Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:37:48.432Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social anxiety disorder: looking back and moving forward

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2018

Matthew P. Hyett
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Curtin University, Kent Street, Bentley, Western Australia, 6021, Australia
Peter M. McEvoy*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Curtin University, Kent Street, Bentley, Western Australia, 6021, Australia Western Australian Department of Health, Centre for Clinical Interventions, 223 James Street, Northbridge, Western Australia, 6000, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Peter M. McEvoy, E-mail: peter.mcevoy@curtin.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Fifty years have passed since social anxiety disorder (SAD) was first differentiated from other phobias. In the years since research has largely aligned with the zeitgeist of categorical classificatory frameworks, and has spanned identifying causes, maintenance factors and innovative interventions. Despite significant advances in the field, the capacity to conceptualise SAD as an independent entity is limited given the heterogeneity and dimensionality of diagnostic criteria, high rates of comorbidity, and non-specificity of aetiological mechanisms, maintaining factors and approaches to treatment. The Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative was developed in an effort to overcome the inherent limitations posed by descriptive diagnostic systems – particularly in terms of reliability and validity – and in doing so seeks to facilitate research into underlying pathophysiological and behavioural mechanisms that cut across traditional diagnostic boundaries. The RDoC framework is furnished with a ‘matrix’, which in essence corresponds to a set of research principles that attempt to reconcile neuroscience and psychopathology. This review outlines a rationale for integrating SAD research with the RDoC approach, and offers examples of how future studies may wish to frame hypotheses and design experiments as the field moves towards classifying dimensions of psychopathology through a mechanistic understanding of underlying neurobiological and behavioural processes.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Overview

A half century has passed since social phobia entered the lexicon of psychiatry as an isolated disorder separable from specific phobias and agoraphobia, with Marks & Gelder (Reference Marks and Gelder1966) first reporting on such differentiation. Since then, the concept of social phobia transitioned from being a relatively neglected condition (Liebowitz et al. Reference Liebowitz, Gorman, Fyer and Klein1985) to being identified as a highly prevalent disorder across the globe (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005a; Stein et al. Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola and Al-Hamzawi2017) – and is now referred to as social anxiety disorder or SAD (APA, 2013). As with all psychiatric conditions, however, diagnosis is made on the basis of presenting signs and symptoms, and no objective tests are available to support clinical judgements. In an attempt to tackle inherent problems of diagnostic subjectivity in psychiatry, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) formed the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative, which aims to identify ‘new ways of classifying mental disorders based on dimensions of observable behaviour and neurobiological measures’ (NIMH, 2008). The RDoC seeks to organise research efforts across differing units of analysis for a range of domains/constructs, whilst acknowledging individual differences in environmental and developmental influences – and is by design a flexible framework insofar as it seeks to accommodate changes to its conceptualisation through integration of emerging knowledge. The RDoC has been heralded as welcome departure from existing diagnostic systems, which arguably lack validity and have hindered efforts in identifying causal mechanisms (Keshavan & Ongur, Reference Keshavan and Ongur2014), but has also been criticised on grounds that it has the potential to de-emphasise psychological, social and contextual factors (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2014). This review will briefly describe the history of SAD classification before discussing limitations of such categorical approaches. We then offer an approach for positioning SAD research within the emerging RDoC framework, highlighting the utility of leveraging specific research methods within this initiative towards developing novel classificatory systems.

The evolution of SAD

Historical descriptions of social anxiety – or related constructs (e.g. speech anxiety) – date back to Hippocrates, with Burton (Reference Burton1621) noting that Cicero ‘… trembled still at the beginning of his speech’ (p. 261). Both Dugas (Reference Dugas1898) and Hartenberg (Reference Hartenberg1901) wrote about such anxieties, with the latter characterising timideté as ‘a combination of fear, shame, and embarrassment felt in social situations, and which affected psychosocial competence through attacks (‘accès’) of fear’ (Berrios, Reference Berrios1996, p. 273). Janet's (Reference Janet1903) concept of ‘psychasthenia’ incorporated the features of social anxiety, and he is credited with coining the term ‘social phobia’ (Berrios, Reference Berrios1996). Whilst social anxiety symptoms were judged as part of the anxiety neuroses throughout the twentieth century (Berrios, Reference Berrios1996), the Japanese viewed the fear of facing other people (‘taijin kyofu-sho’) as separate from other phobias (Prince, Reference Prince, Ghadirian and Lehman1993). The first and second editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (APA, 1952, 1968) allowed diagnosis of phobic reactions/phobic neurosis, but these were not specific to social fears. Social phobia was formally recognised as a separate disorder in DSM-III (APA, 1980), but this definition was viewed as too narrow to adequately capture all individuals with the disorder. For instance, a DSM-III diagnosis of social phobia could not be made if avoidant personality disorder (APD) was also present (Heimberg et al. Reference Heimberg, Hofmann, Liebowitz, Schneier, Smits and Stein2014). This hierarchical rule was challenged (Liebowitz et al. Reference Liebowitz, Gorman, Fyer and Klein1985) prior to the release of DSM-III-R (APA, 1987), which subsequently included a ‘generalised social phobia’ category, to account for non-specific social fears. Social anxiety disorder became the primary naming convention in DSM-5 (APA, 2013). The International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) (WHO, 1992) classifies social phobias alongside phobic anxiety disorders. Common to both nosologies is fear and avoidance of situations (e.g. giving a speech, attending social events) in which individuals believe they will be scrutinised or humiliated. A ‘performance-only’ specifier can be applied in DSM-5, but the utility of this subtype remains unclear (D'Avanzato & Dalrymple, Reference D'Avanzato and Dalrymple2016) given most individuals with diagnosable SAD report fearing more than one situation (Stein et al. Reference Stein, Torgrud and Walker2000).

It is evident that the desire for meaningful classification continues in psychiatry, particularly given its utility in guiding treatment selection, or indeed whether to treat at all (Craddock & Mynors-Wallis, Reference Craddock and Mynors-Wallis2014). However, there are significant limitations to pursuing SAD as a categorical entity on the basis of phenomenology alone. There is mounting evidence that (i) diagnostic criteria for SAD are polythetic, contributing to diagnostic heterogeneity, (ii) its core features exhibit significant dimensionality (i.e. symptoms are also present, albeit to varying degrees, in non-clinical individuals), (iii) there are high rates of comorbidity in SAD (which is likely related to high correlations between phenotypes), and (iv) whilst aetiological, maintenance and treatment models have been proposed as specific to SAD, they can often be applied across diagnoses (i.e. exhibit transdiagnosticity).

Limitations of the categorical approach to SAD

Polythetic categorical diagnostic frameworks and within-category heterogeneity

The DSM and ICD frameworks are polythetic, referring to the fact that diagnostic categories are defined by multiple symptoms, and not all symptoms are required for a diagnosis. Accordingly, different combinations of symptoms can lead to the same diagnosis (Krueger & Bezdjian, Reference Krueger and Bezdjian2009). Whilst all symptoms are required for a SAD diagnosis in DSM-5 (APA, 2013), within-item criteria are polythetic. Criterion A for SAD in DSM-5 requires marked fear or anxiety about one or more social situations, which can vary widely in context. Criterion B relates to fear of negative evaluation, yet the core fear can also vary across individuals. As an example, one individual may have significant fear of being observed eating, believing it will lead to negative evaluation and embarrassment, whilst another may fear conversing in groups as it may lead to rejection, yet both may be diagnosed with SAD (assuming other criteria are also met). Hence, whilst SAD is positioned in DSM (and ICD) as a distinct categorical entity, there is clear heterogeneity in symptom presentations across individuals. As described above, a common distinction historically has been between ‘generalised’ and ‘non-generalised’ SAD. The only subtype specifier in DSM-5 (APA, 2013), namely performance fear, is said to be qualitatively distinct from generalised SAD given its later age-of-onset, less shyness and behavioural inhibition, no familial relationship, and stronger psychophysiological responses (Bogels et al. Reference Bögels, Alden, Beidel, Clark, Pine and Stein2010). Yet the core fear – of (negative or positive) evaluation – is observed across subtypes (Bogels et al. Reference Bögels, Alden, Beidel, Clark, Pine and Stein2010; Weeks et al. Reference Weeks, Norton and Heimberg2009), suggesting that similar cognitive processes may be operating.

The heterogeneity of SAD is further apparent when considering closely related constructs. For instance, distinguishing ‘shyness’ from SAD can be challenging given shyness is frequently an antecedent and central feature of the clinical syndrome (Bruch, Reference Bruch1989). Whilst SAD and shyness have been differentiated in terms of symptoms experienced, functional impairment and quality of life (Heiser et al. Reference Heiser, Turner, Beidel and Roberson-Nay2009), others emphasise that they exist on a continuum (Chavira & Stein, Reference Chavira and Stein1999), with SAD being a form of extreme shyness (Stein, Reference Stein1999). Of course not all shy individuals meet criteria for SAD, and hence this trait may more be a vulnerability factor for the disorder and indeed other forms of psychopathology (Heiser et al. Reference Heiser, Turner and Beidel2003). Thus, whilst shyness can exist independently of SAD, is also closely intertwined with the phenomenology of the disorder. There is also substantial diagnostic overlap between SAD and APD (APA, 2013). Turner et al. (Reference Turner, Beidel, Borden, Stanley and Jacob1991) found that 75% of those with SAD also met criteria for APD. Despite their similarities, Turner et al. (Reference Turner, Beidel and Townsley1992) demonstrated that those with APD and generalised SAD had more severe social anxiety and impaired social functioning compared with those with ‘pure’ generalised SAD, consistent with the view that APD may be a more severe expression of SAD (Herbert et al. Reference Herbert, Hope and Bellack1992). Differentiating shyness from SAD, or SAD from APD, thus often creates a diagnostic dilemma, namely in how to categorise SAD from ‘normative’ but perhaps inhibited levels of social functioning (shyness), and more pervasive and ‘severe’ expressions of social avoidance and interpersonal sensitivity to negative evaluation.

Dimensionality of core features

Social anxiety has also been conceptualised within a dimensional framework (Merikangas et al. Reference Merikangas, Avenevoli, Acharyya, Zhang and Angst2002; Ruscio, Reference Ruscio2010), which acknowledges the importance of differences amongst those who fall at either side of some – often arbitrarily set – categorical diagnostic threshold (Helzer et al. Reference Helzer, Kraemer and Krueger2006). Weeks et al. (Reference Weeks, Norton and Heimberg2009) revealed that the core cognitive features of SAD – fear of negative and positive evaluation – more reflect a dimensional, rather than taxonic, latent structure. That is, these features are not specific to SAD and also manifest in non-clinical individuals. Moreover, such symptoms are suggested to be underpinned by low approach and high avoidance temperamental variables (Rodebaugh et al. Reference Rodebaugh, Levinson, Langer, Weeks, Heimberg and Brown2017), which themselves exhibit dimensionality (Roth & Cohen, Reference Roth and Cohen1986). Huppert et al. (Reference Huppert, Foa, Furr, Filip and Mathews2003) also revealed that negative social interpretation biases are positively correlated with social anxiety symptoms, suggesting that biased information processing may also be dimensional in nature. A caveat of such studies is that they were conducted with non-clinical samples, limiting generalisation to clinical populations. However, Ruscio (Reference Ruscio2010) indicated that DSM-IV diagnostic features of SAD appear to exist on a continuum with less severe social anxiety symptoms, and thus shifting from categorical to dimensional classification models may improve the predictive validity of the diagnosis. Conversely, Weeks et al. (Reference Weeks, Carleton, Asmundson, McCabe and Antony2010) identified a SAD taxon using symptomatic indicators in a community sample, providing support for the utility of categorical diagnosis. Given the contrasting findings, it is difficult to draw conclusions as to whether SAD is best represented dimensionally or categorically, or indeed a combination of both. In spite of the lack of consistency across studies, it has been suggested that the dimensional approach may afford greater utility over categorical models for refining understanding of the mechanisms of disorders such as SAD (Sanislow, Reference Sanislow2016).

The main function of the dimensionally defined behaviours and cognitions noted above is (perceived) harm avoidance, in line with Beck's original formulation (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Emery and Greenberg1985). Continued engagement with avoidance behaviours is believed to maintain threat appraisals in SAD (Clarke & Wells, Reference Clarke, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Bögels & Mansell, Reference Bögels and Mansell2004), but also panic disorder (Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1991), and generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) (Borkovec et al. Reference Borkovec, Alcaine, Behar, Heimberg, Mennin and Turk2004). Of importance, the type of threat is somewhat specific to differing disorders (e.g. attentional bias towards social cues in SAD v. physiological sensations in panic disorder – the caveat here being that biases seemingly isolated to a specific disorder may also occur in other conditions). Hence, a useful distinction for future research may be to investigate dimensional aspects of avoidance and threat appraisal across disorders, giving rise to transdiagnostic ‘functions’, whilst acknowledging the importance of the ‘form’ (content) of differing processes for specific disorders. This dimensional, transdiagnostic approach to SAD is particularly relevant given it is frequently comorbid with other disorders.

Comorbidity and correlated phenotypes

SAD is frequently comorbid with other anxiety disorders including GAD, panic disorder, agoraphobia, as well as major depression, dysthymia, and substance and alcohol abuse in both epidemiological (Schneier et al. Reference Schneier, Johnson, Hornig, Liebowitz and Weissman1992; Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas and Walters2005b) and treatment-seeking samples (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Campbell, Lehman, Grisham and Mancill2001). It has been argued, however, that ‘comorbidity’ is an artefact of categorical diagnostic nosologies that do not accurately account for symptom heterogeneity (Maj, Reference Maj2005). In line with this, it has been suggested that disorders such as SAD may be better conceptualised as part of a broader structure, where correlated features amongst disorders is emphasised (Eaton et al. Reference Eaton, South, Krueger, Millon, Krueger and Simonsen2010).

Latent variable modelling has been used to examine underlying structural relationships between differing conditions. Such analyses indicate that many disorders (e.g. anxiety disorders, unipolar mood disorders, substance use disorders) can be grouped together under two or three dimensions reflecting either internalising or externalising psychopathology. For example, Krueger (Reference Krueger1999) demonstrated that SAD was part of a broad internalising factor along with simple phobia, agoraphobia and panic disorder (also constituting a ‘fear’ subfactor in a three-factor model), as well as major depression, dysthymia and GAD (these latter conditions comprising an ‘anxious-misery’ subfactor in a three-factor model). The internalising construct has recently been incorporated into a new hierarchical classification model (Kotov et al. Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff and Bagby2017) – the Hierarchical Taxonomy Of Psychopathology (HiTOP) – as a ‘spectra’ (consisting of constellations of syndromes). These spectra are said to be influenced by a higher order general psychopathology dimension (e.g. p factor; Caspi et al. Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington and Israel2014). The constructs of fear and distress are subfactors of the internalising spectra, which then relate to the expression of syndromes and disorders. Fear components within the internalising spectra are said to include features such as interactive anxiety, performance anxiety, situational phobia and trait anxiety. Such structural models highlight the non-orthogonality of conditions such as SAD, and position the disorder as just one form of maladjustment where distress is internally focussed. Together, such findings stress the need for refining how we study disorders such as SAD, particularly in terms of whether we continue to conceptualise and treat the condition as if it were a distinct entity.

Non-specificity of aetiological and maintaining factors and treatment approaches

Cognitive models of SAD (Clarke & Wells, Reference Clarke, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Rapee & Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2007; Moscovitch, Reference Moscovitch2009; Heimberg et al. Reference Heimberg, Brozovich, Rapee, Hofmann and DiBartolo2010) state that those with the condition engage in maladaptive cognitive and behavioural strategies (e.g. dysfunctional anticipatory processing, negative social-evaluative cognitions, avoidance and escape behaviour) prior to, during, and following perceived social evaluative situations, which in turn maintains the disorder. Whilst these models have been invaluable in providing an elemental account of the disorder's maintaining factors, it is beneficial to detail their antecedents. Beck & Clark (Reference Beck and Clark1988) proposed an information processing (cf. cognitive) theory of anxiety and depression, following Beck's (Reference Beck1967) original cognitive–behavioural formulation of depression. In essence, the maintaining factors of SAD correspond to three cognitive–behavioural factors, namely, (i) products, (ii) processes and (iii) schemas/core beliefs. The central element, ‘processes’, refers to cognitive processes (e.g. attentional/memory biases) that influence the appraisal of situations (e.g. threat misappraisal). The ‘products’ of such processes may manifest as negative automatic thoughts and/or avoidance behaviours. These products can also influence cognitive processes, given their bidirectional relationship. At the bottom of the hierarchy lie schemas/core beliefs, which influence an individual's view of self, others and the world. Such models have also been applied to GAD, obsessive–compulsive disorder and psychosis (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Emery and Greenberg1985; Beck & Rector, Reference Beck and Rector2005), and hence represent a generic framework for understanding mechanisms of emotional disorders more broadly.

The search for aetiological mechanisms has largely proceeded in alignment with categorical diagnostic frameworks (Spence & Rapee, Reference Spence and Rapee2016). Different risk factor combinations (e.g. genetics, neurobiology) may result in development of SAD (‘equifinality’), but importantly any given risk factor may be associated with other disorders (‘multifinality’). This latter concept is at the heart of transdiagnostic models of psychopathology (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins2011). For instance, Spence & Rapee (Reference Spence and Rapee2016) highlight overly controlling/intrusive parenting as contributing to inhibited temperament in children – and thus risk for the disorder – but note these parenting styles are not unique for increasing risk of SAD. Indeed, such parenting may contribute (or be a response) to a more general trait anxiety early in life (Negreiros & Miller, Reference Negreiros and Miller2014). Spence & Rapee (Reference Spence and Rapee2016) further note that adverse/stressful life events and trauma increase risk for developing SAD. However, these factors again lack specificity to the condition. It is hence increasingly apparent that the field should embrace novel approaches to elucidate the differing influences of transdiagnostic v. disorder-specific aetiological factors (Spence & Rapee, Reference Spence and Rapee2016). In keeping with this, Lahey et al. (Reference Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman and Zald2017) proposed an evidence-based taxonomy of psychopathology based on shared and unique causal factors across differing first-order dimensions (i.e. latent constructs defined by correlations amongst symptoms). Their hierarchical model states that, (i) some genetic and environmental variables increase risk across all first-order dimensions, (ii) there are non-specific causal influences across higher order dimensions (e.g. internalising psychopathology) and (iii) some causal influences are specific to different first-order dimensions, and possibly even subsets of symptoms. This highlights that both specific and non-specific genetic and environmental factors contribute to psychopathology, which has important implications for whether SAD can continue to be conceptualised as discrete condition.

There has been an active search for neurobiological correlates of SAD, but as with all psychiatric conditions findings have been largely non-specific. Although the field has not yet identified specific neuronal biomarkers with prognostic value in isolated disorders including SAD (Linden, Reference Linden2012), it is possible that methodological limitations have obscured identification of casual mechanisms (Poldrack & Farah, Reference Poldrack and Farah2015). Advances to neuroimaging technologies are likely to allow unprecedented insight into multilevel systems (e.g. molecular, cellular, neural populations and circuits), which may assist with identification of neural substrates of psychopathology. For example, whilst the amygdala was broadly identified in early imaging studies as a canonical brain region involved in the pathophysiology of SAD, it has also been implicated in the expression of other anxiety disorders (Etkin & Wager, Reference Etkin and Wager2007), mood disorders (Drevets, Reference Drevets2003), psychosis (Velakoulis et al. Reference Velakoulis, Wood, Wong, McGorry, Yung and Phillips2006) and borderline personality disorder (Herpertz et al. Reference Herpertz, Dietrich, Wenning, Krings, Erberich and Willmes2001). The field has subsequently narrowed its focus, and recent work suggests that the ‘extended amygdala’ – the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) – may be more pertinent to anxiety disorders (Lebow & Chen, Reference Lebow and Chen2016). The BNST is believed to process less specific, long duration threat information and hence may contribute to sustained anxiety rather than fear (i.e. phasic anxiety), aligning with the view that anxiety disorders be recast as disorders of vigilance (Davis & Whalen, Reference Davis and Whalen2001). In contrast, the central amygdala appears more implicated in fear responding than sustained anxiety. It may hence be plausible that amygdala reactivity corresponds to a latent vigilance factor across anxiety disorders, and possibly even across the spectrum of psychiatric conditions. In line with this, grey matter loss in anterior cingulate and insula cortices has been identified as a common factor across diagnostic groups including SAD (Goodkind et al. Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang and Jones-Hagata2015). Furthermore, whilst genetics influence SAD (Hettema et al. Reference Hettema, Neale and Kendler2001), it has been difficult to identify genetic markers at both the disorder and endophenotype level given the heterogeneity of diagnostic features and multiple genetic variants involved (Flint & Munafo, Reference Flint and Munafo2007).

In terms of treatment, cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT), particularly individual-based, has the strongest evidence base, compared with other psychotherapies (Mayo-Wilson et al. Reference Mayo-Wilson, Dias, Mavranezouli, Kew, Clark and Ades2014). CBT for SAD will usually involve a client and therapist working together to target and modify maladaptive cognitive processes (e.g. cognitive restructuring), and dysfunctional behavioural patterns (Rodebaugh et al. Reference Rodebaugh, Holaway and Heimberg2004). Whilst CBT is effective for SAD in helping to modify disorder-specific beliefs around social threat – suggesting a degree of specificity – very similar principles and strategies are used with good effect across psychiatric conditions (Hofmann et al. Reference Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk, Sawyer and Fang2012). Consequently, transdiagnostic CBT approaches have been developed towards the ultimate goal of a ‘unified treatment for emotional disorders’ (Barlow et al. Reference Barlow, Allen and Choate2004). Studies have shown that unified treatments perform better than waitlist across comorbid disorders, and may be comparable to disorder-specific treatments (McEvoy et al. Reference McEvoy, Nathan and Norton2009; Norton & Barrera, Reference Norton and Barrera2012). Although consensus has not been reached on whether unified CBT approaches can be offered in replacement of disorder-specific treatments, evidence points towards the former being complementary to the latter (Rector et al. Reference Rector, Man and Lerman2014). A similar story emerges for the pharmacological treatment of SAD. Both phenelzine and tranylcypromine are effective agents for SAD despite being developed for depression (Sareen & Stein, Reference Sareen and Stein2000). A meta-analysis of placebo-controlled studies revealed that phenelzine had the largest effect size for reducing SAD symptoms (Blanco et al. Reference Blanco, Schneier, Schmidt, Blanco-Jerez, Marshall and Sanchez-Lacay2003). The same meta-analysis reported a very large effect size for clonazepam (used for numerous other psychiatric disorders), and found selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) to be effective in treating SAD (Blanco et al. Reference Blanco, Schneier, Schmidt, Blanco-Jerez, Marshall and Sanchez-Lacay2003). The SSRIs were of course developed initially as ‘antidepressants’ but are broadly effective for anxiety disorders (Baldwin et al. Reference Baldwin, Anderson, Nutt, Bandelow, Bond and Davidson2005). Hence, despite their effectiveness in SAD, the psychotherapeutic and drug treatments offered are not disorder specific, which likely reflects the heterogeneity of conditions being treated (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013).

Despite efficacious treatments for SAD, it remains unclear why a significant number of individuals do not respond to first-line psychotherapeutic or pharmacological interventions. Inaccurate diagnosis, and subsequent inappropriate treatment, has been suggested as one reason for suboptimal outcomes (Stein & Stein, Reference Stein and Stein2008). New approaches to classification, such as that put forward by the RDoC framework, may hence be of benefit in eventually optimising treatment for SAD. Should the field be successful in validating novel domains through integration with neuroscience, it may be possible to develop and select more precise treatments – in essence, ‘precision medicine’ for psychiatry (Insel, Reference Insel2014).

Overcoming limitations posed by categorical psychiatry: embracing RDoC

Despite the limitations detailed above, the last 50 years of research into SAD has dramatically increased understanding of its aetiology, maintenance and treatment. However, there is a need for further clarifying such factors, and it is apparent that the field may benefit by aligning with emerging frameworks that seek to link observable aspects of brain and behaviour with symptom expression (Insel, Reference Insel2014). This constitutes a significant change for psychiatry, with the initial litmus test being whether the field is willing to abandon the status quo of polythetic-categorical models. Even still, embracing RDoC does not necessarily spell the end for SAD as a clinically useful construct. Instead, utilising the enormous body of research that has identified common and shared factors for SAD, in conjunction with novel methodological approaches into pathophysiological mechanisms, may aid discovery of more precise models for optimal treatment selection and prediction. For instance, computational models (e.g. generative embedding) applied to neuroimaging data have offered insight into schizophrenia subtyping (Brodersen et al. Reference Brodersen, Deserno, Schlagenhauf, Lin, Penny and Buhmann2014). Such approaches allow for pathophysiologically informed separation of psychiatric disorders (Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Friston and Frith2009), and may ultimately help inform diagnosis and treatment. Psychiatry is hence equipped with a rich set of methods (in terms of acquisition and analysis of physiological parameters), affording it with a unique opportunity to examine the clinical utility of defining disorders by underlying pathophysiological mechanisms.

The RDoC comprises a two-dimensional matrix spanning domains/constructs and units of analysis – so designed as adaptable constructs, thus enabling incorporation of evolving developments across fields – which are intended to guide research endeavours towards developing a more mechanistic understanding of mental illnesses (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014). A central tenet is in identifying the normal distribution of functioning, with relative deviations to this perhaps assigned disorder or disease status. This overcomes issues faced by DSM/ICD regarding whether disorders are categorically different from normal or whether they vary on a continuum. As Cuthbert (Reference Cuthbert2014) highlights, ‘… many paradigms have been developed that can provide measures of both behavioural performance and of related functional brain activity in a large population, thus providing some sense of the normal distribution’ (p. 31). As psychiatry moves to integrate with the RDoC framework, it has been recommended that studies recruit as broadly as possible (spanning normative to impaired) across the constructs of interest. It is conceded, however, that the RDoC will first be tested in convenience samples of individuals who will likely meet criteria for disorders under current diagnostic systems (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014). For a true representation of dimensionality, it will be important to study individuals with a variety of other psychopathologies, from mild to severe, as well as incorporate those with normative functioning. For example, healthy individuals (with no frank psychopathology) should be recruited alongside those with other anxiety disorders (e.g. GAD, panic disorder and agoraphobia), mood disorders, personality disorders and psychotic disorders. Less ‘severe’, yet still mildly impairing, psychopathologies (including, but not limited to, adjustment disorder) should also be included in the search for aetiological and maintaining factors across the RDoC matrix.

How might existing SAD research inform the RDoC matrix?

Two constructs under the RDoC negative valence systems domain, namely ‘acute threat (‘fear’)’ and ‘potential threat (‘anxiety’)’, parallel a range of SAD features. The former, ‘acute threat’ is defined as ‘activation of the brain's defensive motivational system to promote behaviours that protect the organism from perceived danger … [involving responses to conditioned or unconditioned threat stimuli]’. The latter, ‘potential threat’, is defined as ‘activation of a brain system in which harm may potentially occur but is distant, ambiguous, or low/uncertain in probability … [characterised by high vigilance to low imminence threat]’ (NIMH, 2017). A key feature of SAD, fear of negative evaluation, may hence be conceptualised across the acute threat (‘fear’) and potential threat (‘anxiety’) constructs. The former is particularly salient in light of evidence linking fear of negative evaluation in SAD with conditioned fear (startle) responses (Lissek et al. Reference Lissek, Levenson, Biggs, Johnson, Ameli and Pine2008). Fear of evaluation may also be studied under the sustained threat construct (also under negative valence systems), as well as within the cognitive systems domain (e.g. particularly attentional biases). The behavioural avoidance and safety behaviours commonly seen in SAD have previously been formulated as reflecting efforts to circumvent feared outcomes (Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2007), and may be best operationalised across both the acute threat and potential threat domains in RDoC. It may also be useful to examine features of SAD under RDoC domains other than negative valence systems, for example, across the social processes and positive valence systems domains. The ‘affiliation and attachment’ construct under the former encompasses social information processing (e.g. of social cues), and the behavioural and physiological consequences of disrupted social relationships. This is particularly relevant given cognitive models suggest that those with SAD misinterpret ambiguous social cues (Clark & Wells, Reference Clarke, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995). There is likely to be utility in examining constructs such as ‘approach motivation’ within the positive valence systems domain, reflecting mechanisms and processes that modulate maintenance of approach behaviour, which often manifests as disrupted in SAD via behavioural avoidance (Rapee & Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997).

An essential component of the RDoC is to extend established cognitive and behavioural features, including fear of evaluation, avoidance and safety behaviours, to other conditions. Specifically, the field may seek to further understand these features through the lens of RDoC ‘perceived threat’ across those who may typically be diagnosed with mood and psychotic spectrum conditions and even eating disorders – the latter two also being associated with fear of negative evaluation (Gilbert & Meyer, Reference Gilbert and Meyer2005; Kinoshita et al. Reference Kinoshita, Kingdon, Kinoshita, Kinoshita, Saka and Arisue2011). Within any of the RDoC constructs, it will be critical to move beyond descriptive (e.g. self-report) features, and examine the role of other units of analysis, such as predisposing environmental factors, potential cellular/molecular/brain circuitry contributions, as well as physiology (e.g. psychophysiology) and observable behaviour (e.g. neuropsychology). Fear of negative evaluation, and avoidance/safety behaviours, are just a few examples of what is likely to be many (especially when transdiagnostic factors are examined), but nonetheless offer a useful starting point for reframing our thinking as RDoC-inspired programmes of research gain traction.

The basic elements of RDoC-inspired research

The RDoC was proposed as a framework to better understand how basic dimensions of functioning (from genetics through neurobiology to self-report) underlie the range of human behaviour from normal to abnormal (Insel, Reference Insel2014). The operationalisation of these criteria across differing units of analysis will provide a unique opportunity to understand dimensions of differing psychological constructs (e.g. acute threat, potential threat) using a range of methods from neuroscience (molecular, neurocircuitry, behaviour) (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013). The RDoC matrix details five domains, each with a range of subconstructs, which reference specific psychological processes. The negative valence systems domain has five sub-constructs across seven units of analysis, giving rise to a 5 × 7 matrix. Specific research methods, or ‘elements’, are noted within cells across differing matrices. For instance, the RDoC details a range of elements or factors as potentially contributing to the acute threat (‘fear’) subconstruct. These include differing molecular (e.g. dopamine, glutamate, vasopressin) and cellular (e.g. neuronal, glial) targets, brain circuitry (e.g. ventromedial prefrontal cortex, medial/lateral/basal amygdala), physiology (e.g. skin conductance, heart rate), behaviour (e.g. response inhibition, social approach) and self-report (e.g. quantifying distress) variables, which may be measured during experimental manipulations such as the Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum et al. Reference Kirschbaum, Pirke and Hellhammer1993). In some cases, these elements are also implicated in other sub-constructs or even differing higher level systems; for example, vasopressin is also included in the ‘loss’ and ‘frustrative non-reward’ sub-constructs of negative valence systems, and in the ‘affiliation and attachment’ sub-construct of the social processes domain. In contrast, the (medial/lateral/basal) amygdala is proposed as specific to the acute threat (‘fear’) construct. Ultimately, the specificity of differing elements to various domains will be borne out by future research, albeit each with a finite level of resources through which to test various translational hypotheses. Importantly, the RDoC matrix does not reflect an exhaustive list of domains, constructs and elements, but is rather a dynamic blueprint allowing integration of methodological and conceptual advances, and hence offers an innovative framework through which to reconsider disorders such as SAD (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014).

Whilst the RDoC may assist in contributing to a reconceptualisation of psychiatric illnesses, it has been criticised for being overly ‘neurocentric’ by principally regarding mental symptoms as emergent phenomena of disrupted (neuro)biological processes (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2014). However, such a view – that maladaptive psychological states emerge from aberrant neurobiological systems – aligns with scientific evidence that indeed all subjective experience is dependent on brain functioning (Kendler, Reference Kendler2005). Notwithstanding this, it has been noted that RDoC-inspired investigations should seek to differentiate between neurobiological aetiology and mediation, in essence acknowledging environmental influences on brain function. Additionally, researchers should seek to minimise specific situational factors (e.g. time of day of testing, nature of laboratory setting) that contribute to low cross-situational and temporal consistency (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2014). Further, it will be important to clarify the relative contribution (e.g. in terms of heritability) of endophenotypes and intermediate phenotypes over traditionally studied exophenotypes (comprising signs and symptoms). Indeed, whilst some have argued that the RDoC oversimplifies the complexity of psychopathology (Wakefield, Reference Wakefield2014) by ignoring the richness of disorder or descriptive psychopathology (rather than of disease – by definition, that arising from pathology), the approach arguably offers a tenable alternative, and perhaps a solution, to the limitations inherent in the DSM/ICD approaches to classification. Spence & Rapee (Reference Spence and Rapee2016) note – in overviewing aetiological factors in SAD – the difficulties associated with any identified risk factors being transactional and reciprocal, such that there is a dynamic interplay between expression/emergence of symptoms/vulnerability and eventual disorder onset. Providing answers to these questions will take time, but it is hoped that current and future research endeavours will lead to further advances in identifying susceptible individuals, and classifying, treating and even preventing SAD, if such a category remains over the ensuing years.

Conclusions

In the 50 years that have passed since Marks & Gelder's (Reference Marks and Gelder1966) formative paper the social anxiety literature has proliferated and now spans classification and diagnosis, cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms underlying maintenance, aetiological processes, and both psychological and psychopharmacological treatments. SAD has since emerged as a highly prevalent condition, and whilst the theoretical models that have been described offer hope towards improving and enhancing treatments for the condition, it remains difficult to achieve full symptom resolution, and indeed longer term remission, in such patients. The future of SAD will largely be dependent on research efforts of psychiatry as a whole, and may hinge on what the RDoC framework delivers. Will future treatment interventions be focused on first identifying and targeting transdiagnostic pathophysiological aberrations, followed by specific focus on symptoms unique to the condition? Let us hope the next 50 years are as fruitful as the last.

Acknowledgements

The authors report no competing interests in relation to this manuscript. The authors gratefully acknowledge grant support from the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (Project Grant APP1104007).

References

APA (1952) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 1st edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
APA (1968) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 2nd edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
APA (1980) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
APA (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn – revised. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
APA (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Baldwin, DS, Anderson, IM, Nutt, DJ, Bandelow, B, Bond, A, Davidson, JR et al. British Association for Pharmacology (2005) Evidence-based guidelines for the pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders: recommendations from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology 19, 567596.Google Scholar
Barlow, DH, Allen, LB and Choate, ML (2004) Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders. Behavior Therapy 35, 205230.Google Scholar
Beck, AT (1967) Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Beck, AT and Clark, DA (1988) Anxiety and depression: an information processing perspective. Anxiety Research 1, 2336.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Emery, G and Greenberg, RL (1985) Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: a Cognitive Perspective. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Beck, AT and Rector, NA (2005) Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: theory and therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 1, 577606.Google Scholar
Berrios, GE (1996) The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology Since the Nineteenth Century. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blanco, C, Schneier, FR, Schmidt, A, Blanco-Jerez, CR, Marshall, RD, Sanchez-Lacay, A et al. (2003) Pharmacological treatment of social anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety 18, 2940.Google Scholar
Bögels, SM, Alden, L, Beidel, DC, Clark, LA, Pine, DS, Stein, MB et al. (2010) Social anxiety disorder: questions and answers for the DSM-V. Depression and Anxiety 27, 168189.Google Scholar
Bögels, SM and Mansell, W (2004) Attention processes in the maintenance and treatment of social phobia: hypervigilance, avoidance and self-focused attention. Clinical Psychology Review 24, 827856.Google Scholar
Borkovec, TD, Alcaine, O and Behar, ES (2004) Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In Heimberg, R, Mennin, D, Turk, C (eds). Generalized Anxiety Disroder: Advances in Research and Practice, pp. 77108. New York: Guildford.Google Scholar
Brodersen, KH, Deserno, L, Schlagenhauf, F, Lin, Z, Penny, WD, Buhmann, JM et al. (2014) Dissecting psychiatric spectrum disorders by generative embedding. Neuroimage Clinical 4, 98111.Google Scholar
Brown, TA, Campbell, LA, Lehman, CL, Grisham, JR and Mancill, RB (2001) Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in a large clinical sample. Jorunal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 585599.Google Scholar
Bruch, MA (1989) Familial and developmental antecedents of social phobia: issues and findings. Clinical Psychology Review 9, 3747.Google Scholar
Burton, R (1621) The Anatomy of Melancholy. London, UK: J. M. Dent & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW, Goldman-Mellor, SJ, Harrington, H, Israel, S et al. (2014) The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science 2, 119137.Google Scholar
Chavira, DA and Stein, MB (1999) The shyness spectrum. CNS Spectrums 4, 2029.Google Scholar
Clarke, DM, Wells, A (1995) A cognitive model of social phobia. In Heimberg, RG, Liebowitz, MR, Hope, DA and Schneier, FR (eds). Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Treatment, pp. 6993. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Craddock, N and Mynors-Wallis, L (2014) Psychiatric diagnosis: impersonal, imperfect and important. British Journal of Psychiatry 204, 9395.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN (2014) The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry 13, 2835.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN and Insel, TR (2013) Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine 11, 126.Google Scholar
D'Avanzato, C and Dalrymple, KL (2016) Recent insight into the subtypes of social anxiety disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 18, 50.Google Scholar
Davis, M and Whalen, PJ (2001) The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry 6, 1334.Google Scholar
Drevets, WC (2003) Neuroimaging abnormalities in the amygdala in mood disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 985, 420444.Google Scholar
Dugas, L (1898) Timidité. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Eaton, NR, South, SC and Krueger, RF (2010) The meaning of comorbidity among common mental disorders. In Millon, T, Krueger, RF, Simonsen, E (eds). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-V and ICD-11, pp. 223241. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Etkin, A and Wager, TD (2007) Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 14761488.Google Scholar
Flint, J and Munafo, MR (2007) The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychological Medine 37, 163180.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N and Meyer, C (2005) Fear of negative evaluation and the development of eating psychopathology: a longitudinal study among nonclinical women. International Jorunal of Eating Disorders 37, 307312.Google Scholar
Goodkind, M, Eickhoff, SB, Oathes, DJ, Jiang, Y, Chang, A, Jones-Hagata, LB et al. (2015) Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 305315.Google Scholar
Hartenberg, P (1901) Les Timides et al Timidité. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Heimberg, RG, Brozovich, FA and Rapee, RM (2010) A cognitive-behavioral model of social anxiety disorder. In Hofmann, SG and DiBartolo, PM (eds). Social Anxiety: Clinical, Developmental and Social Perspectives, New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Heimberg, RG, Hofmann, SG, Liebowitz, MR, Schneier, FR, Smits, JA, Stein, MB et al. (2014) Social anxiety disorder in DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety 31, 472479.Google Scholar
Heiser, NA, Turner, SM and Beidel, DC (2003) Shyness: relationship to social phobia and other psychiatric disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy 41, 209221.Google Scholar
Heiser, NA, Turner, SM, Beidel, DC and Roberson-Nay, R (2009) Differentiating social phobia from shyness. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 469476.Google Scholar
Helzer, JE, Kraemer, HC and Krueger, RF (2006) The feasibility and need for dimensional psychiatric diagnoses. Psychological Medicine 36, 16711680.Google Scholar
Herbert, JD, Hope, DA and Bellack, AS (1992) Validity of the distinction between generalized social phobia and avoidant personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101, 332–329.Google Scholar
Herpertz, SC, Dietrich, TM, Wenning, B, Krings, T, Erberich, SG, Willmes, K et al. (2001) Evidence of abnormal amygdala functioning in borderline personality disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry 50, 292298.Google Scholar
Hettema, JM, Neale, MC and Kendler, KS (2001) A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 15681578.Google Scholar
Hofmann, SG (2007) Cognitive factors that maintain social anxiety disorder: a comprehensive model and its treatment implications. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy 36, 193209.Google Scholar
Hofmann, SG, Asnaani, A, Vonk, IJ, Sawyer, AT and Fang, A (2012) The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research 36, 427440.Google Scholar
Huppert, JD, Foa, EB, Furr, JM, Filip, JC and Mathews, A (2003) Interpretation bias in social anxiety: a dimensional perspective. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 569577.Google Scholar
Insel, TR (2014) The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) project: precision medicine for psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 395397.Google Scholar
Janet, P (1903) Les Obsessions et la Psychasthénie. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS (2005) Toward a philosophical structure for psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 433440.Google Scholar
Keshavan, MS and Ongur, D (2014) The journey from RDC/DSM diagnoses toward RDoC dimensions. World Psychiatry 13, 4446.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Merikangas, KR and Walters, EE (2005 a) Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 593602.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Chiu, WT, Demler, O, Merikangas, KR and Walters, EE (2005 b) Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 617627.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, Y, Kingdon, D, Kinoshita, K, Kinoshita, Y, Saka, K, Arisue, Y et al. (2011) Fear of negative evaluation is associated with delusional ideation in non-clinical population and patients with schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 46, 703710.Google Scholar
Kirschbaum, C, Pirke, KM and Hellhammer, DH (1993) The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ – a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 7681.Google Scholar
Kotov, R, Krueger, RF, Watson, D, Achenbach, TM, Althoff, RR, Bagby, RM et al. (2017) The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126, 454477.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF (1999) The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 921926.Google Scholar
Krueger, RF and Bezdjian, S (2009) Enhancing research and treatment of mental disorders with dimensional concepts: toward DSM-V and ICD-11. World Psychiatry 8, 36.Google Scholar
Lahey, BB, Krueger, RF, Rathouz, PJ, Waldman, ID and Zald, DH (2017) A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychological Bulletin 143, 142186.Google Scholar
Lebow, MA and Chen, A (2016) Overshadowed by the amygdala: the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis emerges as key to psychiatric disorders. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 450463.Google Scholar
Liebowitz, MR, Gorman, JM, Fyer, AJ and Klein, DF (1985) Social phobia. Review of a neglected anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 42, 729736.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, SO (2014) The Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): an analysis of methodological and conceptual challenges. Behviour Research and Therapy 62, 129139.Google Scholar
Linden, DE (2012) The challenges and promise of neuroimaging in psychiatry. Neuron 73, 822.Google Scholar
Lissek, S, Levenson, J, Biggs, AL, Johnson, LL, Ameli, R, Pine, DS et al. (2008) Elevated fear conditioning to socially relevant unconditioned stimuli in social anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 124132.Google Scholar
Maj, M (2005) “Psychiatric comorbidity”: an artefact of current diagnostic systems? British Journal of Psychiatry 186, 182184.Google Scholar
Marks, IM and Gelder, MG (1966) Different ages of onset in varieties of phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry 123, 218221.Google Scholar
Mayo-Wilson, E, Dias, S, Mavranezouli, I, Kew, K, Clark, DM, Ades, AE et al. (2014) Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 1, 368376.Google Scholar
McEvoy, PM, Nathan, P and Norton, PJ (2009) Efficacy of transdiagnostic treatments: a review of published outcome studies and future research directions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 23, 2033.Google Scholar
Merikangas, KR, Avenevoli, S, Acharyya, S, Zhang, H and Angst, J (2002) The spectrum of social phobia in the Zurich cohort study of young adults. Biological Psychiatry 51, 8191.Google Scholar
Moscovitch, DA (2009) What is the core fear in social phobia? A new model to facilitate individualized case conceptualization and treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 16, 123134.Google Scholar
Negreiros, J and Miller, LD (2014) The role of parenting in childhood anxiety: etiological factors and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 21, 317.Google Scholar
NIMH (2008) The National Institute of Mental Health Strategic Plan. Bethesda, MD: NIH:.Google Scholar
NIMH (2017) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). Retrieved July 19, 2017 from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/index.shtml.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S and Watkins, ER (2011) A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6, 589609.Google Scholar
Norton, PJ and Barrera, TL (2012) Transdiagnostic versus diagnosis-specific CBT for anxiety disorders: a preliminary randomized controlled noninferiority trial. Depression and Anxiety 29, 874882.Google Scholar
Poldrack, RA and Farah, MJ (2015) Progress and challenges in probing the human brain. Nature 526, 371379.Google Scholar
Prince, RH (1993) Culture bound syndromes: the example of social phobias. In (Ghadirian, A, Lehman, HE (eds). Environment and Psychopathology, pp. 5572. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Rapee, RM and Heimberg, RG (1997) A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy 35, 741756.Google Scholar
Rector, NA, Man, V and Lerman, B (2014) The expanding cognitive-behavioural therapy treatment umbrella for the anxiety disorders: disorder-specific and transdiagnostic approaches. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59, 301309.Google Scholar
Rodebaugh, TL, Holaway, RM and Heimberg, RG (2004) The treatment of social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 24, 883908.Google Scholar
Rodebaugh, TL, Levinson, CA, Langer, JK, Weeks, JW, Heimberg, RG, Brown, PJ et al. (2017) The structure of vulnerabilities for social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research 250, 297301.Google Scholar
Roth, S and Cohen, LJ (1986) Approach, avoidance, and coping with stress. American Psychologist 41, 813819.Google Scholar
Ruscio, AM (2010) The latent structure of social anxiety disorder: consequences of shifting to a dimensional diagnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119, 662671.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, PM (1991) The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive account. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 19, 619.Google Scholar
Sanislow, CA (2016) Connecting psychopathology meta-structure and mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 125, 11581165.Google Scholar
Sareen, L and Stein, M (2000) A review of the epidemiology and approaches to the treatment of social anxiety disorder. Drugs 59, 497509.Google Scholar
Schneier, FR, Johnson, J, Hornig, CD, Liebowitz, MR and Weissman, MM (1992) Social phobia. Comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiologic sample. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 282288.Google Scholar
Spence, SH and Rapee, RM (2016) The etiology of social anxiety disorder: an evidence-based model. Behaviour Research and Therapy 86, 5067.Google Scholar
Stein, DJ, Lim, CCW, Roest, AM, de Jonge, P, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Al-Hamzawi, A et al. WHO World Mental Health Survey Collaborators (2017) The cross-national epidemiology of social anxiety disorder: data from the World Mental Health Survey Initiative. BMC Medicine 15, 143.Google Scholar
Stein, MB (1999) Coming face-to-face with social phobia. American Family Physician 60, 22442245.Google Scholar
Stein, MB and Stein, DJ (2008) Social anxiety disorder. Lancet 371, 11151125.Google Scholar
Stein, MB, Torgrud, LJ and Walker, JR (2000) Social phobia symptoms, subtypes, and severity: findings from a community survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 10461052.Google Scholar
Stephan, KE, Friston, KJ and Frith, CD (2009) Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35, 509527.Google Scholar
Turner, SM, Beidel, DC, Borden, JW, Stanley, MA and Jacob, RG (1991) Social phobia: axis I and II correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100, 102106.Google Scholar
Turner, SM, Beidel, DC and Townsley, RM (1992) Social phobia: a comparison of specific and generalized subtypes and avoidant personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101, 326331.Google Scholar
Velakoulis, D, Wood, SJ, Wong, MT, McGorry, PD, Yung, A, Phillips, L et al. (2006) Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 139149.Google Scholar
Wakefield, JC (2014) Wittgenstein's nightmare: why the RDoC grid needs a conceptual dimension. World Psychiatry 13, 3840.Google Scholar
Weeks, JW, Carleton, RN, Asmundson, GJ, McCabe, RE and Antony, MM (2010) “Social anxiety disorder carved at its joints”: evidence for the taxonicity of social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24, 734742.Google Scholar
Weeks, JW, Norton, PJ and Heimberg, RG (2009) Exploring the latent structure of two cognitive components of social anxiety: taxometric analyses of fears of negative and positive evaluation. Depression and Anxiety 26, E40E48.Google Scholar
WHO (1992) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar