Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T12:33:01.691Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The structure of schizotypal personality traits: a cross-national study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2017

E. Fonseca-Pedrero*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, University of La Rioja, Logroño, Spain Center for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Oviedo, Spain
M. Debbané
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
J. Ortuño-Sierra
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, University of La Rioja, Logroño, Spain
R. C. K. Chan
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, CAS key Laboratory of Mental Health, Beijing, China
D. C. Cicero
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa, Honolulu, HI, USA
L. C. Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
C. Brenner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
E. Barkus
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
R. J. Linscott
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
T. Kwapil
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
N. Barrantes-Vidal
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
A. Cohen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Louisiana, LA, USA
A. Raine
Affiliation:
Departments of Criminology, Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
M. T. Compton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, NY, USA
E. B. Tone
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
J. Suhr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ohio University, Athens, OH, USA
J. Muñiz
Affiliation:
Center for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Oviedo, Spain Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
A. Fumero
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of La Laguna, Santa Cruz de Tenerife, Spain
S. Giakoumaki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Rethymno, Greece
I. Tsaousis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Rethymno, Greece
A. Preti
Affiliation:
Genneruxi Medical Center, Cagliari, Italy
M. Chmielewski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TX, USA
J. Laloyaux
Affiliation:
Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway NORMENT - Norwegian Center of Excellence for Mental Disorders Research, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Psychology and Neuroscience of Cognition Research Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
A. Mechri
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Department, University Hospital of Monastir, Monastir, Tunisia
M. A. Lahmar
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Department, University Hospital of Monastir, Monastir, Tunisia
V. Wuthrich
Affiliation:
Centre for Emotional Health, Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
F. Larøi
Affiliation:
Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway NORMENT - Norwegian Center of Excellence for Mental Disorders Research, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Psychology and Neuroscience of Cognition Research Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
J. C. Badcock
Affiliation:
Centre for Clinical Research in Neuropsychiatry, Division of Psychiatry, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
A. Jablensky
Affiliation:
Centre for Clinical Research in Neuropsychiatry, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
*
*Address for correspondence: E. Fonseca-Pedrero, University of La Rioja, C/Luis de Ulloa, s/n, Edificio VIVES; C.P: 26002, Logroño, La Rioja, Spain. (Email: eduardo.fonseca@unirioja.es)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Schizotypal traits are considered a phenotypic-indicator of schizotypy, a latent personality organization reflecting a putative liability for psychosis. To date, no previous study has examined the comparability of factorial structures across samples originating from different countries and cultures. The main goal was to evaluate the factorial structure and reliability of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) scores by amalgamating data from studies conducted in 12 countries and across 21 sites.

Method

The overall sample consisted of 27 001 participants (37.5% males, n = 4251 drawn from the general population). The mean age was 22.12 years (s.d. = 6.28, range 16–55 years). The SPQ was used. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and Multilevel CFA (ML-CFA) were used to evaluate the factor structure underlying the SPQ scores.

Results

At the SPQ item level, the nine factor and second-order factor models showed adequate goodness-of-fit. At the SPQ subscale level, three- and four-factor models displayed better goodness-of-fit indices than other CFA models. ML-CFA showed that the intraclass correlation coefficients values were lower than 0.106. The three-factor model showed adequate goodness of fit indices in multilevel analysis. The ordinal α coefficients were high, ranging from 0.73 to 0.94 across individual samples, and from 0.84 to 0.91 for the combined sample.

Conclusions

The results are consistent with the conceptual notion that schizotypal personality is a multifaceted construct and support the validity and utility of SPQ in cross-cultural research. We discuss theoretical and clinical implications of our results for diagnostic systems, psychosis models and cross-national mental health strategies.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Schizotypal traits are considered a phenotypic-indicator of schizotypy, a latent personality organization reflecting a putative liability for schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Meehl, Reference Meehl1962). These traits refer to anomalies across cognitive (e.g., hallucinations, ideas of reference), social/emotional (e.g., constricted affect, no close friends) and behavioural (e.g., odd behaviour and language) systems, that do not meet clinical threshold for psychotic disorders (Raine, Reference Raine2006; Kwapil & Barrantes-Vidal, Reference Kwapil and Barrantes-Vidal2015). Recent conceptualizations of the schizotypy framework indicate that it provides a unifying construct that efficiently links a broad continuum of clinical and subclinical psychosis manifestations, as well as normal personality variation (Kwapil & Barrantes-Vidal, Reference Kwapil and Barrantes-Vidal2015). Understanding schizotypal traits in non-clinical samples may help elucidate aetiological mechanisms, provide a window to examine risk and protective factors without certain confounding factors (e.g., medication), and provide a necessary step in the process of developing early detection strategies and preventive interventions for those individuals at risk for psychosis-spectrum disorders (Barrantes-Vidal et al. Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Grant and Kwapil2015).

Previous research has shown that schizotypal traits are a valid putative phenotypic indicator for psychosis-spectrum disorders (e.g., Lenzenweger, Reference Lenzenweger2010; Fonseca Pedrero & Debbané, Reference Fonseca Pedrero and Debbané2017). First, considerable evidence from family, adoption and twin studies have demonstrated that schizotypal traits are related to schizophrenia (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, McGuire, Gruenberg, O'Hare, Spellman and Walsh1993; Walter et al. Reference Walter, Fernandez, Snelling and Barkus2016). Second, independent follow-up studies have shown that individuals from the general population and those at clinical or genetic high risk for psychosis who report schizotypal traits, as well as patients with schizotypal personality disorder, are at elevated risk for transition to psychosis and related conditions (Debbané et al. Reference Debbané, Eliez, Badoud, Conus, Flückiger and Schultze-Lutter2015). Third, schizotypal traits are qualitatively similar, but less severe than the symptoms found in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and ultra-high risk samples. In fact, schizotypal traits have been associated with similar deficits in brain function, eye movements, neurocognition, language, etc., amongst others, to those seen in patients with psychosis (Raine, Reference Raine2006; Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Carpenter, Woods and McGlashan2014; Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Mohr, Ettinger, Chan and Park2015; Ettinger et al. Reference Ettinger, Mohr, Gooding, Cohen, Rapp, Haenschel and Park2015). Fourth, they share many of the same demographic and environmental concomitants as those found in patients with psychosis (e.g., trauma, cannabis use, high levels of urbanicity) (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013). Fifth, isolated schizotypal traits, even those insufficient in severity or impairment to warrant a clinical diagnosis, are associated with increased risk of psychiatric morbidity (e.g., suicidal behaviour, mental health problems, low quality of life) and functional disability (Nuevo et al. Reference Nuevo, Chatterji, Verdes, Naidoo, Arango and Ayuso-Mateos2012; Kwapil et al. Reference Kwapil, Gross, Silvia and Barrantes-Vidal2013; Kelleher et al. Reference Kelleher, Cederlöf and Lichtenstein2014). For example, adolescents in a schizophrenia liability class – those who reported schizotypal traits – showed greater odds of passive suicidal ideation at a 2-year follow-up compared with those not in the liability class [odds ratio (OR) 8.15, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.34–49.60] (Schimanski et al. Reference Schimanski, Mouat, Billinghurst and Linscott2017). These findings reveal an important overlap in schizotypal traits and psychosis-spectrum disorders, supporting the notion of a phenomenological and etiological continuity.

To assess schizotypal traits, several tools have been developed. These instruments permit examination of variations in healthy trait schizotypy, as well as in the latent vulnerability to psychosis-spectrum disorders (e.g., Mason, Reference Mason2015; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Gooding, Debbané, Muñiz and Bartram2016b ). This psychometric high-risk methodology has shown validity and clinical relevance, in line with conventional interview-based high-risk approaches for psychosis (Barrantes-Vidal et al. Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Gross, Sheinbaum, Mitjavila, Ballespí and Kwapil2013; Cicero et al. Reference Cicero, Martin, Becker, Docherty and Kerns2014). Moreover, self-report can be more sensitive to environmental v. heritable effects than to interview-based assessment (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Myers, Torgersen, Neale and Reichborn-Kjennerud2007).

The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (Raine, Reference Raine1991) is a popular, extensively used self-report tool for the assessment of schizotypal traits in both clinical and non-clinical populations. The SPQ measures a broad range of schizotypal traits – originally it encompassed nine subordinate traits that are based on the operational definition of DSM-III-R Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD) [American Psychiatric Association (APA), 1987]. These features also represent the main features of DSM-5 SPD criteria in the chapter on Schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders (APA, 2013). Notably, the DSM-5 also presents SPD in the context of an alternative hybrid (dimensional/categorical) model of personality disorders that is outlined in Section III (APA, 2013). The 74 items of the SPQ are distributed across nine subscales, each containing seven to nine items; these subscales encompass odd beliefs or magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, ideas of reference, paranoid ideation/suspiciousness, excessive social anxiety, no close friends, constricted affect, odd or eccentric behaviour and odd speech. The psychometric properties have been examined in a number of nation- or region-specific studies (e.g., Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014; Gross et al. Reference Gross, Mellin, Silvia, Barrantes-Vidal and Kwapil2014; Tsaousis et al. Reference Tsaousis, Zouraraki, Karamaouna, Karagiannopoulou and Giakoumaki2015; Cicero, Reference Cicero2016).

Although there is no universal agreement on the latent structure of schizotypy or psychosis liability, whether it is dimensional or categorical (Everett & Linscott, Reference Everett and Linscott2015), the literature consistently holds that the phenotypic expression of schizotypal traits is multifaceted (e.g., Vollema & Hoijtink, Reference Vollema and Hoijtink2000; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014; Gross et al. Reference Gross, Mellin, Silvia, Barrantes-Vidal and Kwapil2014; Tsaousis et al. Reference Tsaousis, Zouraraki, Karamaouna, Karagiannopoulou and Giakoumaki2015; Cicero, Reference Cicero2016). This multifaceted nature can be understood in terms of a latent multidimensional or factor structure framework. Using the SPQ (Raine, Reference Raine1991), or its brief version (SPQ-B) (Raine & Benishay, Reference Raine and Benishay1995), the three-factor model proposed by Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994), which comprises Cognitive–Perceptual (Positive), Interpersonal (Negative) and Disorganized dimensions, has been one of the most widely replicated models (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Hsiao and Lin1997; Vollema & Hoijtink, Reference Vollema and Hoijtink2000; Fossati et al. Reference Fossati, Raine, Carretta, Leonardi and Maffei2003; Badcock & Dragovic, Reference Badcock and Dragovic2006; Raine, Reference Raine2006; Wuthrich & Bates, Reference Wuthrich and Bates2006; Bora & Arabaci, Reference Bora and Arabaci2009; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014, Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Debbané, Schneider, Badoud and Eliez2016a ). To a large extent, this factorial structure of schizotypal personality is similar to that for clinical symptoms reported by patients with schizophrenia (Liddle, Reference Liddle1987). The four-factor model proposed by Stefanis et al. (Reference Stefanis, Smyrnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Ntzoufras and Stefanis2004) that includes Cognitive-Perceptual, Interpersonal, Disorganization, and Paranoid dimensions has also been frequently replicated (Bora & Arabaci, Reference Bora and Arabaci2009; Compton et al. Reference Compton, Goulding, Bakeman and McClure-Tone2009; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014; Gross et al. Reference Gross, Mellin, Silvia, Barrantes-Vidal and Kwapil2014). In several studies, the goodness-of-fit indices reported in for Stefanis et al.’s model are similar to, or at times better, than those reported for Raine's model. Item-level examinations of the SPQ have yielded more complex factor solutions (Chmielewski & Watson, Reference Chmielewski and Watson2008; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014), suggesting a need for further replication and deeper analyses.

Variability across studies in the factorial composition of the SPQ may be due in part to the type and size of participant samples as well as the analysis methodologies that were employed. Also, it is noteworthy that different measurement models (e.g., bifactor model) or methodologies (e.g., Multilevel Confirmatory factor analyses – ML-CFA) may better capture the complexity and heterogeneity of schizotypal phenotype, as assessed by the SPQ, when comparing data from multiple countries. These measurement and methodological approaches, however, are not often applied in research on schizotypal traits and the extended psychosis phenotype.

As of yet, there has been no in-depth examination of the factorial structure underlying schizotypal traits, as measured with the SPQ that compares data from diverse countries and cultures. Moreover, no previous studies have examined whether the phenotypic expression of schizotypal traits is similar across sites or countries. It is often assumed that the structure of the schizotypal personality at the individual level is universal; however, this assumption has yet to be assessed empirically using data drawn from different geographical regions.

Therefore, in order to address these possible sources of inconsistency in prior research findings, our aim was to evaluate the factorial structure and reliability of the SPQ scores by amalgamating data from studies conducted in 12 countries and across 21 sites. In particular, the present study: (a) examined associations among self-reported schizotypal traits; (b) tested the factorial structure, at both item and subscale levels, of SPQ scores within and between samples; and (c) estimated the reliability of self-reported schizotypal traits. In line with previous evidence, we hypothesized that three- and four-factor models of the SPQ scores would provide the best fit to the data. Moreover, we further hypothesized that these measurement models of schizotypal personality would fit well in the multilevel analyses.

Method

Participants

This study was undertaken as part of the activities of the 1st International Consortium on Schizotypy Research celebrated in Geneva in 2014 (Debbané & Mohr, Reference Debbané and Mohr2015). Although this is not a meta-analysis, studies using the SPQ in the healthy adult population samples were identified by systematically searching Medline (PubMed and Ovid), PsycINFO, SCOPUS and ISI (Science and Social Science Citation Index) databases between June and August of 2014.

Citations in identified articles were also searched for additional sources. Access to data was sought for studies that met the following inclusion criteria: (a) the sample size was ⩾100; (b) the sample was obtained from the general population, including college or undergraduate populations (samples of non-clinical adolescents, school pupils, patients, or family members of patients with psychosis were excluded); (c) in the case of articles with possible overlapping samples, the study with a larger or more informative sample was selected; and (d) item-level data on the 74 SPQ items and information on the administration procedure (paper-pencil v. computerized) was available.

Table 1 provides a summary of the demographic characteristics of samples that were obtained (See eTable 1 online Supplementary Material). Item level data were obtained from 21 sites across 12 countries (USA, UK, China, Belgium, Spain, Italy, Tunisia, Australia, New Zealand, Canada, Mauritius and Greece). The overall sample consisted of 27 001 participants (n = 4251 drawn from the general population). The mean age was 22.12 years (s.d. = 6.28; range 16–55 years), 15.2% (n = 4113) of participants did not provide age. Only 3.3% (n = 849) of the sample were aged over 35 years. Of participants, 37.5% (n = 10 126) were male, 60.6% (n = 16 368) were female and 1.9% (n = 507) did not specify gender.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample

Studies were reviewed and approved by institutional review boards or ethics committees of the jurisdictions in which studies were undertaken. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation. Studies were conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013). Except for five studies, the data used in the present study were published elsewhere. We deleted from the initial sample those participants with missing values for more than two SPQ items. Based on the SPSS missing value analysis module, the relatively few missing values in the data were replaced by regression-based estimates to which an error component was added.

Instrument

The SPQ (Raine, Reference Raine1991) provided a common index of schizotypal traits across all study sites. Although designed for SPD as defined in the DSM–III-R (APA, 1987), the SPQ is still consistent with the DSM–5 (APA, 2013) because the nine symptoms have not changed (Cicero, Reference Cicero2016). In five studies, the SPQ was administered using a computerized format (studies: 4, 8, 12, 19 and 20) and in two studies a Likert response format (1–5) was used (studies: 8 and 9). For these two studies, the responses were recoded as ‘1–3’ to ‘0’ (No) and ‘4–5’ to ‘1’ (Yes). This data modification produces dichotomous response frequencies consistent with scores from the original SPQ. In the present work we used the SPQ versions adapted and validated for each country: English version (Raine, Reference Raine1991), Spanish (Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Fumero, Paino, de Miguel, Ortuño-Sierra, Lemos Giraldez and Muñiz2014), Italian (Fossati et al. Reference Fossati, Raine, Carretta, Leonardi and Maffei2003), Chinese (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Hsiao and Lin1997), Arabic (Lahmar et al. Reference Lahmar, Gassab, Beltaief and Mechri2014), French (Dumas et al. Reference Dumas, Bouafia, Gutknecht, Saoud, Dalery and d'Amato2000), Creole (Reynolds et al. Reference Reynolds, Raine, Mellingen, Venables and Mednick2000) and Greek (Tsaousis et al. Reference Tsaousis, Zouraraki, Karamaouna, Karagiannopoulou and Giakoumaki2015).

Data analyses

Several analyses were carried out in the present study. First, descriptive statistics and correlations between SPQ subscales were computed.

Second, given the hierarchical structure of the data, with participants nested in sites/countries, a ML-CFA was performed. ML-CFA decomposes the total variance into two components (i.e., within-site variance and between-site variance). Therefore, this approach allows researchers to construct measurement models at both individual and country levels (i.e., within-level and between-level) (e.g., Cheung et al. Reference Cheung, Leung and Au2006; Byrne, Reference Byrne2012). Prior to conducting the ML-CFA, two steps were performed: a) multiple CFA models were tested on the total sample as well as at site level; and b) once we determined the best-fitting measurement model, intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were estimated. The ICC assesses the level of variance in an observed variable that is attributable to membership in its cluster (e.g., site). ICC values range from 0.0 to 1.0. A high ICC implies that the between-group variance dominates the within-group variance. Previously published studies have reported that the presence of ICCs that exceed 0.10 warrants the use of ML-CFA (e.g., Cheung et al. Reference Cheung, Leung and Au2006; Byrne, Reference Byrne2012). These two steps provided initial information about the factor structure of the SPQ, as well as pertinent information used to justify multilevel analyses. Finally, with the best fitting measurement models, two-level CFAs with continuous factor indicators were conducted.

Several measurement models were tested at both item and subscale level. At the item-level, we tested four different factor models by means of CFAs. As SPQ items were binary, we used the weighted least squares means and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2012). Model 1 was a one-factor latent structure; all 74 items loaded on a single factor. Model 2 was a nine-factor oblique structure where the nine factors corresponded to the nine SPQ subscales. Model 3 was a bifactor model with one general factor and nine specific factors (i.e., nine SPQ subscales). Model 4 was a second-order model, that is, one involving a hierarchical structure. Here, three second-order factors (corresponding to cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganization dimensions) loaded on nine lower-order factors. In this model paranoia items were allowed to saturate in both cognitive-perceptual and interpersonal factors, consistent with previous research (Raine et al. Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994). At the subscale level, we tested six models using an MLM estimator. In Model 5, a single factor loaded onto the nine SPQ subscales (e.g., baseline model). Model 6 comprised two correlated factors (i.e., cognitive-perceptual and Interpersonal) (Gross et al. Reference Gross, Mellin, Silvia, Barrantes-Vidal and Kwapil2014). Model 7 comprised three correlated factors (i.e., cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganized). Model 8 was a variation of Model 7 in which the positive and interpersonal factors both loaded on paranoid ideation (Raine et al. Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994). Model 9 was a four-factor model based on Stefanis et al. (Reference Stefanis, Smyrnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Ntzoufras and Stefanis2004) (i.e., cognitive-perceptual, paranoid, interpersonal and disorganized). Model 10 was a subscale-level bifactor model with a general schizotypal factor and three specific factors corresponding to cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganized dimensions.

The goodness-of-fit indices employed were: χ2, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the Root Mean square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) (and 90% CI), the Weighted Root Mean Square Residual (WRMR) for dichotomous indicators, the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) for continuous indicators, and the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). Good fit is indicated when the CFI and TLI are over 0.95 and the RMSEA is under 0.08 (reasonable fit) or under 0.05 (good fit) (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999; Brown, Reference Brown2015). The presence of WRMR values below 1.0 has been suggested as indicative of adequate model fit (Yu & Muthén, Reference Yu and Muthén2002). The AIC and BIC do not have cut-off values. Instead, models with smaller AIC and BIC values have better fit. The AIC and BIC are useful because they penalize more complex models.

Finally, we calculated the internal consistency of the SPQ scores in each country as well as in the total sample, using ordinal α coefficients (Zumbo et al. Reference Zumbo, Gadermann and Zeisser2007). Ordinal α performs well for analysis of dichotomous data and overcomes several problems associated with Cronbach's α (e.g., Dunn et al. Reference Dunn, Baguley and Brunsden2014).

SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp Released, 2013), Mplus 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2012) and R (R Development Core Team, 2011) were used for data analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations between schizotypal traits

Descriptive statistics of the SPQ subscales are reported in eTable 2 (online Supplementary Material). Table 2 shows the mean (and range) of Pearson's correlations among the schizotypal subscales across studies. According to convention, correlations of 0.1, 0.3 and 0.5 are regarded as small, medium and large in effect size, respectively (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). There were several notable findings. First, the magical thinking subscale was relatively independent of the Odd behaviour, Odd speech and No close friends subscales. In fact, these associations were the only ones that were not statistically significant and close to zero. Second, the correlations among the remaining SPQ subscales were of medium to large effect size. No subscales were redundant (i.e., with r > 0.85). Third, the Odd Speech subscale was highly correlated with No Close Friends. Fourth, Excessive Social Anxiety was highly correlated with the Ideas of Reference and Unusual Perceptual Experiences subscales.

Table 2. Pearson correlations between Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire subscales for all studies [mean value (range)]

IREF, Ideas of Reference; ESA, Excessive Social Anxiety; MGT, Magical Thinking; UPE, Unusual Perceptual Experiences; OB, Odd Behaviour; NCF, No Close Friends; OS, Odd Speech; CA, Constricted Affect; PI, Paranoid Ideation.

Structure of schizotypal traits: CFA for the full sample and across samples

Table 3 presents the goodness-of-fit indices for the models tested for the full sample. At the item level, we selected the RMSEA as the primary index of model fit because it has been generally identified as the best performing index for the WLSMV method. RMSEA values of less than 0.06 reliably indicate good model fit for binary outcomes (Yu & Muthén, Reference Yu and Muthén2002). Thus, at the item level, the measurement models that displayed the best goodness-of-fit indices were Model 3 (the nine-first-order model) and Model 4 (the three-second-order model).

Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices of the schizotypal personality models tested for the full sample (n = 27 001)

PI, Paranoid Ideation; χ2, Chi Square; df, Degrees of freedom; RMSEA, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; CI, Confidence Interval; CFI, Comparative Fit Index; TLI, Tucker-Lewis Index; SRMR, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; WRMR, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; AIC, Akaike Information Criterion; BIC, Bayesian Information Criterion.

At the subscale level, the CFA models that showed the best fit were Raine et al.’s model with positive and interpersonal factors both loading on Paranoid ideation, and Stefanis et al.’s four-factor model. In particular, Stefanis et al.’s model yielded better goodness-of-fit indices than the competing factorial models.

eTable 3 (online Supplementary Material) shows the goodness-of-fit indices for the CFA models tested for each subsample. In all sites, the goodness-of-fit indices for the Stefanis et al. (Reference Stefanis, Smyrnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Ntzoufras and Stefanis2004) model met good fit criteria. Other factor models tested, such as the bifactor model and three-factor model of Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994), also showed acceptable fit.

The three and four-factor models tested at the subscale level fit the data well in the full sample and in all 21 subsamples. Based on previous models of schizotypal personality and higher goodness-of-fit indices, we retained the three and four-factor models, at the subscale level, as the models that best accounted for the factor loadings and associations among latent factors.

The three- and four-factor models of schizotypal traits

Table 4 shows the standardized loadings estimated for the total sample, as well as the ranges of standardized factor loadings for the four-factor model for each study. The Unusual Perceptual Experiences and Ideas of Reference subscales had the highest factor loadings across studies. Correlations among the four latent factors ranged from 0.31 (0.10–0.51) for the paranoid-interpersonal factors to 0.68 (0.59–0.79) for the positive-disorganization factors (p < 0.01).

Table 4. Standardized factor loadings for the four-factor model

MGT, Magical Thinking; UPE, Unusual Perceptual Experiences; IREF, Ideas of Reference; ESA, Excessive Social Anxiety; PI, Paranoid Ideation; NCF, No Close Friends; CA, Constricted Affect; OB, Odd Behaviour; OS, Odd Speech.

Note. Brackets shows range values of the standardized factorial loadings estimated across 21 studies. All standardized factorial loadings estimated were statistically significant (p < 0.01).

Table 5 shows the standardized factor loadings for the total sample, as well as the ranges of standardized loadings estimated for the three-factor model for each study. Correlations among the three latent factors ranged from 0.39 (0.14–0.60) for the positive-interpersonal factors to 0.74 (0.62–0.82) for the positive-disorganized factors (p < 0.01).

Table 5. Standardized factorial loadings for the three-factor model in the total sample (n = 27 001)

MGT, Magical Thinking; UPE, Unusual Perceptual Experiences; IREF, Ideas of Reference; ESA, Excessive Social Anxiety, PI: Paranoid Ideation; NCF, No Close Friends; CA, Constricted Affect; OB, Odd Behaviour; OS, Odd Speech.

Note. Brackets shows range values of the standardized factorial loadings estimated across 21 studies. All standardized factorial loadings estimated were statistically significant (p < 0.01).

ML-CFA of the three-factor model

Multilevel four-factor model estimation could not be completed due to a non-positive matrix; consequently, no results could be obtained for this measurement model. Thus, only the three-factor model was tested in multilevel analyses. For the three-factor model, all SPQ subscales showed ICC values lower than 0.106, Thus, the amount of variance attributable to cluster membership (i.e., site) was lower than 11%. ICC values were: Ideas of Reference = 0.097; Unusual Perceptual Experiences = 0.059; Magical Thinking = 0.086; Paranoid Ideation = 0.106; Excessive Social Anxiety = 0.035; No Close Friends = 0.079; Constricted Affect = 0.065; Odd Speech = 0.056; Odd Behaviour = 0.038. These results indicates that a ML-CFA could be warranted; however, the hierarchical nature of the data did not have a clear significant effect on the factor structure of the SPQ (i.e., almost all ICCs values lower than 0.10).

The three-factor model of Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994) showed adequate goodness of fit indices (S-Bχ2 = 3021.22; df = 46; CFI = 0.958; TLI = 0.935; RMSEA = 0.049; SRMRWithin = 0.045; SRMRBetween = 0.128; AIC = 943 269.12; BIC = 943 703.92). eTable 4 (online Supplementary Material) shows the standardized loadings estimated for the three-factor model tested in multilevel analyses. The standardized factor loadings were slightly higher at the site level.

Reliability estimations of the schizotypal traits

Table 6 shows the internal consistency values for the SPQ subscales across studies, as well as for the total sample. Ordinal α coefficients were high and ranged between 0.73 and 0.94 for the subscales in the individual samples, and from 0.84 to 0.91 for the total sample.

Table 6. Ordinal α estimations for Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire subscales across studies and total sample

IREF, Ideas of Reference; ESA, Excessive Social Anxiety; MGT, Magical Thinking; UPE, Unusual Perceptual Experiences; OB, Odd Behaviour; NCF, No Close Friends; OS, Odd Speech; CA, Constricted Affect; PI, Paranoid Ideation.

Studies 1–21 refer to the studies listed in Table 1.

Discussion

The SPQ (Raine, Reference Raine1991) is one of the most frequently used self-report tools for assessing schizotypal traits in samples of the general population as well as in clinical samples. Moreover, the SPQ may have utility as a screening instrument that can identify individuals who may be at increased risk for psychosis-spectrum disorders (Mason, Reference Mason2015; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Gooding, Debbané, Muñiz and Bartram2016b ). To date, there have been no comprehensive reports on the structure of the schizotypal personality using large and representative multi-national or multi-ethnic samples. We sought to bring clarity to this matter by examining the SPQ's factorial structure and reliability across different studies and countries. Notably, this is the first study to include data from multiple continents and is the largest SPQ dataset to be collated to date. Such a cross-national investigation of the SPQ has potential to advance our understanding of the manifestation of schizotypal traits across the world. In addition, a multisite data set is helpful because it contributes to knowledge about the external validity and generalizability of schizotypal personality.

Examination of the factorial structure underlying the SPQ scores indicates that schizotypal traits have a multidimensional rather than a unidimensional structure. At the item level, the nine factor and second-order factor models had adequate goodness-of-fit (i.e., based on RMSEA indices), especially given the factorial complexity of these measurement models (i.e., 74 categorical items and nine first-order factors or three-higher order factors). Moreover, almost all factorial loadings were high and statistically significant across studies and countries. These results are consistent with the theoretical grouping of the nine SPQ subscales as well as with a three-factor model of schizotypal personality (Raine, Reference Raine1991). To date, no previous studies have tested these CFA measurement models of the SPQ at the item level. In future studies, these findings need replication and deeper analysis; for example, it will be important to study measurement invariance of the SPQ items across countries.

At the subscale level, the three-factor model of Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, Scerbo, Triphon and Kim1994) and the four-factor model of Stefanis et al. (Reference Stefanis, Smyrnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Ntzoufras and Stefanis2004) were the best fitting across studies. First, the three-factor model in which the Paranoid subscale saturated on both positive and interpersonal dimensions, showed a better fit to the data for the full sample and across samples. Our findings converge with those obtained in studies that focus on abbreviated versions of the SPQ (Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Lemos-Giráldez, Paino, Villazón-García and Muñiz2009; Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Matthews, Najolia and Brown2010) as well as previous factorial studies in both clinical and non-clinical samples (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Hsiao and Lin1997; Vollema & Hoijtink, Reference Vollema and Hoijtink2000; Fossati et al. Reference Fossati, Raine, Carretta, Leonardi and Maffei2003; Badcock & Dragovic, Reference Badcock and Dragovic2006; Wuthrich & Bates, Reference Wuthrich and Bates2006; Bora & Arabaci, Reference Bora and Arabaci2009; Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Debbané, Schneider, Badoud and Eliez2016a ). Similar factorial solutions have been found for other measures of schizotypy in samples of the general population (Fonseca-Pedrero et al. Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Ortuño-Sierra, Sierro, Daniel, Cella, Preti, Mohr and Mason2015). Moreover, this factorial structure is to a large extent similar to that reported in studies of patients with psychosis (Liddle, Reference Liddle1987). Just as the manifestation of schizophrenia is heterogeneous – encompassing a broad range of emotional, cognitive, perceptual, social and behavioural functions – schizotypy also involves a diverse set of traits (Cohen & Fonseca-Pedrero, Reference Cohen and Fonseca-Pedreroin press).

Second, Stefanis et al.’s (Reference Stefanis, Smyrnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Ntzoufras and Stefanis2004) model yielded the best goodness-of-fit indices in comparison with the other measurement models. These results are convergent with those reported by previous researchers (e.g, Bora & Arabaci, Reference Bora and Arabaci2009). Our results show that the SPQ may be particularly useful for tapping positive, interpersonal, paranoid and disorganized schizotypal features. However, the results for the four-factor model should be interpreted cautiously. Specifically, in Stefanis et al.’s model the interpersonal and paranoid factors have two subscales in common – Excessive Social Anxiety and Paranoid Ideation. From a psychometric point of view, such cross-loading of subscales renders interpretation problematic. In particular, it becomes difficult to understand what each dimension measures. These limitations have to be taken into account when interpreting the significance of results within a CFA framework.

Third, correlated three-factor multilevel model with loading freely estimated across levels indicated good fit of the model to the data. This schizotypal measurement model seems to be similar at both individual and country levels. The findings presented in this study favour the use of the three-factor model of the SPQ, at least in the countries included in this study. In adition, our multilevel results provide new insight into the construct of schizotypal personality. However, more research is needed; it will be important to replicate these findings in samples drawn randomly from the general population, to test scalar and strong measurement invariance at multilevel data, and to add data from new countries. According to Cohen and Fonseca-Pedrero (Reference Cohen and Fonseca-Pedreroin press) resolving the structure of schizotypal personality is an important step towards: (a) understanding the number and content of schizotypy symptoms, (b) resolving whether schizotypy reflects multiple processes or a single construct with varied expressions, and (c) developing more sophisticated measures and operational definitions for empirical and clinical use. Moreover, a sound and reliable factorial solution may harmonize clinical and empirical research on schizotypal traits worldwide.

The SPQ scores showed adequate levels of internal consistency across studies and countries. The reliability of the SPQ scores, estimated with ordinal α, were above 0.75. SPQ scores showed adequate psychometric properties across countries and hold implications for the use of this tool in cross-cultural research as well as for early detection of those individuals at risk for psychosis-spectrum disorders and mental health disorders. The SPQ is a tool that covers a wide variety of facets related to schizotypal personality, and, therefore, it can be considered as an accurate and useful tool to measure the wide scope of phenomena captured by this construct included within DSM-5 and ICD-10. Moreover, psychometric measurement of schizotypal personality allows us to understand the various manifestations of psychosis-spectrum risk. The psychometric assessment of schizotypal traits offers unique benefits – it is relatively inexpensive, non-invasive and useful for screening large samples of the general population. This research further extends the knowledge of the reliability of schizotypal traits, measured using the SPQ, in non-clinical samples from different countries.

The results of the present study should be interpreted in the light of the following limitations. First, the majority of the participants were college students and this fact may affect generalization of the results to other populations of interest. Counter-balancing these cautions, we note that findings from general population samples (Studies 10, 11, 12, 15 and 21), birth cohort (Study 6) and older samples (Studies 11, 20 and 21) yielded findings consistent with those from college samples. Second, the study is subject to the problems inherent to any research based on self-reports. This notwithstanding, self-report has the advantages that it is free of independent observer biases and can be more sensitive to underlying causal processes (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Myers, Torgersen, Neale and Reichborn-Kjennerud2007). Third, the infrequency response was not systematically employed in all samples. Fourth, we have not considered whether the latent structure of the SPQ is best conceived as dimensional or categorial in nature. That is, our findings do not speak to whether we should think of the latent structure of schizotypal personality as comprising latent classes, latent dimensions, or some combination of dimensions and classes (Linscott, Reference Linscott2013).

Conclusions

Schizotypal personality is a heterogeneous construct closely linked to psychosis-spectrum disorders supported by an extensive body of theoretical and empirical knowledge. This study is the first to comprehensively examine the underlying structure and reliability of self-reported schizotypal traits using a multinational sample. First, the results strengthen the conceptual notion that schizotypal personality is a multifaceted rather than a unitary construct. Second, the SPQ, a tool that covers a wide variety of facets of schizotypal personality, showed adequate psychometric properties across countries. The current findings have important theoretical and clinical implications for psychosis-spectrum disorders, aetiological models and international diagnostic systems. Advances in the field of measurement open up new horizons for the assessment of schizotypal personality traits and allow a better understanding of the structure and content of this construct across western and non-western countries.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001829

Acknowledgements

E.F.P was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (MICINN) (PSI2014-56114-P), by the Instituto Carlos III, Center for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), and by 2015 edition of the BBVA Foundation Grants for Researchers and Cultural Creators. M.D was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (100019_159440). R.Ch. was supported by the Beijing Training Project for Leading Talents in S&T (Z151100000315020), the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission Grant (Z161100000216138), and the CAS/SAFEA International Partnership Programme for Creative Research Teams (Y2CX131003). S. G and I.T. were supported by the ‘ARISTEIA II’ Action of the Operational Programme Education and Lifelong Learning and was co-funded by the European Social Fund (ESF) and National Resources [grant number KA 2990].

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn. revised) (DSM-III-R). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Badcock, JC, Dragovic, M (2006). Schizotypal personality in mature adults. Personality and Individual Differences 40, 7785.Google Scholar
Barrantes-Vidal, N, Grant, P, Kwapil, T (2015). The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S408-S416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrantes-Vidal, N, Gross, G, Sheinbaum, T, Mitjavila, M, Ballespí, S, Kwapil, TR (2013). Positive and negative schizotypy are associated with prodromal and schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms. Schizophrenia Research 145, 5055.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Arabaci, LE (2009). Confirmatory factor analysis of schizotypal personality traits in university students. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry 20, 339345.Google Scholar
Brown, TA (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd edn. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Byrne, B (2012). Structural Equation Modeling with Mplus: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Chen, WJ, Hsiao, CK, Lin, CCH (1997). Schizotypy in community samples: the three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106, 649654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, MW-L, Leung, K, Au, K (2006). Evaluating multilevel models in cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37, 522541.Google Scholar
Chmielewski, M, Watson, D (2008). The heterogeneous structure of schizotypal personality disorder: item-level factors of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and Their associations with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms, dissociative tendencies, and normal personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 117, 364376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicero, DC (2016). Measurement Invariance of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire in Asian, Pacific Islander, White, and Multiethnic Populations. Psychological Assessment 28, 351361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicero, DC, Martin, EA, Becker, TM, Docherty, AR, Kerns, JG (2014). Correspondence between psychometric and clinical high risk for psychosis in an undergraduate population. Psychological Assessment 26, 901915.Google Scholar
Cohen, A, Fonseca-Pedrero, E (in press). Towards a schizotypy core: convergence and divergence of two empirically-derived self-report measures from a nonclinical sample. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology.Google Scholar
Cohen, A, Mohr, C, Ettinger, U, Chan, RCK, Park, S (2015). Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S427-S435.Google Scholar
Cohen, AS, Matthews, RA, Najolia, GM, Brown, LA (2010). Toward a more psychometrically sound brief measure of schizotypal traits: introducing the SPQ-Brief Revised. Journal of Personality Disorders 24, 516537.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Compton, MT, Goulding, SM, Bakeman, R, McClure-Tone, EB (2009). Confirmation of a four-factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire among undergraduate students. Schizophrenia Research 111, 4652.Google Scholar
Debbané, M, Eliez, S, Badoud, D, Conus, P, Flückiger, R, Schultze-Lutter, F (2015). Developing psychosis and its risk states through the lens of schizotypy. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S396-S407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Debbané, M, Mohr, C (2015). Integration and development in schizotypy research: an introduction to the special supplement. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S363S365.Google Scholar
Dumas, P, Bouafia, S, Gutknecht, C, Saoud, M, Dalery, J, d'Amato, T (2000). [Validation of the French version of the Raine Schizotypal Personality Disorder Questionnaire – categorial and dimensional approach to schizotypal personality traits in a normal student population]. Encephale 26, 2329.Google Scholar
Dunn, TJ, Baguley, T, Brunsden, V (2014). From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. British Journal of Psychology 105, 399412.Google Scholar
Ettinger, U, Mohr, C, Gooding, D, Cohen, A, Rapp, A, Haenschel, C, Park, S (2015). Cognition and brain function in schizotypy: a selective review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S417-S426.Google Scholar
Everett, KV, Linscott, RJ (2015). Dimensionality vs taxonicity of schizotypy: some new data and challenges ahead. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S465S474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonseca Pedrero, E, Debbané, M (2017). Schizotypal traits and psychotic-like experiences during adolescence: an update. Psicothema 29, 517.Google Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Debbané, M, Schneider, M, Badoud, D, Eliez, S (2016a). Schizotypal traits in adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: validity, reliability and risk for psychosis. Psychological Medicine 46, 10051013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Fumero, A, Paino, M, de Miguel, A, Ortuño-Sierra, J, Lemos Giraldez, S, Muñiz, J (2014). Schizotypal personality questionnaire: new sources of validity evidence in college students. Psychiatry Research 219, 214220.Google Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Gooding, D, Debbané, M, Muñiz, J (2016b). Psychopathology: psychosis assessment and high-risk paradigms. In The ITC International Handbook of Testing and Assessment (ed. Bartram, D.), pp. 147170. Oxford University Press: UK.Google Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Lemos-Giráldez, S, Paino, M, Villazón-García, U, Muñiz, J (2009). Validation of the schizotypal personality questionnaire brief form in adolescents. Schizophrenia Research 111, 5360.Google Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Ortuño-Sierra, J, Sierro, G, Daniel, C, Cella, M, Preti, A, Mohr, C, Mason, OJ (2015). The measurement invariance of schizotypy in Europe. European Psychiatry 30, 837844.Google Scholar
Fossati, A, Raine, A, Carretta, I, Leonardi, B, Maffei, C (2003). The three-factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across age and gender. Personality and Individual Differences 35, 10071019.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Carpenter, WT, Woods, SW, McGlashan, TH (2014). Attenuated psychosis syndrome: ready for DSM-5.1? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 10, 155192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, GM, Mellin, J, Silvia, PJ, Barrantes-Vidal, N, Kwapil, TR (2014). Comparing the factor structure of the Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales and the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 5, 397405.Google Scholar
Hu, L-T, Bentler, PM (1999). Cut off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling 6, 155.Google Scholar
IBM Corp Released (2013). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. IBM Corp: Armonk, NY.Google Scholar
Kelleher, I, Cederlöf, M, Lichtenstein, P (2014). Psychotic experiences as a predictor of the natural course of suicidal ideation: a Swedish cohort study. World Psychiatry 13, 184188.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, McGuire, M, Gruenberg, AM, O'Hare, A, Spellman, M, Walsh, D (1993). The Roscommon Family Study: III. Schizophrenia-related personality disorders in relatives. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 781788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Myers, J, Torgersen, S, Neale, MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud, T (2007). The heritability of cluster A personality disorders assessed by both personal interview and questionnaire. Psychological Medicine 37, 655665.Google Scholar
Kwapil, TR, Barrantes-Vidal, N (2015). Schizotypy: looking back and moving forward. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S366-S373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwapil, TR, Gross, GM, Silvia, PJ, Barrantes-Vidal, N (2013). Prediction of psychopathology and functional impairment by positive and negative schizotypy in the Chapmans’ ten-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 807815.Google Scholar
Lahmar, ML, Gassab, L, Beltaief, F, Mechri, A (2014). Psychometric properties of the Arabic version of the schizotypal personality questionnaire in Tunisian university students. Tunisie Medicale 92, 318322.Google Scholar
Lenzenweger, MF (2010). Schizotypy and Schizophrenia: The View from Experimental Psychopathology. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Liddle, PF (1987). The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia. A re-examination of the positive-negative dichotomy. British Journal of Psychiatry 151, 145151.Google Scholar
Linscott, RJ (2013). The taxonicity of schizotypy: does the same taxonic class structure emerge from analyses of different attributes of schizotypy and from fundamentally different statistical methods? Psychiatry Research 210, 414421.Google Scholar
Linscott, RJ, van Os, J (2013). An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine 43, 11331149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, O (2015). The assessment of schizotypy and its clinical relevance. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, S374-S385.Google Scholar
Meehl, PE (1962). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychologist 17, 827838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, LK, Muthén, BO (1998–2012). Mplus User's Guide, 7th edn. Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Nuevo, R, Chatterji, S, Verdes, E, Naidoo, N, Arango, C, Ayuso-Mateos, JL (2012). The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross-national study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 475485.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2011). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing (http://www.R-project.org): Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Raine, A (1991). The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17, 555564.Google Scholar
Raine, A (2006). Schizotypal personality: neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2, 291326.Google Scholar
Raine, A, Benishay, D (1995). The SPQ-B: a brief screening instrument for schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 9, 346355.Google Scholar
Raine, A, Reynolds, C, Lencz, T, Scerbo, A, Triphon, N, Kim, D (1994). Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality . Schizophrenia Bulletin 20, 191201.Google Scholar
Reynolds, CA, Raine, A, Mellingen, K, Venables, PH, Mednick, SA (2000). Three-factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 603618.Google Scholar
Schimanski, ID, Mouat, KL, Billinghurst, BL, Linscott, RJ (2017). Preliminary evidence that schizophrenia liability at age 15 predicts suicidal ideation two years later. Schizophrenia Research 181, 6062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefanis, NC, Smyrnis, N, Avramopoulos, D, Evdokimidis, I, Ntzoufras, I, Stefanis, CN (2004). Factorial composition of self-rated schizotypal traits among young males undergoing military training. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30, 335350.Google Scholar
Tsaousis, I, Zouraraki, C, Karamaouna, P, Karagiannopoulou, L, Giakoumaki, SG (2015). The validity of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire in a Greek sample: tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences. Comprehensive Psychiatry 62, 5162.Google Scholar
Vollema, MG, Hoijtink, H (2000). The multidimensionality of self-report schizotypy in a psychiatric population: an analysis using multidimensional Rasch models. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 565575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, EE, Fernandez, F, Snelling, M, Barkus, E (2016). Genetic consideration of schizotypal traits: a review. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 1769.Google Scholar
World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Journal of the American Medical Association 310, 21912194.Google Scholar
Wuthrich, VM, Bates, TC (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and Chapman schizotypy scales. Journal of Personality Assessment 87, 292304.Google Scholar
Yu, CY, Muthén, BO (2002). Evaluation of Model Fit Indices for Latent Variable Models with Categorical and Continuous Outcomes (Technical Report). UCLA, Graduate School of Education and Information Studies: Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Zumbo, BD, Gadermann, AM, Zeisser, C (2007). Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and theta for Likert rating scales. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods 6, 2129.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Pearson correlations between Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire subscales for all studies [mean value (range)]

Figure 2

Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices of the schizotypal personality models tested for the full sample (n = 27 001)

Figure 3

Table 4. Standardized factor loadings for the four-factor model

Figure 4

Table 5. Standardized factorial loadings for the three-factor model in the total sample (n = 27 001)

Figure 5

Table 6. Ordinal α estimations for Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire subscales across studies and total sample

Supplementary material: File

Fonseca-Pedrero supplementary material

Fonseca-Pedrero supplementary material

Download Fonseca-Pedrero supplementary material(File)
File 53.2 KB