Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-hpxsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T13:30:17.453Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verification of Ingroup Identity as a Longitudinal Mediator between Intergroup Contact and Outgroup Evaluation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2013

Angel Gómez*
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
Anja Eller
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional Autónoma (Mexico)
Alexandra Vázquez
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ángel Gómez. Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia. Social and Organizational Psychology Department Psychology. C/ Juan del Rosal 10. Despacho 1.58. 28040 Madrid (Spain). Phone: +34-913987747. E-mails: agomez@psi.uned.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Almost six decades of research have consistently demonstrated that intergroup contact is one of the most powerful ways of improving intergroup attitudes. At least two important limitations, however, still compel researchers to continue work in this area: the issue of long-term effects of contact, and the processes underlying such effects. This report makes a theoretical and empirical contribution with regard to these two aspects introducing a new mediator of the effects of contact: verification of qualities of typical ingroup members that may or may not characterize individual group members (e.g. verification of ingroup identities). One hundred and forty-two high school students participated in a two-wave longitudinal study with 12 weeks’ lag in Spain. Cross-sectional and longitudinal mediational analyses using multiple imputation data showed that intergroup contact improves general outgroup evaluation through increasing verification of ingroup identities. This research demonstrates the relevance of considering verification of ingroup identity as a mediator for the positive effects of intergroup contact.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

Intergroup contact has been amply shown to be one of the most powerful tools for reducing prejudice and improving intergroup attitudes. However, recent revisions point to two specific topics that should be further investigated in future research (Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006). The first one is more extensive, longitudinal research. The second one is searching for underlying mechanisms between intergroup contact and its effects. This report attempts to make a theoretical and empirical contribution uncovering a new mediator that integrates two theories from two distinct literatures: intergroup relations and the self. Based on the broad assumption that higher quality of intergroup contact should be associated with more positive intergroup attitudes, we suggest that this effect will be produced, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, at least in part because it increases the perception that outgroup members know and understand the characteristics of ingroup members, thus verifying ingroup self-perception. Specifically, intergroup contact will improve intergroup attitudes through increasing verification of ingroup identities – the match of ingroup and outgroup perceptions regarding the qualities of typical ingroup members (that may or may not characterize individual group members).

Intergroup contact and intergroup attitudes

There is a sizeable literature that reflects the trajectory and developments of the contact hypothesis during the last six decades, that is, when, how, and why it works (e.g. Dovidio, Gartner, & Kawakami, Reference Dovidio, Gaertner and Kawakami2003; Pettigrew, Reference Pettigrew1998; Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006). Nevertheless, although intergroup contact has been consistently recognized as extremely useful for improving intergroup orientations, there is still a dearth of research testing the effects of intergroup contact over time. The literature on longitudinal effects of intergroup contact is quite limited when we consider that its tradition goes back more than five decades (e.g. Binder et al., Reference Binder, Zagefka, Brown, Funke, Kessler, Mummendey and Leyens2009; Brown, Eller, Leeds, & Stace, Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004; Feddes, Noack, & Rutland, Reference Feddes, Noack and Rutland2009; Hamilton & Bishop, Reference Hamilton and Bishop1976; Levin, van Laar, & Sidanius, Reference Levin, van Laar and Sidanius2003). In those contexts where experimental or quasi-experimental designs are difficult to implement, longitudinal research represents a compelling strategy to test causal relationships between intergroup contact and intergroup attitudes (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Finkel, Reference Finkel1995; Pettigrew, Reference Pettigrew1996). The causal direction from contact to attitude change in natural settings is one aspect that remains relatively unexplored and makes this kind of investigation particularly pertinent.

Longitudinal research on intergroup contact has generally shown that contact has positive effects on intergroup attitudes over time (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004; Hamilton & Bishop, Reference Hamilton and Bishop1976; Levin et al., Reference Levin, van Laar and Sidanius2003), but quality rather than quantity of contact tends to be most predictive of positive intergroup attitudes. Our first set of general predictions is that contact quality will be associated with more positive evaluations of the outgroup, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally.

Mediators of the effect of intergroup contact on intergroup attitudes

Revealing the underlying processes explaining the positive effects of intergroup contact on intergroup relations has perhaps been the key point of interest during the new century (see Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2008, for a review). Pettigrew (Reference Pettigrew1998) posits four processes through which intergroup contact operates: a) learning about the outgroup (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004; Eller, Abrams, & Zimmermann, Reference Eller, Abrams and Zimmermann2011), b) changing behavior (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004), c) generating affective ties by reducing intergroup anxiety (Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns, & Voci, Reference Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns and Voci2004; Turner, Hewstone, & Voci, Reference Turner, Hewstone and Voci2007; Turner, Hewstone, Voci, Paolini, & Christ, Reference Turner, Hewstone and Voci2007) or by increasing self-disclosure (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Hewstone and Voci2007), perspective-taking (Craig, Cairns, Hewstone, & Voci, Reference Craig, Cairns, Hewstone and Voci2002) or interpersonal closeness (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004), and finally d) promoting an ingroup reappraisal through “deprovincialization” (Verkuyten, Thijs, & Bekhuis, Reference Verkuyten, Thijs and Bekhuis2010). Additionally, other contextual factors as the perceived importance of contact (van Dick et al., Reference van Dick, Wagner, Pettigrew, Christ, Wolf, Petzel and Jackson2004) or belongingness to a common ingroup identity (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004, Reference Eller and Abrams2006; Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2000) appear to be important.

The novel mechanism we propose here to understand how contact reduces prejudice differs from some previously identified mediators in four ways. First, while previous mediators are mainly outgroup-focused processes, verification of ingroup identity is ingroup-focused. This point is relevant because it means that manipulating variables that affect ingroup self-perception, without affecting the outgroup or the ingroup-outgroup relation, could improve intergroup orientations. Second, none of these previous underlying mechanisms posits the ingroup member “in the eyes” of the outgroup to see how they perceive the ingroup. Third, verification of ingroup identity involves validating an existing identity, which should be easier than modifying certain aspects of that identity. And fourth, whereas much research suggests that affective mediators of intergroup contact are more powerful than cognitive mediators (e.g. Pettigrew & Tropp, Reference Pettigrew, Tropp and Oskamp2000, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2008; see also Swart, Hewstone, Christ, & Voci, Reference Swart, Hewstone, Christ and Voci2011), it is conceivable that the latter have not been sufficiently explored and our work could help to incite a resurgence of a focus on cognitive / learning-based processes. We will develop such a focus in the next few paragraphs.

These findings notwithstanding, an important variable that is under-studied in the intergroup contact literature and that is ingroup-focused are meta-stereotypes - ingroup members’ perceptions about how outgroup members view the ingroup (Gómez, Reference Gómez2002; Vorauer, Main, & O´Connell, Reference Vorauer, Main and O’Connell1998). Past research suggests that people generally assume that outgroup members have a negative image of the ingroup (e.g. Kramer & Wei, Reference Kramer, Wei, Tyler, Kramer and John1999; Sigelman & Tuch, Reference Sigelman and Tuch1997). But more importantly, people also expect a relatively high level of inconsistency between ingroup self-stereotypes and meta-stereotypes (Gómez, Reference Gómez2002; Klein & Azzi, Reference Klein and Azzi2001; Vorauer et al., Reference Vorauer, Main and O’Connell1998). These expectations of being seen inaccurately or being stereotyped lead people to avoid intergroup interactions (Stephan & Stephan, Reference Stephan and Stephan1985; Tropp, Reference Tropp2003; Vorauer et al., Reference Vorauer, Main and O’Connell1998), which obviously make intergroup contact not viable as a strategy to improve intergroup attitudes.

Gómez, Huici, and Morales (Reference Gómez, Huici and Morales2004) demonstrated that intergroup contact improved the evaluation of the outgroup and also increased verification of the ingroup identity. Ingroup identity refers to qualities of typical ingroup members that may or may not characterize individual group members (see Gómez, Seyle, Huici, & Swann, Reference Gómez, Seyle, Huici and Swann2009; Swann, Reference Swann, Van Lang, Kruglanski and Higgins2011). Thus, verification of ingroup identity is the overlap between how ingroup members perceive their group, and how they think that the source (outgroup members in the context of intergroup relations) perceives the ingroup, no matter whether such perceptions describe or do not describe individual group members (Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Seyle, Huici and Swann2009). The greater the overlap between these two perceptions, the higher the verification of ingroup identity. Nevertheless, it has not been tested whether fostering the verification of ingroup identity improves intergroup attitudes. Moreover, no research thus far has examined whether verification of ingroup identity can explain why direct intergroup contact ameliorates intergroup attitudes, and if such a mediating effect holds over time. This is the main focus of the present report.

Verification of ingroup identity

The process of verification of ingroup identity is based on a well-established theory that has been supported by empirical evidence during the last three decades: self-verification theory (Swann, Reference Swann, Suls and Greenwald1983, Reference Swann, Van Lang, Kruglanski and Higgins2011). This theory stipulates that people prefer others to see them in the same way they see themselves (Swann, Reference Swann, Suls and Greenwald1983). Self-verification theory assumes that people base their identities on the treatment they receive from others (e.g., Cooley, Reference Cooley1902; Mead, Reference Mead1934). Identities have an important function because people use them to make predictions about their worlds, guide behavior, and maintain the perception that the world is knowable and coherent. These functions lead people to be strongly motivated to maintain their identities. Interestingly, this motivation occurs whether the identities happen to be positive or negative.

For decades, research on self-verification theory focused on people’s efforts to confirm their personal selves, which refer to qualities that make them unique (see Swann, Chang-Schneider, & Angulo, 2007, for a review). But recently, researchers have demonstrated that self-views should have the same motivational properties when they refer to attributes of the group with which the person is aligned. Chen and co-workers (Chen, Chen, & Shaw, Reference Chen, Chen and Shaw2004; Chen, Shaw, & Jeung, Reference Chen, Shaw and Jeung2006) have shown that people work to verify personal self-views that are linked to group membership or “collective self-views”. Along these lines, Gómez et al. (Reference Gómez, Seyle, Huici and Swann2009) demonstrated that people strive to verify qualities of typical group members of their group, or engage in “verification of ingroup identities” even when they do not themselves possess these qualities and even when such qualities are negative. Importantly, people prefer to interact with and evaluate more positively those who confirm their group identities than those who disconfirm them.

Previous longitudinal research into self-verification supports the prediction that people seek and find verification over time in naturally occurring situations (McNulty & Swann, Reference McNulty and Swann1994). Some other research offers testimony for the longitudinal effects of self-verification (Swan, Kwan, Polzer, & Milton, 2003; Swann, Milton, & Polzer, Reference Swann, Milton and Polzer2000). In the intergroup realm, the most straightforward way to bring about this process of mutual knowing and understanding is through intergroup contact. As a consequence, we predict that the positive effect of contact quality on outgroup evaluation through verification of ingroup identity will be produced also over time, that is, longitudinally. The general lack of longitudinal research in the area could have the consequence that some mediators could go unnoticed because their effects on intergroup attitudes do not surface instantly but only over time (Hovland & Weiss, Reference Hovland and Weiss1951). The present research provides support for such a mediator, never tested up to date in the intergroup contact literature.

Overview of the present research

The present study investigated whether the longitudinal effect of intergroup contact on outgroup evaluations is produced because such contact increases perceptions of verification of ingroup identity over time. The current study focused on Spanish high school students and their intergroup contact and attitudes toward immigrants in Spain. In the last decade, the number of immigrants in Spain has increased substantially, and in Madrid, where the present study was conducted, around 17.12% of the population consists of immigrants Footnote 1 .

Participants were asked about the quality of their intergroup contact with immigrants, how they thought that immigrants perceive Spaniards, how they perceived Spaniards themselves, and about their general evaluation of immigrants. We operationalize verification of ingroup identity as the overlap between how participants think that immigrants perceive Spaniards (i.e. meta-stereotype) and how participants themselves perceive Spaniards (i.e. ingroup stereotype). The higher the overlap, the higher the verification of ingroup identity.

We expect that contact quality will improve the general evaluation of immigrants and verification of ingroup identity. Importantly, we predict that contact quality will improve general evaluation of immigrants through increasing verification of ingroup identity cross-sectionally, and also over time.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Participants at T1 were 142 Spanish high school students. They took part in the present study with permission of the school and their parents. At T2, 12 weeks later, there were 56 girls and 60 boys (mean age = 16.5, SD = .60). The questionnaires at both waves were identical and completed during class, on an individual basis. Participation was voluntary and respondents were informed that they were free to withdraw from the study at any time and that all their responses would be treated completely confidentially. At completion of the longitudinal study, participants were debriefed and thanked.

Measures

Predictor variable: Quality of contact (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003; Islam & Hewstone, Reference Islam and Hewstone1993) was measured by asking participants on 7-point scales ranging from (1) to (7), whether contact with immigrants was perceived to be involuntary or voluntary (completely involuntary–completely voluntary), competitive or cooperative (completely competitive—completely cooperative), positive or negative (completely negative—completely positive), and superficial or intimate (completely superficial—completely intimate), Cronbach’s alphas = .87 at T1 and .88 at T2.

Mediating variables: Verification of ingroup identity was a composite index subtracting meta-stereotypes from ingroup stereotypes. Ingroup stereotypes were measured by asking participants what percentage of Spaniards they think in general possess the following attributes: honest, friendly, intelligent, and hard-working Footnote 2 , Cronbach’s alphas = .70 at T1 and .72 at T2. Meta-stereotypes were measured by asking participants what percentage of Spaniards possess the attributes in question, in the eyes of immigrants, Cronbach’s alphas = .68 at T1 and .66 at T2. Alphas for the index of verification of ingroup identity were .68 and .66 at T1 and T2, respectively. Factor analyses of the scales show that items load into a single factor at both T1 and T2, explaining 42.04% and 49.73% of the variance, respectively. Means displayed in Table 1 indicate that the indices at T1 and T2 are positive. Positive values signify that ingroup-stereotypes are more positive than meta-stereotypes, and the lower the index, the higher the verification of ingroup identity.

Table 1. Changes of Means over Time

Note:

Unless otherwise indicated, numbers are means, standard deviations are in parentheses. *** p < .001

Criterion variable: The General Evaluation Scale (Wright, Aron, McLaughlin-Volpe, & Ropp, Reference Wright, Aron, McLaughlin-Volpe and Ropp1997) instructed respondents to ‘indicate how you feel about immigrants in general’ by using the following bipolar adjective pairs separated by a 7-point scale: cold—warm, negative—positive, friendly—hostile, suspicious—trusting, respect—contempt, disgust—admiration (pairs 3 and 5 were reversed). Responses were scored such that the more positive adjective received the higher score, Cronbach’s alphas were T1 = .80 and T2 = .81, respectively.

Results

Our analytic strategy was first to compare the T1 participants that remained in or dropped out of the study between T1 and T2 to see whether the groups were compatible on the different measures. We then examined changes in scores between the two time points and conducted cross-sectional mediation analysis. Further, to deal with missing data longitudinally, we performed multiple imputation of data. The main analyses then focus on the effect of quality of contact on verification of ingroup identity and general outgroup evaluation and the mediating role of verification of ingroup identity, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally.

Panel attrition and comparison of participants

A MANOVA across the set of measures at T1 yielded a significant multivariate effect, F(4, 133) = 3.05, p < .02, partial η2 = 08. Those students who dropped out of the study showed less positive general evaluations of immigrants (M = 3.37, SD = 1.28) than those who remained in the sample (M = 3.98, SD = 1.02). There were no significant differences in quality of contact or verification of ingroup identity, ps > .47. However, dropping out of the study was not based on self-selection, but was rather due to practical issues: The students that dropped out simply were not present on the day of T2 data collection, but no participants refused to take part in the study. We recognize, however, that the possible generalizability of our longitudinal findings might be limited given the dropout of participants with less positive attitudes toward immigrants.

Changes of Means over Time

A repeated-measures MANOVA revealed that scores on the measures changed significantly over time, F(4, 108) = 5.35, p = .002, partial η2 = 13. Table 1 shows that there was one significant univariate effect of time. Quality of contact increased over the course of the study.

Interrelationships among variables

Table 2 displays the results of correlation analysis among the variables. At both time points quality of contact is associated with more positive meta-stereotypes, higher verification of ingroup identity (i.e. the negative correlation indicates that the higher the quality of contact, the lower the difference between meta-stereotype and ingroup stereotype), and more positive general evaluation of immigrants. More positive general outgroup evaluation is also related to more positive meta-stereotypes and higher verification of ingroup identity. Importantly, a test of the difference between the correlations showed that quality of contact is related significantly more strongly to verification of ingroup identity than to meta-stereotypes, both at T1, z = 4.10, p < .001, and T2, z = 4.83, p < .001. General evaluation of the outgroup is also more strongly correlated with verification of ingroup identity than with meta-stereotypes, both at T1, z = 4.33, p < .001, and T2, z = 3.93, p < .001.

Table 2. Interrelationships of Variables at T1 (above the diagonal) and T2 (below the diagonal). Correlations between the same variables at T1 and T2 are reported in the diagonal

Note:

IG = Ingroup. Numbers are Pearson’s correlations (r).

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Cross-sectional mediation analyses

To test whether the relationship between quality of contact and general evaluation of the outgroup was mediated by verification of ingroup identity, we conducted mediation analyses, separately for T1 and T2. Using the SPSS macro provided by Preacher and Hayes (Reference Preacher and Hayes2008), we conducted a bootstrapping test (n samples = 5,000) for the model. Quality of contact and verification of ingroup identity were standardized prior to using the macro. Figure 1 displays the mediation analysis for T1, and Figure 2 shows the results of the analysis for T2. The effect sizes are ab ps ´s Footnote 3 = .032, for T1, and .035 for T2 (see Preacher & Kelley, Reference Preacher and Kelley2011). The results of both analyses indicate that verification of ingroup identity partially mediated the effect of contact quality on general evaluation of the outgroup, as predicted.

Figure 1. Verification of ingroup identity partially mediates the effect of quality of contact on general outgroup evaluation at T1.

Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). Numbers in parentheses describe total effects. CI Confidence interval. *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Figure 2. Verification of ingroup identity partially mediates the effect of quality of contact on general outgroup evaluation at T2.

Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). Numbers in parentheses describe total effects. CI Confidence interval. *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Figure 3. Path diagram showing a longitudinal analysis of group identity verification as a mediator between contact and general outgroup evaluation.

Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Using Multiple Imputation to Deal With Missing Data

There was sample attrition within the longitudinal sample. We therefore considered the options of listwise deletion of cases or using multiple imputation as a compensatory method of analysis (Enders, Reference Enders2010). Typically, and particularly in longitudinal datasets (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2004), missing data are dealt with by deletion of missing participants, which compromises the power of the tests. Listwise deletion procedures are based on the assumption of Missing Completely At Random (MCAR), which could result in seriously biased estimates with present levels of missingness. SPSS was used to calculate the fraction of missing data. This weights the proportion of missing information in the dataset by the number and quality of data imputations. We used 100 imputations to estimate the fraction missing. This was 6.9% in the longitudinal sample.

Multiple imputation, which is based on the assumption of Missing at Random (MAR), is superior to the method of participant deletion (Rubin, Reference Rubin1987). Data are MAR “if missingness is related to other measured variables in the analysis model, but not to the underlying values of the incomplete variable (i.e., the hypothetical values that would have resulted had the data been complete)” (Baraldi & Enders, Reference Baraldi and Enders2010, p. 7). Given sufficient numbers of covariates to aid imputation (in the present research these included age, sex, and year of study), the assumption of MAR provides results that are less biased than listwise deletion (Graham, Reference Graham2003; Schafer & Olsen, Reference Schafer and Olsen1998). Thus we were able to treat missing data as MAR and to impute the missing data using all variables present in the different datasets. Schafer and Graham (Reference Schafer and Graham2002) recommend 20 imputations in order to generate an accurate final imputed dataset. In each imputation a copy of the dataset is created containing unique imputed values. The multiple sets of parameter estimates and standard errors across imputed data sets are subsequently combined into a single set of results (Baraldi & Enders, Reference Baraldi and Enders2010). To obtain results that are as reliable as possible we conservatively imputed our dataset 100 times, using SPSS (Eller et al., Reference Eller, Abrams and Zimmermann2011; Graham, Olchowski, & Gilreath, Reference Graham, Olchowski and Gilreath2007).

Longitudinal analysis

We used multiple regression analysis to examine the relationships between T1 quality of contact on (a) T2 verification of ingroup identity, controlling for T1 verification of ingroup identity, and on (b) T2 general evaluation, controlling for T1 general evaluation. We also tested the effect of T2 verification of ingroup identity on T2 general evaluation, controlling for T1 general evaluation (cf. Eller et al., Reference Eller, Abrams and Zimmermann2011). T1 quality of contact predicted verification of ingroup identity Footnote 4,5 , b = −2.35, t (109) = −2.02, p < .05, and general outgroup evaluation, b = .16, t(113) = 2.00, p < .05, at T2. T2 verification of ingroup identity was associated with general outgroup evaluation at T2, controlling for general outgroup evaluation at T1, b = −.01, t(109) = −2.16, p = .03. Importantly, the contact-general evaluations relationship was reduced to, b = .13, t(108) = 1.46, p = .14, when verification was included in the regression equation. The size was ab ps ´s =.023 (see Preacher & Kelley, Reference Preacher and Kelley2011), see Figure 3.

Reversed longitudinal analysis

To investigate the causal direction of the variables in the longitudinal model, we employed an identical strategy to the one described above, but reversed the roles of predictor and criterion variables (cf. Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003). There were no significant longitudinal effects.

Discussion

Past research has shown that intergroup contact improves intergroup attitudes, cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Previous work has even identified some underlying mechanisms responsible for such improvements, such as intergroup anxiety, interpersonal closeness, self-disclosure, perspective-taking, behavior modification, knowledge of the outgroup, belongingness to a common ingroup identity, or the perceived importance of intergroup contact (Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004; Paolini et al., Reference Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns and Voci2004; Pettigrew et al., Reference Pettigrew, Christ, Wagner and Stellmacher2007). In this report, we offer testimony that intergroup contact might improve intergroup attitudes, at least in part, through a new cognitive, ingroup-focused mechanism not tested to date, verification of ingroup identity.

Our findings indicate that the quality of intergroup contact is related to a positive evaluation of the outgroup, cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Quality of contact and evaluation of the outgroup correlated more strongly with verification of ingroup identity than with meta-stereotypes, which reinforces the role of verification of ingroup identity rather than of meta-stereotypes. Importantly, as predicted, the positive effect of contact quality on the evaluation of the outgroup was mediated by the perception of verification of ingroup identity, not merely cross-sectionally, but also over time. We should be careful, however, with the generalization of our findings because although the mediation effect seems to be consistent (there is a cross-sectional as well as a longitudinal effect), the sizes of these effects are comparatively small). In summary, the present study shows the positive effects of high-quality contact on verification of ingroup identity, and the potential of verification of ingroup identity to mediate between contact and intergroup attitudes.

Moreover, consistent with Allport’s (Reference Allport1954) original hypothesis (see also Brown & Hewstone, Reference Brown and Hewstone2005), the present study provided additional longitudinal evidence that the quality of contact is particularly influential on intergroup attitudes. Intergroup contact has long-lasting effects and we provided further evidence that the causal direction is from contact to improved attitudes rather than the other way around (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004; Levin et al., Reference Levin, van Laar and Sidanius2003; Stephan & Rosenfield, 1978). It should be recognized, however, that as is the case with the present research, most of the previous investigations considered to be “longitudinal” are really half-longitudinal designs (see Cole & Maxwell, Reference Cole and Maxwell2003), and include only two time points. For a fully longitudinal test of the effects of the mediators, future research should add a third time point (see, for example, Swart et al., Reference Swart, Hewstone, Christ and Voci2011).

In addition, the present research theoretically contributes to the literature of intergroup contact. Pettigrew and Tropp’s (Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006) meta-analyses of intergroup contact showed that affective mediators are more powerful than cognitive ones. However, it is conceivable that the literature of the former is more extensive than that of the latter. Our work opens the door for examining more cognitive and learning-based processes that could be underlying the effects of intergroup contact on intergroup attitudes (for a justification of why verification is a cognitive factor, see Swann, Griffin, Predmore, & Gaines, Reference Swann, Griffin, Predmore and Gaines1987).

Critics might argue that there are at least three limitations to our work. First, given that we have focused on positive traits in our measure of ingroup identity verification, it could be argued that all we have shown is that positive contact fosters positive appraisals, which in turn improve intergroup attitudes. Moreover, it might be also argued that we did not ask participants whether the traits offered to describe the ingroup were self-descriptive or not, which could restrict the validity and generalizability of our findings. To counter these arguments, numerous replications using diverse methodologies have demonstrated that verification strivings are independent of the content or the valence of such identities and even of the extent to which the identities are self-descriptive (see Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Seyle, Huici and Swann2009). But perhaps even more importantly, correlation analysis conducted in the present investigation demonstrates that both contact and outgroup evaluation are more strongly correlated with verification of ingroup identity than with positive meta-stereotypes.

Second, some research needs to be done to understand why verification of ingroup identity mediates the effect of intergroup contact on intergroup attitudes. We see at least two possible mechanisms here. First, it might be the case that verification of ingroup identity would make individuals conscious that in the same way they stereotype the outgroup, the outgroup also stereotypes the ingroup. Being conscious of the fact that the outgroup perceives the ingroup in a similar way that they see themselves would increase intergroup trust or intergroup empathy and/or reduce anxiety (see North & Swann, 2009; Tausch, Hewstone, Schmid, Hughes, & Cairns, Reference Tausch, Hewstone, Schmid, Hughes and Cairns2011). Or second, via a cognitive process, individuals might perceive the outgroup as more intelligent or insightful than they expected, because they possess the capacity of thinking about the ingroup. In line with this reasoning, Gómez et al. (Reference Gómez, Seyle, Huici and Swann2009) found that participants who received a verifying feedback perceived the evaluator as more intelligent and competent than participants who received a disconfirming, enhancing feedback. This last finding, showing that the perceived competence of the evaluator is related to verification rather than enhancement, also helps to reduce the possible limitation of the present study that it included positive traits only.

Finally, someone might ask about the mediating role of verification of ingroup identity over and above the role of other, well-established mediators. We see at least two possibilities here. The first would be that verification of group identity is affecting intergroup orientations through its relation with other existing mediators. For example, verification of ingroup identity implies learning about the outgroup, because ingroup members try to predict how outgroup members see the ingroup. Some other affective mechanisms, such as perspective-taking, or interpersonal closeness, also involve considering the perspective of the outgroup. The second possibility is that verification of ingroup identity is influencing intergroup orientations but without affecting or being affected by other potential mediators. We are more inclined to the first possibility, but the present report cannot speak to these alternatives. Further studies should explore the isolated mediator effect of verification of ingroup identity on intergroup attitudes, but also its mediating properties in interaction with other mediators.

To conclude, some might also criticize the nature of our outcome measure (e.g. Verification of ingroup identity). We maintain that such an index has some disadvantages but also advantages. The main disadvantage is that changes in a composite index may result from either one of the measures that form the index or from the other (in the present case, the ingroup self-stereotype or the meta-stereotypes). Changes only in the ingroup self-stereotype, or changes only in the meta-stereotype can produce the same result for the final index. However, two key advantages of a composite index are that 1) it is more appropriate to capture participants’ unconscious perceptions because it is more difficult for them to be aware that the interest of the researcher is based on a composite measure rather than on two independent measures, and 2) it make sense theoretically because verification of ingroup identity is by definition the comparison between how we perceive ourselves, and how we think we are perceived by others.

The present paper extends theory and research into intergroup contact in two ways, in response to the call by Pettigrew and Tropp (Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006). First, we showed that verification of ingroup identity, an ingroup-focused process, is a relevant mediator of the effects of intergroup contact on intergroup attitudes, cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Our research represents another milestone in the study of mediators that can help to improve the efficacy of intergroup contact on improving intergroup attitudes in general, and reducing intergroup prejudice, in particular. Moreover, we demonstrated the relevance of intragroup processes in intergroup contact. Second, we replicated the causal effect of long-term intergroup contact on intergroup attitudes. In particular, our research supports other evidence showing that the quality of the contact reduces prejudice over time (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Eller, Leeds and Stace2007; Levin et al., Reference Levin, van Laar and Sidanius2003; Eller & Abrams, Reference Eller and Abrams2003, Reference Eller and Abrams2004).

Footnotes

This research and the preparation of this manuscript were supported by Research Fund Grant PSI2009–07008 from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation to Ángel Gómez, and a British Academy Post-Doctoral Fellowship to Anja Eller. We are grateful to Bill Swann for their comments on earlier versions of this paper.

1 These traits were obtained in a preliminary study with 20 participants from a comparable population as the main study (10 girls and 10 boys, mean age = 16.32, SD = .86). Participants were asked to list three positive and three negative traits they thought described Spaniards. The most cited traits were friendly (60%), lazy (50 %), intelligent (45%) and dishonest (40%). Based on discussions with participants in the preliminary study and also with their teachers, they all agreed that asking for the meta-stereotype is a complex task, particularly for young people, so following their suggestion we used all the traits in the same direction, and we transformed lazy and dishonest into hard-working and honest.

2 We assumed that the degree of intergroup contact of our participants with immigrants was high and quite similar among them. We measured the quantity of direct contact and we found that on a scale from 0 (never) to 7 (very often), the mean was significantly higher than the midpoint of the scale (4), M = 4.59, SD = 1.52, t (115) = 4.15, p < .001. In addition, contact quantity was not significantly correlated with the key variables of our study, rs < .14, ps > .13.

3 ab ps is the partially standardized product of paths “a” (effect of the predictor on mediator) and “b” (effect of the mediator on the outcome measure). See Preacher and Kelley (Reference Preacher and Kelley2011).

4 Pooled multiple imputation results only provide the unstandardized regression coefficient (b), not the standardized one (ß).

5 In some cases, the degrees of freedom do not indicate the full sample (N = 116). The differences in N (only 7 in most cases) are due to missing values by some participants on some variables across the two time points.

References

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Baraldi, A. N., & Enders, C. K. (2010). An introduction to modern missing data analyses. Journal of School of Psychology, 48, 537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2009.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, J., Zagefka, H., Brown, R., Funke, F., Kessler, T., Mummendey, A., … Leyens, J.-Ph. (2009). Does contact reduce prejudice or does prejudice reduce contact? A longitudinal test of the contact hypothesis amongst majority and minority groups in three European countries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 843856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013470 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R., Eller, A., Leeds, S., & Stace, K. (2007). Intergroup contact and intergroup attitudes: A longitudinal study. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 692703. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.384 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Hewstone, M. (2005). An integrative theory of intergroup contact. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 255343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(05)37005-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S., Chen, K. Y., & Shaw, L. (2004). Self-verification motives at the collective level of self-definition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 7794. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087890 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S., Shaw, L., & Jeung, K. Y. (2006). Collective self-verification among members of a naturally-occurring group: Possible antecedents and long-term consequences. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 101115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp2802_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. London, UK: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Craig, J., Cairns, E., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2002). Young people’s attitudes to and contact with members of the religious out-group. Unpublished manuscript. University of Ulster, Londonderry, UK.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J., Gaertner, S., & Kawakami, K. (2003). Intergroup contact: The past, present and the future. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430203006001009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eller, A., & Abrams, D. (2003). “Gringos” in Mexico: Cross-sectional and longitudinal effects of language school-promoted contact on intergroup bias. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 5575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430203006001012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eller, A., & Abrams, D. (2004). Come together: Longitudinal comparisons of Pettigrew’s reformulated intergroup contact model and the common ingroup identity model in Anglo-French and Mexican-American contexts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 229256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.194 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eller, A., & Abrams, D. (2006). A people’s entente cordiale? The role of implicit attitude in the relationship between English-French contact, levels of categorization, and explicit intergroup attitudes. Current Research in Social Psychology, 11, 92110.Google Scholar
Eller, A., Abrams, D., & Zimmermann, A. (2011). Two degrees of deparation: Longitudinal and cross-cultural effects of extended contact. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14, 175191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430210391120 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Feddes, A. R., Noack, P., & Rutland, A. (2009). Direct and extended friendship effects on minority and majority children’s interethnic attitudes: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 80, 377390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01266.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, S. E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, A. (2002). Metastereotypes and intergroup relations. If my group stereotypes others, others stereotype my group… and we know. Concept, research lines and future perspectives of metastereotypes. Revista de Psicología Social, 17, 253282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1174/02134740260372982 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, A., Huici, C., & Morales, J. F. (2004). ¡Nos gusta que nos vean... como somos! Implicaciones de la teoría de la auto-verificación a nivel intergrupal [We like others to see us… the way we are. Implications of self-verification theory at a group level]. Revista de Psicología Social, 19, 139157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1174/021347404773820954 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, A., Seyle, C., Huici, C., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2009). Can self-verification strivings fully transcend the self-other barrier? Seeking verification of ingroup identities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 10211044. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016358 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. W. (2003). Adding missing-data relevant variables to FIML-based structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 10, 80100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. W., Olchowski, A. E., & Gilreath, T. D. (2007). How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prevention Science, 8, 206213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0070-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, D. L., & Bishop, G. D. (1976). Attitudinal and behavioral effects of initial integration of White suburban neighborhoods. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 4767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1976.tb02494.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W., (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 635650. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/266350 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 700710. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167293196005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, O., & Azzi, A. E. (2001). The strategic confirmation of metastereotypes: How group members attempt to tailor an out-group’s representation of themselves. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 279293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466601164759 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R. M., & Wei, J. (1999). Social uncertainty and the problem of trust in social groups: The social self in doubt. In Tyler, T. R., Kramer, R. M., & John, O. P. (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 145168). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Levin, S., van Laar, C., & Sidanius, J. (2003). The effects of ingroup and outgroup friendships on ethnic attitudes in college: A longitudinal study. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 7692. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430203006001013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNulty, S. E., & Swann, W. B. (1994). Identity negotiation in roommate relationships: The self as architect and consequence of social reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 10121023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
North, R. J. & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2009). Self-Verification 360°: Illuminating the light and dark sides. Self and Identity, 8, 131146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298860802501516 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., & Voci, A. (2004). Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on judgments of catholics and protestants in Northern Ireland: The mediating role of an anxiety-reduction mechanism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 770786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262848 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. (1996). How to think like a social scientist. New York, NY: Harper Collins College Publishers.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 6585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wagner, U., & Stellmacher, J. (2007). Direct and indirect intergroup contact effects on prejudice: A normative interpretation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 31, 411425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2006.11.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2000). Does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Recent meta-analytic findings. In Oskamp, S. (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 93114). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 922934. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.504 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16, 93115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022658 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. B. (1987) Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York, NY: J. Wiley & Sons CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schafer, J. L., & Olsen, M. K. (1998). Multiple imputation for multivariate missing-data problems: A data analyst’s perspective. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 545571. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3304_5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigelman, L., & Tuch, S. A. (1997). Metasterotypes: Blacks’ perceptions of whites’ stereotypes of blacks. Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 87101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/297788 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues 41, 157175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01134.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In Suls, J. & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 3366). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr. (2011). Self-verification theory. In Van Lang, P., Kruglanski, A., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 2342). London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Chang-Schneider, C., & Angulo, S. (2007). Self-verification in relationships as an adaptive process. In Wood, J., Tesser, A., & Holmes, J. (Eds.), Self and relationships (pp 4972). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Griffin, J. J., Predmore, S., & Gaines, B. (1987). The cognitive-affective crossfire: When self-consistency confronts self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 881889. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.881 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, W. B. Jr., Kwan, V. S. Y., Polzer, J. T., & Milton, L. P. (2003). Fostering group identification and creativity in diverse groups: The role of individuation and self-verification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 13961406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203256868 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, W. B. Jr., Milton, L. P., & Polzer, J. T. (2000). Should we create a niche or fall in line? Identity negotiation and small group effectiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 238250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swart, H., Hewstone, M., Christ, O., & Voci, A. (2011). Affective mediators of intergroup contact: A three-wave longitudinal study in South Africa. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 12211238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024450 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., Schmid, K., Hughes, J., & Cairns, E. (2011). Extended contact effects as a function of closeness of relationship with ingroup contacts. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 14, 239254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430210390534 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tropp, L. R. (2003). The psychological impact of prejudice: Implications for intergroup contact. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 131149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430203006002001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2007). Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: The mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 369388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.369 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., Voci, A., Paolini, S., & Christ, O. (2007). Reducing prejudice via direct and extended cross-group friendship. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 212255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10463280701680297 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wolf, C., Petzel, T., … Jackson, J. S. (2004). Role of perceived importance in intergroup contact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 211227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.211 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verkuyten, M., Thijs, J., & Bekhuis, H. (2010). Intergroup contact and ingroup reappraisal: Examining the deprovincialization thesis. Social Psychology Quarterly, 73, 398416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0190272510389015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., Main, K. J., & O’Connell, G. B. (1998). How do individuals expect to be viewed by members of lower status groups? Content and implications of meta-stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 917937. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.75.4.917 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, S. C., Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., & Ropp, S. A. (1997). The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 7390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.73 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Changes of Means over Time

Figure 1

Table 2. Interrelationships of Variables at T1 (above the diagonal) and T2 (below the diagonal). Correlations between the same variables at T1 and T2 are reported in the diagonal

Figure 2

Figure 1. Verification of ingroup identity partially mediates the effect of quality of contact on general outgroup evaluation at T1.Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). Numbers in parentheses describe total effects. CI Confidence interval. *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Verification of ingroup identity partially mediates the effect of quality of contact on general outgroup evaluation at T2.Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). Numbers in parentheses describe total effects. CI Confidence interval. *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Path diagram showing a longitudinal analysis of group identity verification as a mediator between contact and general outgroup evaluation.Note: Negative coeficients involving Verification of Ingroup Identity indicate high verification. Numbers are standardized partial regression coeficients (betas). *p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.