Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-hpxsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T10:21:05.670Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Startle Modification and P50 Gating in Schizophrenia Patients and Controls: Russian Population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 March 2016

Zinaida I. Storozheva*
Affiliation:
Serbsky National Research Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry (Russia)
Anna.V. Kirenskaya
Affiliation:
Serbsky National Research Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry (Russia)
Vladimir Y. Novototsky-Vlasov
Affiliation:
Serbsky National Research Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry (Russia)
Klavdia Y. Telesheva
Affiliation:
Serbsky National Research Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry (Russia)
Mikhail Pletnikov
Affiliation:
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (USA)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Zinaida I. Storozheva. E-mail: storozheva_zi@mail.ru
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Prepulse modification of the acoustic startle response (ASR) and P50 gating are potential neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia and may be used in the construction of valid clinical biomarkers. Such approach requires a large amount of data obtained in the representative samples from different gender, socio-typological and ethnic groups, replicating studies using the similar protocols and meta-analyses. This is a replication study of ASR and the first study of P50 suppression in Russian patients with schizophrenia (n = 28) and healthy controls (n = 25). ASR and P50 were estimated according to standard protocols. Patients exhibited increased baseline ASR latency (d = 0.35, p = .026) and reduced prepulse inhibition (PPI) at 60 ms interval (d = 0.39, p = .003) and 120 ms interval (d = 0.37, p = .005) relative to controls. In the P50 test patients displayed greater S2 response amplitude (d = 0.24, p = .036) and deficit of P50 suppression (d = 0.43, p = .001). No correlations of PPI and P50 suppression were found in both groups. Only in controls prepulse ASR facilitation (at 2500 ms interval) positively correlated with P50 suppression (r = –.514, p = .013). In patients PPI displayed significant correlations with Difficulty in abstract thinking (N5: r = –.49, p = .005) and Hallucination (P3: r = .40, p = .036) PANSS scales. Logistic regression showed that the combination of PPI and P50 suppression could serve as a diagnostic predictor. Obtained results demonstrated that both PPI and P50 could be regarded as potential schizophrenia biomarkers in Russian population.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2016 

Deficits in early information processing potentially leading to sensory overload have been considered a central feature of schizophrenia. Sensory-motor and sensory gating are measured by prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response (ASR) and P50 suppression of the auditory event-related potential respectively.

PPI normally occurs when a weak prestimulus precedes a strong “startling” stimulus by 50–300 ms. This weak prestimulus inhibits the response to startling stimulus measured by m. orbicularis oculi EMG, i.e. motor response. Other startle parameters, such as habituation, baseline amplitude and latency also may be useful measures of information processing. PPI is deficient in schizophrenia patients and unaffected relatives (Kumari, Das, Zachariah, Ettinger & Sharma, Reference Kumari, Das, Zachariah, Ettinger and Sharma2005; Wynn et al., Reference Wynn, Dawson, Schell, McGee, Salveson and Green2004).

The P50 is an early positive component of the auditory averaged response with latency about 50 ms. When using a paired click paradigm with a 500-ms interval, a reduced P50 response after the second click (S2, or test stimulus) compared with P50 response after the first click (S1, or conditioning stimulus) is observed in healthy subjects. P50 suppression is thought to be related to auditory sensory gating (Freedman, Waldo, Bickford-Wimer, & Nagamoto, Reference Freedman, Waldo, Bickford-Wimer and Nagamoto1991), because the dependent measure is an event related potential (ERP) rather than a motor response. P50 suppression deficits in schizophrenia patients are persistent and found in both acutely ill and more stable schizophrenic outpatients as well as in their unaffected relatives (Thaker, Reference Thaker2008; Turetsky et al., Reference Turetsky, Calkins, Light, Olincy, Radant and Swerdlow2007).

Being the putative measures of early information gating P50 and PPI generally are not associated and “diverge” in normal subjects (Brenner, Edwards, Carroll, Kieffaber, & Hetrick, Reference Brenner, Edwards, Carroll, Kieffaber and Hetrick2004; Schwarzkopf, Lamberti & Smith, Reference Schwarzkopf, Lamberti and Smith1993) and in schizophrenia patients (Braff, Light, & Swerdlow, Reference Braff, Light and Swerdlow2007; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Summerfelt, Wonodi, Adami, Buchanan and Thaker2007).

The lack of association between P50 suppression and PPI is not surprising because of different stimulus parameters (usually clicks vs. noises), different responses (cortical vs. muscular) and different optimal lead intervals (500 ms vs. 60–120 ms). Even being elicited by the same stimuli and the same lead intervals PPI and P50 suppression don’t display significant relation (Oranje, Geyer, Bocker, Kenemans, & Verbaten, Reference Oranje, Geyer, Bocker, Kenemans and Verbaten2006).

PPI is subserved by various forebrain structures including hippocampus and cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic circuitry. The existing data suggest that P50 suppression is regulated by wide-ranging neural circuitry, involving hippocampal structures, temporo-parietal and prefrontal cortical regions (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Olincy, Waldo, Harris, Griffith, Stevens and Freedman1998; Arciniegas et al., Reference Arciniegas, Topkoff, Rojas, Sheeder, Teale, Young and Adler2001; Grunwald et al., Reference Grunwald, Butros, Pezer, von Oertzen, Fernández, Schaller and Elger2003; Turetsky et al., Reference Turetsky, Calkins, Light, Olincy, Radant and Swerdlow2007). Thus, P50 generation circuitry has neural substrates interacting and overlapping with those of PPI, especially in mesial temporal lobe structures (Li, Du, Li, Wu, & Wu, Reference Li, Du, Li, Wu and Wu2009; Swerdlow, Geyer, & Braff, Reference Swerdlow, Geyer and Braff2001) and frontal cortex (Oranje et al., Reference Oranje, Geyer, Bocker, Kenemans and Verbaten2006). Both common and distinct neurochemical mechanisms were also revealed for PPI and P50 suppression (Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Gault, Hopkins, Logel, Vianzon, Short and Freedman2002; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Croft, Scholes, Dunne, O'Neill, Leung and Nathan2008).

PPI and P50 suppression are promising as neurophysiological probes in psychiatric researches and as biomarkers in clinical trials (Kumari et al., Reference Kumari, Premkumar, Fannon, Aasen, Raghuvanshi, Anilkumar and Kuipers2012). These measures are also viewed as valid candidates to endophenotypes of schizophrenia, i.e., quantitative traits in the putative pathophysiologic pathway from the genotype to the phenotype (Turetsky et al., Reference Turetsky, Calkins, Light, Olincy, Radant and Swerdlow2007).

The absence of close association between these biomarkers seems to be advantageous for their concurrent use for elaboration of complex strategy in the neurophysiological and genetic subtyping of the disorder. According to opinion of leading investigators in this field (Swerdlow, Weber, Qu, Ligh, & Braff, Reference Swerdlow, Weber, Qu, Light and Braff2008; Turetsky et al., Reference Turetsky, Calkins, Light, Olincy, Radant and Swerdlow2007) the development of such complex strategy needs combination of multiple measures, in particular, PPI, P50 suppression and antisaccades.

At the same time, constructing of valid clinical biomarkers and endophenotypes needs closer examination of the traits using representative samples from different gender and ethnic groups, replicating studies using the similar protocols and several biomarkers, and meta-analyses (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Dobie, Cadenhead, Olincy, Freedman, Green and Braff2007). The most possible ethnic variability of experimental cohort is of great advantage in the study of any psychophysiological tests especially those, which may be promising as disease hallmarks.

The aim of the present study was the concurrent estimation of PPI and P50 suppression in Russian population of schizophrenia patients and healthy participants. We also intended preliminary estimation of these measures and their combination validity as possible tools for differentiation of healthy participants and schizophrenia patients.

Materials and method

Participants

53 male subjects (aged 19–54 years) participated in the study: 28 schizophrenia patients (SCH group) and 25 healthy volunteers (CON group).

The patients (mean age 35.3 ± 2.5 years) were admitted to Serbsky National Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry to undergo examination: 25 patients were criminal offenders, and 3 persons were examined within the framework of civil litigation. Most of patients (23 subjects) were classified as paranoid (F20.06), one patient had undifferentiated schizophrenia (F20.3), another one had residual schizophrenia (F20.5) and three patients had simple schizophrenia (F20.6) according to ICD-10. Patients’ current symptomatology was assessed with Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS). Mean sum of PANSS scales was 86.83 ± 2.34.

All of the patients were free from medication for the period exceeding one month before the investigation.

Healthy participants (25 subjects, mean age 26.4 ± 1.3 years) were ethnic Russians recruited from the central part of European Russia; 4 subjects were criminal offenders.

The subjects of the control group were evaluated by a psychiatrist to exclude any psychiatric diagnosis. Subjects with a history of neurological disorder, head trauma with loss of consciousness, substance abuse, or other medical condition that might affect brain functioning were excluded from participation in the study.

Written informed consent for the investigation was obtained from all subjects.

Each subject participated in two experimental sessions: (1) acoustic startle response (ASR) recording, and (2) P50 recording. Sessions (1) and (2) were carried out separately in two days. The sequence of sessions was counterbalanced.

Participants were asked to refrain from smoking for 1 hour prior to the testing.

Procedure

The testing took place in a quiet, lighted, and electrically shielded room. During the experimental session, participants were seated comfortably in a reclining chair and instructed to look straight ahead at a neutral picture and keep their eyes open.

Acoustic Startle Measurement

Experimental design

The procedure was developed on the basis of Consortium on Genetics of Schizophrenia recommendations (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Dobie, Cadenhead, Olincy, Freedman, Green and Braff2007).

ASR was evoked using 110 dB, 40-ms bursts of white noise. The prepulse stimuli were 85 dB, 20-ms bursts of white noise presented with leading interval (LI) of 60, 120 and 2500 ms prior to the startle stimulus. The stimuli were delivered binaurally from a computer sound card via earphones.

The startle session began with a 60 second acclimation period consisting of 70 dB white noise, which continued as the background noise throughout the session. The session included 4 experimental blocks. First block was a habituation series of 5 pulse alone stimuli (HAB1). HAB1 was followed by two PPI-BLOCKS. Each PPI-BLOCK consisted of 8 pulse alone trials and 8 prepulse trials (prepulse plus pulse) at each of the three designated prepulse intervals (60, 120 and 2500 ms), for a total of 32 startle stimuli presented in a pseudorandom order. Finally, a second habituation block (HAB2) of five pulse alone stimuli was presented at the end of the session. Inter-trial intervals were 8–22 s (M = 15 s).

Signal Recording

Two Skintact 10 mm Ag/AgCl electrodes (Leonard Lang GMBH, Austria) were positioned below and lateral to each eye over m. orbicularis oculi, with a ground electrode on the middle of forehead. Electrode impedance was kept below 10 kΩ. The eye-blink component of the acoustic startle response was measured using a 4-channel NeuroMyograph-01 (MBN, Russia). Sampling rate was 1000 Hz, band-pass filters 0.5–200 Hz. Recorded EMG activity was analyzed visually to remove artifact trials. Trials containing marked muscle activity from 20 ms before to 20 ms after the main stimulus delivery were discarded.

Integrated EMG traces were obtained by digital high-pass filtration with a cut-off frequency of 10 Hz (to remove low-frequency components), followed by rectification and smoothing with a moving average filter. Amplitude and latency of the maximum deviation from the baseline were determined automatically at 20–200 ms after the main stimulus presentation in integrated EMG traces.

Definition of Variables

Baseline startle magnitude was calculated as a mean for 5 pulse alone trials during the 1st (habituation) block. Percent habituation was calculated using the first and last five pulse alone blocks ([(mean amplitude HAB1-mean amplitude HAB2)/mean amplitude HAB1] ×100).

PPI was calculated for each of the three leading intervals: LI = 60ms, LI = 120ms and LI = 2500 ms as:

100 × (1 – [(mean magnitude on prepulse trials) / (mean magnitude on PPI-BLOCK pulse alone trials)]).

PPI calculation was performed separately for 2nd and 3rd blocks as well as total for whole experiment.

P50 suppression

Experimental design

Auditory stimuli (85 dB, 1 ms) were delivered using Stim2 software (Compumedics NeuroScan) through earphones. The subjects were presented with pairs of auditory clicks (S1, conditioning click, S2, testing click) with a 500 ms inter-click interval. Inter-trial intervals were randomized between 8 and 15 s. Two runs, each consisting of 50 identical pairs of clicks (100 total trials) were recorded.

Signal Recording

Electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings were made with 19 Ag/AgCl electrodes mounted according to the international 10–20 system and left and right earlobes. All active electrodes were referred to the left mastoid. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. Prior to further analysis, all channels were recalibrated (off-line) with respect to linked (averaged) earlobes.

EEG was amplified and monitored with Synamp2 (Compumedics NeuroScan) amplifier. Data were collected at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz with an analog band-pass filter of 1–100 Hz.

EEG records were filtered with high-pass filter 10Hz, 6-dB slope. The trials then were visually examined to reject muscle, EOG and other apparent artifacts.

Time-locked evoked potentials were obtained by averaging all artifact-free epochs. The vertex channel Cz was used for P50 data analysis (Nagamoto, Adler, Waldo, Griffith, & Freedman, Reference Nagamoto, Adler, Waldo, Griffith and Freedman1991).

Definition of Variables

For S1, P50 was defined as the most positive deflection from 40 ms to 90 ms after stimulus presentation. For S2, P50 was selected within a window of the S1 latency ± 10 ms for each subject. P50 amplitude was defined as the absolute difference between the P50 peak and the preceding negative trough. P50 suppression was defined as percent of P50 amplitudes ratio [(S1 amplitude – S2 amplitude)/ S1 amplitude x 100].

Subjects were not included in analyses if they had unacceptably small initial P50 amplitude (i.e., < 1.0 μV) or were P50 “nonresponders”. Therefore eight subjects had to be excluded from analysis and the data of 20 control subjects and 25 schizophrenia patients were analyzed in the P50 suppression paradigm.

Statistical analysis

Non-parametric Mann-Whitney test was applied for between-group difference estimation. Spearmen Rank Order Correlations were calculated to assess association of PPI and P50 suppression rates.

Logit analysis was applied for preliminary study of probable validity of PPI, P50 suppression rate, and their combination for differentiation of participants groups.

STATISTICA 6.0 software was used in all analytical procedures.

Results

Baseline parameters and habituation of ASR

No significant between-group difference in baseline startle amplitude was seen. SCH group displayed significantly longer baseline latency of response recorded from the right eye and a trend to increase of response latency at the left eye (Table 1).

Table 1. Baseline parameters and habituation of ASR in CON and SCH groups

Prepulse inhibition of ASR

In SCH group PPI deficit relative to control level was observed at LI 60 ms and 120 ms. At the left eye PPI impairment was significant during 2nd and 3rd blocks, and in whole experiment. At the right eye PPI decrease in SCH group relative to healthy participants was significant during 2nd block and in whole experiment whereas data obtained during 3rd block displayed between-group differences of borderline significance.

No PPI was found at LI 2500 ms in either group (Table 2). Sixty-seven and fifty-five of participants in CON and SCH groups respectively displayed prepulse facilitation at this LI so that response to prepulse + pulse trials was increased relative to response to pulse alone ones. No between-group difference in prepulse modulation at LI 2500ms was revealed by Mann-Whitney test.

Table 2. PPI at different lead intervals in CON and SCH groups (%%)

P50 suppression

The data of 20 control subjects and 25 schizophrenia patients were analyzed.

Comparison of P50 amplitudes to conditioning S1 click revealed no significant between-group difference (Table 3). At the same time, P50 amplitudes to S2 click were significantly higher in SCH compared to CON group. This suppression difference was reflected in P50 rate that was significantly lower in patients than in healthy subjects.

Table 3. Comparison of Group Means (Mann-Whitney test) for P50 suppression measures in healthy subjects and schizophrenia patients

The effect of age

Age was differentially distributed across groups; the SCH group was older than the CON group (35.3 ± 2.5 vs. 26.4 ± 1.3 years, p = .0022). Correlation analysis revealed no significant effects of age on P50, ASR and PPI measures in either of groups, as well as in total cohort of participants (R < 0.26, p > .209 in CON group, and R < 0.19 p > .162 in Sch group). At that, the pattern of between-group differences in ASR and P50 measures remained unchanged after exclusion of the youngest 20% of healthy controls and the oldest 20% of patients (mean age SCH vs. CON = 30.4±6,0 y vs. 27,8±5,3 y, p = .18).

Comparison of the mean values of PPI at the right and left eyes in groups adjusted for age (Table 4) by Wilcoxon matched pairs test for dependent variables revealed in SCH group significantly higher level of PPI (LI = 60 ms) at the right than at the left eye (n = 22, T = 65, p = .045). In CON group the difference between PPI levels obtained at the left and right eye was not significant. The between-group difference of PPI at LI 120 ms was significant at the left eye and was at the level of trend at the right eye.

Table 4. PPI and P50 measures in CON and SCH groups adjusted for age

Correlations of ASR and P50 measures

Analysis was performed in 20 control subjects and 25 schizophrenia patients. Baseline ASR amplitude, habituation rate, PPI at LI 60 ms and 120 ms didn’t display any significant correlations with S1 and S2 amplitudes or P50 suppression in either group. In CON but not in SCH group baseline ASR latency at the right eye displayed moderate negative correlation with P50 suppression (r = –0.514, p = .013).

Only in CON but not in SCH group PPI at LI 2500 ms recorded at the right eye during the 2nd block positively correlated with S2 amplitude of P50 (r = 0.62, p = .0029) and negatively with P50 suppression rate (r = –0.57, p = .0073).

Correlations with PANSS scales

Baseline ASR amplitude displayed significant positive correlation with scale O2 – Anxiety and O4 - Tension. ASR habituation rate positively correlated with N3 scale - Poor rapport, O11 scale – Poor attention and P6 scale - Suspiciousness /persecution.

Surprising positive correlation was revealed between PPI level and P3 scale - Hallucinations. At the same time PPI displayed negative correlation with N5 scale – Difficulty in abstract thinking (Table 5). Correlation between N5 and P3 scales was not significant (r = –0.20, p = .28).

Table 5. Correlations of ASR measures with PANSS scales in SCH group

Note: R – Spearmen rank-order correlation, ns – non-significant R.

No correlations between P50 gating and PANSS scales were detected.

Predictive logit-models

Results in the previous sections show that SCH patients differed significantly from controls on P50 suppression rate and on PPI level, and these two measures were not associated in either group. We tried then to examine whether P50 suppression rate and PPI or their combination could serve as a predictor for groups differentiation, using binary logistic regression, where P50 suppression rates and PPI (measured from the left eye, whole experiment) were taken as independent variables, and group (y = 0 for CON and y = 1 for SCH) as the dependent variable.

Three equations had been generated by STATISTICA 6.0 (Table. 6) where y was a probability of tested person attribution to SCH group and three sets of predictors (PPI, P50, P50+PPI) were used as arguments.

Table 6. Results of logit analysis of PPI and P50 suppression as predictors for differentiation of CON and SCH groups

Thus, relative high level of specificity (attribution of healthy subjects to CONl group) – about 85–90% - was observed for all three models (Table 6). At the same time sensitivities (attribution of patients to SCH group) of models developed with P50 gating only or with PPI only as predictors were quite low (35.71 and 57.41% respectively). The combination of these measures increased sensitivity of model up to 71.43%. It can be concluded that complex use of P50 suppression rate and PPI improves the model quality and predictive validity.

Discussion

This article presents the results of replication study of acoustic startle prepulse modification and P50 suppression in Russian schizophrenia patients’ and healthy subjects population.

We didn’t observe any difference in baseline ASR amplitude and habituation rate between patients and healthy persons. These results are in agreement with those obtained by some (Hasenkamp et al., Reference Hasenkamp, Epstein, Green, Wilcox, Boshoven, Lewison and Duncan2010; Quednow et al., Reference Quednow, Frommann, Berning, Kuhn, Maier and Wagner2008; Wynn et al., Reference Wynn, Dawson, Schell, McGee, Salveson and Green2004) but not all (Braff, Grillon, & Geyer, Reference Braff, Grillon and Geyer1992; Geyer, Swerdlow, Mansbach, & Braff, Reference Geyer, Swerdlow, Mansbach and Braff1990; Ludewig, Geyer, & Vollenweider, Reference Ludewig, Geyer and Vollenweider2003; Quednow et al., Reference Quednow, Wagner, Westheide, Beckmann, Bliesener, Maier and Kühn2006) authors. ASR habituation is regarded as a kind of information gating measures (Blumental, Reference Blumental1997), but its impairment isn’t seen in schizophrenics as stable as PPI deficit and this problem is to be solved.

Differences between schizophrenics and healthy participants in ASR latency have been reported previously (Braff, Swerdlow, & Geyer, Reference Braff, Swerdlow and Geyer1999; Geyer & Braff, Reference Geyer and Braff1982; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Light, Cadenhead, Sprock, Hsieh and Braff2006), but not in all studies (Braff et al., Reference Braff, Grillon and Geyer1992; Parwani et al., Reference Parwani, Duncan, Bartlett, Madonick, Efferen, Rajan and Rotrosen2000). High level of heritability of ASR latency was displayed by Hasenkamp et al. (Reference Hasenkamp, Epstein, Green, Wilcox, Boshoven, Lewison and Duncan2010) and significant effect of ethnicity was proved for this measure (Hasenkamp et al., Reference Hasenkamp, Norrholm, Green, Lewison, Boshoven, Keyes and Duncan2008; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Sprock, Light, Cadenhead, Calkins, Dobie and Braff2007). In the present study, longer ASR latency was shown to be inherent for Russian patients with schizophrenia. In our previous study (16 healthy and 16 patients with schizophrenia) between-group difference in ASR latency was at the level of a trend (59.4 ± 2.3 ms compared to 65.0 ± 2.4, p = .076) (Storozheva et al., Reference Storozheva, Kirenskaya, Lazarev, Novototskii-Vlasov, Samylkin and Fastovtsov2012).

In agreement with our previous results (Storozheva et al., Reference Storozheva, Kirenskaya, Lazarev, Novototskii-Vlasov, Samylkin and Fastovtsov2012) this replication study revealed deficit of PPI in SCH group at LI 60 and 120 ms, which was more prominent at the left eye. Our results contradict the data obtained by other authors who found mainly right-sided PPI impairment in schizophrenia (Braff, Grillon, & Geyer Reference Braff, Grillon and Geyer1992; Filion, Dawson, & Schell, Reference Filion, Dawson and Schell1993; Hasenkamp et al., Reference Hasenkamp, Epstein, Green, Wilcox, Boshoven, Lewison and Duncan2010; Parwani et al., Reference Parwani, Duncan, Bartlett, Madonick, Efferen, Rajan and Rotrosen2000). This discrepancy may be due to ethnic peculiarity of our participants or be specific for personality of our participants who were mainly criminal offenders. The difference in PPI between offenders and non-offenders suffering from schizophrenia as well as the possible influence of eye dominance have to be studied in larger cohort of participants.

Data about association of PPI and psychotic symptom severity in schizophrenia are controversial (Hasenkamp et al., Reference Hasenkamp, Kelley, Egan, Green, Wilcox, Boshoven and Duncan2011; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Light, Cadenhead, Sprock, Hsieh and Braff2006). We found negative correlations between Difficulty in abstract thinking scale and PPI. In our previous investigation the correlations of PPI parameters with P3 and N5 PANSS scales reached the level of a trend (.07 < p < .1) only in cohort without experience of psychoactive substances (16 persons). This somewhat corresponds to data obtained by Perry & Braff (Reference Perry and Braff1994) who showed association of PPI decrease and Ego Impairment Index. Positive correlations between PPI and Hallucinations scale revealed in our study may reflect compensatory mechanisms. It was proposed by Cadenhead (Reference Cadenhead2011) who showed that greater PPI in prodromal state associated with higher probability of further psychosis development. Ambiguous associations between PPI and psychotic symptoms were shown in experiments with dopamine agonists and NMDA antagonists, which evoked psychotic-like state in healthy humans and at the same time might increase PPI in some individuals depending on substances’ doses (Abel, Allin, Hemsley, & Geyer, Reference Abel, Allin, Hemsley and Geyer2003; Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Madonick, Parwani, Angrist, Rajan, Chakravorty and Rotrosen2001; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Lelham, Sutherland Owens, Chang, Sassen and Talledo2009; Talledo, Sutherland Owens, Schortinghuis, & Swerdlow, Reference Talledo, Sutherland Owens, Schortinghuis and Swerdlow2009; It’s worth to note that hallucinations are viewed by some investigators as reflection of compensatory mechanisms in diseased brain (Sperling, Bleich, Maihöfner, & Reulbach, Reference Sperling, Bleich, Maihöfner and Reulbach2009).

Another kind of information gating, i.e. P50 suppression, also was studied for the first time in Russian population. In the P50 paradigm, we didn’t observe any between-group difference in S1 response amplitude. At the same time, S2 response amplitude was significantly greater and P50 suppression was impaired in patients group. These results are in agreement with data presented by many authors (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Pachtman, Franks, Pecevich, Waldo and Freedman1982; Adler et al., Reference Adler, Olincy, Waldo, Harris, Griffith, Stevens and Freedman1998; Braff et al., Reference Braff, Light and Swerdlow2007; Freedman et al., Reference Freedman, Adler, Myles-Worsley, Nagamoto, Miller, Kisley and Waldo1996; Hall, Taylor, Salisbury, & Levy, Reference Hall, Taylor, Salisbury and Levy2011; Nagamoto et al., Reference Nagamoto, Adler, Waldo, Griffith and Freedman1991; Oranje et al., Reference Oranje, Geyer, Bocker, Kenemans and Verbaten2006,). We didn’t reveal any correlations of P50 suppression and PANSS scales, which also corresponds to results of the most of studies (Braff et al., Reference Braff, Light and Swerdlow2007; Brenner et al., 2007; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Summerfelt, Wonodi, Adami, Buchanan and Thaker2007; Potter, Summerfelt, Gold, & Buchanan, Reference Potter, Summerfelt, Gold and Buchanan2006; Schwarzkopf et al., Reference Schwarzkopf, Lamberti and Smith1993).

We have found no correlations between PPI and P50 suppression in either group. Some authors (Braff et al., Reference Braff, Light and Swerdlow2007; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Summerfelt, Wonodi, Adami, Buchanan and Thaker2007) pointed out that an absence of association between P50 suppression and PPI is at least partially due to the difference in the length of stimuli onset asynchrony. In this study, we have revealed correlation between long-lead prepulse facilitation of ASR (at LI 2500 ms) and P50 suppression. Similar correlation was found by Hong et al. (Reference Hong, Summerfelt, Wonodi, Adami, Buchanan and Thaker2007) for LI 500 ms (i.e. the equal to the length of S1-S2 interval in P50 suppression test). ASR facilitation by discrete prepulse at long-lead interval was shown to be purely human phenomenon, and it wasn’t observed in animals (Hoffman & Wible, Reference Hoffman and Wible1969, Putnam & Vanman, Reference Putnam, Vanman, Dawson, Schell and Bohmelt1999).). It is thought to be a measure of sustained attention to irrelevant stimuli, especially at the beginning of stimuli delivery when prepulse hadn’t acquired conditional signal quality yet (Filion et al., Reference Filion, Dawson and Schell1993, Filion, Dawson, & Schell, Reference Filion, Dawson and Schell1994). In such way correlations between ASR prepulse facilitation and P50 suppression may reflect the existence of common brain mechanisms for sensory (but not sensory-motor) gating and sustained attention to irrelevant signals.

Negative correlation of ASR baseline latency and P50 suppression needs further study and can be regarded as an evidence of significance of information processing velocity for sensory gating. It should be noted that associations of P50 and ASR measures were observed only in healthy participants but not in schizophrenia patients. This difference of correlation pattern may reflect a change in interaction of brain structures involved in information processing which occurs in patients.

Preliminary analysis showed that neither P50 suppression nor PPI could serve alone as valid diagnostic tool. PPI impairment is observed in several other neuropsychiatric disorders including obsessive-compulsive disorder, comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and tic disorder, and Huntington’s Disease (Carroll, Vohs,O'donnell, Shekhar, & Hetrick, Reference Carroll, Vohs, O’donnell, Shekhar and Hetrick2007; Conzelmann et al., Reference Conzelmann, Pauli, Mucha, Jacob, Gerdes, Romanos and Weyers2010; Giakoumaki et al., Reference Giakoumaki, Roussos, Rogdaki, Karli, Bitsios and Frangou2007; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Paulsen, Braff, Butters, Geyer and Swenson1995). P50 suppression deficit has been found after traumatic brain injury (Arciniegas et al, Reference Arciniegas, Topkoff, Rojas, Sheeder, Teale, Young and Adler2001), and in bipolar illness (Olincy & Martin, Reference Olincy and Martin2005; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Hall, McDonald, Marshall, Walshe, Murray and Bramon2007). But according to supposition of Swerdlow et al. (Reference Swerdlow, Weber, Qu, Light and Braff2008) we have shown that concurrent use of PPI and P50 paradigms significantly increased the quality of diagnostic model. It can be concluded that complex study of multiple measures of forebrain inhibitory functions may help to identify patterns of normal vs. deficient functions.

In Russian population increased baseline startle latency at the right eye, impaired PPI (more prominent at the left eye), increased P50 amplitude to S2 click, and decreased level of P50 suppression were found in schizophrenia patients (most of patients were criminal offenders) compared to healthy subjects. P50 suppression level didn’t correlate with PPI in any group, but in healthy control it displayed negative correlation with baseline ASR latency and positive correlation with long-lead prepulse facilitation of ASR. P50 suppression and PPI level could be regarded as potential disease biomarkers that are promising for the development of additional psychophysiological methods of diagnosis and examination in studied ethnic and socio-typological population.

The work was supported by Russian Humanitarian Scientific Foundation grants 10–06–00701a and 12–06–00809a. The work was carried out in Serbsky National Research Centre for Social and Forensic Psychiatry, Moscow, Russia. The authors of this work have no competing interests.

References

Abel, K. M., Allin, M. P. G, Hemsley, D. R., & Geyer, M. A. (2003). Low dose ketamine increases prepulse inhibition in healthy men. Neuropharmacology, 44, 729737. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(03)00073-X Google Scholar
Adler, L. E., Pachtman, E., Franks, R. D., Pecevich, M., Waldo, M. C., & Freedman, R. (1982). Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 639654.Google Scholar
Adler, L. E., Olincy, A., Waldo, M., Harris, J. G., Griffith, J., Stevens, K., … Freedman, R. (1998). Schizophrenia, sensory gating, and nicotinic receptors. Schizophrenia Bulletin 24, 189202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033320 Google Scholar
Arciniegas, D. B., Topkoff, J. L., Rojas, D. C., Sheeder, J., Teale, P., Young, D. A., … Adler, L. E. (2001). Reduced hippocampal volume in association with p50 nonsuppression following traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 13, 213221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13.2.213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumental, T. D. (1997). Prepulse inhibition decreases as startle reactivity habituates. Psychophysiology, 34, 446450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02388.x Google Scholar
Braff, D., Swerdlow, N., & Geyer, M. (1999). Symptom correlates of prepulse inhibition deficits in male schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 596602.Google Scholar
Braff, D. L., Grillon, C., & Geyer, M. A. (1992). Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 206215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030038005 Google Scholar
Braff, D. L., Light, G. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression are both deficient but not correlated in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 12041207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brenner, C. A., Edwards, C. R., Carroll, C. A., Kieffaber, P. D., & Hetrick, W. P. (2004). P50 and acoustic startle gating are not related in healthy participants. Psychophysiology, 41, 702708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00206.x Google Scholar
Cadenhead, K. S. (2011). Startle reactivity and prepulse inhibition in prodromal and early psychosis: effects of age, antipsychotics, tobacco and cannabis in a vulnerable population. Psychiatry Research, 188, 208216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, M. E., Dobie, D. J., Cadenhead, K. S., Olincy, A., Freedman, R., Green, M. F., … Braff, D. L. (2007). The consortium on the genetics of endophenotypes in schizophrenia: Model recruitment, assessment, and endophenotyping methods for a multisite collaboration. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(1), 3348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl044 Google Scholar
Carroll, C. A., Vohs, J. L., O’donnell, B. F., Shekhar, A., & Hetrick, W. P. (2007). Sensorimotor gating in manic and mixed episode bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 9, 221229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00415.x Google Scholar
Conzelmann, A., Pauli, P., Mucha, R. F., Jacob, C. P., Gerdes, A. B. M., Romanos, J., … Weyers, P. (2010). Early attentional deficits in an attention-to-prepulse paradigm in ADHD adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 594603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019859 Google Scholar
Duncan, E. J., Madonick, S. H., Parwani, A., Angrist, B., Rajan, R., Chakravorty, S., … Rotrosen, J. P. (2001). Clinical and sensorimotor gating effects of ketamine in normals. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(1), 7283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00240-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filion, D. L., Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1993). Modification of the acoustic startle-reflex eye blink: A tool for investigating early and late attentional processes. Biological Psychology, 35, 185200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(93)90001-O Google Scholar
Filion, D. L., Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1994). Probing the orienting response with startle modification and secondary reaction time. Psychophysiology, 31(1), 6878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01026.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freedman, R., Adler, L. E., Myles-Worsley, M., Nagamoto, H. T., Miller, C., Kisley, M., … Waldo, M. (1996). Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects. Human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 11141121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120052009 Google Scholar
Freedman, R., Waldo, M., Bickford-Wimer, P., & Nagamoto, H. (1991). Elementary neuronal dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 4, 233243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(91)90035-P Google Scholar
Geyer, M. A., Swerdlow, N. R., Mansbach, R. S., & Braff, D. L. (1990). Startle response models of sensorimotor gating and habituation deficits in schizophrenia. Brain Research Bulletin, 25, 485498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(90)90241-Q Google Scholar
Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (1982). Habituation of the Blink reflex in normals and schizophrenic patients. Psychophysiology, 19(1), 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02589.x Google Scholar
Giakoumaki, S. G., Roussos, P., Rogdaki, M., Karli, C., Bitsios, P., & Frangou, S. (2007). Evidence of disrupted prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of bipolar disorder patients. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 14181422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunwald, T., Butros, N. N., Pezer, N., von Oertzen, J., Fernández, G., Schaller, C., & Elger, C. E. (2003). Neuronal substrates of sensory gating within the human brain. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 511519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01673-6 Google Scholar
Hall, M. H., Taylor, G., Salisbury, D. F., & Levy, D. L. (2011). Sensory gating event-related potentials and oscillations in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected relatives. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37, 11871199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasenkamp, W., Kelley, M., Egan, G., Green, A., Wilcox, L., Boshoven, W., … Duncan, E. (2011). Lack of relationship between acoustic startle and cognitive variables in schizophrenia and control subjects. Psychiatry Research, 187, 324328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.011 Google Scholar
Hasenkamp, W., Epstein, M. P., Green, A., Wilcox, L., Boshoven, W., Lewison, B., & Duncan, E. (2010). Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Research, 178, 236243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.11.012 Google Scholar
Hasenkamp, W., Norrholm, S. D., Green, A., Lewison, B., Boshoven, W., Keyes, M., & Duncan, E. (2008). Differences in startle reflex and prepulse inhibition in European-Americans and African-Americans. Psychophysiology, 45, 876882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00680.x Google Scholar
Hoffman, H. S., & Wible, B. I. (1969). Temporal parameters in startle facilitation by steady background signals. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 45, 712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1911377 Google Scholar
Hong, L. E., Summerfelt, A., Wonodi, I., Adami, H., Buchanan, R., & Thaker, G. (2007). Independent domains of inhibitory gating in schizophrenia and the effect of stimulus interval. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 6165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.1.61 Google Scholar
Kumari, V., Premkumar, P., Fannon, D., Aasen, I., Raghuvanshi, S., Anilkumar, A. P., … Kuipers, E. (2012). Sensorimotor gating and clinical outcome following cognitive behavior therapy for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 134, 232238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.020 Google Scholar
Kumari, V., Das, M., Zachariah, E., Ettinger, U., & Sharma, T. (2005). Reduced prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Psychophysiology. 42, 588594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00346.x Google Scholar
Leonard, J., Gault, J., Hopkins, J., Logel, R., & Vianzon, M., Short, M., ... Freedman, R. (2002). Association of promoter variants in the alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene with an inhibitory deficit found in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 10851096. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1085 Google Scholar
Li, L., Du, Y., Li, N., Wu, X., & Wu, Y. (2009). Top–down modulation of prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in humans and rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 11571167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.001 Google Scholar
Ludewig, K., Geyer, M. A., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2003). Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 121128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01925-X Google Scholar
Mann, C., Croft, R. J., Scholes, K. E., Dunne, A., O'Neill, B. V., Leung, S., … Nathan, P. J. (2008). Differential effects of acute serotonin and dopamine depletion on prepulse inhibition and p50 suppression measures of sensorimotor and sensory gating in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 16531666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301556 Google Scholar
Nagamoto, H. T., Adler, L. E., Waldo, M. C., Griffith, J., & Freedman, R. (1991). Gating of auditory response in schizophrenics and normal controls. Effects of recording site and stimulation interval on the P50 wave. Schizophrenia Research, 4(1), 3140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(91)90007-E CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olincy, A., & Martin, L. (2005). Diminished suppression of the P50 auditory evoked potential in bipolar disorder subjects with a history of psychosis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(1), 4349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.43 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oranje, B., Geyer, M. A., Bocker, K. B. E., Kenemans, J. L., & Verbaten, M. N. (2006). Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression: Commonalities and dissociations. Psychiatry Research, 143, 147158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.002 Google Scholar
Parwani, A., Duncan, E. J., Bartlett, E., Madonick, S., Efferen, T. R., Rajan, R., … Rotrosen, J. P. (2000). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 662669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00148-1 Google Scholar
Perry, W., & Braff, D. L. (1994) Information-processing deficits and thought disorder in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 363367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.151.3.363 Google Scholar
Potter, D., Summerfelt, A., Gold, J., & Buchanan, R. W. (2006). Review of clinical correlates of P50 sensory gating abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 692700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj050 Google Scholar
Putnam, L. E., & Vanman, L. E. (1999). Long lead interval startle modification. In. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Bohmelt, A. H. (Eds.), Startle Modification (pp. 7292). Cambridge, UK: University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511665523.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quednow, B. B., Frommann, I., Berning, J., Kuhn, K. U., Maier, W., & Wagner, M. (2008). Impaired sensorimotor gating of the acoustic startle response in the prodrome of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 766773. http://dx.doi.org/10.5167/uzh-5723 Google Scholar
Quednow, B. B., Wagner, M., Westheide, J., Beckmann, K., Bliesener, N., Maier, W., & Kühn, K. U. (2006). Sensorimotor gating and habituation of the startle response in schizophrenic patients randomly treated with amisulpride or olanzapine and is regarded as an evidence of impaired gating. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 536545.Google Scholar
Schulze, K. K., Hall, M. H., McDonald, C., Marshall, N., Walshe, M., Murray, R. M., & Bramon, E. (2007). P50 auditory evoked potential suppression in bipolar disorder patients with psychotic features and their unaffected relatives. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 121128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.006 Google Scholar
Schwarzkopf, S. B., Lamberti, J. S., & Smith, D. A. (1993). Concurrent assessment of acoustic startle and auditory P50 evoked potential measures of sensory inhibition. Biological Psychiatry, 33, 815828. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(93)90023-7 Google Scholar
Sperling, W., Bleich, S., Maihöfner, C., & Reulbach, U. (2009) Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia - outcry of a diseased brain? Medical Hypotheses, 72, 213216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2008.09.005 Google Scholar
Storozheva, Z. I., Kirenskaya, A. V., Lazarev, I. E., Novototskii-Vlasov, V. Yu., Samylkin, D. V., & Fastovtsov, G. A. (2012). Prepulse modification of the acoustic startle reaction in healthy subjects and patients with schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 42, 128132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11055-011-9545-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Lelham, S. A., Sutherland Owens, A. N., Chang, W. L., Sassen, S. D. T., & Talledo, J. A. (2009). Pramipexole effects on startle gating in rats and normal men. Psychopharmacology, 205, 689698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1577-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swerdlow, N. R., Weber, M., Qu, Y., Light, G. A., & Braff, D. L. (2008). Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology, 199, 331388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1072-4 Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Sprock, J., Light, G. A., Cadenhead, K., Calkins, M. E., Dobie, D. J., … Braff, D. L. (2007). Multi-site studies of acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in humans: Initial experience and methodological considerations based on studies by the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 92, 237251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.012 Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Light, G. A., Cadenhead, K. S., Sprock, J., Hsieh, M. H., & Braff, D. L. (2006). Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: Relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 13251335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (2001). Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: Current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology, 156, 194215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002130100799 Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Paulsen, J., Braff, D. L., Butters, N., Geyer, M. A., & Swenson, M. R. (1995). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 58, 192200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192 Google Scholar
Talledo, J. A., Sutherland Owens, A. N., Schortinghuis, T., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2009). Amphetamine effects on startle gating in normal women and female rats. Psychopharmacology, 204(1), 165175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1446-7 Google Scholar
Thaker, G. K. (2008). Neurophysiological endophenotypes across bipolar and schizophrenia psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34, 760773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn049 Google Scholar
Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. E., Light, G. A., Olincy, A., Radant, A. D., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: The viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(1), 6994. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl060 Google Scholar
Wynn, J. K., Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., McGee, M., Salveson, D., & Green, M. F. (2004). Prepulse facilitation and prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 518523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.018 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline parameters and habituation of ASR in CON and SCH groups

Figure 1

Table 2. PPI at different lead intervals in CON and SCH groups (%%)

Figure 2

Table 3. Comparison of Group Means (Mann-Whitney test) for P50 suppression measures in healthy subjects and schizophrenia patients

Figure 3

Table 4. PPI and P50 measures in CON and SCH groups adjusted for age

Figure 4

Table 5. Correlations of ASR measures with PANSS scales in SCH group

Figure 5

Table 6. Results of logit analysis of PPI and P50 suppression as predictors for differentiation of CON and SCH groups