Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:58:40.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of oxytocin in empathy to the pain of conflictual out-group members among patients with schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 April 2014

A. Abu-Akel*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
M. Fischer-Shofty
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Israel
Y. Levkovitz
Affiliation:
The Emotion-Cognition Research Center, Shalvata Mental Health Care Center, Hod-Hasharon, Israel
J. Decety
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, USA
S. Shamay-Tsoory
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Israel
*
* Address for correspondence: A. Abu-Akel, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK. (Email: ama289@bham.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

Oxytocin (OT) is associated with our ability to empathize and has been shown to play a major role in mediating social behaviors within the context of intergroup dynamics. Schizophrenia is associated with impaired empathy, and with a dysfunctional oxytocinergic system. The effect of OT on the empathic responses of patients with schizophrenia within the context of intergroup relationships has not been studied. The present study examined the effect of OT on the patients' empathic responses to pain experienced by in-group, conflictual out-group and neutral out-group members.

Method.

In a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subject cross-over design, the responses on the Pain Evaluation Task of 28 male patients with schizophrenia were compared to 27 healthy male controls. All participants received a single intranasal dose of 24 IU OT or placebo, 1 week apart.

Results.

OT induced an empathy bias in the healthy controls towards the conflictual out-group members. Although this effect was absent in the patient group, OT seems to heighten an empathic bias in the patient group towards the in-group members when rating non-painful stimuli.

Conclusions.

The study demonstrates that the administration of OT can result in empathic bias towards adversary out-group members in healthy controls but not in patients with schizophrenia. However, the OT-induced bias in both the patients (in the no-pain condition towards the in-group members) and the healthy controls (in the no-pain and pain conditions towards the adversary out-group) suggests that OT enhances the distinction between conflictual in-group and out-group members.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Introduction

Empathy can generally be defined as an affective response to another's individual distress, which requires an accurate understanding of another's mental state or frame of reference (Batson et al. Reference Batson, Early and Salvarani1987). In addition to its association with a myriad of sociocognitive dysfunctions, schizophrenia is associated with impaired empathy (Abu-Akel & Abushua'leh, Reference Abu-Akel and Abushua'leh2004; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Gokcen and Veznedaroglu2008; Derntl et al. Reference Derntl, Finkelmeyer, Toygar, Hulsmann, Schneider, Falkenberg and Habel2009; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Horan, Cobia, Karpouzian, Fox, Reilly and Breiter2013). For example, Bora et al. (Reference Bora, Gokcen and Veznedaroglu2008) reported that, compared to healthy controls, patients with schizophrenia exhibited severe empathy dysfunctions as measured by the Empathy Quotient questionnaire (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright2004). They also found impairments in the ability of the patients with schizophrenia to recognize and reason about emotions, which are considered core components of empathy (Blair, Reference Blair2005; Shamay-Tsoory, Reference Shamay-Tsoory2011). Similarly, Derntl et al. (Reference Derntl, Finkelmeyer, Toygar, Hulsmann, Schneider, Falkenberg and Habel2009) found that patients with schizophrenia exhibited a generalized deficit in all core components of empathy, including emotion recognition, perspective taking and affective responsiveness. More recently, Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Zaki, Harvey, Ochsner and Green2011) investigated empathic accuracy (defined as the ability to accurately infer the affective state of another person) and reported that patients with schizophrenia not only have lower empathic accuracy compared to controls but also are less sensitive to the social cues of others when making those judgments.

Several studies have suggested that the neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) mediates empathy (Rosenfeld et al. Reference Rosenfeld, Lieberman and Jarskog2011; Striepens et al. Reference Striepens, Kendrick, Maier and Hurlemann2011). This has been demonstrated in both healthy (Hurlemann et al. Reference Hurlemann, Patin, Onur, Cohen, Baumgartner, Metzler, Dziobek, Gallinat, Wagner, Maier and Kendrick2010) and pathological populations such as those with autism (Guastella et al. Reference Guastella, Einfeld, Gray, Rinehart, Tonge, Lambert and Hickie2010). Studies have also shown abnormal levels of OT in the plasma (Goldman et al. Reference Goldman, Marlow-O'Connor, Torres and Carter2008) and cerebrospinal fluid (Linkowski et al. Reference Linkowski, Geenen, Kerkhofs, Mendlewicz and Legros1984; Beckmann et al. Reference Beckmann, Lang and Gattaz1985; Legros et al. Reference Legros, Gazzotti, Carvelli, Franchimont, Timsit-Berthier, von Frenckell and Ansseau1992) of patients with schizophrenia and, more recently, variations in the OT receptor gene have been linked to schizophrenia (Souza et al. Reference Souza, Ismail, Meltzer and Kennedy2010; Montag et al. Reference Montag, Brockmann, Bayerl, Rujescu, Muller and Gallinat2012a , Reference Montag, Brockmann, Lehmann, Muller, Rujescu and Gallinat b ). Because of these associations, research concerned with the therapeutic potential of OT in schizophrenia has intensified over the past few years (Davis et al. Reference Davis, Lee, Horan, Clarke, McGee, Green and Marder2013). Findings suggest that endogenous OT levels are significantly associated with social functioning (Keri et al. Reference Keri, Kiss and Kelemen2009; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Carter, Drogos, Pournajafi-Nazarloo, Sweeney and Maki2010; Sasayama et al. Reference Sasayama, Hattori, Teraishi, Hori, Ota, Yoshida, Arima, Higuchi, Amano and Kunugi2012) and that the administration of OT improves symptomatology (Bujanow, Reference Bujanow1974; Caldwell et al. Reference Caldwell, Stephens and Young2009; Feifel et al. Reference Feifel, Macdonald, Nguyen, Cobb, Warlan, Galangue, Minassian, Becker, Cooper, Perry, Lefebvre, Gonzales and Hadley2010; Pedersen et al. Reference Pedersen, Gibson, Rau, Salimi, Smedley, Casey, Leserman, Jarskog and Penn2011), social perception (Fischer-Shofty et al. Reference Fischer-Shofty, Brune, Ebert, Shefet, Levkovitz and Shamay-Tsoory2013) and verbal memory (Feifel et al. Reference Feifel, Macdonald, Cobb and Minassian2012). In addition, OT has been shown to improve emotion recognition (Averbeck et al. Reference Averbeck, Bobin, Evans and Shergill2012) and theory of mind abilities (Pedersen et al. Reference Pedersen, Gibson, Rau, Salimi, Smedley, Casey, Leserman, Jarskog and Penn2011). These associations suggest that OT might have a bearing on both the psychopathology and sociocognitive functioning of individuals with schizophrenia. Thus, in considering the general role of OT in social interaction and empathy, the established abnormalities of individuals with schizophrenia in sociocognitive and empathic abilities and the positive normalizing effect of OT on various aspects of social dysfunction seen in schizophrenia, it can be hypothesized that the normalizing effect of OT in schizophrenia can also be extended to empathy.

One of the most rudimentary empathy mechanisms is that of empathy to pain, a concept that describes our tendency to automatically experience distress when facing someone else's pain. Previous human imaging studies focusing on empathy for others' pain have consistently shown activations in regions also involved in direct pain experience (Jackson & Decety, Reference Jackson and Decety2004; Singer et al. Reference Singer, Seymour, O'Doherty, Kaube, Dolan and Frith2004; Decety & Lamm, Reference Decety and Lamm2006). Specifically, a neural network including the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula was reported to respond to both felt and observed pain (Decety et al. Reference Decety, Echols and Correll2010). The same sets of regions have been reliably observed across a wide range of individuals and circumstances, suggesting that empathy to pain is, at least in part, an automatic, bottom-up process and perhaps an evolved adaptation (Decety & Svetlova, Reference Decety and Svetlova2012). However, research strongly suggests that empathy is also mediated by top-down processing. Indeed, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the empathic response to pain is either strengthened or weakened when contextual and interpersonal variables are manipulated, including the intent of the inflictor of pain to harm the target (Akitsuki & Decety, Reference Akitsuki and Decety2009), and whether the person in pain belongs to a stigmatized group (Tarrant et al. Reference Tarrant, Dazeley and Cottom2009; Decety et al. Reference Decety, Echols and Correll2010). This suggests that empathy to pain is also modulated by top-down processes such as group membership. Although this ‘in-group empathy bias’ has been repeatedly reported in healthy participants (Trawalter et al. Reference Trawalter, Hoffman and Waytz2012), it is not known whether patients with schizophrenia also exhibit this bias.

The role of OT and empathy has been extensively researched in the context of intergroup relationships in healthy controls, showing that the administration of OT favorably promotes empathy towards in-group members (De Dreu et al. Reference De Dreu, Greer, Van Kleef, Shalvi and Handgraaf2011). The work of De Dreu et al. (Reference De Dreu, Greer, Handgraaf, Shalvi, Van Kleef, Baas, Ten Velden, Van Dijk and Feith2010, Reference De Dreu, Greer, Van Kleef, Shalvi and Handgraaf2011) has also shown that raising OT levels can lead to negative reactions towards out-group members such as out-group derogations and defensive aggression. To our knowledge, the effect of the administration of OT on such bias among patients with schizophrenia has not been investigated. In addition, we are not aware of any studies that have examined whether patients with schizophrenia have inherent social biases towards in-group versus out-group members. With respect to studying the effect of OT on empathy in schizophrenia, we are aware of only one study (Davis et al. Reference Davis, Lee, Horan, Clarke, McGee, Green and Marder2013). In this double-blind, placebo-controlled study, the administration of a single dose of 40 IU intranasal OT did not improve the patients' empathic responses as measured by the Emotional Perspective Taking Task (EPTT; Derntl et al. Reference Derntl, Finkelmeyer, Toygar, Hulsmann, Schneider, Falkenberg and Habel2009), in which participants were required to infer the emotional expression of a masked-face individual taking part in a two-person social interaction. Of note, however, the authors reported a positive effect on the composite score of several higher-level processes, including the detection of sarcasm and deception in addition to empathy.

Accordingly, using a double-blind cross-over design, we sought to examine in a group of individuals with schizophrenia the effect of intranasal administration of OT on their empathic responses to the pain of others. A unique aspect of our study is that we tested the influence of OT on the empathic responses of patients within the context of the Israeli–Palestinian conflict in Israel. This allowed us to examine, in an ecologically valid environment, the role of this neuropeptide in modulating the patients' empathic responses to members of a group who are part of a protracted intergroup violent conflict. Accordingly, this study evaluated whether patients with schizophrenia have a group bias, often exhibited by healthy participants, towards in-group versus out-group members, and the extent to which this bias, or lack thereof, is moderated by the administration of intranasal OT.

Method

Participants

Twenty-eight healthy men, ranging in age from 20 to 37 years (mean age = 27.43 years, s.d. = 3.44), and 28 male patients with schizophrenia, ranging in age from 22 to 45 years (mean age = 32.21 years, s.d. = 6.05) were included in the study. Because of known significant hormonal interactions between OT and estrogen in women (McCarthy, Reference McCarthy1995), an all-male sample eliminates this possible confound. During the course of the experiment, one participant from the control group dropped out, bringing the final sample to a total of 55 participants. The data reported here are thus for 27 healthy participants and 28 patients with schizophrenia. All participants were Israeli Jews and living in Israel at the time of assessment. Healthy participants were recruited through advertisement posted across campus at the University of Haifa, and the patients were recruited from out-patient and day clinics. Diagnosis of schizophrenia was based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) and confirmed by two psychiatrists. The individuals with schizophrenia were stabilized on fixed doses of an equivalent antipsychotic medication with or without trihexyphenidyl (Artane, 5–0 mg/day) or biperiden (Dekinet, 2–4 mg/day). Exclusion criteria were acute, unstable, significant or untreated medical illnesses (including arrhythmia, psychiatric conditions and head injury), mental retardation (IQ < 75), and disturbances in visuomotor coordination. The study was approved by the National Institutional Review Board of Israel and all participants gave their signed informed consent.

Clinical assessment

The patients with schizophrenia were assessed by a clinical psychologist with validated clinical tests suitable for evaluating their condition and the severity of specific symptoms. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al. Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1987) was used to evaluate the presence/absence and severity of positive, negative and general psychopathology. This was assessed during the screening session. The Clinical Global Impression (CGI) scale was used to estimate the severity of each patient's illness. This was administered during the first and second sessions of the treatment protocol. Finally, the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS; Shipley, Reference Shipley1940) was used to assess the intellectual abilities of the participants.

After giving their oral and written informed consent, the participants were instructed to avoid using psychotropic substances (e.g. caffeine and nicotine) for at least 12 h prior to the experiment.

Treatment administration

A double-blind, within-subject, cross-over design was used, with participants randomly assigned to groups for the first administration of either OT or placebo 45 min prior to performing the behavioral task. Those initially receiving OT were administered a single intranasal dose of 24 IU (Syntocinon-Spray, Novartis, Switzerland) by three puffs in each nostril (each puff contains 4 IU). The placebo contained all inactive ingredients without the neuropeptide. Seven days later, at the second session of the experiment, participants underwent the same procedure with the other substance (i.e. placebo or OT). Previous studies show that the elevation of OT following intranasal administration (24 IU) usually lasts for only 1–2 h (Born et al. Reference Born, Lange, Kern, McGregor, Bickel and Fehm2002; Gossen et al. Reference Gossen, Hahn, Westphal, Prinz, Schultz, Grunder and Spreckelmeyer2012). Hence, it was safe to assume that there was no carry-over effect between the two sessions.

Assessment of empathy to pain: the Pain Evaluation Task

In this study, we used the Pain Evaluation Task designed by Jackson et al. (Reference Jackson, Meltzoff and Decety2005). We chose this task because previous research using this task and similar empathy-for-pain paradigms has shown that it reliably induces an empathy bias towards in-group compared to out-group members (Hein et al. Reference Hein, Silani, Preuschoff, Batson and Singer2010; Shamay-Tsoory et al. Reference Shamay-Tsoory, Abu-Akel, Palgi, Sulieman, Fischer-Shofty, Levkovitz and Decety2013). The task consists of showing a series of digital color photographs depicting right hands and right feet in painful and non-painful situations as follows: (1) right hands in painful situations, (2) right hands in neutral situations, (3) right feet in painful situations, and (4) right feet in neutral situations. All situations depict familiar events that can occur in everyday life. Various types of pain (mechanical, thermal and pressure) are represented. For each painful situation there is a corresponding neutral picture that involved the same setting without any painful component. Stimuli were presented randomly, following a 750-ms presentation of different common names of Jews (representing the in-group members), Palestinian Arabs (representing the adversary out-group members) or Europeans (representing the neutral out-group members). Following each name, a picture showing either a painful or a non-painful situation was presented (see Fig. 1). Participants were then asked to rate, as quickly as possible, the intensity of the pain experienced by the target using a visual analog scale (VAS) using the computer mouse (from 0 = no pain to 10 = most painful). The experiment consisted of 30 trials in total (15 painful and 15 non-painful stimuli). The same name was always tagged with the same picture in all sessions, but the order of presentation of the pain and no-pain stimuli were randomized separately for each participant. The task began with three practice trials, followed by the test blocks. The task was administered using E-prime 2.1 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., USA).

Fig. 1. Sample presentation of a right hand in a pain and no-pain situation.

Selection of the protagonist names

The selection of the targets' names was based on a preliminary study in which 120 students from the University of Haifa (60 Palestinian Arabs and 60 Jews), all Israeli citizens, were asked to report what they believe are the five most common names of Palestinian Arabs, Jews and Europeans. From a total list of 96 names, five names with the highest frequency were selected for each group. Because, in the current study, all the participants were Israeli Jews, we only used the prototypical names reported by the Jewish students. Accordingly, the stereotypical Jewish names were Moshe, Avi, Yits'hak, Yesrael and Shimon. The Arab names were Ahmad, Mohammed, Abed, Saleem and Ali. The European (neutral out-group) names were Chris, John, Mark, Martin and Paul. The use of names to prime group membership is motivated by studies showing that the presentation of prototypical names is sufficient to prime affiliative biases of in-group and out-group membership (De Dreu et al. Reference De Dreu, Greer, Van Kleef, Shalvi and Handgraaf2011; Bruneau et al. Reference Bruneau, Dufour and Saxe2012; Shamay-Tsoory et al. Reference Shamay-Tsoory, Abu-Akel, Palgi, Sulieman, Fischer-Shofty, Levkovitz and Decety2013).

Statistical approach and analysis

The double-blind, placebo-controlled design adopted in the current study enabled us to assess within and across groups the effect of OT versus placebo on the empathy rating of perceived pain when viewing in-group, adversary or neutral out-group members (primed by their respective prototypical names) in painful and non-painful familiar situations. The current study thus used a 2 × 3 × 2 × 2 design, where treatment (placebo versus OT), target (Jew versus Palestinian Arab versus European) and stimuli (painful versus non-painful) were entered as within-subject factors and patients versus healthy controls as between-subjects factors. In addition, independent-sample t tests and correlation analyses were carried out. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS version 21 (SPSS Inc., USA).

Results

Independent-sample t tests indicated that the control and patient groups differed in age (t 53 = −3.54, p = 0.001) but not intelligence (t 53 = 1.74, p = 0.09). None of the empathy responses of both groups correlated with either the intelligence score or age. In addition, none of the empathy responses within the schizophrenia group correlated with age of disease onset, duration of illness, positive or negative symptoms, or general psychopathology. There were also no associations between the patients' responses and severity of illness as measured by the CGI scale during the first or the second session. Moreover, there was no difference in the patients' severity of illness during the first versus the second session. The clinical and demographic data for the patient and healthy control groups are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Clinical and demographic data of the patient and healthy control groups

SILS, Shipley Institute of Living Scale; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; CGI, Clinical Global Impression.

Values given as mean ± standard deviation.

Effect of OT on empathy to pain

The full model of the study design included three within-subject variables (target, pain condition and treatment) and one between-subjects variable (group). To examine the interaction between treatment (placebo/OT), target (Arab/European/Jew), pain condition (pain/no-pain) and group (control/schizophrenia), a four-way (2 × 3 × 2 × 2) repeated-measures ANOVA was performed. The results show a non-significant treatment effect (F 1,53 = 1.02, p = 0.32), a non-significant target effect (F 2,106 = 0.75, p = 0.47) and a non-significant group effect (F 1,53 = 0.45, p = 0.51). The pain condition effect was highly significant (F 1,53 = 48.50, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.90), indicating that overall the ratings for the painful stimuli were higher than the non-painful stimuli. Furthermore, the two-way interactions of group × treatment (F 1,53 = 2.52, p = 0.12) and target × group (F 2,106 = 2.75, p = 0.07) were also not significant. Importantly, the three-way interaction of treatment × target × group (F 2,106 = 4.05, p = 0.020, η p 2 = 0.07) was significant, indicating that the treatment affected the groups differently when judging the stimuli associated with the different targets. The within-subjects contrast for this interaction is linearly significant (F 1,53 = 8.17, p = 0.006, η p 2 = 0.13), suggesting that the participants' ratings across the three targets followed a linear trend. The four-way interaction of treatment × target × condition × group was not significant (F 2,106 = 0.260, p = 0.772).

To further explore the source of the three-way interaction, we carried out separate repeated-measures ANOVAs for each group. Using a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, we examined the interaction between treatment (placebo/OT), target (Arab/European/Jew) and pain condition (pain/no-pain) within the healthy controls. As shown in Fig. 2, a significant treatment by target effect (F 2,53 = 6.74, p = 0.002, η p 2 = 0.21) indicated that, in the healthy controls, the treatment differentially affected the pain ratings of the different target groups. Although the pain condition was highly significant (F 1,26 = 183.95, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.88), indicating higher pain ratings in the pain versus no-pain condition, the three-way interaction of treatment × target × pain condition was not significant (F 2,52 = 0.66, p = 0.52). The target (F 2,52 = 0.434, p = 0.65) and treatment (F 2,52 = 2.62, p = 0.12) effects were also not significant. To further explore the source of the two-way treatment by target interaction, follow-up t tests were carried out. This analysis indicated that OT had a significant effect on increasing the pain rating for the Arab protagonists (t 26 = 2.06, p = 0.049) but it did not affect the pain rating for the European (t 26 = − 0.59, p = 0.56) or Jewish targets (t 26 = − 0.56, p = 0.58). Of note, similarly to the pain ratings, in the no-pain condition, OT had a significant effect on increasing the empathy to pain ratings for the Arab targets (t 26 = 3.27, p = 0.003) and for the European targets (t 26 = 2.31, p = 0.029) but non-significantly for the Jewish targets (t 26 = 1.37, p = 0.18).

Fig. 2. Mean empathic ratings (± standard error) of the healthy controls as a function of treatment condition (placebo versus oxytocin), pain condition (pain versus no-pain) and target (Arabs versus Europeans versus Jews). VAS, Visual analog scale.

Within the patient group, we similarly examined the interaction between treatment (placebo/OT), target (Arab/European/Jew) and pain condition (pain/no-pain) using a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA. As shown in Fig. 3, a non-significant treatment by target effect (F 2,54 = 0.82, p = 0.45) indicated that, in the patient group, the treatment did not differentially affect their empathy to the pain of the different targets. Although the pain condition was highly significant (F 1,27 = 318.07, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.92), indicating higher pain ratings in the pain versus no-pain condition, the three-way interaction of treatment × target × pain condition was not significant (F 2,54 = 0.12, p = 0.89). The target (F 2,54 = 2.70, p = 0.11) and treatment (F 2,54 = 0.23, p = 0.64) effects were also not significant.

Fig. 3. Mean empathic ratings (± standard error) of the patients with schizophrenia as a function of treatment condition (placebo versus oxytocin), pain condition (pain versus no-pain) and target (Arabs versus Europeans versus Jews). VAS, Visual analog scale.

When examining the within-subject contrasts reported above, no significant differences were discerned across the targets in the placebo condition for both the painful and non-painful stimuli. However, under the OT condition, the patients, but not the controls, rated the non-painful stimuli differently across the targets. Paired t tests reveal that the Jewish targets received significantly higher ratings than both the Arab targets (t 27 = 3.17, p = 0.004) and the European targets (t 27 = 2.38, p = 0.024). The European targets also received higher rating than the Arab targets, but the difference did not reach significance (t 27 = 1.34, p = 0.19).

Discussion

The current study sought to investigate the effect of OT on the empathic responses of male patients with schizophrenia and male healthy controls within the context of intergroup relationships. The healthy and the patient groups behaved differently in their empathic responses towards the conflictual group members as a function of OT administration. Within the controls, OT induced a significant empathy bias towards the Arab targets in the painful condition. A similar but a more robust bias towards the Arab targets was also observed under the OT condition when rating non-painful stimuli. Within the schizophrenia group, the empathy ratings of the targets did not differ as a function of treatment condition when evaluating others in pain. However, OT seems to heighten the bias within the patients when rating non-painful stimuli. Under this condition, the patients significantly conferred higher empathy ratings to the Jewish targets (i.e. the in-group members), followed by the European targets and then the Arab targets. Thus it seems that OT exerts opposing effects on the rating of non-painful stimuli within the healthy and patient groups; although OT increased the level of empathy in the healthy participants towards out-group members, it increased the bias among the patients towards the in-group members.

From a clinical perspective, OT did not increase the patients' empathic responses in the painful condition. This finding partially overlaps with the study by Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Lee, Horan, Clarke, McGee, Green and Marder2013), who reported that OT did not improve the empathic responses of their patients as measured by the EPTT. However, as noted earlier, that study found a positive effect on a composite score of several higher-level cognitive processes, including empathy. The absence of the effect of OT among the patients can be attributed to variations within the patients' neurochemical profile with which OT interacts, including variation in endogenous OT and the possible modulatory effect it may have on exogenous OT (Bartz et al. Reference Bartz, Zaki, Bolger and Ochsner2011). As such, the lack of a baseline measure of endogenous OT levels is a limitation in our study that should be addressed in future studies. Another possibility is that the administration of one dose of OT may not be sufficient to induce an effect on complex sociocognitive abilities, such as empathy, in the patient group. It is conceivable that a higher dosage and/or a protracted regimen of daily intranasal OT administration may be necessary for such an effect to transpire (Feifel et al. Reference Feifel, Macdonald, Nguyen, Cobb, Warlan, Galangue, Minassian, Becker, Cooper, Perry, Lefebvre, Gonzales and Hadley2010; Pedersen et al. Reference Pedersen, Gibson, Rau, Salimi, Smedley, Casey, Leserman, Jarskog and Penn2011).

A surpising finding is that OT increased empathic responses towards the non-painful stimuli, albeit in opposite trends, in both the patient and healthy participants. Although the opposing effect in terms of the in-group bias (in the patient group) versus the out-group bias (in the heathy controls) is open to speculation, our findings suggest that increasing OT levels facilitate social categorization of others as in-group versus out-group (Kavaliers & Choleris, Reference Kavaliers and Choleris2011; De Dreu, Reference De Dreu2012). Moreover, the enhanced empathic response towards non-painful stimuli after the administration of OT might be a consequence of an overdrive of endogenous OT leading to hyper-vigilance/sensitivity. This may in turn have led participants to attribute social meaning to visual information with when none is required. This is akin to inducing some form of hyper-mentalism, which is a common feature in schizophrenia present with positive symptoms (Abu-Akel & Bailey, Reference Abu-Akel and Bailey2000; Frith, Reference Frith2004; Abu-Akel, Reference Abu-Akel2008; Montag et al. Reference Montag, Dziobek, Richter, Neuhaus, Lehmann, Sylla, Heekeren, Heinz and Gallinat2011; Walss-Bass et al. Reference Walss-Bass, Fernandes, Roberts, Service and Velligan2013). Another explanation is that both pain and non-pain stimuli rely on similar neural substrates including the thalamus, insula and anterior cingulate cortex (Mouraux et al. Reference Mouraux, Diukova, Lee, Wise and Iannetti2011; Hayes & Northoff, Reference Hayes and Northoff2012), whose activity correlates significantly with the perceived saliency of the stimulus (Mouraux et al. Reference Mouraux, Diukova, Lee, Wise and Iannetti2011). Speculatively, the modulation of the oxytocinergic system of the regional, along with the connectivity between these different regions (Bethlehem et al. Reference Bethlehem, van Honk, Auyeung and Baron-Cohen2013), may explain the increased ratings we observe for both the painful and non-painful stimuli.

Within the healthy controls, there was an increase in the pain ratings to both painful and non-painful stimuli in the OT condition, which suggests that OT may have a general effect on pain perception. This finding is commensurate with evidence suggesting that OT potently affects the nociceptive system when administered directly (Juif & Poisbeau, Reference Juif and Poisbeau2013). Moreover, our study can inform research using intergroup conflict paradigms, particularly in light of the finding that OT did not lead the healthy participants to change their overall ratings towards their in-group members. Indeed, our finding suggests that OT can induce an empathic response towards out-group members even if the out-group members are part of a social group with which the participants are engaged in a protracted political and violent conflict. As such, our finding does not support claims suggesting that OT promotes, in healthy controls, out-group derogation or enhances in-group bias (De Dreu et al. Reference De Dreu, Greer, Handgraaf, Shalvi, Van Kleef, Baas, Ten Velden, Van Dijk and Feith2010, Reference De Dreu, Greer, Van Kleef, Shalvi and Handgraaf2011), and replicates our earlier results in this regard (Shamay-Tsoory et al. Reference Shamay-Tsoory, Abu-Akel, Palgi, Sulieman, Fischer-Shofty, Levkovitz and Decety2013). This finding might be explained within the framework of prosocial theories positing that trust and concern for others tend to increase towards others in anticipation of future interaction (Komorita & Parks, Reference Komorita and Parks1995). It is conceivable that the administration of OT increases the salience of this likely interaction, which could explain why the effect was only observed when rating the Palestinian Arabs (with whom there is a greater likelihood of interaction) and not the Europeans. Other possible interpretations pertain to the role of OT in increasing approach-related behaviors (Kemp & Guastella, Reference Kemp and Guastella2011), abolishing negative affect (Petrovic et al. Reference Petrovic, Kalisch, Singer and Dolan2008) and increasing attention to socially relevant information (Leknes et al. Reference Leknes, Wessberg, Ellingsen, Chelnokova, Olausson and Laeng2013), which together constitute mechanisms that could facilitate increased empathic responses towards others. The selectivity of OT to increasing the empathic response towards the out-group rather than the in-group suggests that, in certain contexts, exogenous OT may not necessarily have an additive value to what is already salient to the individual (i.e. the in-group member). In this regard there is evidence showing that the administration of OT to individuals with high emotional sensitivity afforded little or no improvement in detecting subtle social cues (Leknes et al. Reference Leknes, Wessberg, Ellingsen, Chelnokova, Olausson and Laeng2013). Thus, our finding provides further support for the view suggesting that the effect of OT on sociocognitive abilities is not uniform and is susceptible to changes in context, whether it is the environment or the persons with whom we interact (Bartz et al. Reference Bartz, Zaki, Bolger and Ochsner2011).

To conclude, this study demonstrates that the administration of OT induces an empathic bias towards the adversary out-group members in healthy male controls but not in male individuals with schizophrenia. This suggests that OT, within the context of intergroup conflict, does not affect the patients' empathic judgments of either neutral or adversary out-group members. In this context we should note that the reliance on an all-male sample may limit the generalizability of our findings, particularly in light of evidence suggesting that male and female individuals with schizophrenia differ in baseline OT levels and OT response (Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Carter, Drogos, Pournajafi-Nazarloo, Sweeney and Maki2010), and also in their affective mentalizing abilities (Abu-Akel & Bo, Reference Abu-Akel and Bo2013). However, OT does seem to enhance the distinction between the conflictual in-group and out-group in both the healthy and patient groups. This is evidenced by the OT-induced bias in both the patient (in the no-pain condition towards the in-group members) and the healthy control groups (in both the pain and no-pain conditions towards the adversary out-group members), and the absence of such an effect towards the neutral European group. This finding thus underscores the importance of using both neutral and conflict groups in future studies concerned with intergroup dynamics (see also Bruneau et al. Reference Bruneau, Dufour and Saxe2012). The effect of OT on enhancing the salience and/or social relevance of non-painful stimuli is intriguing and warrants further investigation, particularly within the framework of the ‘aberrant salience’ hypothesis of schizophrenia (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Binational Science Foundation (BSF). J.D. was supported by a grant from the John Templeton Foundation.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Abu-Akel, A (2008). Theory of mind in autism, schizophrenia, and in-between. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, 261262.Google Scholar
Abu-Akel, A, Abushua'leh, K (2004). ‘Theory of mind’ in violent and nonviolent patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 69, 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abu-Akel, A, Bailey, AL (2000). The possibility of different forms of theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. Psychological Medicine 30, 735738.Google Scholar
Abu-Akel, A, Bo, H (2013). Superior mentalizing abilities of female patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 210, 794799.Google Scholar
Akitsuki, Y, Decety, J (2009). Social context and perceived agency affects empathy for pain: an event-related fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 47, 722734.Google Scholar
Averbeck, BB, Bobin, T, Evans, S, Shergill, SS (2012). Emotion recognition and oxytocin in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 42, 259266.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S (2004). The Empathy Quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 163175.Google Scholar
Bartz, JA, Zaki, J, Bolger, N, Ochsner, KN (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 301309.Google Scholar
Batson, CD, Early, S, Salvarani, G (1987). Perspective taking: imagining how another feels versus imaging how you would feel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23, 751758.Google Scholar
Beckmann, H, Lang, RE, Gattaz, WF (1985). Vasopressin–oxytocin in cerebrospinal fluid of schizophrenic patients and normal controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 10, 187191.Google Scholar
Bethlehem, RA, van Honk, J, Auyeung, B, Baron-Cohen, S (2013). Oxytocin, brain physiology, and functional connectivity: a review of intranasal oxytocin fMRI studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 962974.Google Scholar
Blair, RJR (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition 15, 698718.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Gokcen, S, Veznedaroglu, B (2008). Empathic abilities in people with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 160, 2329.Google Scholar
Born, J, Lange, T, Kern, W, McGregor, GP, Bickel, U, Fehm, HL (2002). Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 5, 514516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruneau, EG, Dufour, N, Saxe, R (2012). Social cognition in members of conflict groups: behavioural and neural responses in Arabs, Israelis and South Americans to each other's misfortunes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. Series B, Biological Sciences 367, 717730.Google Scholar
Bujanow, W (1974). Letter: Is oxytocin an anti-schizophrenic hormone? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 19, 323.Google Scholar
Caldwell, HK, Stephens, SL, Young, WS 3rd (2009). Oxytocin as a natural antipsychotic: a study using oxytocin knockout mice. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, MC, Lee, J, Horan, WP, Clarke, AD, McGee, MR, Green, MF, Marder, SR (2013). Effects of single dose intranasal oxytocin on social cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 147, 393397.Google Scholar
Decety, J, Echols, S, Correll, J (2010). The blame game: the effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, 985997.Google Scholar
Decety, J, Lamm, C (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Scientific World Journal 6, 11461163.Google Scholar
Decety, J, Svetlova, M (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2, 124.Google Scholar
De Dreu, CK (2012). Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: an integrative review and research agenda. Hormones and Behavior 61, 419428.Google Scholar
De Dreu, CK, Greer, LL, Handgraaf, MJ, Shalvi, S, Van Kleef, GA, Baas, M, Ten Velden, FS, Van Dijk, E, Feith, SW (2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328, 14081411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Dreu, CK, Greer, LL, Van Kleef, GA, Shalvi, S, Handgraaf, MJ (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108, 12621266.Google Scholar
Derntl, B, Finkelmeyer, A, Toygar, TK, Hulsmann, A, Schneider, F, Falkenberg, DI, Habel, U (2009). Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 108, 197206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feifel, D, Macdonald, K, Cobb, P, Minassian, A (2012). Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin improves verbal memory in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 139, 207210.Google Scholar
Feifel, D, Macdonald, K, Nguyen, A, Cobb, P, Warlan, H, Galangue, B, Minassian, A, Becker, O, Cooper, J, Perry, W, Lefebvre, M, Gonzales, J, Hadley, A (2010). Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin reduces symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry 68, 678680.Google Scholar
Fischer-Shofty, M, Brune, M, Ebert, A, Shefet, D, Levkovitz, Y, Shamay-Tsoory, SG (2013). Improving social perception in schizophrenia: the role of oxytocin. Schizophrenia Research 146, 357362.Google Scholar
Frith, CD (2004). Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine 34, 385389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman, M, Marlow-O'Connor, M, Torres, I, Carter, CS (2008). Diminished plasma oxytocin in schizophrenic patients with neuroendocrine dysfunction and emotional deficits. Schizophrenia Research 98, 247255.Google Scholar
Gossen, A, Hahn, A, Westphal, L, Prinz, S, Schultz, RT, Grunder, G, Spreckelmeyer, KN (2012). Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration – a study in healthy men. Neuropeptides 46, 211215.Google Scholar
Guastella, AJ, Einfeld, SL, Gray, KM, Rinehart, NJ, Tonge, BJ, Lambert, TJ, Hickie, IB (2010). Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry 67, 692694.Google Scholar
Hayes, DJ, Northoff, G (2012). Common brain activations for painful and non-painful aversive stimuli. BMC Neuroscience 13, 60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hein, G, Silani, G, Preuschoff, K, Batson, CD, Singer, T (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68, 149160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurlemann, R, Patin, A, Onur, OA, Cohen, MX, Baumgartner, T, Metzler, S, Dziobek, I, Gallinat, J, Wagner, M, Maier, W, Kendrick, KM (2010). Oxytocin enhances amygdala-dependent, socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 49995007.Google Scholar
Jackson, PL, Decety, J (2004). Motor cognition: a new paradigm to study self-other interactions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14, 259263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, PL, Meltzoff, AN, Decety, J (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage 24, 771779.Google Scholar
Juif, PE, Poisbeau, P (2013). Neurohormonal effects of oxytocin and vasopressin receptor agonists on spinal pain processing in male rats. Pain 154, 14491456.Google Scholar
Kapur, S (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 323.Google Scholar
Kavaliers, M, Choleris, E (2011). Sociality, pathogen avoidance, and the neuropeptides oxytocin and arginine vasopressin. Psychological Science 22, 13671374.Google Scholar
Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, Opler, LA (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261276.Google Scholar
Kemp, AH, Guastella, AJ (2011). The role of oxytocin in human affect: a novel hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 20, 222231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keri, S, Kiss, I, Kelemen, O (2009). Sharing secrets: oxytocin and trust in schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience 4, 287293.Google Scholar
Komorita, SS, Parks, CD (1995). Interpersonal relations: mixed-motive interaction. Annual Review of Psychology 46, 183207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J, Zaki, J, Harvey, PO, Ochsner, K, Green, MF (2011). Schizophrenia patients are impaired in empathic accuracy. Psychological Medicine 41, 22972304.Google Scholar
Legros, JJ, Gazzotti, C, Carvelli, T, Franchimont, P, Timsit-Berthier, M, von Frenckell, R, Ansseau, M (1992). Apomorphine stimulation of vasopressin- and oxytocin-neurophysins. Evidence for increased oxytocinergic and decreased vasopressinergic function in schizophrenics. Psychoneuroendocrinology 17, 611617.Google Scholar
Leknes, S, Wessberg, J, Ellingsen, DM, Chelnokova, O, Olausson, H, Laeng, B (2013). Oxytocin enhances pupil dilation and sensitivity to ‘hidden’ emotional expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8, 741749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linkowski, P, Geenen, V, Kerkhofs, M, Mendlewicz, J, Legros, JJ (1984). Cerebrospinal fluid neurophysins in affective illness and in schizophrenia. European Archive of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 234, 162165.Google Scholar
McCarthy, MM (1995). Estrogen modulation of oxytocin and its relation to behavior. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology 395, 235245.Google Scholar
Montag, C, Brockmann, EM, Bayerl, M, Rujescu, D, Muller, DJ, Gallinat, J (2012 a). Oxytocin and oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and risk for schizophrenia: a case-control study. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 14, 500508.Google Scholar
Montag, C, Brockmann, EM, Lehmann, A, Muller, DJ, Rujescu, D, Gallinat, J (2012 b). Association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and self-rated ‘empathic concern’ in schizophrenia. PLoS One 7, e51882.Google Scholar
Montag, C, Dziobek, I, Richter, IS, Neuhaus, K, Lehmann, A, Sylla, R, Heekeren, HR, Heinz, A, Gallinat, J (2011). Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Research 186, 203209.Google Scholar
Mouraux, A, Diukova, A, Lee, MC, Wise, RG, Iannetti, GD (2011). A multisensory investigation of the functional significance of the ‘pain matrix’. NeuroImage 54, 22372249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, CA, Gibson, CM, Rau, SW, Salimi, K, Smedley, KL, Casey, RL, Leserman, J, Jarskog, LF, Penn, DL (2011). Intranasal oxytocin reduces psychotic symptoms and improves Theory of Mind and social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 132, 5053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrovic, P, Kalisch, R, Singer, T, Dolan, RJ (2008). Oxytocin attenuates affective evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 66076615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenfeld, AJ, Lieberman, JA, Jarskog, LF (2011). Oxytocin, dopamine, and the amygdala: a neurofunctional model of social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 10771087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, LH, Carter, CS, Drogos, L, Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H, Sweeney, JA, Maki, PM (2010). Peripheral oxytocin is associated with reduced symptom severity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 124, 1321.Google Scholar
Sasayama, D, Hattori, K, Teraishi, T, Hori, H, Ota, M, Yoshida, S, Arima, K, Higuchi, T, Amano, N, Kunugi, H (2012). Negative correlation between cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin levels and negative symptoms of male patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 139, 201206.Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, SG (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17, 1824.Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, SG, Abu-Akel, A, Palgi, S, Sulieman, R, Fischer-Shofty, M, Levkovitz, Y, Decety, J (2013). Giving peace a chance: oxytocin increases empathy to pain in the context of the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 31393144.Google Scholar
Shipley, WC (1940). A self-administering scale for measuring intellectual impairment and deterioration. Journal of Psychology 9, 371377.Google Scholar
Singer, T, Seymour, B, O'Doherty, J, Kaube, H, Dolan, RJ, Frith, CD (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 11571162.Google Scholar
Smith, MJ, Horan, WP, Cobia, DJ, Karpouzian, TM, Fox, JM, Reilly, JL, Breiter, HC (2013). Performance-based empathy mediates the influence of working memory on social competence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. Published online: 31 December 2013 . doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt084.Google ScholarPubMed
Souza, RP, Ismail, P, Meltzer, HY, Kennedy, JL (2010). Variants in the oxytocin gene and risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 121, 279280.Google Scholar
Striepens, N, Kendrick, KM, Maier, W, Hurlemann, R (2011). Prosocial effects of oxytocin and clinical evidence for its therapeutic potential. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 32, 426450.Google Scholar
Tarrant, M, Dazeley, S, Cottom, T (2009). Social categorization and empathy for outgroup members. British Journal of Social Psychology 48, 427446.Google Scholar
Trawalter, S, Hoffman, KM, Waytz, A (2012). Racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. PLoS One 7, e48546.Google Scholar
Walss-Bass, C, Fernandes, JM, Roberts, DL, Service, H, Velligan, D (2013). Differential correlations between plasma oxytocin and social cognitive capacity and bias in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 147, 387392.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Sample presentation of a right hand in a pain and no-pain situation.

Figure 1

Table 1. Clinical and demographic data of the patient and healthy control groups

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Mean empathic ratings (± standard error) of the healthy controls as a function of treatment condition (placebo versus oxytocin), pain condition (pain versus no-pain) and target (Arabs versus Europeans versus Jews). VAS, Visual analog scale.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Mean empathic ratings (± standard error) of the patients with schizophrenia as a function of treatment condition (placebo versus oxytocin), pain condition (pain versus no-pain) and target (Arabs versus Europeans versus Jews). VAS, Visual analog scale.