Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-09T16:17:43.404Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social context and the real-world consequences of social anxiety

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 August 2019

Juyoen Hur*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA
Kathryn A. DeYoung
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA Department of Family Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA
Samiha Islam
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA
Allegra S. Anderson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN37240, USA
Matthew G. Barstead
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA
Alexander J. Shackman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA Maryland Neuroimaging Center, University of Maryland, College Park, MD20742, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Juyoen Hur, E-mail: juh614@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Social anxiety lies on a continuum, and young adults with elevated symptoms are at risk for developing a range of psychiatric disorders. Yet relatively little is known about the factors that govern the hour-by-hour experience and expression of social anxiety in the real world.

Methods

Here we used smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to intensively sample emotional experience across different social contexts in the daily lives of 228 young adults selectively recruited to represent a broad spectrum of social anxiety symptoms.

Results

Leveraging data from over 11 000 real-world assessments, our results highlight the central role of close friends, family members, and romantic partners. The presence of such close companions was associated with enhanced mood, yet socially anxious individuals had fewer confidants and spent less time with the close companions that they do have. Although higher levels of social anxiety were associated with a general worsening of mood, socially anxious individuals appear to derive larger benefits – lower levels of negative affect, anxiety, and depression – from their close companions. In contrast, variation in social anxiety was unrelated to the amount of time spent with strangers, co-workers, and acquaintances; and we uncovered no evidence of emotional hypersensitivity to these less-familiar individuals.

Conclusions

These findings provide a framework for understanding the deleterious consequences of social anxiety in emerging adulthood and set the stage for developing improved intervention strategies.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Socially anxious individuals are prone to heightened fear, anxiety, and avoidance of social interactions and situations associated with potential scrutiny (Alden and Taylor, Reference Alden and Taylor2004; Heimberg et al., Reference Heimberg, Hofmann, Liebowitz, Schneier, Smits, Stein, Hinton and Craske2014). In addition to heightened negative affect (NA), socially anxious individuals tend to report lower levels of positive affect (PA) (Anderson and Hope, Reference Anderson and Hope2008; Kashdan and Collins, Reference Kashdan and Collins2010; Kashdan et al., Reference Kashdan, Weeks and Savostyanova2011; Geyer et al., Reference Geyer, Fua, Daniel, Chow, Bonelli, Huang, Barnes and Teachman2018). Social anxiety symptoms lie on a continuum and, when extreme, can become debilitating (Lipsitz and Schneier, Reference Lipsitz and Schneier2000; Katzelnick et al., Reference Katzelnick, Kobak, DeLeire, Henk, Greist, Davidson, Schneier, Stein and Helstad2001; Kessler, Reference Kessler2003; Rapee and Spence, Reference Rapee and Spence2004; Craske et al., Reference Craske, Stein, Eley, Milad, Holmes, Rapee and Wittchen2017; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi, Hu, Karam, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Wojtyniak, Xavier, Kessler and Scott2017; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton, Ruggero, Simms, Widiger, Achenbach, Bach, Bagby, Bornovalova, Carpenter, Chmielewski, Cicero, Clark, Conway, DeClercq, DeYoung, Docherty, Drislane, First, Forbush, Hallquist, Haltigan, Hopwood, Ivanova, Jonas, Latzman, Markon, Miller, Morey, Mullins-Sweatt, Ormel, Patalay, Patrick, Pincus, Regier, Reininghaus, Rescorla, Samuel, Sellbom, Shackman, Skodol, Slade, South, Sunderland, Tackett, Venables, Waldman, Waszczuk, Waugh, Wright, Zald and Zimmerman2018; Conway et al., Reference Conway, Forbes, Forbush, Fried, Hallquist, Kotov, Mullins-Sweatt, Shackman, Skodol, South, Sunderland, Waszczuk, Zald, Afzali, Bornovalova, Carragher, Docherty, Jonas, Krueger, Patalay, Pincus, Tackett, Reininghaus, Waldman, Wright, Zimmerman, Bach, Bagby, Chmielewski, Cicero, Clark, Dalgleish, DeYoung, Hopwood, Ivanova, Latzman, Patrick, Ruggero, Samuel, Watson and Eaton2019; Ruscio, Reference Ruscio2019). Social anxiety disorder is among the most prevalent mental illnesses; contributes to the development of other psychiatric disorders, such as depression; and is challenging to treat (Schneier et al., Reference Schneier, Johnson, Hornig, Liebowitz and Weissman1992; Rodebaugh et al., Reference Rodebaugh, Holaway and Heimberg2004; Acarturk et al., Reference Acarturk, Cuijpers, Van Straten and De Graaf2009; Mathew et al., Reference Mathew, Pettit, Lewinsohn, Seeley and Roberts2011; Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, von Auer, Murray, Petermann, Helbig-Lang and Gerlach2013; Craske et al., Reference Craske, Stein, Eley, Milad, Holmes, Rapee and Wittchen2017; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi, Hu, Karam, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Wojtyniak, Xavier, Kessler and Scott2017). Relapse and recurrence are common, and pharmaceutical treatments are associated with significant adverse effects (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Yonkers, Otto, Eisen, Weisberg, Pagano, Shea and Keller2005; Rhebergen et al., Reference Rhebergen, Batelaan, de Graaf, Nolen, Spijker, Beekman and Penninx2011; Scholten et al., Reference Scholten, Batelaan, van Balkom, Wjh Penninx, Smit and van Oppen2013, Reference Scholten, Batelaan, Penninx, van Balkom, Smit, Schoevers and van Oppen2016; Gordon and Redish, Reference Gordon, Redish, Redish and Gordon2016; Spinhoven et al., Reference Spinhoven, Batelaan, Rhebergen, van Balkom, Schoevers and Penninx2016; Batelaan et al., Reference Batelaan, Bosman, Muntingh, Scholten, Huijbregts and van Balkom2017). Yet the situational factors that govern the momentary experience and expression of social anxiety in the real world remain incompletely understood. To date, most of what it known is based on either retrospective report or acute laboratory challenges (Alden and Wallace, Reference Alden and Wallace1995; Beck et al., Reference Beck, Davila, Farrow and Grant2006; Buote et al., Reference Buote, Pancer, Pratt, Adams, Birnie-Lefcovitch, Polivy and Wintre2007; Afram and Kashdan, Reference Afram and Kashdan2015; Crişan et al., Reference Crişan, Vulturar, Miclea and Miu2016).

As part of an on-going prospective-longitudinal study focused on individuals at risk for the development of mood and anxiety disorders, we used smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to intensively sample momentary levels of NA and PA in the daily lives of 228 young adults. Subjects were selectively recruited from a pool of 6594 individuals screened for individual differences in dispositional negativity (i.e. negative emotionality), the tendency to experience more intense, frequent, or persistent levels of depression, worry, fear and anxiety – including social anxiety (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Tromp, Stockbridge, Kaplan, Tillman and Fox2016; Hur et al., Reference Hur, Stockbridge, Fox and Shackman2019). This ‘enrichment’ strategy enabled us to examine a broader spectrum of social anxiety symptoms than alternate approaches, such as convenience sampling. Because EMA data are captured in real time (e.g. Who are you with?), they circumvent the biases that can distort retrospective reports and provide insights into how emotional experience dynamically responds to moment-by-moment changes in social context (Barrett, Reference Barrett1997; Shiffman et al., Reference Shiffman, Stone and Hufford2008; Csikszentmihalyi et al., Reference Csikszentmihalyi, Mehl and Conner2013; Lay et al., Reference Lay, Gerstorf, Scott, Pauly and Hoppmann2017). We focused on young adulthood because it is a time of profound, often stressful developmental transitions (e.g. moving away from home, forging new social relationships; Hays and Oxley, Reference Hays and Oxley1986; Alloy and Abramson, Reference Alloy and Abramson1999; Arnett, Reference Arnett2000; Pancer et al., Reference Pancer, Hunsberger, Pratt and Alisat2000). In fact, more than half of undergraduate students report overwhelming anxiety, with many experiencing the first onset or a recurrence of anxiety and mood disorders during this period (Auerbach et al., Reference Auerbach, Alonso, Axinn, Cuijpers, Ebert, Green, Hwang, Kessler, Liu, Mortier, Nock, Pinder-Amaker, Sampson, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Andrade, Benjet, Caldas-de-Almeida, Demyttenaere, Florescu, de Girolamo, Gureje, Haro, Karam, Kiejna, Kovess-Masfety, Lee, McGrath, O'Neill, Pennell, Scott, Ten Have, Torres, Zaslavsky, Zarkov and Bruffaerts2016, Reference Auerbach, Mortier, Bruffaerts, Alonso, Benjet, Cuijpers, Demyttenaere, Ebert, Green, Hasking, Murray, Nock, Pinder-Amaker, Sampson, Stein, Vilagut, Zaslavsky, Kessler and Collaboratorsin press; American College Health Association, 2016; Global Burden of Disease Collaborators, 2016; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi, Hu, Karam, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Wojtyniak, Xavier, Kessler and Scott2017; Lipson et al., Reference Lipson, Lattie and Eisenberg2018). Those with elevated levels of social anxiety tend to experience substantial distress and impairment and are more likely to develop psychopathology (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, Avenevoli, Acharyya, Zhang and Angst2002).

We were particularly interested in understanding how the momentary emotional experience of socially anxious individuals varies as a function of social context. Emotion is profoundly social (Fox and Shackman, Reference Fox, Shackman, Fox, Lapate, Shackman and Davidson2018). Emotional experiences are routinely shared and dissected with friends, family, and romantic partners (Rime, Reference Rime2009). Humans and other primates routinely seek the company of close companions in response to stressors, and increased social engagement promotes positive affect (Cottrell and Epley, Reference Cottrell, Epley, Suls and Miller1977; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018). Indeed, there is abundant evidence that close companions play a critical role in coping with stress and regulating negative affect (Bolger and Eckenrode, Reference Bolger and Eckenrode1991; Myers, Reference Myers, Kahneman, Diener and Schwartz1999; Wade and Kendler, Reference Wade and Kendler2000; Buote et al., Reference Buote, Pancer, Pratt, Adams, Birnie-Lefcovitch, Polivy and Wintre2007; Marroquin, Reference Marroquin2011; Zaki and Williams, Reference Zaki and Williams2013; Kendler and Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2014; Coan and Sbarra, Reference Coan and Sbarra2015; Ramsey and Gentzler, Reference Ramsey and Gentzler2015; Reeck et al., Reference Reeck, Ames and Ochsner2016). Many of these beneficial effects appear to be disrupted in socially anxious individuals (Alden and Taylor, Reference Alden and Taylor2004).

We began by testing whether social anxiety is associated with the amount of time allocated to different social contexts (e.g. with close companions) and whether this reflects the number of self-reported confidants. Social avoidance is diagnostic of social anxiety disorder, is a key component of dimensional measures of social anxiety, and contributes to functional impairment and reduced quality of life (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Beidel, Dancu and Keys1986; Liebowitz, Reference Liebowitz1987; Beidel et al., Reference Beidel, Turner and Morris1999; Strahan and Conger, Reference Strahan and Conger1999; APA, 2013). Among community samples, adults with elevated levels of social anxiety are less likely to have a close friend and more likely to be unmarried by mid-life (Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Hughes, George and Blazer1994). They are also more likely to be lonely (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Rodebaugh, Zyphur and Gleeson2016). Recent work using unobtrusive, smartphone-based global positioning system (GPS) data provides additional evidence suggestive of social inhibition and avoidance (Boukhechba et al., Reference Boukhechba, Chow, Fua, Teachman and Barnes2018), demonstrating that socially anxious university students spend significantly less time at ‘leisure’ (e.g. gymnasiums, pubs, cinemas, and coffee shops) and ‘food’ (e.g. restaurants, food courts, and dining halls) locations during peak hours in the evening. Socially anxious students also spent more time at home or off-campus (e.g. parents’ home), particularly on weekends, and visited fewer locations overall, suggesting a more restricted range of activities (see also Chow et al., Reference Chow, Fua, Huang, Bonelli, Xiong, Barnes and Teachman2017). Whether this pattern reflects generalized avoidance, specific avoidance of socially ‘distant’ individuals (e.g. strangers, acquaintances), or a lack of confidants remains unknown.

Next, we used a series of multilevel models (MLMs) to understand the interactive effects of social anxiety and the social environment on momentary affect. This enabled us to test whether socially anxious individuals experience heightened NA and attenuated PA in the presence of distant others, as one would expect based on laboratory studies of interactions with unfamiliar peers and researchers (Meleshko and Alden, Reference Meleshko and Alden1993; Creed and Funder, Reference Creed and Funder1998; Coles et al., Reference Coles, Turk and Heimberg2002; Kashdan and Roberts, Reference Kashdan and Roberts2004, Reference Kashdan and Roberts2006, Reference Kashdan and Roberts2007; Heerey and Kring, Reference Heerey and Kring2007; Kashdan et al., Reference Kashdan, Ferssizidis, Farmer, Adams and McKnight2013b; Crişan et al., Reference Crişan, Vulturar, Miclea and Miu2016). This expectation is reinforced by evidence from EMA studies that children with social anxiety disorder experience diminished PA in the presence of distant others (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Lee, Wright, Gilchrist, Forbes, McMakin, Dahl, Ladouceur, Ryan and Silk2017). Whether this pattern is also evident in adults remains unknown.

Using a MLM approach, we also tested two competing predictions about the consequences of close companions. One possibility is that socially anxious individuals derive increased emotional benefits (e.g. lower levels of NA) from close companions. Consistent with this view, the presence of a friend has been shown to normalize behavioral signs of anxiety and reduce negative self-thoughts in socially anxious adults exposed to an experimental speech challenge (Pontari, Reference Pontari2009). Likewise, diary studies suggest that spousal support plays a key role in dampening negative affect among patients with social anxiety disorder (Zaider et al., Reference Zaider, Heimberg and Iida2010) and EMA studies show that the presence of close companions is associated with disproportionately enhanced PA in children with social anxiety disorder (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Lee, Wright, Gilchrist, Forbes, McMakin, Dahl, Ladouceur, Ryan and Silk2017) and adults with elevated levels of dispositional negativity (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018). In fact, a variety of work suggests that individuals with low levels of psychological well-being and patients with depression reap larger emotional benefits from positive daily events (Rottenberg, Reference Rottenberg2017; Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Swendsen, Cui, Husky, Johns, Zipunnikov and Merikangas2018). Although socially anxious adults often show symptoms of depression and anhedonia, it is unclear whether similar benefits extend to this population.

A competing possibility is that socially anxious individuals fail to capitalize on available socio-emotional support. Indeed, socially anxious individuals tend to be less emotionally expressive, disclosing, and intimate with companions (Vernberg et al., Reference Vernberg, Abwender, Ewell and Beery1992; Meleshko and Alden, Reference Meleshko and Alden1993; Sparrevohn and Rapee, Reference Sparrevohn and Rapee2009; Cuming and Rapee, Reference Cuming and Rapee2010; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Morelli, Ong and Zaki2018). They perceive themselves as receiving less social support (La Greca and Lopez, Reference La Greca and Lopez1998; Torgrud et al., Reference Torgrud, Walker, Murray, Cox, Chartier and Kjernisted2004; Cuming and Rapee, Reference Cuming and Rapee2010); perceive their friendships to be of a lower quality (Rodebaugh, Reference Rodebaugh2009; Rodebaugh et al., Reference Rodebaugh, Lim, Shumaker, Levinson and Thompson2015); are less satisfied with friends, family, and romantic partners (Stein and Kean, Reference Stein and Kean2000; Starr and Davila, Reference Starr and Davila2008; Wong et al., Reference Wong, Sarver and Beidel2012); and are prone to emotional neediness and overreliance (Davila and Beck, Reference Davila and Beck2002; Darcy et al., Reference Darcy, Davila and Beck2005). Perhaps as a consequence, socially anxious individuals report elevated levels of interpersonal conflict (Cuming and Rapee, Reference Cuming and Rapee2010) and many patients show profound impairment of interpersonal relationships (Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Fuetsch, Sonntag, Müller and Liebowitz2000; Rapaport et al., Reference Rapaport, Clary, Fayyad and Endicott2005; Olatunji et al., Reference Olatunji, Cisler and Tolin2007; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi, Hu, Karam, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Wojtyniak, Xavier, Kessler and Scott2017). Taken together, these observations motivate the prediction that socially anxious individuals derive smaller emotional benefits or even costs (e.g. higher levels of NA) from close companions.

Discovering the situational factors associated with the real-world experience of social anxiety is important. The identification of potentially modifiable targets, such as social context, has the potential to guide the development of improved intervention strategies.

Method

Overview

As part of an on-going prospective-longitudinal study focused on individuals at risk for the development of internalizing disorders, we used well-established measures of dispositional negativity (often termed neuroticism or negative emotionality; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Tromp, Stockbridge, Kaplan, Tillman and Fox2016; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018) to screen 6594 young adults (57.1% female; 59.0% White, 19.0% Asian, 9.9% African American, 6.3% Hispanic, 5.8% Multiracial/Other; M = 19.2 years, s.d. = 1.1 years). Screening data were stratified into quartiles (top quartile, middle quartiles, bottom quartile) separately for males and females. Individuals who met preliminary inclusion criteria were independently recruited from each of the resulting six strata. Approximately half the subjects were recruited from the top quartile, with the remainder split between the middle and bottom quartiles (i.e. 50% high, 25% medium, and 25% low). Given the typically robust relations between measures of dispositional negativity and social anxiety – R 2 = 0.25 in the present sample – this ‘enrichment’ strategy allowed us to examine a relatively wide range of social anxiety symptoms without gaps or discontinuities. All subjects were first-year university students in good physical health with consistent access to a smartphone. All reported the absence of a lifetime psychotic, bipolar, neurological, or developmental disorder. Given the focus of the larger prospective-longitudinal study on risk for the development of mental illness, all subjects reported the absence of current alcohol/substance abuse, suicidal ideation, internalizing disorder (past 2 months), and psychiatric treatment. To maximize the range of psychiatric risk, subjects with a lifetime history of anxiety and mood disorders were not excluded, as in prior studies of high-risk populations (Alloy et al., Reference Alloy, Abramson, Hogan, Whitehouse, Rose, Robinson, Kim and Lapkin2000). At the baseline laboratory session, subjects provided informed written consent, were familiarized with the EMA protocol (see below), and completed the social anxiety and social network assessments. Beginning the next day, subjects completed up to 8 EMA surveys/day for 7 days. All procedures were approved by the University of Maryland Institutional Review Board. The sample does not overlap with that detailed in prior work by our group (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018).

Subjects

Two-hundred and forty-two subjects completed both the baseline assessment and EMA protocol. Fourteen subjects were excluded from analyses: 2 withdrew and 12 (~5%) failed to complete >39 survey prompts (70% compliance). The final sample included 228 subjects (51.3% female; 62.7% White, 17.5% Asian, 8.3% African American, 4.9% Hispanic, 6.6% Multiracial/Other; M = 18.76 years, s.d. = 0.35 years).

Power analysis

Sample size was determined a priori as part of the application for the award that supported this research (R01-MH107444). The target sample size (N ≈ 240) was chosen to afford acceptable power and precision given available resources (Schönbrodt and Perugini, Reference Schönbrodt and Perugini2013). At the time of study design, G-power 3.1.9.2 (http://www.gpower.hhu.de) indicated >99% power to detect a benchmark medium-sized effect (r = 0.30) with up to 20% planned attrition (N = 192 usable datasets) using α = 0.05 (two-tailed).

Social anxiety

At baseline, the self-report version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS) was used to quantify social anxiety (Liebowitz, Reference Liebowitz1987). Subjects used a 0 (none) to 3 (severe) scale to rate the amount of fear and anxiety they typically experience in response to 24 everyday situations (e.g. going to a party, meeting strangers, returning goods to a store, speaking up at meeting). They used a 0 (never) to 3 (usually) rating scale to rate the frequency of avoiding the 24 situations. Social anxiety was quantified by summing the 48 responses. As shown in Fig. 1, LSAS scores were approximately normally distributed (M = 41.7, s.d. = 22.0, Range = 1–121, α = 0.95) and somewhat higher than that previously reported in large university convenience samples (N = 856, M = 34.7, s.d. = 20.4; Russell and Shaw, Reference Russell and Shaw2009)Footnote 1, Footnote 2Footnote .

Fig. 1. Social anxiety. Social anxiety was assessed at baseline using the self-report version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS). The two highest cases were not excluded because they are sensible – given the nature of the scale and the sample – and because they did not exert undue statistical leverage (Hoaglin et al., Reference Hoaglin, Iglewicz and Tukey1986; Hoaglin and Iglewicz, Reference Hoaglin and Iglewicz1987). Exploratory analyses indicated that the exclusion of these cases did not meaningfully alter the results (not reported).

Social network size

At baseline, the self-reported number of close companions was measured using an item (How many people do you know where you have a close, confiding relationship and can share your most private feelings?) from the modified Kendler Social Support Inventory (MKSSI; Spoozak et al., Reference Spoozak, Gotman, Smith, Belanger and Yonkers2009). Single-item measures of social network size are routinely used in epidemiology research (e.g. Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2005; Van Lente et al., Reference Van Lente, Barry, Molcho, Morgan, Watson, Harrington and McGee2012; Kocalevent et al., Reference Kocalevent, Berg, Beutel, Hinz, Zenger, Harter, Nater and Brahler2018). The resulting descriptive statistics (M = 5.6, s.d. = 4.1, Range = 0–30) are broadly consistent with the results of past work focused on confidant networks in university students (Sarason et al., Reference Sarason, Levine, Basham and Sarason1983; Freberg et al., Reference Freberg, Adams, McGaughey and Freberg2010) and friendship networks in community-dwelling adults (Wang and Wellman, Reference Wang and Wellman2010).

EMA procedures

SurveySignal (Hofmann and Patel, Reference Hofmann and Patel2015) was used to automatically deliver 8 text messages/day to each subject's smartphone. On weekdays, messages were delivered every 1.5 to 3 h (M = 115 min, s.d. = 25) between 8:30 AM and 10:30 PM. As in prior work by our group (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018), weekday messages were delivered during the ‘passing periods’ between regularly scheduled university courses to maximize compliance. On weekends, messages were delivered every 1.5 to 2.5 h (M = 99 min, s.d. = 17) between 11:00 AM and 11:00 PM. Messages were delivered according to a fixed schedule that varied across days (e.g. the third message was delivered at 12:52 PM on Mondays and 12:16 PM on Tuesdays). Messages contained a link to a secure on-line survey. Subjects were instructed to respond within 30 min (Latency: Median = 2 min, s.d. = 7 min, Interquartile Range = 9 min) and to refrain from responding at unsafe or inconvenient moments (e.g. while driving). A reminder was sent when subjects failed to respond within 15 min. During the baseline laboratory session, several well-established procedures were used to maximize compliance (Palmier-Claus et al., Reference Palmier-Claus, Myin-Germeys, Barkus, Bentley, Udachina, Delespaul, Lewis and Dunn2011), including: (1) delivering a test message in the laboratory and confirming that the subject was able to successfully complete the on-line EMA survey, (2) 24/7 technical support, and (3) monetary bonuses. Base compensation was $10, with $20 bonuses for ⩾70% and ⩾80% compliance, respectively (Total = $10–$50). In the final sample, EMA compliance was acceptable (M = 87.9%, s.d. = 6.2%, Minimum = 71.4%, Total = 11 224) and unrelated to social anxiety, p = 0.77.

EMA survey

Current NA (afraid, nervous, worried, hopeless, sad) and PA (calm, cheerful, content, enthusiastic, joy, relaxed) at the time of the survey prompt was rated using a 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) scale. Subjects also indicated their current social context (‘At the time of ping, who was around?’): alone, close friend(s), family, friend(s), romantic partner, acquaintance(s), co-worker(s), and/or stranger(s). Composite measures of NA and PA were computed by averaging the relevant items (αs > 0.92). To enable follow-up assessments of generality, composite anxiety (afraid, nervous, worried) and depression (hopeless, sad) facet scales were computed (αs > 0.88). Building on prior work by our group and others (Diener and Seligman, Reference Diener and Seligman2002; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018), friends, close friends, family, and romantic partners were re-coded as ‘Close’ companions. Acquaintances, co-workers, and strangers were re-coded as ‘Distant’ companions. This approach is conceptually similar to the distinction between ‘strong’ and ‘weak’ social connections (Granovetter, Reference Granovetter1973). Analyses indicated that assessments completed in the presence of a mixture of Close and Distant companions (8%) showed intermediate effects and are omitted from the present report.

Analytic strategy

The overarching aim of the present study was to understand the joint explanatory influence of Social Anxiety (LSAS) and Social Context (EMA) on real-world Affect (EMA-derived NA, PA). In all cases, hypothesis testing employed a continuous measure of Social Anxiety.

We began by testing whether variation in Social Anxiety prospectively predicts the aggregate amount of time allocated to different social contexts. A standard multivariate mediation framework was then used to test whether relations between Social Anxiety and Social Context were statistically attributable, at least in part, to variation in Social Network Size (e.g. elevated social anxiety → fewer confidants → less time with close companions) (Hayes, Reference Hayes2017), where Size was indexed using the MKSSI. As in prior work by our group (Stout et al., Reference Stout, Shackman, Pedersen, Miskovich and Larson2017), the significance of the indirect effect (‘mediation’) was assessed using non-parametric bootstrapping (5000 samples). Although the mediation framework provides useful information, it rests on strong assumptions and positive results do not license causal inferences (Bullock et al., Reference Bullock, Green and Ha2010; Green et al., Reference Green, Ha and Bullock2010). Pirateplots were created using the yaRrr package for R (Phillips, Reference Phillips2017). Hotelling's test for dependent correlations was computed using FZT (https://psych.unl.edu/psycrs/statpage/comp.html).

Next, a series of MLMs was implemented in SPSS (version 24.0.0.0) with momentary assessments of Affect and Social Context nested within subjects and intercepts free to vary across subjects. Separate MLMs were computed for NA and PA. Level 2 variables (i.e. Social Anxiety) were mean centered.

The equations defined below outline the basic structure of our final MLMs in standard notation (Raudenbush and Bryk, Reference Raudenbush and Bryk2002). At the first level, Affect during EMA t for individual i was modeled as a function of Social Context:

(1)$${\rm Affec}{\rm t}_{ti} = \pi _{0i} + \pi _{1i}({\rm Distant}) + \pi _{2i}({\rm Close}) + e_{ti}$$

Alone served as the dummy-coded reference category for primary analyses (as in Equation 1). Distant companions served as the reference category for follow-up analysesFootnote 3.

At the second level, the association between Social Context and Affect was modeled as a function of individual differences in Social Anxiety:

(2)$$\pi _{0i} = \beta _{00} + \beta _{01}({\rm Anxiet}{\rm y}_i) + r_{0i}$$
(3)$$\pi _{1i} = \beta _{10} + \beta _{11}({\rm Anxiet}{\rm y}_i) + r_{1i}$$
(4)$$\pi _{2i} = \beta _{20} + \beta _{21}({\rm Anxiet}{\rm y}_i) + r_{2i}$$

Conceptually, this enabled us to test prospective relations between Social Anxiety and Affect, cross-sectional relations between EMA-derived measures of Social Context and Affect, and the potentially interactive effects of Social Anxiety and Social Context. We also explored the impact of incorporating variation in the amount of time allocated to different contexts as a nuisance variable. For significant effects, we examined generality across NA facets (i.e. anxious and depressed mood). As an additional validity check, we confirmed that similar results were obtained when two authors independently analyzed the data using SPSS (J.H.) and R (M.G.B.), respectively.

Results

Momentary emotional experience covaries with social context

Consistent with other work in young adults (Larson, Reference Larson1990; Berry and Hansen, Reference Berry and Hansen1996; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018), our sample spent slightly more than half their time with others (Close = 44.1%, Distant = 13.4%, Alone = 42.5%), although there were marked individual differences in the amount of time devoted to each social environment (Fig. 2). Individuals who spent more time with close others reported lower average levels of NA (r = −0.14, p = 0.03) and higher average levels of PA (r = 0.31, p < 0.000). Conversely, those who spent more time alone reported higher average levels of NA (r = 0.14, p = 0.03) and lower average levels of PA (r = −0.28, p < 0.001), replicating past work (e.g. Diener and Seligman, Reference Diener and Seligman2002; Oishi et al., Reference Oishi, Diener and Lucas2007; Diener et al., Reference Diener, Seligman, Choi and Oishi2018; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Updegraff, Iida, Dishion, Doane, Corbin, Van Lenten and Ha2018; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Weinstein, Hudja, Bloomer, Barstead, Fox and Lemay2018). The average amount of time spent with distant others was unrelated to average mood (ps > 0.20). In sum, individuals who spend more time with close companions report modestly enhanced mood, whereas those who are prone to seclusion show the opposite effect.

Fig. 2. Percentage of usable momentary assessments acquired in the presence of close companions, distant companions, or alone. Figure depicts the data (jittered gray points; individual participants), density distribution (colored bean plots), Bayesian 95% highest density interval (HDI; white bands), and mean (black bars) for each social context. HDIs permit population-generalizable visual inferences about mean differences and were estimated using 1000 samples from a posterior Gaussian distribution.

Socially anxious individuals spend less time with close companions and have smaller confidant networks

On average, individuals with higher levels of social anxiety spent significantly less time in the company of close companions (r = −0.16, p = 0.01) and showed a trend to spend more time alone (r = 0.13, p = 0.06), consistent with prior work (Alden and Taylor, Reference Alden and Taylor2004; Afram and Kashdan, Reference Afram and Kashdan2015). A mediation analysis suggested that this reflects reduced access to close companions. As shown in Fig. 3, individuals with higher levels of social anxiety report fewer confidants (a = −0.19, p = 0.005), which is also consistent with prior work (Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Haemmerlie and Edwards1991; Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Hughes, George and Blazer1994; La Greca and Lopez, Reference La Greca and Lopez1998). In turn, individuals with fewer confidants were less likely to be in the presence of close companions (b = 0.31, p < 0.001) at the time of momentary assessmentFootnote 4. Bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect excluded zero, indicating significant mediation. Likewise, the direct effect of social anxiety on the amount of time spent with close companions was no longer significant after accounting for variation in the number of confidants (c′ path in Fig. 3; p > 0.10)Footnote 5. Interestingly, social anxiety was not significantly related to the amount of time spent with distant companions (p = 0.20), contraindicating a general bias to avoid others. The association between social anxiety and the amount of time allocated to close companions was significantly stronger than that with distant companions, t Hotelling = 2.18, p = 0.03.

Fig. 3. The negative association between social anxiety and the amount of time with close companions is significantly explained by reduced access to confidants. Figure depicts the corresponding mediation model. Path labels indicate standardized regression coefficients, with c′ indicating the coefficient while controlling for variation in the self-reported number of confidants. Socially anxious individuals report fewer confidants, and individuals with fewer confidants were, in turn, less likely to be in the presence of close companions at the time of momentary assessment.

Social anxiety is associated with diminished real-world emotional experience

MLM analyses demonstrated that social anxiety is associated with reduced quality of real-world emotional experience. Individuals with higher levels of social anxiety report significantly increased NA (t = 25.2, b = 0.12, s.e. = 0.005, p < 0.001) and reduced PA (t = −24.1, b = −0.19, s.e. = 0.008, p < 0.001), consistent with past research (Kashdan, Reference Kashdan2004; Kashdan and Steger, Reference Kashdan and Steger2006; Kashdan et al., Reference Kashdan, Farmer, Adams, Ferssizidis, McKnight and Nezlek2013a, Reference Kashdan, Ferssizidis, Farmer, Adams and McKnight2013b)Footnote 6.

The quality of momentary emotional experience covaries with the presence of close companions

Relative to seclusion or the presence of distant others, MLM results showed that close companions are associated with lower levels of NA (Alone: t = −0.7.51, b = −0.09, s.e. = 0.012, p < 0.001; Distant: t = −6.71, b = −0.10, s.e. = 0.015, p < 0.001) and higher levels of PA (Alone: t = 15.79, b = 0.31, s.e. = 0.019, p < 0.001; Distant: t = 15.03, b = 0.37, s.e. = 0.025, p < 0.001). Relative to seclusion, distant companions are associated with lower levels of PA (PA: t = −2.59, b = −0.06, s.e. = 0.024, p = 0.01; NA:p > 0.30). Results were similar when controlling for variation in the amount of time allocated to different social contexts (online Supplementary Table S1). These findings reinforce the conclusion that the quality of momentary emotional experience is positively associated with the presence of close friends, family, and romantic partners.

Socially anxious individuals derive larger emotional benefits from close companions

We next considered the joint impact of social anxiety and social context on momentary mood (Table 1). As shown in the upper panel of Fig. 4, the results of this more comprehensive MLM revealed that socially anxious individuals derive larger emotional benefits – indexed by significantly lower levels of NA – from close companions relative to seclusion (Social Anxiety × Close-Alone, t = −2.27, b = −0.03, s.e. = 0.012, p = 0.02). In short, individuals with higher levels of social anxiety tend to experience the least intense, most normative levels of NA in the company of friends, family, and romantic partners. This effect remained significant after controlling for the amount of time allocated to different social contexts (online Supplementary Table S2)Footnote 7. Other interactions were not significant for NA or PA (p > 0.80). Social anxiety was not associated with an exaggerated emotional response in the presence of co-workers, strangers, and other distant companions (Fig. 4 and Table 1). The same general pattern of results was evident for analyses focused on the anxious and depressed facets of momentary NA (online Supplementary Tables S3 and S4).

Table 1. The impact of social anxiety and social context on momentary emotional experience

Note: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Fig. 4. The deleterious impact of social anxiety on momentary emotional experience depends on social context. Individuals with higher levels of social anxiety derive larger emotional benefits – larger decrements in negative affect (NA) – from close companions relative to being alone as shown in the upper panel. Hypothesis testing relied on a continuous measure of social anxiety. For illustrative purposes, predicted values derived from multilevel modeling are depicted for extreme levels (±1 s.d.) of social anxiety.

Discussion

Social anxiety lies on a continuum, from mild to debilitating, and young adults with elevated symptoms of social anxiety are more likely to show significant impairment and develop frank psychopathology. The present study provides new insights into the ways in which real-world emotional experience varies as a function of social anxiety and the social environment. Our results demonstrate that the presence of close companions is associated with lower levels of momentary NA (Fig. 4), including anxiety and depression. Importantly, individuals with higher levels of social anxiety were found to spend significantly less time with close companions and a mediation analysis suggested that this association is partially explained by smaller confidant networks (Fig. 3). Social anxiety was unrelated to the number of assessments completed in the presence of co-workers, strangers, and other distant companions, contraindicating a general social avoidance bias. Although social anxiety at baseline was prospectively associated with a diminished quality of momentary emotional experience (i.e. increased NA and reduced PA), MLM analyses demonstrated that individuals with higher levels of social anxiety derive significantly larger benefits – manifesting as lower levels of NA, anxiety, and depression – from the company of close companions (Fig. 4). In contrast, socially anxious individuals were not disproportionately sensitive to the presence of distant companions (Table 1 and Fig. 4). Indeed, they showed similarly high levels of NA when they were alone. Although social anxiety research and treatment has predominantly focused on responses to novelty and potential threat, our results underscore the centrality of friends, family, and romantic partners. These findings provide a framework for understanding the deleterious consequences of extreme social anxiety and guiding the development of improved interventions.

The present findings extend developmental and laboratory research highlighting the importance of social and interpersonal processes for emotion regulation and mental wellbeing (Maresh et al., Reference Maresh, Beckes and Coan2013; Zaki and Williams, Reference Zaki and Williams2013; Coan and Sbarra, Reference Coan and Sbarra2015; Reeck et al., Reference Reeck, Ames and Ochsner2016; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Barstead, Smith, Bowker, Pérez-Edgar and Fox2018). Our observations motivate the hypothesis that the pervasive NA characteristic of socially anxious young adults partially reflects difficulties forming or maintaining close relationships, consistent with work focused on children and adolescents at risk for developing social anxiety disorder (Ladd et al., Reference Ladd, Kochenderfer-Ladd, Eggum, Kochel and McConnell2011; Frenkel et al., Reference Frenkel, Fox, Pine, Walker, Degnan and Chronis-Tuscano2015; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Tromp, Stockbridge, Kaplan, Tillman and Fox2016; Markovic and Bowker, Reference Markovic and Bowker2017; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Barstead, Smith, Bowker, Pérez-Edgar and Fox2018). With fewer confidants, socially anxious individuals spend significantly less time with close companions and are less frequent beneficiaries of their mood-enhancing effects (Figs 3 and 4). Socially anxious individuals appear to have an intact capacity for social mood enhancement. Indeed, they show lower levels of NA in the company of close companions, in broad accord with work focused on depressed samples (Rottenberg, Reference Rottenberg2017). This perspective is also well aligned with evidence from prospective longitudinal studies indicating that close friendships and other kinds of social support and intimacy reduce the risk of developing anxiety symptoms in adolescence and early adulthood (Teachman and Allen, Reference Teachman and Allen2007; Rodebaugh, Reference Rodebaugh2009; Tillfors et al., Reference Tillfors, Persson, Willen and Burk2012; Frenkel et al., Reference Frenkel, Fox, Pine, Walker, Degnan and Chronis-Tuscano2015; Narr et al., Reference Narr, Allen, Tan and Loeb2019). Likewise, among patients undergoing treatment for social anxiety, higher levels of perceived social support are associated with a more favorable prognosis (Rapee et al., Reference Rapee, Peters, Carpenter and Gaston2015).

Naturally, our results do not license causal inferences. We cannot rule out the possibility that reduced access to confidants begets higher levels of social anxiety or, more likely, that these two constructs exert bi-directional effects (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Barstead, Smith, Bowker, Pérez-Edgar and Fox2018). Likewise, it could be that socially anxious individuals actively seek out the company of close companions when they are experiencing lower levels of NA. Nevertheless, randomized laboratory studies reinforce the conclusion that close companions play a key role in mitigating NA. For example, the presence of a close companion has been shown to normalize negative affect and catastrophic cognitions (‘I'm going to die’) in anxiety patients exposed to a panic-inducing CO2 challenge (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Hollon, Carson and Shelton1995) and to normalize behavioral signs of anxiety in socially anxious young adults during a videotaped speech challenge (Pontari, Reference Pontari2009). Taken with the present results, these observations motivate the hypothesis that friends, romantic partners, and family members serve as a regulatory ‘prosthesis’ for socially anxious individuals.

Relying on close companions is risky. This is particularly true for socially anxious individuals, who tend to experience elevated levels of interpersonal conflict (Cuming and Rapee, Reference Cuming and Rapee2010) and, among patients, marked impairment of interpersonal relationships (Wittchen et al., Reference Wittchen, Fuetsch, Sonntag, Müller and Liebowitz2000; Rapaport et al., Reference Rapaport, Clary, Fayyad and Endicott2005; Olatunji et al., Reference Olatunji, Cisler and Tolin2007; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi, Hu, Karam, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Wojtyniak, Xavier, Kessler and Scott2017). Relationship distress and dissolution reduces or eliminates the possibility of interpersonal emotion regulation and, ultimately, can contribute to the development, maintenance, and recurrence of psychopathology (Rehman et al., Reference Rehman, Gollan and Mortimer2008; Marroquin, Reference Marroquin2011; Whisman and Baucom, Reference Whisman and Baucom2012; Baucom et al., Reference Baucom, Belus, Adelman, Fischer and Paprocki2014). Even in the absence of relationship problems, as young adults transition to full-time employment, marriage, and parenting, social network size naturally begins to decline and more time is spent with distant companions or alone (Larson, Reference Larson1990; Lansford et al., Reference Lansford, Sherman and Antonucci1998; Wrzus et al., Reference Wrzus, Hanel, Wagner and Neyer2013, Reference Wrzus, Wagner and Riediger2016; Sander et al., Reference Sander, Schupp and Richter2017) – effects that may be amplified in more recent cohorts, which tend to allocate less time to face-to-face social interaction and experience elevated levels of loneliness (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Spitzberg and Campbell2019). Many middle-aged and older adults report that they have no confidant (McPherson and Smith-Lovin, Reference McPherson and Smith-Lovin2006), depriving them of the emotional benefits of close companions. This is likely to be exacerbated among individuals with elevated levels of social anxiety, who are less likely to have close friends and more likely to be unmarried by mid-life (Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Haemmerlie and Edwards1991; Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Hughes, George and Blazer1994; La Greca and Lopez, Reference La Greca and Lopez1998). Extending the present approach to earlier and later developmental periods is an important challenge for future research, and prospective-longitudinal studies are likely to be especially fruitful.

Social anxiety is often cast as an increased sensitivity to scrutiny from others, especially unfamiliar others, which manifests as heightened avoidance, fear (‘phobia’), and anxiety (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Our results underscore the need to broaden this perspective. As indexed by EMA, social anxiety was unrelated to the amount of time spent with distant companions. Moreover, socially anxious individuals did not report heightened NA when they were in the presence of distant companions (Table 1 and Fig. 4). This finding suggests that socially anxious individuals tend to experience normative emotional responses to distant companions in the absence of clear signs of rejection, scrutiny, or threat. Another possibility is that hyper-reactivity to strangers is specific to pathological levels of social anxiety or is only evident in a subset of socially anxious individuals. Adjudicating between these accounts represents another important avenue for future research.

From a clinical perspective, these observations suggest that naturally occurring social relationships are a potentially important target for intervention. Existing treatments for social anxiety typically focus on the individual, but our results highlight the value of simultaneously considering the role of close companions and developing supplementary interventions to enhance social connection, acceptance, and support. This could take the form of nurturing social-cognitive skills (e.g. emotional disclosure), cultivating stronger and more frequent ties with existing companions and social networks (e.g. via behavioral activation approaches), or reducing maladaptive thoughts and behaviors (e.g. neediness, overreliance) that promote conflict and rejection (Masi et al., Reference Masi, Chen, Hawkley and Cacioppo2011; Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Grippo, London, Goossens and Cacioppo2015; Kok and Singer, Reference Kok and Singer2017). The development of smartphone-based interventions would provide a scalable and cost-effective alternative to more traditional modalities. Already, 77% of U.S. adults, and 94% of U.S. adults under the age of 30 own a smartphone (Pew Research Center, 2018). Mobile applications may be particularly useful for individuals who are unable or unwilling to use traditional care delivery systems and for subclinical presentations of social anxiety that do not warrant resource-intensive treatments (Ruscio, Reference Ruscio2019). Regardless of delivery method, intervention research would also provide a crucial opportunity for testing the causal contribution of close companions to the everyday experience of social anxiety.

Our results highlight some additional avenues for future research. To understand the generalizability of our inferences, it will be useful to extend the present approach to larger and more representative samples and to populations with more severe symptoms, distress, and impairment. Future EMA studies may benefit from using larger sampling windows or selectively targeting periods of increased stress or disrupted social intimacy (e.g. transition from high school or university, or from university to full-time work) in order to capture a wider range of social interactions and their association with momentary affect. It will also be helpful to examine the nature and quality of naturalistic social interactions – including momentary perceptions of social connection, emotional support, and conflict – in more detail using either EMA (e.g. context- or event-triggered) or behavioral observations. Developing a clearer understanding of the processes that promote heightened levels of NA during periods of solitude – when both social support and social threat are absent (Fig. 4) – is also likely to be useful (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Tromp, Stockbridge, Kaplan, Tillman and Fox2016).

In conclusion, the present study suggests that close companions play an important role in the momentary experience of socially anxious young adults. The use of well-established techniques for intensive EMA and a relatively large sample selectively recruited from a pool of more than 6000 young adults increases our confidence in the reproducibility and translational relevance of these findings.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002022.

Data

Raw data have been or will be made available via the National Institute of Mental Health's RDoC Database (https://data-archive.nimh.nih.gov/rdocdb).

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the assistance of C. Garbin, L. Friedman, R. Tillman, and members of the Affective and Translational Neuroscience laboratory and constructive feedback from four anonymous reviewers and K. Rubin. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (DA040717, MH107444) and University of Maryland. Authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Author contributions

A.J.S. and K.A.D. designed the study. K.A.D., A.S.A., S.I. collected data. K.A.D., S.I., J.H. processed data. J.H., A.J.S., M.G.B., and K.A.D. designed the analytic strategy. J.H., M.G.B., and A.J.S. performed analyses. J.H. drafted the paper. J.H., M.G.B., and A.J.S. created figures and tables. A.J.S. supervised and funded the work. All authors edited the paper and approved the final version.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

Contributed equally

The notes appear after the main text.

1 The mean and dispersion of the present sample is similar to that of unselected individuals drawn from the same university population. For example, exploratory analyses of data collected as part of the University of Maryland Department of Psychology's on-line survey during the 2015–2018 academic years (N = 1596) revealed that among the subset of respondents 18–19 years old, women reported significantly greater social anxiety (N = 601, M = 46.8, s.d. = 23.2) than men (N = 229, M = 40.3, s.d. = 22.7), t = 3.67, p = 0.001. When the on-line survey data were adjusted to reflect the percentage of women in the EMA study (51.3%), the resulting distribution (M = 43.6, Range = 2–122) was similar to the present EMA sample (M = 41.7, Range = 1–121).

2 For descriptive purposes, depression was assessed using the General Depression subscale of the revised Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms (IDAS-II) Watson et al. (Reference Watson, O'Hara, Naragon-Gainey, Koffel, Chmielewski, Kotov, Stasik and Ruggero2012). As expected, levels of depression were somewhat elevated in the present sample (M = 39.9, s.d. = 12.8), which corresponds to the 60th percentile in U.S. normative data; Nelson et al. (Reference Nelson, O'Hara and Watson2018).

3 Similar results were obtained using the log-transformed NA scores as a DV (not reported).

4 The zero-order correlation between self-reported social network size and the proportion of momentary assessments completed in the presence of close companions was r = 0.29, p < 0.001.

5 Although the complementary pattern (elevated social anxiety → fewer confidants → greater solitude) was evident for a model focused on the amount of time spent alone, we refrain from reporting or interpreting it, given the strong dependency between time allocated to close companions vs. solitude. That is, social contexts were mutually exclusive (Fig 2), and most assessments were completed either in the presence of close companions or alone. From this perspective, the results of the ‘alone’ model are almost entirely predictable knowing the results of the ‘close companions’ model.

6 Momentary NA and PA were negatively correlated within momentary assessments (t = −18.7, b = −0.26, s.e. = 0.014, p < 0.001).

7 It also remained significant when controlling for variation in depressive symptoms, indexed using the General Depression subscale of the IDAS-II (Social Anxiety × Close-Alone, t = −2.28, b = −0.03, s.e. = 0.012, p = 0.02).

References

Acarturk, C, Cuijpers, P, Van Straten, A and De Graaf, R (2009) Psychological treatment of social anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 39, 241254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Afram, A and Kashdan, TB (2015) Coping with rejection concerns in romantic relationships: an experimental investigation of social anxiety and risk regulation. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 4, 151156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alden, LE and Taylor, CT (2004) Interpersonal processes in social phobia. Clinical Psychology Review 24, 857882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alden, LE and Wallace, ST (1995) Social phobia and social appraisal in successful and unsuccessful social interactions. Behaviour Research and Therapy 33, 497505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, LB and Abramson, LY (1999) The Temple-Wisconsin Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression Project: conceptual background, design, and methods. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 13, 227262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloy, LB, Abramson, LY, Hogan, M E, Whitehouse, WG, Rose, DT, Robinson, MS, Kim, RS and Lapkin, JB (2000) The Temple-Wisconsin Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression Project: Lifetime history of Axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 403418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American College Health Association (2016) American College Health Association-National College Health Assessment II: Reference Group Executive Summary Fall 2015. Hanover, MD: American College Health Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Anderson, ER and Hope, DA (2008) A review of the tripartite model for understanding the link between anxiety and depression in youth. Clinical Psychology Review 28, 275287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnett, JJ (2000) Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist 55, 469480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, RP, Alonso, J, Axinn, WG, Cuijpers, P, Ebert, DD, Green, JG, Hwang, I, Kessler, RC, Liu, H, Mortier, P, Nock, MK, Pinder-Amaker, S, Sampson, NA, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Al-Hamzawi, A, Andrade, LH, Benjet, C, Caldas-de-Almeida, JM, Demyttenaere, K, Florescu, S, de Girolamo, G, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Karam, EG, Kiejna, A, Kovess-Masfety, V, Lee, S, McGrath, JJ, O'Neill, S, Pennell, BE, Scott, K, Ten Have, M, Torres, Y, Zaslavsky, AM, Zarkov, Z and Bruffaerts, R (2016) Mental disorders among college students in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine 46, 29552970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, RP, Mortier, P, Bruffaerts, R, Alonso, J, Benjet, C, Cuijpers, P, Demyttenaere, K, Ebert, DD, Green, JG, Hasking, P, Murray, E, Nock, MK, Pinder-Amaker, S, Sampson, NA, Stein, DJ, Vilagut, G, Zaslavsky, AM, Kessler, RC and Collaborators, WW-I (in press) WHO world mental health surveys international college student project: prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 127, 623638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, LF (1997) The relationships among momentary emotion experiences, personality descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23, 11001110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batelaan, NM, Bosman, RC, Muntingh, A, Scholten, WD, Huijbregts, KM and van Balkom, A (2017) Risk of relapse after antidepressant discontinuation in anxiety disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis of relapse prevention trials. BMJ 358, j3927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baucom, DH, Belus, JM, Adelman, CB, Fischer, MS and Paprocki, C (2014) Couple-based interventions for psychopathology: a renewed direction for the field. Family Process 53, 445461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, JG, Davila, J, Farrow, S and Grant, D (2006) When the heat is on: romantic partner responses influence distress in socially anxious women. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44, 737748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beidel, DC, Turner, SM and Morris, TL (1999) Psychopathology of childhood social phobia. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 38, 643650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, DS and Hansen, JS (1996) Positive affect, negative affect, and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71, 796809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolger, N and Eckenrode, J (1991) Social relationships, personality, and anxiety during a major stressful event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61, 440449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boukhechba, M, Chow, P, Fua, K, Teachman, BA and Barnes, LE (2018) Predicting social anxiety from global positioning system traces of college students: feasibility study. JMIR Mental Health 5, e10101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, SE, Yonkers, KA, Otto, MW, Eisen, JL, Weisberg, RB, Pagano, M, Shea, MT and Keller, MB (2005) Influence of psychiatric comorbidity on recovery and recurrence in generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, and panic disorder: a 12-year prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 11791187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullock, JG, Green, DP and Ha, SE (2010) Yes, but what's the mechanism? (don't expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 98, 550558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buote, VM, Pancer, SM, Pratt, MW, Adams, G, Birnie-Lefcovitch, S, Polivy, J and Wintre, MG (2007) The importance of friends. Friendship and adjustment among 1st-year university students. Journal of Adolescent Research 22, 665689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, S, Grippo, AJ, London, S, Goossens, L and Cacioppo, JT (2015) Loneliness: clinical import and interventions. Perspectives on Psychological Science 10, 238249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, MM, Hollon, SD, Carson, RE and Shelton, RC (1995) Effects of a safe person on induced distress following a biological challenge in panic disorder with agoraphobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104, 156163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chow, PI, Fua, K, Huang, Y, Bonelli, W, Xiong, H, Barnes, LE and Teachman, BA (2017) Using mobile sensing to test clinical models of depression, social anxiety, state affect, and social isolation among college students. Journal of Medical Internet Research 19, e62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coan, JA and Sbarra, DA (2015) Social baseline theory: the social regulation of risk and effort. Current Opinion in Psychology 1, 8791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coles, ME, Turk, CL and Heimberg, RG (2002) The role of memory perspective in social phobia: immediate and delayed memories for role-played situations. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 30, 415425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, CC, Forbes, MK, Forbush, KT, Fried, EI, Hallquist, MN, Kotov, R, Mullins-Sweatt, SN, Shackman, AJ, Skodol, AE, South, SC, Sunderland, M, Waszczuk, MA, Zald, DH, Afzali, MH, Bornovalova, MA, Carragher, N, Docherty, AR, Jonas, KG, Krueger, RF, Patalay, P, Pincus, AL, Tackett, JL, Reininghaus, U, Waldman, ID, Wright, AGC, Zimmerman, J, Bach, B, Bagby, RM, Chmielewski, M, Cicero, DC, Clark, LA, Dalgleish, T, DeYoung, CG, Hopwood, CJ, Ivanova, MY, Latzman, RD, Patrick, CJ, Ruggero, CJ, Samuel, DB, Watson, D and Eaton, NR (2019) A hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology can reform mental health research. Perspectives on Psychological Science 14, 419436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cottrell, NB and Epley, SW (1977) Affiliation, social comparison, and socially mediated stress reduction. In Suls, JM and Miller, RL (eds), Social Comparison Processes: Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives. Washington, DC: Hemisphere, pp. 4368.Google Scholar
Craske, MG, Stein, MB, Eley, TC, Milad, MR, Holmes, A, Rapee, RM and Wittchen, H-U (2017) Anxiety disorders. Nature Reviews Disease Primers 3, 118.Google ScholarPubMed
Creed, A and Funder, D (1998) Social anxiety: from the inside and outside. Personality and Individual Differences 25, 1933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crişan, LG, Vulturar, R, Miclea, M and Miu, AC (2016) Reactivity to social stress in subclinical social anxiety: emotional experience, cognitive appraisals, behavior, and physiology. Frontiers in Psychiatry 7, 5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M, Mehl, MR and Conner, TS (2013) Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life. New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Cuming, S and Rapee, RM (2010) Social anxiety and self-protective communication style in close relationships. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darcy, K, Davila, J and Beck, JG (2005) Is social anxiety associated with both interpersonal avoidance and interpersonal dependence? Cognitive Therapy and Research 29, 171186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, JR, Hughes, DC, George, LK and Blazer, DG (1994) The boundary of social phobia: exploring the threshold. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 975983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davila, J and Beck, JG (2002) Is social anxiety associated with impairment in close relationships? A preliminary investigation. Behavior Therapy 33, 427446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E and Seligman, ME (2002) Very happy people. Psychological Science 13, 8184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E, Seligman, MEP, Choi, H and Oishi, S (2018) Happiest people revisited. Perspectives on Psychological Science 13, 176184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, AS and Shackman, AJ (2018) How are emotions embodied in the social world. In Fox, AS, Lapate, RC, Shackman, AJ and Davidson, RJ (eds), The Nature of Emotion. Fundamental Questions, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 237239.Google Scholar
Freberg, K, Adams, R, McGaughey, K and Freberg, L (2010) The rich get richer: online and offline social connectivity predicts subjective loneliness. Media Psychology Review 3, 103115.Google Scholar
Frenkel, TI, Fox, NA, Pine, DS, Walker, OL, Degnan, KA and Chronis-Tuscano, A (2015) Early childhood behavioral inhibition, adult psychopathology and the buffering effects of adolescent social networks: a twenty-year prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56, 10651073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geyer, EC, Fua, KC, Daniel, KE, Chow, PI, Bonelli, W, Huang, Y, Barnes, LE and Teachman, BA (2018) I did OK, but did I like it? Using ecological momentary assessment to examine perceptions of social interactions associated with severity of social anxiety and depression. Behavior Therapy 49, 866880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Global Burden of Disease Collaborators (2016) Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 diseases and injuries, 1990–2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. The Lancet 388, 15451602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, JA and Redish, AD (2016) On the cusp. Current challenges and promises in psychiatry. In Redish, AD and Gordon, JA (eds), Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 314.Google Scholar
Granovetter, MS (1973) The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology 78, 13601380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DP, Ha, SE and Bullock, JG (2010) Enough already about “black box” experiments: studying mediation is more difficult than most scholars suppose. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 628, 200208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, AF (2017) Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hays, RB and Oxley, D (1986) Social network development and functioning during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50, 305313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heerey, EA and Kring, AM (2007) Interpersonal consequences of social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 125134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heimberg, RG, Hofmann, SG, Liebowitz, MR, Schneier, FR, Smits, JA, Stein, MB, Hinton, DE and Craske, MG (2014) Social anxiety disorder in DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety 31, 472479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoaglin, DC and Iglewicz, B (1987) Fine-tuning some resistant rules for outlier labeling. Journal of the American Statistical Association 82, 11471149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoaglin, DC, Iglewicz, B and Tukey, JW (1986) Performance of some resistant rules for outlier labelling. Journal of the American Statistical Association 81, 991999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W and Patel, PV (2015) Surveysignal: a convenient solution for experience sampling research using participants’ own smartphones. Social Science Computer Review 33, 235253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hur, J, Stockbridge, MD, Fox, AS and Shackman, AJ (2019) Dispositional negativity, cognition, and anxiety disorders: an integrative translational neuroscience framework. Progress in Brain Research 247, 375436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB (2004) The neglected relationship between social interaction anxiety and hedonic deficits: differentiation from depressive symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 18, 719730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB and Collins, RL (2010) Social anxiety and the experience of positive emotion and anger in everyday life: an ecological momentary assessment approach. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 23, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, TB and Roberts, JE (2004) Social anxiety's impact on affect, curiosity, and social self-efficacy during a high self-focus social threat situation. Cognitive Therapy and Research 28, 119141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, TB and Roberts, JE (2006) Affective outcomes in superficial and intimate interactions: roles of social anxiety and curiosity. Journal of Research in Personality 40, 140167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, TB and Roberts, JE (2007) Social anxiety, depressive symptoms, and post-event rumination: affective consequences and social contextual influences. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21, 284301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB and Steger, MF (2006) Expanding the topography of social anxiety: an experience-sampling assessment of positive emotions, positive events, and emotion suppression. Psychological Science 17, 120128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB, Weeks, JW and Savostyanova, AA (2011) Whether, how, and when social anxiety shapes positive experiences and events: a self-regulatory framework and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 786799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB, Farmer, AS, Adams, LM, Ferssizidis, P, McKnight, PE and Nezlek, JB (2013a) Distinguishing healthy adults from people with social anxiety disorder: evidence for the value of experiential avoidance and positive emotions in everyday social interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 645655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, TB, Ferssizidis, P, Farmer, AS, Adams, LM and McKnight, PE (2013b) Failure to capitalize on sharing good news with romantic partners: exploring positivity deficits of socially anxious people with self-reports, partner-reports, and behavioral observations. Behaviour Research and Therapy 51, 656668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katzelnick, DJ, Kobak, KA, DeLeire, T, Henk, HJ, Greist, JH, Davidson, JR, Schneier, FR, Stein, MB and Helstad, CP (2001) Impact of generalized social anxiety disorder in managed care. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 19992007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS and Gardner, CO (2014) Sex differences in the pathways to major depression: a study of opposite-sex twin pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 426435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Myers, J and Prescott, CA (2005) Sex differences in the relationship between social support and risk for major depression: a longitudinal study of opposite-sex twin pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 250256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC (2003) The impairments caused by social phobia in the general population: implications for intervention. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 108, 1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kocalevent, RD, Berg, L, Beutel, ME, Hinz, A, Zenger, M, Harter, M, Nater, U and Brahler, E (2018) Social support in the general population: standardization of the Oslo social support scale (OSSS-3). BMC Psychology 6, 31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kok, BE and Singer, T (2017) Effects of contemplative dyads on engagement and perceived social connectedness over 9 months of mental training: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 74, 126134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, RF, Kotov, R, Watson, D, Forbes, MK, Eaton, NR, Ruggero, CJ, Simms, LJ, Widiger, TA, Achenbach, TM, Bach, B, Bagby, RM, Bornovalova, MA, Carpenter, WT, Chmielewski, M, Cicero, D, Clark, LA, Conway, C, DeClercq, B, DeYoung, CG, Docherty, AR, Drislane, LE, First, MB, Forbush, KT, Hallquist, M, Haltigan, JD, Hopwood, CJ, Ivanova, MY, Jonas, KG, Latzman, RD, Markon, KE, Miller, JD, Morey, LC, Mullins-Sweatt, SN, Ormel, J, Patalay, P, Patrick, CJ, Pincus, AL, Regier, DA, Reininghaus, U, Rescorla, LA, Samuel, DB, Sellbom, M, Shackman, AJ, Skodol, A, Slade, T, South, SC, Sunderland, M, Tackett, JL, Venables, NC, Waldman, ID, Waszczuk, MA, Waugh, MH, Wright, AGC, Zald, DH and Zimmerman, J (2018) Progress in achieving empirical classification of psychopathology. World Psychiatry 17, 282293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladd, GW, Kochenderfer-Ladd, B, Eggum, ND, Kochel, KP and McConnell, EM (2011) Characterizing and comparing the friendships of anxious-solitary and unsociable preadolescents. Child Development 82, 14341453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
La Greca, AM and Lopez, N (1998) Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 26, 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamers, F, Swendsen, J, Cui, L, Husky, M, Johns, J, Zipunnikov, V and Merikangas, KR (2018) Mood reactivity and affective dynamics in mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 127, 659669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansford, JE, Sherman, AM and Antonucci, TC (1998) Satisfaction with social networks: an examination of socioemotional selectivity theory across cohorts. Psychology and Aging 13, 544552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, RW (1990) The solitary side of life: an examination of the time people spend alone from childhood to old age. Developmental Review 10, 155183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lay, JC, Gerstorf, D, Scott, SB, Pauly, T and Hoppmann, CA (2017) Neuroticism and extraversion magnify discrepancies between retrospective and concurrent affect reports. Journal of Personality 85, 817829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebowitz, MR (1987) Social phobia. Modern Problems in Pharmacopsychiatry 22, 141173.Google Scholar
Lim, MH, Rodebaugh, TL, Zyphur, MJ and Gleeson, JF (2016) Loneliness over time: the crucial role of social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 125, 620630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipsitz, JD and Schneier, FR (2000) Social phobia. Epidemiology and cost of illness. Pharmacoeconomics 18, 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipson, SK, Lattie, EG and Eisenberg, D (2018) Increased rates of mental health service utilization by U.S. college students: 10-year population-level trends (2007–2017). Psychiatric Services 70, 6063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maresh, EL, Beckes, L and Coan, JA (2013) The social regulation of threat-related attentional disengagement in highly anxious individuals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markovic, A and Bowker, JC (2017) Friends also matter: examining friendship adjustment indices as moderators of anxious-withdrawal and trajectories of change in psychological maladjustment. Developmental Psychology 53, 14621473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marroquin, B (2011) Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in depression. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 12761290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masi, CM, Chen, HY, Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2011) A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15, 219266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathew, AR, Pettit, JW, Lewinsohn, PM, Seeley, JR and Roberts, RE (2011) Co-morbidity between major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders: shared etiology or direct causation? Psychological Medicine 41, 20232034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPherson, M and Smith-Lovin, L (2006) Social isolation in America: changes in core discussion networks over two decades. American Sociological Review 71, 353375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meleshko, KG and Alden, LE (1993) Anxiety and self-disclosure: toward a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64, 10001009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikangas, KR, Avenevoli, S, Acharyya, S, Zhang, H and Angst, J (2002) The spectrum of social phobia in the Zurich cohort study of young adults. Biological Psychiatry 51, 8191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, RL, Haemmerlie, FM and Edwards, M (1991) Social, personal, and interpersonal deficits in socially anxious people. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 6, 859872.Google Scholar
Morgan, JK, Lee, GE, Wright, AG, Gilchrist, DE, Forbes, EE, McMakin, DL, Dahl, RE, Ladouceur, CD, Ryan, ND and Silk, JS (2017) Altered positive affect in clinically anxious youth: the role of social context and anxiety subtype. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 45, 14611472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, DG (1999) Close relationships and quality of life. In Kahneman, D, Diener, E and Schwartz, N (eds), Well-being. New York: Sage, pp. 374391.Google Scholar
Narr, RK, Allen, JP, Tan, JS and Loeb, EL (2019) Close friendship strength and broader peer group desirability as differential predictors of adult mental health. Child Development 90, 298313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, GH, O'Hara, MW and Watson, D (2018) National norms for the expanded version of the inventory of depression and anxiety symptoms (IDAS-II). Journal of Clinical Psychology 74, 953968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K, von Auer, M, Murray, E, Petermann, F, Helbig-Lang, S and Gerlach, AL (2013) Internet-delivered attention modification training as a treatment for social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy 51, 8797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oishi, S, Diener, E and Lucas, RE (2007) The optimum level of well-being: can people be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science 2, 346360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, BO, Cisler, JM and Tolin, DF (2007) Quality of life in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review 27, 572581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmier-Claus, JE, Myin-Germeys, I, Barkus, E, Bentley, L, Udachina, A, Delespaul, P, Lewis, SW and Dunn, G (2011) Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 123, 1220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pancer, SM, Hunsberger, B, Pratt, MW and Alisat, S (2000) Cognitive complexity of expectations and adjustment to university in the first year. Journal of Adolescent Research 15, 3857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center (2018) Mobile fact sheet. Available at http://www.pewinternet.org/fact-sheet/mobile/.Google Scholar
Phillips, ND (2017) Yarrr! The pirate's guide to R. APS Observer 30, 2223.Google Scholar
Pontari, BA (2009) Appearing socially competent: the effects of a friend's presence on the socially anxious. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35, 283294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, MA and Gentzler, AL (2015) An upward spiral: bidirectional associations between positive affect and positive aspects of close relationships across the life span. Developmental Review 36, 58104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapaport, MH, Clary, C, Fayyad, R and Endicott, J (2005) Quality-of-life impairment in depressive and anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 11711178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, RM and Spence, SH (2004) The etiology of social phobia: empirical evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology Review 24, 737767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapee, RM, Peters, L, Carpenter, L and Gaston, JE (2015) The Yin and Yang of support from significant others: influence of general social support and partner support of avoidance in the context of treatment for social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 69, 4047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudenbush, SW and Bryk, AS (2002) Hierarchical Linear Models. Applications and Data Analysis Methods, 2nd Edn. Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Reeck, C, Ames, DR and Ochsner, KN (2016) The social regulation of emotion: an integrative, cross-disciplinary model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20, 4763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehman, US, Gollan, J and Mortimer, AR (2008) The marital context of depression: research, limitations, and new directions. Clinical Psychology Review 28, 179198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhebergen, D, Batelaan, NM, de Graaf, R, Nolen, WA, Spijker, J, Beekman, AT and Penninx, BW (2011) The 7-year course of depression and anxiety in the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 123, 297306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rime, B (2009) Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: theory and empirical review. Emotion Review 1, 6085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodebaugh, TL (2009) Social phobia and perceived friendship quality. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 872878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodebaugh, TL, Holaway, RM and Heimberg, RG (2004) The treatment of social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 24, 883908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodebaugh, TL, Lim, MH, Shumaker, EA, Levinson, CA and Thompson, T (2015) Social anxiety and friendship quality over time. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy 44, 502511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, AA, Updegraff, KA, Iida, M, Dishion, TJ, Doane, LD, Corbin, WC, Van Lenten, SA and Ha, T (2018) Trajectories of positive and negative affect across the transition to college: the role of daily interactions with parents and friends. Developmental Psychology 54, 21812192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rottenberg, J (2017) Emotions in depression: what do we really know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 13, 241263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, KH, Barstead, MG, Smith, KA and Bowker, JC (2018) Peer relations and the behaviorally inhibited child. In Pérez-Edgar, K and Fox, NA (eds), Behavioral Inhibition: Integrating Theory, Research, and Clinical Perspectives. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing, pp. 157184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruscio, AM (2019) Normal versus pathological mood: implications for diagnosis. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 15, 179205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, GS and Shaw, S (2009) A study to investigate the prevalence of social anxiety in a sample of higher education students in the United Kingdom. Journal of Mental Health 18, 198206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sander, J, Schupp, J and Richter, D (2017) Getting together: social contact frequency across the life span. Developmental Psychology 53, 15711588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarason, IG, Levine, HM, Basham, RB and Sarason, BR (1983) Assessing social support: the social support questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44, 127139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneier, FR, Johnson, J, Hornig, CD, Liebowitz, MR and Weissman, MM (1992) Social phobia: comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiologic sample. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 282288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholten, WD, Batelaan, NM, van Balkom, AJ, Wjh Penninx, B, Smit, JH and van Oppen, P (2013) Recurrence of anxiety disorders and its predictors. Journal of Affective Disorders 147, 180185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholten, WD, Batelaan, NM, Penninx, BW, van Balkom, AJ, Smit, JH, Schoevers, RA and van Oppen, P (2016) Diagnostic instability of recurrence and the impact on recurrence rates in depressive and anxiety disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders 195, 185190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönbrodt, FD and Perugini, M (2013) At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality 47, 609612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shackman, AJ, Tromp, DPM, Stockbridge, MD, Kaplan, CM, Tillman, RM and Fox, AS (2016) Dispositional negativity: an integrative psychological and neurobiological perspective. Psychological Bulletin 142, 12751314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, AJ, Weinstein, JS, Hudja, SN, Bloomer, CD, Barstead, MG, Fox, AS and Lemay, EP Jr. (2018) Dispositional negativity in the wild: social environment governs momentary emotional experience. Emotion 18, 707724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffman, S, Stone, AA and Hufford, MR (2008) Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 4, 132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparrevohn, RM and Rapee, RM (2009) Self-disclosure, emotional expression and intimacy within romantic relationships of people with social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 10741078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinhoven, P, Batelaan, N, Rhebergen, D, van Balkom, A, Schoevers, R and Penninx, BW (2016) Prediction of 6-yr symptom course trajectories of anxiety disorders by diagnostic, clinical and psychological variables. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 44, 92101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spoozak, L, Gotman, N, Smith, MV, Belanger, K and Yonkers, KA (2009) Evaluation of a social support measure that may indicate risk of depression during pregnancy. Journal of Affective Disorders 114, 216223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starr, LR and Davila, J (2008) Differentiating interpersonal correlates of depressive symptoms and social anxiety in adolescence: implications for models of comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 37, 337349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, MB and Kean, YM (2000) Disability and quality of life in social phobia: epidemiologic findings. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 16061613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, DJ, Lim, CCW, Roest, AM, de Jonge, P, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Al-Hamzawi, A, Alonso, J, Benjet, C, Bromet, EJ, Bruffaerts, R, de Girolamo, G, Florescu, S, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Harris, MG, He, Y, Hinkov, H, Horiguchi, I, Hu, C, Karam, A, Karam, EG, Lee, S, Lepine, JP, Navarro-Mateu, F, Pennell, BE, Piazza, M, Posada-Villa, J, Ten Have, M, Torres, Y, Viana, MC, Wojtyniak, B, Xavier, M, Kessler, RC, Scott, KM and Collaborators, W. H. O. W. M. H. S. (2017) The cross-national epidemiology of social anxiety disorder: data from the World Mental Health Survey Initiative. BMC Medicine 15, 143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, DM, Shackman, AJ, Pedersen, WS, Miskovich, TA and Larson, CL (2017) Neural circuitry governing anxious individuals’ mis-allocation of working memory to threat. Scientific Reports 7, 87428752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strahan, EY and Conger, AJ (1999) Social anxiety and social performance: why don't we see more catastrophes? Journal of Anxiety Disorders 13, 399416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teachman, BA and Allen, JP (2007) Development of social anxiety: social interaction predictors of implicit and explicit fear of negative evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 35, 6378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tillfors, M, Persson, S, Willen, M and Burk, WJ (2012) Prospective links between social anxiety and adolescent peer relations. Journal of Adolescence 35, 12551263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgrud, LJ, Walker, JR, Murray, L, Cox, BJ, Chartier, M and Kjernisted, KD (2004) Deficits in perceived social support associated with generalized social phobia. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy 33, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, SM, Beidel, DC, Dancu, CV and Keys, DJ (1986) Psychopathology of social phobia and comparison to avoidant personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95, 389394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, JM, Spitzberg, BH and Campbell, WK (2019) Less in-person social interaction with peers among U.S. adolescents in the 21st century and links to loneliness. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 36, 18921913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lente, E, Barry, MM, Molcho, M, Morgan, K, Watson, D, Harrington, J and McGee, H (2012) Measuring population mental health and social well-being. International Journal of Public Health 57, 421430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernberg, EM, Abwender, DA, Ewell, KK and Beery, SH (1992) Social anxiety and peer relationships in early adolescence: a prospective analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 21, 189196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wade, TD and Kendler, KS (2000) The relationship between social support and major depression: cross-sectional, longitudinal, and genetic perspectives. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 188, 251258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H and Wellman, B (2010) Social connectivity in America: changes in adult friendship network size from 2002 to 2007. American Behavioral Scientist 53, 11481169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D, O'Hara, MW, Naragon-Gainey, K, Koffel, E, Chmielewski, M, Kotov, R, Stasik, SM and Ruggero, CJ (2012) Development and validation of new anxiety and bipolar symptom scales for an expanded version of the IDAS (the IDAS-II). Assessment 19, 399420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whisman, MA and Baucom, DH (2012) Intimate relationships and psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 15, 413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, WC, Morelli, SA, Ong, DC and Zaki, J (2018) Interpersonal emotion regulation: implications for affiliation, perceived support, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 115, 224254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H-U, Fuetsch, M, Sonntag, H, Müller, N and Liebowitz, M (2000) Disability and quality of life in pure and comorbid social phobia. Findings from a controlled study. European Psychiatry 15, 4658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, N, Sarver, DE and Beidel, DC (2012) Quality of life impairments among adults with social phobia: the impact of subtype. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 26, 5057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrzus, C, Hanel, M, Wagner, J and Neyer, FJ (2013) Social network changes and life events across the life span: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 139, 5380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrzus, C, Wagner, GG and Riediger, M (2016) Personality-situation transactions from adolescence to old age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 110, 782799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaider, TI, Heimberg, RG and Iida, M (2010) Anxiety disorders and intimate relationships: a study of daily processes in couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119, 163173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J and Williams, WC (2013) Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion 13, 803810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Social anxiety. Social anxiety was assessed at baseline using the self-report version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS). The two highest cases were not excluded because they are sensible – given the nature of the scale and the sample – and because they did not exert undue statistical leverage (Hoaglin et al., 1986; Hoaglin and Iglewicz, 1987). Exploratory analyses indicated that the exclusion of these cases did not meaningfully alter the results (not reported).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Percentage of usable momentary assessments acquired in the presence of close companions, distant companions, or alone. Figure depicts the data (jittered gray points; individual participants), density distribution (colored bean plots), Bayesian 95% highest density interval (HDI; white bands), and mean (black bars) for each social context. HDIs permit population-generalizable visual inferences about mean differences and were estimated using 1000 samples from a posterior Gaussian distribution.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. The negative association between social anxiety and the amount of time with close companions is significantly explained by reduced access to confidants. Figure depicts the corresponding mediation model. Path labels indicate standardized regression coefficients, with c′ indicating the coefficient while controlling for variation in the self-reported number of confidants. Socially anxious individuals report fewer confidants, and individuals with fewer confidants were, in turn, less likely to be in the presence of close companions at the time of momentary assessment.

Figure 3

Table 1. The impact of social anxiety and social context on momentary emotional experience

Figure 4

Fig. 4. The deleterious impact of social anxiety on momentary emotional experience depends on social context. Individuals with higher levels of social anxiety derive larger emotional benefits – larger decrements in negative affect (NA) – from close companions relative to being alone as shown in the upper panel. Hypothesis testing relied on a continuous measure of social anxiety. For illustrative purposes, predicted values derived from multilevel modeling are depicted for extreme levels (±1 s.d.) of social anxiety.

Supplementary material: File

Hur et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Hur et al. supplementary material(File)
File 37.3 KB