Emotional and behavioral dysregulation (e.g. affective variability, reactive verbal and physical aggression) represent transdiagnostic indicators that permeate nearly all psychological disorders in youth (Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2003; McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin, & Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011). Enhancing our understanding of their neurobiological underpinnings has the potential to elucidate key etiological mechanisms that could improve current interventions. Along these lines, individual variability in peripheral physiology, specifically variation in tonic (resting) and phasic (reactivity) respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), may underlie risk for dysregulated emotion and behavior (Beauchaine & Bell, Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Beauchaine and Crowell2019). The transition to adolescence may be a particularly critical period for assessing this potential biomarker given normative increases in physiological reactivity and interpersonal conflict (Granic, Hollenstein, Dishion, & Patterson, Reference Granic, Hollenstein, Dishion and Patterson2003; Steinberg & Morris, Reference Steinberg and Morris2001). However, few studies examine RSA during this sensitive developmental window. Moreover, tonic and phasic RSA are often examined independently as predictors of disorder-specific outcomes, despite evidence that their interaction may help specify unique physiological profiles of at-risk youth (Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg, & Kovacs, Reference Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg and Kovacs2013; Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg, & Kovacs, Reference Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg and Kovacs2014). The current study expands on previous literature using a transdiagnostic sample of clinically referred youth to examine baseline RSA, RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict, and their interaction as predictors of dysregulated emotion and behavior in daily life.
Dysregulated emotion and behavior as transdiagnostic indicators of psychopathology
Dysregulated emotion and behavior are multifaceted transdiagnostic constructs that reflect heightened reactivity to internal or external events (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Dysregulated emotion has been defined as intense, variable, or prolonged affective episodes (Linehan, Reference Linehan1993). Some research suggests that affective variability – frequent and intense fluctuations in one's emotional experience – may be a particularly important facet of dysregulated emotion, as variability in negative emotions (e.g. anger, sadness) has been associated with various forms of psychopathology (Houben, Van Den Noortgate, & Kuppens, Reference Houben, Van Den Noortgate and Kuppens2015; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Victor, Kaufman, Beeney, Byrd, Vine and Stepp2020). Dysregulated behavior has been most commonly studied in youth in terms of discrete verbal and physical acts of aggression. Verbal and physical ‘reactive’ aggression – impulsive, emotionally-driven aggressive acts – have received growing attention in light of their transdiagnostic relevance and predictive utility (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; Schaeffer, Petras, Ialongo, Poduska, & Kellam, Reference Schaeffer, Petras, Ialongo, Poduska and Kellam2003). Notably, dysregulated emotion and behavior have been linked prospectively to worsening internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescence, providing support for the notion that these transdiagnostic indicators function to increase the risk for a wide range of psychopathology during this developmentally sensitive period (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011). These findings highlight the importance of continuing to examine these constructs among transdiagnostic samples of youth to improve our understanding of psychopathology as it develops along common and divergent pathways (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996).
RSA and risk for psychopathology
RSA refers to the amount of variability between heartbeats (i.e. inter-beat interval) that rhythmically fluctuates with respiration (Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Thomas Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann, Malik and Stone1997). This non-invasive index is of particular interest, given its unique association with parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) function, specifically vagal control of the sinoatrial node (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, Reference Berntson, Cacioppo and Quigley1994), and its implicated role in self-regulation (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Porges, Reference Porges2007). During rest, tonic PNS activation functions via the vagus nerve to slow heart rate and increase its variability (i.e. higher RSA), which facilitates energy and emotion regulation. During stress (e.g. perceived threat, environmental challenge), withdrawal of PNS control – a releasing of vagal control – results in increased heart rate and reduced RSA variability, allowing one to mobilize the metabolic resources needed to optimally respond to environmental demands (i.e. RSA reactivity; Porges, Reference Porges2007). Consistent with this theoretical perspective, better mental health and functional outcomes are generally linked to higher tonic RSA, and to moderate RSA reactivity (specifically RSA withdrawal) during environmental challenges and stress (Graziano & Derefinko, Reference Graziano and Derefinko2013).
Individual variability in tonic and phasic RSA has been the focus of a growing body of literature aimed at identifying neurobiological indices of risk for psychopathology in youth (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015). Specifically, research in clinical and community samples shows that lower tonic RSA increases the risk for a wide range of psychiatric disorders, suggesting a reduced capacity for responsiveness to environmental demands (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Knapton, McDonough-Caplan, Shader and Zisner2019). Additionally, excessive RSA reactivity (i.e. greater RSA withdrawal relative to peers) in response to challenge and/or stress (i.e. passively viewing emotionally evocative stimuli) is linked to internalizing and externalizing pathology (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Knapton, McDonough-Caplan, Shader and Zisner2019; Fanti et al., Reference Fanti, Eisenbarth, Goble, Demetriou, Kyranides, Goodwin and Cortese2019). While moderate RSA withdrawal is considered an adaptive and necessary response to stress, excessive withdrawal may be problematic, as it is associated with overly rapid mobilization of fight or flight responding (Porges, Reference Porges2007). Although truly dangerous circumstances may require a more pronounced physiological reaction to enable necessary emotional and behavioral responses, it may be less contextually appropriate, and by extension maladaptive, to marshal a pronounced RSA response to everyday stressors or challenges, like interpersonal conflict.
While tonic and phasic RSA have been studied as independent predictors of various forms of psychopathology, recent research highlights the importance of examining these indicators in combination to better understand within-individual changes to context as a risk factor for psychopathology. For example, studies examining internalizing disorders in youth have shown depression to be associated with the interaction between tonic RSA and RSA reactivity to an emotionally evocative film (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2009; Yaroslavsky et al., Reference Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg and Kovacs2013, Reference Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg and Kovacs2014). Specifically, this research points to a physiological profile characterized by low tonic RSA and excessive RSA withdrawal, a pattern of responding that may be associated with exaggerated emotional reactivity to environmental stressors and the use of maladaptive behavioral responses. Taken together, this work underscores the importance of examining within-individual interplay between tonic and phasic RSA as it applies to symptoms of broader dimensions of psychopathology.
Along these lines, there is a growing emphasis on examining associations between RSA and transdiagnostic endophenotypes, like dysregulated emotion and behavior. Research has documented associations between tonic RSA and RSA reactivity and dysregulation (Beauchaine & Bell, Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Beauchaine and Crowell2019). In line with the shift from discrete, disorder-specific outcomes to transdiagnostic indicators of psychopathology, the current study examined RSA as a predictor of dysregulated emotion and behavior in a transdiagnostic sample of clinically referred youth during the sensitive transition to adolescence. Moreover, the current study expands on prior literature, which has traditionally examined these constructs using laboratory-based assessments of discrete disorders, by capturing the frequency of these endophenotypes in daily life using an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) protocol.
Transition to adolescence as key developmental window
The transition to adolescence is characterized by normative increases in physiological reactivity that reflect substantial neurobiological changes (Steinberg & Morris, Reference Steinberg and Morris2001). Research suggests that these changes are thought to underlie normative increases in dysregulated emotion and behavior. However, developmental changes in physiological reactivity appear to be amplified among at-risk youth and these amplifications occur during a time of heightened social stress and increases in interpersonal conflict, especially parent-child conflict (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Hollenstein, Dishion and Patterson2003; Steinberg & Morris, Reference Steinberg and Morris2001). Combined, these factors may contribute to increased vulnerability for dysregulated emotion and behavior, intensifying risk for the range of psychopathology that also emerges during this developmental window (Spear, Reference Spear2009). Examining physiological reactivity to stressors that reflect real-life, social experiences (e.g. parent-child conflict) during this sensitive developmental window may enhance our etiological understanding of risk for psychopathology. However, as noted above, the literature in this area has traditionally assessed physiological (RSA) reactivity while passively viewing emotionally evocative stimuli (see Cui et al., Reference Cui, Morris, Harrist, Larzelere, Criss and Houltberg2015; Woody et al., Reference Woody, James, Foster, Owens, Feurer, Kudinova and Gibb2019 for exceptions), making it less clear how findings may generalize to developmentally salient stressors like parent-child conflict.
Current study
The primary goal of this study was to examine baseline RSA, RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict, and their interaction as predictors of dysregulated emotion and behavior during the critical transition to adolescence. Importantly, the current study utilized a transdiagnostic sample of clinically referred youth and examined these endophenotypes in daily life using an EMA protocol. This unique study design allowed for the first known empirical investigation to address these aims.
Methods
Sample
Participants were 162 youth (M age = 12.03 years, s.d. = 0.92 years; 47% female; 60% racial/ethnic minority). Youth and their parents (88% biological mothers; M age = 39.84; s.d. = 7.25; 94% female; 48% racial/ethnic minority) were recruited from pediatric primary care clinics within a large, Mid-Atlantic urban, academic hospital-based setting, and from ambulatory psychiatric treatment clinics in the same geographic region. All youth were receiving psychiatric treatment for any mood or behavior problem and were oversampled for emotional reactivity based on the Affective Instability subscale from the Personality Assessment Inventory-Adolescent version (M = 13.05, s.d. = 2.90; scores >11 indicating clinical significance; Morey, Reference Morey2007). This recruitment strategy was designed to capture a transdiagnostic sample of youth and diagnostic information can be found in Table 1. Any youth with an IQ estimate <70 (n = 5), not currently in treatment for a mood or behavior problem (n = 4), with an organic neurological medical condition (n = 1), diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder or in a current manic or psychotic episode were excluded. All caregivers had legal custody and primary physical custody (>50% of the time). Caregivers reported having M = 3.24 children (s.d. = 1.68) and 49% reported living with their romantic partners. While 64% of households had at least one employed parent, 19% reported an annual household income between $20 000 and $39 000 and 31% reported annual income <$20 000. Additional demographic information is available upon request.
Note. Diagnostic information was obtained from child- and parent-report on the KSAD-S for all disorders except for borderline personality disorder, which was obtained from child- and parent-report on the CI-BPD.
Procedure
The present data were drawn from this longitudinal study where youth and their parents completed three assessments, each of which were every 9 months apart. Assessments included semi-structured diagnostic interviews, questionnaires, and a parent–child interaction task during which multiple physiological indices were continuously recorded. Following each laboratory assessment, youth and their parent also completed a 4-day EMA protocol. The current study focuses on the first assessment, specifically on RSA measured during the parent–child interaction task, and dysregulated emotion and behavior assessed during the EMA protocol. All study procedures were approved by the Human Research Protection Office (HRPO) and the Clinical and Translational Science Institute (CTSI) pediatric practice-based research network. Youth and their parents provided written informed consent and were compensated for their time.
Physiological reactivity to parent-child conflict
Youth RSA was assessed continuously as an index of parasympathetic function during three, 2-minute vanilla baselines (i.e. youth reading silently, youth listening to parent read, and youth thinking about a discussion topic), and an 8-minute parent-child conflict discussion task. Vanilla baselines were designed following best-practice recommendations (Jennings, Kamarck, Stewart, Eddy, & Johnson, Reference Jennings, Kamarck, Stewart, Eddy and Johnson1992). For the conflict discussion, youth and their parent identified areas of disagreement using a 25-item questionnaire that included common areas of parent-child conflict (e.g. behavior toward siblings, internet/cell phone usage, behavior in school). If an area of conflict was endorsed, respondents rated frequency (1 = once in past month to 6 = more than once per day) and intensity (1 = not at all bad to 5 = extremely bad). Research assistants identified two conflict topics that were rated highly in terms of frequency (M = 5.23; s.d. = 1.13) and severity (M = 3.99; s.d. = 0.92) by both members of the dyad. Dyads were then asked to discuss topics with a goal of resolving disagreements in the future.
Physiological recording and assessment
Electrocardiogram (ECG) signals were obtained from disposable Ag/Ag-Cl spot electrodes positioned in a modified lead-II configuration using Mindware BioLab software (MindWare Technologies, Ltd., Gahanna, OH). Two trained scorers visually inspected each recoded waveform (Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Thomas Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann, Malik and Stone1997) and manually corrected artifacts using Mindware HRV 3.1.4 software (MindWare, Gahanna, OH). Any discrepancies arising in this process were resolved by consensus between first and second authors. The interbeat interval (IBI) series was resampled in equal 250 ms intervals, linearly detrended, and tapered using a Hanning window. Heart rate variability (HRV) was calculated using Fast Fourier transformation analysis of the IBI series, and RSA was defined as high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) associated with the log-transformed high frequency (HF) respiratory power band (0.12–0.50 Hz range; see Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Thomas Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann, Malik and Stone1997).Footnote 1 RSA analyses included a total of 154 youth with usable physiological data.Footnote 2
RSA was estimated separately for each vanilla baseline (mean range = 6.55–6.83; s.d. range = 1.00–1.08) and for the conflict discussion task (mean = 6.87; s.d. = 0.90). To take advantage of the multiple baseline assessments, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in MPlus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2012) was employed to assess a latent baseline RSA construct using three baseline indicators. Overall model fit was acceptable (χ2 = 3.55, p > 0.05; CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.13) and the average interitem correlation was 0.82 indicating a high degree of unidimensionality. To measure within-individual RSA reactivity, a difference score was calculated by subtracting RSA during baseline from RSA during the conflict discussion. Thus, negative scores reflect RSA withdrawal (reduced RSA during conflict relative to baseline), while positive scores reflect RSA augmentation (increased RSA during conflict relative to baseline). In this sample, mean RSA reactivity was slightly positive (mean = 0.31; s.d. = 0.64; range = −1.27–2.01), meaning the majority of those below the mean experienced RSA withdrawal, while all of those above the mean experienced RSA augmentation.
Dysregulated emotion and behavior in daily life
All youth completed a 4-day EMA protocol consisting of 10 time-based prompts over the course of a long weekend, with two of the days always including Saturday and Sunday (e.g. Friday: midday, nighttime; Saturday–Sunday: morning, midday, nighttime; Monday: midday, nighttime). Youth were prompted to complete assessments via a ‘beep’ on a study-provided phone. Compliance was high, with the average youth completing 91.1% of assessments.
Dysregulated emotion
At each assessment (n = 10), participants rated a series of negative emotions on a 4-point Likert scale (i.e. 0 = not at all; 1 = a little; 2 = a medium amount; 3 = a lot). Specifically, participants responded to the prompt ‘In the past 15 minutes, how much have you felt…’ (1) ‘sad (blue, unhappy),’ (2) ‘angry (mad),’ (3) ‘nervous (worried, uneasy),’ (4) ‘stressed,’ (5) ‘ashamed,’ (6) ‘guilty,’ (7) ‘lonely,’ and (8) ‘empty.’ To capture emotional variability or dysregulation, the mean squared successive difference (MSSD) was calculated for each negative emotion. MSSD assesses the difference between emotional intensity at consecutive assessments (e.g. sadness at T2–sadness at T1; sadness at T3–sadness at T2) to capture the magnitude of change (i.e. stronger emotional ups-and-downs) while taking temporal dependency into account (Trull et al., Reference Trull, Solhan, Tragesser, Jahng, Wood, Piasecki and Watson2008). Higher MSSD indicates a more variable or dysregulated emotional profile, representative of larger moment-to-moment fluctuations in intensity and heightened reactivity to internal or external events.
MSSD calculations for each of the eight emotions were used as manifest indicators of a latent dysregulated emotion construct. To determine the appropriate measurement model for dysregulated emotion, we conducted exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses in MPlus. Manifest indicators (MSSD for each emotion) were entered into an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with an oblique rotation (i.e. promax), which allows all items to load on all factors. The number of factors extracted was determined by several criteria (e.g. strength of loadings, number of eigenvalues >1.0, interpretability of the solution). In line with theoretical and empirical distinctions (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986; Ortony & Turner, Reference Ortony and Turner1990), a 2-factor solution emerged as optimal. Variability in sadness, anger, nervousness and stress loaded on one factor (dysregulated basic emotion; range of loadings: 0.414–0.914) and variability in shame, guilt, loneliness, and emptiness loaded on a second factor (dysregulated complex emotion; range of loadings: 0.579–0.639). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using the two identified emotion factors confirmed good model fit: χ2 = 27.725, p = 0.03; CFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.07.
Dysregulated behavior
At the end of each of the 4 days, participants completed an end-of-day assessment: ‘When you were feeling your worst, did you let your feelings out by….’: (1) ‘crying/screaming,’ (2) ‘yelling at someone,’ or (3) ‘hitting, slamming or punching someone or something.’ Youth could endorse any of the above and a count score (range = 0–4) was generated for each behavior reflecting the number of days each behavior was endorsed (i.e. 0 = did not engage in behavior on any day, 4 = engaged in behavior every day). Nearly half of the sample reported verbal aggression at least once (crying/screaming (41.2%), yelling at someone (43.8%)), and 20.3% reported physical aggression. Counts for each behavior were used as manifest indicators of the latent dysregulated behavior construct. The small number of indicators necessitated the examination of a one-factor solution. Overall model fit was good (χ2 = 0.659, p > 0.05; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA < 0.0001) and the average interitem correlation was 0.35 which reflects sufficient unidimensionality for self-reported behavioral constructs (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995).
Covariates
Demographic variables
Participant age, gender (0 = male; 1 = female) and minority status (0 = Caucasian; 1 = racial/ethnic minority) were included in analyses.
Stimulant use
All participants completed a questionnaire that included current medication (e.g. stimulant) usage and 18% reported taking stimulants on the day of study participation. Given the influence of stimulants on heart rate, a dichotomized construct (0 = no stimulant usage; 1 = same-day stimulant usage) was included in analyses.
Data analytic strategy
Using SPSS version 25 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL), preliminary analyses included obtaining descriptive statistics, and examining bivariate correlations between all study variables using extracted latent scores for primary predictors and outcomes. . Primary study hypotheses were tested in MPlus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2012), and age, gender, minority status, and same-day stimulant use were included as covariates. Our model was estimated using maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) and included all primary predictors (baseline RSA, RSA reactivity, and their interaction)Footnote 3 and outcomes (dysregulated basic emotion, dysregulated complex emotion, dysregulated behavior) (see Fig. 1)Footnote 4. All significant interactions were probed and plotted for interpretation using RSA values ±1 s.d. above and below the mean. Fraley's (Reference Fraley2018) online utility for parsing the two-way interactions was used to assess simple slopes and regions of significance.
Results
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations
Table 2 provides bivariate correlations for all study variables. Male youth were more likely to take stimulant medication on the day of the laboratory assessment. Female youth reported higher levels of dysregulated basic and complex emotion in daily life. Minority youth displayed higher baseline RSA. Higher baseline RSA was associated with RSA reactivity (greater withdrawal) and with more dysregulated behavior in daily life. Dysregulated basic and complex emotion were positively correlated, and higher levels of both were associated with dysregulated behavior in daily life.
RSA, respiratory sinus arrhythmia.
Significant associations are bolded, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, †p = 0.05.
RSA as a predictor of dysregulated emotion and behavior in daily life
Table 3 depicts results controlling for the effects of gender, minority status, and same-day stimulant use. RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict was associated with dysregulated basic emotion in daily life, with greater RSA withdrawal predicting higher levels of dysregulated basic emotion. There was also a significant interaction between baseline RSA and RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict in the prediction of dysregulated complex emotion and behavior in daily life.Footnote 5
RSA, respiratory sinus arrhythmia.
Note: Significant associations are bolded and represent effects after controlling for age, gender, minority status, and same-day stimulant use.
Post-hoc probing of these interactions revealed two distinct patterns of moderation. Simple slopes analyses showed that baseline RSA was related to dysregulated complex emotion (b = −0.30, s.e. = 0.07, p = 0.003) and dysregulated behavior (b = 0.62, s.e. = 0.25, p = 0.015) as a function of RSA withdrawal (−1 s.d.); in contrast, baseline RSA was unrelated to dysregulated complex emotion (b = 0.12, s.e. 0.15, p > 0.40) and dysregulated behavior (b = −0.13, s.e. = 0.18, p > 0.45) as a function of RSA augmentation (+1 s.d.). Region of significance analyses indicated that lower baseline RSA and greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict were associated with dysregulated complex emotion in daily life (Fig. 2) whereas higher baseline RSA and greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict were associated with dysregulated behavior in daily life (Fig. 3).
Discussion
The current study is the first to examine baseline RSA, RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict, and their interaction as predictors of dysregulated emotion and behavior in daily life. Importantly, these associations were examined using a transdiagnostic sample of clinically referred youth during the transition to adolescence, a particularly sensitive period characterized by normative increases in physiological reactivity and interpersonal conflict (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Hollenstein, Dishion and Patterson2003; Steinberg & Morris, Reference Steinberg and Morris2001). Results demonstrated that youth with greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict reported greater dysregulated basic emotion (sadness, anger, nervousness, stress) in daily life. Additionally, two interactions emerged such that baseline RSA was related to dysregulated complex emotion and dysregulated behavior as a function of RSA reactivity to conflict. Specifically, for youth experiencing greater RSA reactivity to parent-child conflict, lower baseline RSA was associated with dysregulated complex emotion (shame, guilt, loneliness, emptiness) in daily life while higher baseline RSA was associated with dysregulated behavior in daily life. Importantly, these effects were demonstrated within a multivariate structural equation model in which all possible associations between predictors and outcomes were examined simultaneously.
RSA reactivity was uniquely related to dysregulated emotion, namely variability in the intensity of basic emotions like anger, sadness, and anxiety. Youth with greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict were characterized by more extreme fluctuations in basic emotions in daily life. This is consistent with previous research linking large reductions in RSA during passive, emotionally evocative tasks to symptoms of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology (Beauchaine & Thayer, Reference Beauchaine and Thayer2015) and extends this work by illustrating a similar physiological response in the context of a more developmentally salient and ecologically-valid stressor (i.e. parent-child conflict). Additionally, our findings suggest that variability in the intensity of anger, sadness, and anxiety might explain the association between excessive RSA withdrawal and internalizing and externalizing disorders, in line with assertions that RSA reactivity underlies emotional lability specifically (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Beauchaine & Bell, Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Beauchaine and Crowell2019). While RSA withdrawal is a normative stress response that enables attention allocation and the mobilization of metabolic resources (Porges, Reference Porges2007), research suggests that the adaptiveness of this response is non-linear, and largely context-dependent (see Davis, Brooker, & Kahle, Reference Davis, Brooker and Kahle2020). Because of its association with the deployment of defensive action (i.e. fight or flight responding), excessive RSA withdrawal may be less adaptive within the context of everyday stressors, like interpersonal conflict, and may lower one's threshold for experiencing dysregulated emotion (Porges, Reference Porges2007). Overall, our findings suggest that a strong autonomic response to the conflict may serve as a marker for intense emotional ups and downs in daily life and indicate risk for various forms of psychopathology during the transition to adolescence.
We also observed two distinct interactions. First, youth with lower baseline RSA and greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict reported extreme variability in the intensity of complex emotion (shame, guilt, loneliness, emptiness) in daily life. This atypical pattern of RSA responding was one of the two profiles previously identified as a marker of depression risk (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2009; Yaroslavsky et al., Reference Yaroslavsky, Bylsma, Rottenberg and Kovacs2013, Reference Yaroslavsky, Rottenberg and Kovacs2014). The resemblance of our complex emotion results to findings in depression risk is not surprising, given that dysregulation of emotions like shame, guilt, and loneliness has been linked to internalizing disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This combination of low tonic RSA and excessive RSA withdrawal may have a synergistic impact on cardiac arousal that functions to deplete regulatory capacity, with vagal control over heart rate both chronically low and further removed under stress. Reduced tonic RSA is associated with increased sympathetic arousal (Porges, Reference Porges2007; Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, & Maiti, Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt and Maiti1994), and excessive RSA reactivity to conflict has been linked with sustained attention to emotional stimuli (Woody et al., Reference Woody, James, Foster, Owens, Feurer, Kudinova and Gibb2019); together, these processes might exaggerate the potency of emotional stimuli and impede the ability to access more adaptive executive resources.
The second interaction found higher baseline RSA and greater RSA withdrawal to parent-child conflict to predict dysregulated behavior (i.e. verbal and physical reactive aggression) in daily life, suggesting this profile increases risk for extreme behavioral expressions of dysregulation. This is consistent with research linking high resting RSA and excessive RSA reactivity to reactive forms of aggression (Zhang & Gao, Reference Zhang and Gao2015), and suggests that suddenly removing vagal control from a system ‘at rest’ might signal the presence of threat. This is in line with theoretical conceptualizations of reactive aggression as a threat response that occurs at the height of a freeze-flight-fight defense cascade (Gray, Reference Gray1987). While this finding appears to stand in contrast to literature linking low tonic RSA to psychopathology (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Bell, Knapton, McDonough-Caplan, Shader and Zisner2019; Fanti et al., Reference Fanti, Eisenbarth, Goble, Demetriou, Kyranides, Goodwin and Cortese2019), it echoes the work demonstrating a robust link between low resting heart rate and aggression across development (Jennings, Pardini, & Matthews, Reference Jennings, Pardini and Matthews2017; Ortiz & Raine, Reference Ortiz and Raine2004). Although we failed to find a main effect of baseline RSA on dysregulation in daily life, the high inverse correlation between heart rate and RSA suggests future work may seek to disentangle these associations. Taken together, a nuanced picture emerges, whereby youth with the low-baseline-plus-excessive-withdrawal combination experienced more intense (complex) emotional upheavals, an endophenotype that is more consistent with internalizing pathology. By contrast, youth characterized by high-baseline-plus-excessive-withdrawal were more likely to display extreme behavioral dysregulation like verbal and physical reactive aggression, an endophenotype more consistent with externalizing pathology.Footnote 6 Combined findings suggest that, among disordered youth, baseline RSA may be neither universally ‘adaptive’ or ‘maladaptive’ on its own, and instead underscore the importance of also assessing one's physiological response to stress. Along these lines, youth with greater RSA withdrawal to conflict showed extreme fluctuations of basic emotions, an endophenotype shared across internalizing and externalizing pathologies. This echoesresearch suggesting that high rates of comorbidity among internalizing andexternalizing disorders are associated with greater RSA withdrawal than either disorder alone (Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Pang & Beauchaine, Reference Pang and Beauchaine2013). Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of continuing to examine the within individual interplay of tonic and phasic RSA among transdiagnostic samples of youth.
Findings from the current study should be considered within the context of several limitations. First, the current study used a transdiagnostic sample of clinically referred youth, limiting our ability to make any definitive conclusions about the extent to which the physiological profiles identified in the current sample generalize to community samples of typically developing youth. While this reflects our primary aim to examine associations between physiological profiles and transdiagnostic indicators of psychopathology as they manifest in daily life and echoes research highlighting the importance of assessing these association within clinical populations (see Graziano & Derefinko, Reference Graziano and Derefinko2013), future inclusion of a comparison group subjected to the same experimental tasks would be useful. Second, while several important covariates (i.e. age, gender, minority status, stimulant medication use) were included in the model, our study was not adequately powered to examine potential moderation and future work may assess how these factors influence findings. Additionally, while our assessment of peripheral physiology expands on previous laboratory-based assessments (i.e. passive viewing of emotionally evocative stimuli) by utilizing dyadic interactions, we recognize this approach limits internal validity and acknowledge the potential benefits of utilizing standardized methods across studies (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Brooker and Kahle2020). To balance the need for internal and external validity we standardized many aspects of the interaction. First, vanilla baselines were comprised of uniform tasks (e.g. reading, listening, thinking) in attempt to provide a more stable and reliable comparison condition (Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Kamarck, Stewart, Eddy and Johnson1992), and allow a more precise estimate of physiological reactivity during the experimental task of interest. Second, discussion topics were selected from a standard list based on frequency and severity ratings, and the length of discussion was consistent across dyads; however, dyadic scripts were not standardized to promote spontaneously conversation, maximizing ecological validity. Finally, our physiological assessment did not explicitly measure respiration; thus, we cannot determine whether respiration influenced our findings. Additionally, although we focused on RSA as an indicator of parasympathetic function, we understand that other autonomic influences are likely at play. Future studies may seek to investigate alternative physiological indices (i.e. impedance cardiography) in isolation and in conjunction with RSA to more comprehensively examine autonomic processes and their relationship to psychopathology.
The current study broadens our understanding of RSA as a biomarker for dysregulated emotion and behavior. Specifically, results point to excessive RSA withdrawal to conflict as a risk factor for extreme fluctuations in basic emotions like anger, sadness, and anxiety in daily life. Furthermore, findings bridge a gap in the literature by examining the interaction between baseline RSA and RSA reactivity in a transdiagnostic sample of pre-adolescent youth. Results highlight two unique physiological profiles, both characterized by excessive RSA withdrawal to conflict, that increase risk for dysregulated complex emotion and dysregulated behavior in daily life. These physiological profiles were identified in clinically referred youth and suggest that intervention strategies targeting reactivity to stress, specifically interpersonal conflict, may be particularly useful in helping to reduce symptoms associated with dysregulated emotion and behavior in this population. Finally, our findings highlight the potential importance of assessment and intervention during the critical transition to adolescence, a vulnerable developmental period before severe forms of psychopathology become entrenched.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all the families who took part in this study, and to the MoodY study team, which includes interviewers and their supervisors, data managers, student workers, and volunteers. This study was supported by grants awarded to Dr. Stephanie Stepp and Dr. Amy Byrd from the National Institute on Mental Health (R01 MH101088, F32 MH110077). Additional funding from the National Institute of Health also supported this work (T32 MH018951, K01 MH109859).
Conflict of interest
None.