sleep (Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi, & Halasz, Reference Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi and Halasz2001; Goder et al., Reference Goder, Aldenhoff, Boigs, Braun, Koch and Fritzer2006). Notably, during SWS, reactivation of some recently encoded memories is thought to take place, as SWS hippocampal firing patterns parallel those present during waking (Pavlides & Winson, Reference Pavlides and Winson1989; Wilson & McNaughton, Reference Wilson and McNaughton1994), and reactivating recently learned memories via external cues during SWS benefits declarative memory (Rasch, Buchel, Gais, & Born, Reference Rasch, Buchel, Gais and Born2007; Rudoy, Voss, Westerberg, & Paller, Reference Rudoy, Voss, Westerberg and Paller2009).

Declarative memory consolidation may nonetheless depend on sleep mechanisms beyond slow-wave activity. Other results implicate spindle activity (Clemens, Fabo, & Halasz, Reference Clemens, Fabo and Halasz2005; Schabus et al., Reference Schabus, Gruber, Parapatics, Sauter, Klosch, Anderer and Zeithofer2004) and theta power that predominates during rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep (Fogel, Smith, & Cote, Reference Fogel, Smith and Cote2007; Nishida, Pearsall, Buckner, & Walker, Reference Nishida, Pearsall, Buckner and Walker2009). Also, fluctuating levels of acetylcholine across non-REM and REM sleep may mediate hippocampal-neocortical information exchange and synaptic plasticity (Power, Reference Power2004).

Alzheimer's pathology interferes with sleep physiology. Sleep abnormalities typically observed in AD patients include reductions in sleep efficiency, spindle activity, SWS, and REM, along with an increased arousal index (Bliwise, Reference Bliwise1993; McCurry & Ancoli-Israel, Reference McCurry and Ancoli-Israel2003; Rauchs et al., Reference Rauchs, Schabus, Parapatics, Bertran, Clochon, Hot and Anderer2008). Patients with aMCI express subjective sleep complaints (Beaulieu-Bonneau & Hudon, Reference Beaulieu-Bonneau and Hudon2009), and in a recent study, such complaints were found to correlate with later memory (Westerberg et al., Reference Westerberg, Lundgren, Florczak, Mesulam, Weintraub, Zee and Paller2010). However, the extent to which objective neurophysiological sleep parameters are altered in aMCI patients is unknown.

To determine whether sleep physiology is deficient in aMCI and if the degree of these deficiencies is related to degree of declarative memory impairment, we examined memory/sleep relationships in aMCI patients and age- and education-matched cognitively healthy older adults (Table 1). Polysomnographic (PSG) data were acquired during two experimental nights, with memory testing before and after sleep each night (Figure 1). Memory tests included two declarative memory tests (word-pair recall, fact recognition) and a nondeclarative memory test (object priming).

Table 1 Neuropsychological testing results for the two groups (with SE in parentheses)

Note. CERAD = Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease; WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment. * = aMCI patients significantly worse than controls [t(23), p < .05].

Fig. 1 Timeline of events for each of the polysomnographic recording nights.

Method

This study was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board. We complied with ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants

Eighteen cognitively healthy older adults and 10 aMCI patients recruited from the Northwestern Alzheimer's Disease Center participated in exchange for monetary compensation. Data from 1 control participant were excluded due to previously undetected sleep apnea, and 1 control and 2 patients elected not to complete the full protocol, resulting in a final group of 16 controls (3 male) and 8 aMCI patients (1 male). Mean age and years of education were matched across control and aMCI groups [age: 72.7 (63.2–79.1; SD = 5.1) and 75.6 years (62.3–82.8; SD = 7.2), respectively, p > .3; education: 15.6 (12–20; SD = 2.5) and 14.5 years (10–18; SD = 3.0), respectively, p > .3].

Participants were clinically evaluated and given a neuropsychological assessment (Table 1). Diagnosis of aMCI followed current guidelines (Petersen, Reference Petersen2004), and reflected scores of 1.5 or more standard deviations below the mean for individuals of comparable age, gender, and education level in one or more cognitive domains including declarative memory, no impairments in daily living activities as assessed with the Functional Assessment Questionnaire (Pfeffer, Kurosaki, Harrah, Chance & Filos, Reference Pfeffer, Kurosaki, Harrah, Chance and Filos1982) and the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (Jorm, Reference Jorm1994), and failure to reach clinical criteria for dementia. No aMCI patients were taking acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. Exclusion criteria included history of central nervous system disease, major psychiatric disorder, alcohol or substance abuse, serious medical illness (thyroid disorder, renal, hepatic, cardiac, or pulmonary insufficiency, unstable diabetes, uncontrolled hypertension, cancer), chronic use of psychoactive or hypnotic medications, and one or more sleep disorders (uncontrolled sleep apnea, restless leg syndrome, narcolepsy). Three of the 16 controls were not given a full clinical evaluation but had no memory complaints and did not meet any exclusion criteria.

General Procedure

During a preliminary interview, experimental procedures were explained, written consent was obtained, and questionnaires regarding recent sleep habits, sleep quality, and daytime sleepiness were administered (Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, Reference Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman and Kupfer1989; Johns, Reference Johns1991). No significant group differences were found [sleep habits/quality: p > .6; daytime sleepiness: p > .4].

At home, participants wore an activity sensor, recorded bed and wake times, and completed questionnaires regarding sleep quality (Akerstedt, Hume, Minors, & Waterhouse, Reference Akerstedt, Hume, Minors and Waterhouse1994) for 1 week. These data verified that participants adhered to a regular sleep schedule.

Participants spent two experimental nights at the Clinical Research Unit of Northwestern Memorial Hospital. On average, 11 days intervened between the two nights (range: 7–14 days). Participants also underwent an adaptation night that directly preceded the first experimental night. During the adaptation night, participants were familiarized with procedures and screened for sleep disorders. Data from the adaptation night were not included in any analyses reported here. Each night, the experimental procedures were identical, with the exception that specific stimuli tested were different each night. Preparation for PSG began 3 hrs before regular bedtime. Approximately 1.5 hrs before bedtime, participants completed the Positive and Negative Affectivity Scale (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988), answered the question “How awake do you feel right now?” on a 1–5 scale (1 = very sleepy, 5 = wide awake), and then completed the memory tests. PANAS responses and answers to the sleepiness question did not predict memory, nor were group differences present in these two measures (p values > .1). Lights were turned off according to typical bedtimes for each participant, and participants were allowed to sleep uninterrupted until they woke up for the day on their own or until their typical wake-up time, whichever came first. After waking, participants were given 1 hr to clean up and eat breakfast. Participants then completed a subjective sleep questionnaire, the PANAS, and three memory tests.

Memory Tests

Two declarative memory tests and one nondeclarative memory test were administered in the same order each night. Different sets of stimuli were introduced each night. Each morning, the three memory tests were administered again (without additional encoding), in the same order each morning (Figure 1). Memory scores were computed for each evening and each morning test, and were submitted to across-group comparisons. Change scores for the two declarative memory tests were computed by subtracting each evening score from the corresponding morning score. For the nondeclarative test, evening priming scores were subtracted from morning priming scores. Change scores were then correlated with sleep measures.

Word-pair recall

At encoding, participants studied 44 related word pairs (e.g., story-article), adapted from previous investigations of sleep and memory (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid and Born2004, Reference Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle and Born2006; Plihal & Born, Reference Plihal and Born1997). Words were presented centrally on a computer screen, one above the other, at a rate of one pair every 4 s. Participants were told to memorize each pair. Next, they completed math problems for 1 min, to minimize word rehearsal. Then, the first word of each pair was presented for 4 s and participants were asked to say the other member of the pair aloud. After 4 s, a tone sounded and the correct answer appeared directly below the first word, and both remained for 4 s. After the last pair, participants completed math problems for 1 min and then took the same test again, to ensure robust learning of the word pairs. Only scores from the second test were used to compute evening memory scores. In the morning, the test was given once more using the same format. Each time the test was given, the same word pairs were presented in a different random order except that the first two and last two pairs studied during encoding were always the first and last two tested, respectively (these trials were excluded from analyses to minimize serial position effects).

Fact recognition

At encoding, participants viewed a set of monochrome facial images (six male, five female). Each face was presented for 15 s above four biographical facts (e.g., Christine/was homeless/won the state lottery/bought a health food store) meant to simulate meeting each person. The first fact was always a name. Then, the screen showed a 5-point rating scale, and participants rated the emotionality of the description (1 = very emotional, 5 = not emotional) to ensure they were encoding the descriptions. After 11 encoding trials, participants solved math problems for 1 min. Then, 10 test trials began using all the studied stimuli except the first studied face and associated facts. On the first trial, 1 face appeared on the left and 10 names on the right. Participants were asked to press the number corresponding to the correct name. If they did not know the correct name, they were asked to guess. After a button was pressed, a list of 10 facts appeared on the right, and participants attempted to press the number corresponding to the correct fact. This procedure continued for the final two facts, and then a new face appeared on the left side and the testing process repeated for the remaining nine faces. Following the evening test, each face and associated facts were presented for 15 s each, providing an additional learning opportunity. No additional testing was completed in the evening. The morning test was otherwise identical to the evening test. Across the evening and morning tests, faces, and facts appeared in a different random order. Within each test, the facts were presented in the same order for each of the 10 trials.

Object priming

At encoding, participants viewed 30 color pictures of common objects for 4 s each. Participants were asked to say aloud the name of each object as soon as it appeared. Immediately following each object presentation, a rating scale appeared and participants were asked to rate how much they liked each object (1 = like very much, 4 = dislike very much). Participants were not informed that memory for these objects would be subsequently tested, nor were they informed when tested that any previously seen objects would be used. At test, the same 30 objects were randomly intermixed with 30 new objects, each flashed for 102 ms followed by a mask for 102 ms. Participants pressed “b” if they recognized the identity of the object and “n” if not, and if they pressed “b” they named the object aloud as quickly and accurately as possible. A “b” press was only counted as correct if the spoken object name was accurate. The morning test was the same except there was a different set of 30 new objects and a different random order. The percent of new objects correctly recognized was subtracted from the percent of old objects correctly recognized to obtain a priming score for each test.

PSG recording and analyses

Sleep EEG was measured using electrodes placed at eight sites from the 10-20 system (C3, C4, O1, O2, F3, F4, P3, and P4), referenced to average mastoids, along with electrooculogram, chin electromyogram, and electrocardiogram channels. For three aMCI patients and two controls, electrodes were placed only at C3, C4, O1, and O2 due to recording limitations. On the adaptation night, nasal/oral airflow, abdominal and chest respiration, pulse oximetry, and leg electromyogram were also monitored. Signals were sampled at 200 Hz (Neurofax EEG-1100, Nihon Khoden) and amplified using a 0.27- to 70-Hz bandpass filter. Sleep staging was accomplished using standard criteria (Iber, Ancoli-Israel, Chesson, & Quan, Reference Iber, Ancoli-Israel, Chesson and Quan2007).

EEG spectral analyses were conducted following artifact removal based on visual inspection. Fast Fourier transform was applied using a Hanning function and 4-s intervals with 50% overlap, yielding a frequency resolution of 0.25 Hz. Estimates were averaged for 30-s epochs aligned with sleep stages and absolute power computed for delta (0.5–4.5 Hz), theta (4.5–8.5 Hz), alpha (8.5–12.5 Hz), and sigma (12.5–15.5 Hz) frequency bands. Reported EEG power values were averaged across the four electrode locations recorded in all participants unless otherwise stated.

Sleep spindles in the frequency range from 12.5–15.5 Hz were detected automatically at each electrode (amplitude > 12 μV; duration 0.5–3.0 s). Fast (13–15 Hz) and slow (11–13 Hz) spindles were also detected with the same criteria. Scoring, artifact rejection, spectral analyses, and spindle detection were completed with Prana software (Phitools).

Results

Memory Dysfunction in aMCI

For each test type, performance was averaged across the two evening sessions to yield an evening memory score, as preliminary analyses indicated no significant differences between night 1 and night 2 evening scores. Likewise, performance was averaged across the two morning sessions to yield a morning memory score, as preliminary analyses indicated no significant differences between the two morning scores. To compare memory performance across groups, a 2 × 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with test time (evening, morning) as the within-subjects variable and group (control, aMCI) as the between-subjects variable was used to assess test performance. A Bennett-Box test conducted with each ANOVA ensured homogeneity in across-group variance for all three tests (p values > .2). Results from all tests are depicted in Figure 2.

Fig. 2 (a) Word-pair recall, (b) fact recognition, and (c) priming accuracy for evening and morning test sessions averaged across the two nights for control (n = 16) and aMCI (n = 8) groups.

Word-pair recall

Recall scores from the second test each evening were entered into the ANOVA to assess performance. Recall was better in the control group than in the aMCI group (66% and 32%, respectively), as shown by a significant main effect of group [F(1,22) = 15.6; p < .001]. The test time × group interaction was also significant [F(1,22) = 9.6; p < .01]. Controls improved their recall in the morning relative to that in the evening [t(15) = 2.2; p < .05], whereas aMCI patients did not, instead recalling less in the morning compared to the evening [t(7) = 3.4; p < .05]. The main effect of test time was not significant (p > .9).

To determine whether immediate feedback given to participants on each trial of this test was differentially effective at improving memory across aMCI and control groups, evening recall improvement scores were calculated by subtracting performance on the first evening recall test from performance on the second evening recall test for each night. Evening recall improvement scores did not significantly differ between groups [night 1: t(23) = 1.7; p > .05; night 2: t(23) = 1.7; p < .05].

Fact recognition

Recognition was better in controls than in aMCI patients (56% and 23%, respectively), as shown by a main effect of group [F(1,22) = 17.3; p < .001]. The test time × group interaction was marginal [F(1,22) = 3.8; p < .07]; controls improved their scores in the morning relative to the evening [t(15) = 2.9; p < .05] whereas aMCI patients did not (p > .8). The main effect of test time was not significant (p > .1).

Object priming

Priming scores were computed as the difference in naming accuracy between old and new objects. Priming magnitude was nearly identical in the control and aMCI groups (21% and 22%, respectively). The main effect of group was not significant (p > .9), but the main effect of test time was [F(1,22) = 10.0; p < .005]. Priming was stronger on the evening test (25%) than on the morning test (18%), presumably because of the shorter delay from encoding. The test time × group interaction was not significant (p > .5).

Sleep disruptions in aMCI

Measures computed for each participant did not significantly differ across nights. Accordingly, to compare potential differences in sleep across groups, sleep latency, total sleep time, minutes in each stage (and corresponding percentages spent in each stage relative to total sleep time), minutes of wake-after sleep onset (WASO), REM latency, and sleep efficiency (total sleep time divided by total recording time) were averaged across nights. Large group differences were present in SWS and modest group differences were present in other measures (Table 2). The aMCI group spent fewer minutes in SWS [t(22) = 2.8; p < .05], with a correspondingly lower SWS percentage than in the control group [t(22) = 2.7; p < .05]. Marginal group differences were present in REM minutes [t(22) = 2.0; p < .07], REM percentage [t(22) = 2.1; p < .06], WASO minutes [t(22) = 1.8; p < .08], WASO percentage [t(22) = 2.0; p < .06], REM latency [t(22) = 2.0; p < .07], and sleep efficiency [t(22) = 2.0; p < .06].

Table 2 Sleep parameters averaged across the two nights for the two groups (with SE in parentheses)

Note. SWS = slow-wave sleep; REM = rapid eye movement; WASO = wake after sleep onset; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Percent measures are in relation to sum of total sleep time and WASO. ** = aMCI group significantly less than the control group [t(23), p < .05]. * = aMCI group marginally different from the control group [t(23), p < .08].

To determine whether the magnitude of aMCI disruption relative to controls differed between SWS and REM, as has been suggested for healthy older versus younger adults (Van Cauter, Leproult, & Plat, Reference Van Cauter, Leproult and Plat2000), a ratio of SWS min to REM min was computed for each participant. These ratios were significantly smaller for aMCI patients than for controls [.007 vs. .12, respectively; t(22) = 2.6; p < .05]. Although all participants spent more time in REM than SWS, by this metric, the amount of SWS relative to REM is smaller in aMCI patients compared with controls, highlighting the disproportionate SWS disruption in aMCI.

EEG power averaged across non-REM (stages 1, 2, and SWS) and REM sleep periods for the two nights also revealed reliable group differences. Table 3 shows power values and results from across-group t-tests at each recording location for non-REM and REM sleep periods. During non-REM, delta and theta were reduced in aMCI patients compared to controls [t(22) = 2.3; p < .05, and t(22) = 2.2; p < .05, respectively]. These differences remained present when stage 2 was examined alone [delta: t(22) = 2.1; p < .05; theta: t(22) = 2.2; p < .05], reflecting the large contribution of stage-2 to the non-REM category. During REM, the aMCI group also showed a significant reduction in theta [t(22) = 2.9; p < .05], but not in delta [t(22) = 1.8; p < .09]. Alpha and sigma power did not differ between groups during non-REM or REM periods (p values > .4).

Table 3 Average absolute power (μV2) for non-REM and REM sleep periods averaged across the two nights at individual electrode sites in the delta, theta, alpha, and sigma bands (with SE in parentheses)

Note. REM = rapid eye movement; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Averages for C3, C4, O1, O2 sites include n = 16 controls, n = 8 aMCI patients (df for t-tests = 23; averages for F3, F4, P3, P4 sites include n = 14 controls, n = 5 aMCI patients (df for t-tests = 18).

Spindle counts focused on frontal and parietal locations during stage 2 for all participants with data from these recording sites (n = 5 aMCI patients, n = 13 controls), given prior reports that spindle activity is maximal at these locations and during this stage (Zygierewicz et al., Reference Zygierewicz, Blinowska, Durka, Szelenberger, Niemcewicz and Androsiuk1999). Spindle counts were reduced in aMCI patients at F3 and F4 recording sites compared to controls [F3: aMCI average = 86, control average = 318; t(17) = 2.9; p < .01; F4: aMCI average = 85; control average = 331; t(17) = 3.1; p < .01], but not at parietal sites (p values > .7). Consistent with evidence that fast spindles may be more affected by AD pathology than slow spindles (Rauchs et al., Reference Rauchs, Schabus, Parapatics, Bertran, Clochon, Hot and Anderer2008), stage-2 frontal reductions in the aMCI group were present in fast spindles but not in slow spindles [F3: t(17) = 2.8; p < .05; F4: t(17) = 2.9; p < .05].

Declarative memory retention is related to physiological aspects of sleep

Further analyses were undertaken to isolate aspects of sleep physiology contributing to memory consolidation. Memory change scores were correlated separately with delta power, theta power, and stage-2 spindle counts from frontal electrodes from the intervening night, as previous studies have reported relationships between memory and these aspects of sleep in younger adults. Although SWS has also been implicated in consolidation, the lack of SWS in nine of our participants precluded correlation analyses with this variable. Initial analyses included all participants, and for each, EEG power values were computed during sleep across the whole night. Correlations were computed separately for each of the two nights.

On night 1, word-pair recall change scores were predicted by delta (r = .55; p < .01) and theta (r = .52; p < .01), but not frontal stage-2 spindles (p values > .1). Increases in both delta and theta power were associated with positive change scores, as depicted in Figure 3. These correlations were likely not attributable to type I errors, as both correlations were significant at a stringent p < .01 level. Additional analyses showed that these correlations were also present when EEG power was computed separately for non-REM and REM sleep periods, and when each group was examined separately (Table 4). Correlation coefficients were very similar across the two groups, though in the aMCI group alone they were not significant, likely due to the small size of that group.

Fig. 3 (a) Delta power and (b) theta power were positively related to memory change in word-pair recall from evening to morning [r(22), p < .05].

Table 4 Pearson correlation coefficients for recall correlations with delta and theta power

Note. *** = r(22), p < .01; ** = r(22), p < .05; * = r(14), p < .05. REM = rapid eye movement; aMCI = amnestic mild cognitive impairment.

On night 2, a similar pattern of results was observed. Word-pair recall scores were positively correlated with theta (r = .59; p < .01) and marginally positively correlated with delta (r = .41; p < .08), but not with frontal stage-2 spindle counts (p values > .2). Correlations with delta and theta were also significant when non-REM and REM sleep periods were analyzed separately. When each group was examined separately, correlation coefficients just failed to reach significance (p values < .1) but reflected the same patterns as when both groups were examined together. See Table 4 for correlation coefficients for all analyses.

Connections between sleep and either biographical fact recognition or nondeclarative memory were less clear-cut. Correlation analyses did not reveal any significant relations between these memory tests and sleep parameters.

Sleep may also benefit memory by increasing morning alertness. However, if alertness or other nonspecific factors contributed to the correlations reported above, then one might expect sleep quality to predict response time in the morning. Response time to make a recognition decision on the object-priming test was measured for all objects. Yet, response-time decrease from evening to morning (58 ms and 162 ms for MCI patients and controls, respectively) was not correlated with any sleep parameter (r's < .2).

Discussion

Whereas disruptions of sleep physiology have previously been observed in AD, the present results are, to our knowledge, the first to demonstrate physiological abnormalities in aMCI patients. In comparisons with healthy individuals matched on age and education, differences were prominent in delta and theta power and in SWS. Importantly, aspects of sleep disrupted in aMCI were also implicated in declarative memory consolidation, suggesting that sleep disruptions in aMCI can thwart overnight consolidation and thus contribute to memory deficits that these patients experience.

SWS was dramatically reduced in aMCI patients, in tandem with borderline changes in REM, WASO, REM latency, and sleep efficiency. These differences are consistent with reports in AD (Bliwise, Reference Bliwise1993; McCurry & Ancoli-Israel, Reference McCurry and Ancoli-Israel2003) and suggest that alterations in sleep physiology beyond what occurs in healthy aging could signal neurodegenerative pathology. The finding that group differences were greater in SWS than in REM parallels findings in healthy aging (Benca, Obermeyer, Thisted, & Gillin, Reference Benca, Obermeyer, Thisted and Gillin1992; Ehlers & Kupfer, Reference Ehlers and Kupfer1989; Van Cauter et al., Reference Van Cauter, Leproult and Plat2000). We thus speculate that SWS begins to decline in healthy aging and then declines further in aMCI, whereas in advanced stages of AD, REM decline accelerates such that SWS no longer stands out as a selectively targeted sleep stage (Prinz, Poceta, & McCurry, Reference Prinz, Poceta and McCurry2002).

Sleep spindles, typically maximal during stage 2, have also been implicated in consolidation (Clemens et al., Reference Clemens, Fabo and Halasz2005; Schabus et al., Reference Schabus, Gruber, Parapatics, Sauter, Klosch, Anderer and Zeithofer2004, Reference Schabus, Hoedlmoser, Pecherstorfer, Anderer, Gruber, Parapatics and Zeithofer2008; Tamaki, Matsuoka, Nittono, & Hori, Reference Tamaki, Matsuoka, Nittono and Hori2008). Here, reduced stage-2 spindle counts at frontal recording sites in aMCI patients are consistent with reports of reduced spindle activity in AD (Bliwise Reference Bliwise1993; McCurry & Ancoli-Israel, Reference McCurry and Ancoli-Israel2003; Rauchs et al., Reference Rauchs, Schabus, Parapatics, Bertran, Clochon, Hot and Anderer2008). Our finding that fast but not slow spindles showed reductions is also in keeping with evidence that fast spindles (typically 13–15 Hz) are most disrupted in AD (Rauchs et al., Reference Rauchs, Schabus, Parapatics, Bertran, Clochon, Hot and Anderer2008).

In some ways, however, the findings in aMCI diverged sharply from what is typically found in AD. In particular, aMCI patients exhibited substantial reductions in delta and theta, whereas AD patients can show faster mean theta frequencies (Hot et al., Reference Hot, Rauchs, Bertran, Denise, Desgranges, Clochon and Eustache2011) and often show an overall shift to predominantly delta and theta both during sleep and during wakefulness (Bliwise, Reference Bliwise1993; Petit, Montplaisir, Lorrain, & Gauthier, Reference Petit, Montplaisir, Lorrain and Gauthier1992; Prinz, Larsen, Moe, & Vitiello, Reference Prinz, Larsen, Moe and Vitiello1992). Yet, levels of delta and theta during sleep are typically diminished in older adults relative to younger adults (Ehlers & Kupfer, Reference Ehlers and Kupfer1989; Landolt & Borbely, Reference Landolt and Borbely2001).

Importantly, our correlational results provided a key insight into the functional repercussions of reduced delta and theta power. Not only were these two measures reduced in aMCI patients compared to controls, but these measures were also the most severely reduced for those individuals who gained the least overnight benefit, or obtained no overnight benefit, in word-pair recall (Figure 3). These results are not easily attributable to a nonspecific benefit of sleep, such as increased alertness, because these two sleep parameters correlated with changes in recall but not with changes in recognition, priming, or response speed. Likewise, these correlations did not arise merely because the two groups differed in both EEG and recall measures. When correlations were examined for each group separately, controls and aMCI patients showed the same general pattern of associations. Although relationships did not reach statistical significance in all conditions when the modestly sized groups were examined separately, correlation coefficients were similar across the two groups and these values were similar to those from the combined sample. Presumably, this pattern of results indicates that these relationships hold regardless of neuropathology. Delta and theta power thus appear to index processes related to the overnight stabilization of memory storage that involves hippocampal binding and facilitates later recall.

Of course, factors beyond sleep-dependent consolidation likely contributed to overnight changes in memory. For example, circadian effects likely influenced performance on the evening and morning tests (Hogan et al., Reference Hogan, Kelly, Verrier, Newell, Hasher and Robertson2009). Also, similar memory changes might have occurred during an 8-hr delay during the day. Whether or not such memory changes occur during the day, the most important conclusions are based on the correlations with delta/theta, which suggest that memory processing during sleep is relevant for these memory changes, consistent with the speculation that poor sleep contributes to poor memory in MCI.

Thus, the neuropathology in aMCI not only produces dysfunction at the moment of memory acquisition, but it seems to disrupt delta and theta activity along with memory reactivation and stabilization during sleep. Word-pair recall performance also supports this conclusion, as recall was superior in the morning compared to the prior evening in controls, whereas recall declined overnight in the aMCI group, suggesting that sleep-dependent consolidation processes were more effective in controls. We cannot rule out a contribution from circadian factors to the differential overnight memory change. Recall feedback might also have been relevant if it was more effective in controls than in aMCI patients. This is unlikely, however, as an analysis of memory improvement from first to second evening test yielded no evidence for differential benefit from feedback between the groups. Still, differences could emerge at longer delays. Despite these complexities, the different patterns of overnight memory change in the two groups remain an interesting topic for future investigation.

Delta power during sleep has been associated with the consolidation of many types of memory (Bodizs et al., Reference Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi and Halasz2001; Goder et al., Reference Goder, Aldenhoff, Boigs, Braun, Koch and Fritzer2006; Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini, & Tononi, Reference Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini and Tononi2004; Wamsley, Tucker, Payne, & Stickgold, Reference Wamsley, Tucker, Payne and Stickgold2010). In particular, overnight improvement in nondeclarative memory and in maze navigation learning have been associated with delta from the intervening night. In studies that tested declarative memory via recall or recognition (Bodizs et al., Reference Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi and Halasz2001; Goder et al., Reference Goder, Aldenhoff, Boigs, Braun, Koch and Fritzer2006), delta was related to memory that was assessed before sleep, but overnight change in memory was not evaluated. Our results support and extend these findings, as here we demonstrated a relationship between delta and overnight memory change, which more strongly implicates delta in memory consolidation. This emphasis on delta activity is consistent with theories positing that during SWS, when delta is most predominant, consolidation of recently acquired information is accomplished via neural changes resulting from interactions between hippocampal and neocortical networks (Diekelmann & Born, Reference Diekelmann and Born2010). The persistence of delta correlations during REM suggests that even low levels of slow-wave activity can influence memory processing. REM delta could reflect the persistence of hippocampal/neocortical interactions from non-REM sleep, or other processes (e.g., hippocampal plasticity). Relationships between REM and declarative memory consolidation have been previously reported (Bodizs et al., Reference Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi and Halasz2001; Rauchs et al., Reference Rauchs, Bertran, Guillery-Girard, Desgranges, Kerrouche, Denise and Eustache2004), and are in agreement with theories positing neural processes contributing to declarative memory consolidation likely occur across multiple sleep stages (e.g., Diekelmann & Born, Reference Diekelmann and Born2010).

Time spent in SWS each night, defined by conventional sleep-staging methods, declines considerably in aging (Bliwise, Reference Bliwise1993; Danker-Hopfe et al., Reference Danker-Hopfe, Schafer, Dorn, Anderer, Saletu, Gruber and Dorffner2005; Van Cauter et al., Reference Van Cauter, Leproult and Plat2000). In two controls and in five aMCI patients, no epochs of SWS were observed across both nights. Notably, conventional sleep staging methods are insensitive to low levels of slow-wave activity, as slow-wave amplitudes must be greater than 75 microvolts to count as SWS. On the other hand, delta power (slow-wave activity computed in the delta band) provides a more fine-grained measure across the whole night, especially in individuals with lower-amplitude slow waves. Accordingly, correlational analyses of memory decline in aging and aMCI can be more powerful when using delta power than amount of SWS, and here this approach allowed us to link slow-wave activity with memory consolidation. Future research in populations with diminished SWS levels may benefit from comparable analyses.

Relationships between overnight recall change and theta were also observed. These findings are consistent with recent results showing a positive relationship between theta during SWS and delayed recall in AD patients (Hot et al., Reference Hot, Rauchs, Bertran, Denise, Desgranges, Clochon and Eustache2011). Two other studies implicated theta during REM in declarative memory consolidation (Fogel et al., Reference Fogel, Smith and Cote2007; Nishida et al., Reference Nishida, Pearsall, Buckner and Walker2009). Here, recall was related to theta across all sleep periods, as well as during REM and non-REM analyzed separately. Hippocampal theta is prominent during REM, and is hypothesized to reflect synchronization of neuron ensembles within different hippocampal subunits and limbic structures in the service of synaptic plasticity (Buzsaki, Reference Buzsaki2002; Montgomery, Sirota, & Buzsaki, Reference Montgomery, Sirota and Buzsaki2008). Theta during waking has also been associated with declarative memory (Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Russegger, & Pachinger, Reference Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Russegger and Pachinger1996; Weiss & Rappelsberger, Reference Weiss and Rappelsberger2000), and intracranial EEG recordings suggest that wake theta shares the same neural generators as those observed during REM sleep (Babiloni et al., Reference Babiloni, Vecchio, Mirabella, Buttiglione, Sebastiano, Picardi and Eusebi2009). Yet, the extent to which scalp EEG theta during sleep is influenced by hippocampal theta rhythms is unknown. Thus, future investigations are necessary to determine how hippocampal theta may contribute to theta recorded at the scalp and whether REM and non-REM theta reflect similar neurophysiological events relevant for consolidation.

The current investigation included two tests of declarative memory, a word-pair recall test and a fact recognition test. Connections between sleep and consolidation were found only for recall. There are multiple reasons why recognition may not be as sensitive as recall in detecting sleep/memory relationships. First, recall places more demands on producing information from memory, whereas recognition is accomplished by selecting information from a set of choices. Consolidation may aid the strategic retrieval required for recall more than the potentially less extensive retrieval that can support recognition. Lower mean accuracy for recognition (44% correct) compared to recall (55% correct) may also be relevant. Correct answers via guessing were more likely for recognition, especially as some choices could often be eliminated based on prior trials. Also, for biographical facts that were not well learned, less benefit may have accrued from sleep-dependent consolidation because memory storage was too weak (Stickgold, Reference Stickgold2009). Another factor that may be relevant pertains to the types of associations formed. The two memory tests relied on different sorts of associations, which can be relevant for which brain areas are involved in storage (Mayes, Montaldi, & Migo, Reference Mayes, Montaldi and Migo2007), and can impact sleep/memory relationships (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Peigneux, Muto, Schenkel, Knoblauch, Munch and Cajochen2006). At any rate, word-pair recall not only provided evidence in support of sleep/memory relationships here, but has also been used in other experiments implicating sleep in the consolidation of declarative memories (Fogel et al., Reference Fogel, Smith and Cote2007; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid and Born2004, Reference Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle and Born2006).

The present study characterizes physiological sleep abnormalities in aMCI for the first time, and correlational data supported the hypothesis that sleep is involved in memory processing. Future research exploring the effects of direct manipulations of sleep on memory will further understanding of how memories are processed during sleep (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid and Born2004, Reference Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle and Born2006; Rasch et al., Reference Rasch, Buchel, Gais and Born2007; Rudoy et al., Reference Rudoy, Voss, Westerberg and Paller2009). A thorough understanding of sleep/memory relationships and changes in these relationships in aMCI patients can help reveal how remembering a lifetime of memories depends not only on information acquisition and retrieval, but also on intervening consolidation during sleep. Disrupted memory processing during sleep may contribute to memory problems in neurodegenerative diseases and in aging generally. Further efforts in this vein could lead to sleep-focused treatments designed to offset cognitive decline. The present results provide evidence that neural dysfunction in aMCI hinders some aspects of sleep, and that these same aspects of sleep are important for memory consolidation, supporting the conclusion that the memory problems these patients experience partially reflect deficient information processing during sleep. The delta and theta reductions in aMCI—and their connection with declarative memory—indicate that these specific aspects of sleep impact memory consolidation.

Acknowledgments

We thank Erik Naylor for technical assistance with polysomnography, Aparna Arjunan, Courtney Clark, Lori Koch, Dylan Murray, Kari Sveum, and Tiffany St. James for assistance with data collection and preliminary analyses, and Paul Raffeld for assistance with statistical analyses. This work was supported by the Northwestern Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Center (M.M.M., NIH P30 AG13854), a Senator Mark Hatfield Award from the Alzheimer's Association (K.A.P.), the Illinois Department of Public Health Alzheimer's Disease Research Fund (K.A.P.), the Northwestern Clinical Research Unit (NIH UL1RR025741), and the donors of Alzheimer's Disease Research, a program of the American Health Assistance Foundation (C.W., K.A.P.). The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Akerstedt, T., Hume, K., Minors, D., Waterhouse, J. (1994). The subjective meaning of good sleep, an intraindividual approach using the Karolinska Sleep Diary. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 287296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Mirabella, G., Buttiglione, M., Sebastiano, F., Picardi, A., Eusebi, F. (2009). Hippocampal, amygdala, and neocortical synchronization of theta rhythms is related to an immediate recall during rey auditory verbal learning test. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 20772089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaulieu-Bonneau, S., Hudon, C. (2009). Sleep disturbances in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics, 21, 654666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benca, R.M., Obermeyer, W.H., Thisted, R.A., Gillin, J.C. (1992). Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 651668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliwise, D.L. (1993). Sleep in normal aging and dementia. Sleep, 16, 4081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodizs, R., Bekesy, M., Szucs, A., Barsi, P., Halasz, P. (2001). Sleep-dependent hippocampal slow activity correlates with waking memory performance in humans. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78, 441457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braak, H., Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buysse, D.J., Reynolds, C.F. III, Monk, T.H., Berman, S.R., Kupfer, D.J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzsaki, G. (2002). Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron, 33, 325340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chee, M.W., Chuah, L.Y. (2008). Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21, 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clemens, Z., Fabo, D., Halasz, P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132, 529535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danker-Hopfe, H., Schafer, M., Dorn, H., Anderer, P., Saletu, B., Gruber, G., Dorffner, G. (2005). Percentile reference charts for selected sleep parameters for 20- to 80-year-old healthy subjects from the SIESTA database. Somnologie, 9, 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diekelmann, S., Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 114126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drosopoulos, S., Wagner, U., Born, J. (2005). Sleep enhances explicit recollection in recognition memory. Learning & Memory, 12, 4451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, C.L., Kupfer, D.J. (1989). Effects of age on delta and REM sleep parameters. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 118125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., Cohen, N.J. (2001). Conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, S.M., Smith, C.T., Cote, K.A. (2007). Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and non-REM sleep in declarative and procedural memory systems. Behavioural Brain Research, 180, 4861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goder, R., Aldenhoff, J.B., Boigs, M., Braun, S., Koch, J., Fritzer, G. (2006). Delta power in sleep in relation to neuropsychological performance in healthy subjects and schizophrenia patients. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 18, 529535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, M.J., Kelly, C.A., Verrier, D., Newell, J., Hasher, L., Robertson, I.H. (2009). Optimal time-of-day and consolidation of learning in younger and older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 107128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hot, P., Rauchs, G., Bertran, F., Denise, P., Desgranges, B., Clochon, P., Eustache, F. (2011). Changes in sleep theta rhythm are related to episodic memory imapriment in early Alzheimer's disease. Biological Psychology, 87, 334339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, R., Ghilardi, M.F., Massimini, M., Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature, 430, 7881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A.L., Quan, S.F. (2007). The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine.Google Scholar
Ji, D., Wilson, M.A. (2007). Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 100107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, M.W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep, 14, 540545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A.F. (1994). A short form of the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE): Development and cross-validation. Psychological Medicine, 24, 145153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston naming test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Russegger, H., Pachinger, T. (1996). Theta band power in the human scalp EEG and the encoding of new information. Neuroreport, 7, 12351240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landolt, H.P., Borbely, A.A. (2001). Age-dependent changes in sleep EEG topography. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 369377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maquet, P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294, 10481052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, L., Born, J. (2007). The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 442450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, L., Helgadottir, H., Molle, M., Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature, 444, 610613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, L., Molle, M., Hallschmid, M., Born, J. (2004). Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 99859992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 126135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCurry, S.M., Ancoli-Israel, S. (2003). Sleep dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease and other dementias. Current Treatment Options in Neurology, 5, 261272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M. (2004). The cholinergic lesion of Alzheimer's disease: Pivotal factor or side show? Learning and Memory, 11, 4349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M., Shaw, P., Mash, D., Weintraub, S. (2004). Cholinergic nucleus basalis tauopathy emerges early in the aging-MCI-AD continuum. Annals of Neurology, 55, 815828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, S.M., Sirota, A., Buzsaki, G. (2008). Theta and gamma coordination of hippocampal networks during waking and rapid eye movement sleep. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 67316741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J.C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R.C., Hughes, J.P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., Clark, C. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 39, 11591165.Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M., Nadel, L., Winocur, G., Gilboa, A., Rosenbaum, R.S. (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 179190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R.L., Walker, M.P. (2009). REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 11581166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paller, K.A. (1997). Consolidating dispersed neocortical memories: The missing link in amnesia. Memory, 5, 7388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paller, K.A. (2009). Memory consolidation: Systems. In L.R. Squire (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Oxford: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pavlides, C., Winson, J. (1989). Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. Journaal of Neuroscience, 9, 29072918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Parisi, J.E., Dickson, D.W., Johnson, K.A., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Kokmen, E. (2006). Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 63, 665672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petit, D., Montplaisir, J., Lorrain, D., Gauthier, S. (1992). Spectral analysis of the rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram in right and left temporal regions: A biological marker of Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology, 32, 172176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfeffer, R.I., Kurosaki, T.T., Harrah, C.H. Jr., Chance, J.M., Filos, S. (1982). Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 323329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plihal, W., Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 534547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, A.E. (2004). Slow-wave sleep, acetylcholine, and memory consolidation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 17951796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, P.N., Larsen, L.H., Moe, K.E., Vitiello, M.V. (1992). EEG markers of early Alzheimer's disease in computer selected tonic REM sleep. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 83, 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, P.N., Poceta, J.S., McCurry, S. (2002). Sleep in the dementing disorders. In T.L. Lee-Chiong, M.J. Sateia, & M.A. Carskadon (Eds.), Sleep medicine. Philadelphia: Hanley & Belfus, Inc.Google Scholar
Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S., Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315, 14261429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauchs, G., Bertran, F., Guillery-Girard, B., Desgranges, B., Kerrouche, N., Denise, P., Eustache, F. (2004). Consolidation of strictly episodic memories mainly requires rapid eye movement sleep. Sleep, 27, 395401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rauchs, G., Schabus, M., Parapatics, S., Bertran, F., Clochon, P., Hot, P., Anderer, P. (2008). Is there a link between sleep changes and memory in Alzheimer's disease? Neuroreport, 19, 11591162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R.M. (1992). Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1970). L'examen Clinique en Psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Rudoy, J.D., Voss, J.L., Westerberg, C.E., Paller, K.A. (2009). Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep. Science, 326, 1079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schabus, M., Gruber, G., Parapatics, S., Sauter, C., Klosch, G., Anderer, P., Zeithofer, J. (2004). Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep, 27, 14791485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schabus, M., Hoedlmoser, K., Pecherstorfer, T., Anderer, P., Gruber, G., Parapatics, S., Zeithofer, J. (2008). Interindividual sleep spindle differences and their relation to learning-related enhancements. Brain Research, 1191, 127135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, C., Peigneux, P., Muto, V., Schenkel, M., Knoblauch, V., Munch, M., Cajochen, C. (2006). Encoding difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 89768982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D., Buzsaki, G. (2003). Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 20652069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437, 12721278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stickgold, R. (2009). How do I remember? Let me count the ways. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13, 305308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, R.J., Lehmann, H. (2011). Alternative conceptions of memory consolidation and the role of the hippocampus at the systems level in rodents. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 21, 446451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H., Hori, T. (2008). Fast sleep spindle (13–15 hz) activity correlates with sleep-dependent improvement in visuomotor performance. Sleep, 31, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 861868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wamsley, E.J., Tucker, M.A., Payne, J.D., Stickgold, R. (2010). A brief nap is beneficial for human route-learning: The role of navigation experience and EEG spectral power. Learning and Memory, 17, 332336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychololgy, 54, 10631070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1981). The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Manual. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weiss, S., Rappelsberger, P. (2000). Long-range EEG synchronization during word encoding correlates with successful memory performance. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westerberg, C.E., Lundgren, E.M., Florczak, S.M., Mesulam, M.M., Weintraub, S., Zee, P.C., Paller, K.A. (2010). Sleep influences the severity of memory disruption in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Results from sleep self-assessment and continuous activity monitoring. Alzheimer's Disease and Associated Disorders, 24, 325333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M.A., McNaughton, B.L. (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science, 265, 676679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zygierewicz, J., Blinowska, K.J., Durka, P.J., Szelenberger, W., Niemcewicz, S., Androsiuk, W. (1999). High resolution study of sleep spindles. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 21362147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Neuropsychological testing results for the two groups (with SE in parentheses)

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Timeline of events for each of the polysomnographic recording nights.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 (a) Word-pair recall, (b) fact recognition, and (c) priming accuracy for evening and morning test sessions averaged across the two nights for control (n = 16) and aMCI (n = 8) groups.

Figure 3

Table 2 Sleep parameters averaged across the two nights for the two groups (with SE in parentheses)

Figure 4

Table 3 Average absolute power (μV2) for non-REM and REM sleep periods averaged across the two nights at individual electrode sites in the delta, theta, alpha, and sigma bands (with SE in parentheses)

Figure 5

Fig. 3 (a) Delta power and (b) theta power were positively related to memory change in word-pair recall from evening to morning [r(22), p < .05].

Figure 6

Table 4 Pearson correlation coefficients for recall correlations with delta and theta power