Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T07:46:55.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychotic experiences and disorders in adolescents and young adults with borderline intellectual functioning and intellectual disabilities: evidence from a population-based birth cohort in the United Kingdom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2025

Christina Dardani*
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway PsychGen Centre for Genetic Epidemiology and Mental Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Jack Underwood
Affiliation:
Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Neuroscience and Mental Health Innovation Institute, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
Hannah Jones
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Alexandros Rammos
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Neuroscience and Mental Health Innovation Institute, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
Sarah Sullivan
Affiliation:
Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Laura Hull
Affiliation:
Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Department of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Golam Khandaker
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Stan Zammit
Affiliation:
Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Neuroscience and Mental Health Innovation Institute, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Dheeraj Rai
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Bristol Autism Spectrum Service, Avon and Wiltshire Partnership NHS Mental Health Trust, Bristol, UK
Paul Madley-Dowd
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
*
Corresponding author: Christina Dardani; Email: christina.dardani@bristol.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Individuals with borderline intellectual functioning and intellectual disabilities (intellectual impairment) may be at increased risk of psychosis. However, studies have been limited by small and selected samples. Moreover, the role of early life trauma, a key risk factor for psychosis, in the associations is unknown.

Methods

Using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort, we investigated the associations between intellectual impairment, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences, and assessed the mediating role of trauma in childhood. Individuals with intellectual impairment were identified using a multisource measure utilizing indicators from ALSPAC combined with health and administrative records. Psychotic disorder diagnoses were extracted through linkage to primary care records. Psychotic experiences were assessed at ages 18 and 24 using the semi-structured Psychosis-Like Symptoms interview (PLIKSi). Trauma between ages 5 and 11 was assessed with questionnaires and interviews administered to children and parents at multiple ages. Multiple imputation was performed to mitigate bias due to missing data.

Results

The maximum sample after multiple imputation was 9,407. We found associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders (OR = 4.57; 95%CI: 1.56–13.39). Evidence was weaker in the case of psychotic experiences (OR = 1.63; 95%CI: 0.93–2.84). There was some evidence suggesting a mediating role of trauma in the associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic experiences (OR = 1.09; 95%CI: 1.03–1.15). Complete records analyses yielded comparable estimates.

Conclusions

Intellectual impairment is associated with psychotic disorders and experiences in adulthood. Research into the contribution of trauma could shape intervention strategies for psychotic disorders in this population.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Borderline intellectual functioning and Intellectual disabilities (henceforth intellectual impairment)refer to difficulties in cognitive and adaptive functioning that manifest early in childhood and have a substantial impact on education, independent living skills, employment, and access to social support and health care across the lifespan (Patel et al., Reference Patel, Apple, Kanungo and Akkal2018). Intellectual impairment has a neurodevelopmental origin and is not the result of later neurocognitive changes due to injury or disease. Recent meta-analytic evidence suggests that the lifetime prevalence of mental health conditions in intellectual impairment may be higher than the general population (pooled lifetime prevalence: ≈32% vs ≈ 29% in the general population)(Mazza et al., Reference Mazza, Rossetti, Crespi and Clerici2020; Steel et al., Reference Steel, Marnane, Iranpour, Chey, Jackson, Patel and Silove2014). Co-occurring mental health conditions in intellectual impairment have been associated with adverse behavioral, educational, and social outcomes for the affected individuals as well as lower quality of life for their families and carers (Maes et al., Reference Maes, Broekman, Došen and Nauts2003). On this basis, understanding the links between intellectual impairment and mental health conditions is among the top global research priorities in the field (Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Yasamy, Emerson, Officer, Richler and Saxena2014).

Affective and non-affective psychotic disorders (henceforth psychotic disorders) are among the most common co-occurring mental health conditions in intellectual impairment. The prevalence of psychotic disorders in intellectual impairment appears to be higher than their estimated lifetime prevalence in the general population, with schizophrenia reaching approximately 4.8% (vs ≈ 0.9% in the general population), unspecified psychotic disorder reaching 3.9% (vs ≈ 0.5%) and bipolar disorder approximately 2% (vs ≈ 0.2%)(Aman et al., Reference Aman, Naeem, Farooq and Ayub2016; Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Glasson, Chen, Epstein, Leonard, Skoss, Jacoby, Blackmore, Srinivasjois, Bourke, Sanders and Downs2020; Mazza et al., Reference Mazza, Rossetti, Crespi and Clerici2020; Perälä et al., Reference Perälä, Suvisaari, Saarni, Kuoppasalmi, Isometsä, Pirkola, Partonen, Tuulio-Henriksson, Hintikka and Kieseppä2007). However, most studies so far have been limited by small and selected samples (predominantly inpatient), with limited control for potential confounding factors. Furthermore, these studies have been predominantly based on diagnoses of psychotic disorders in this population (Mazza et al., Reference Mazza, Rossetti, Crespi and Clerici2020), offering limited insights into sub-clinical symptoms and potential precursors of psychotic disorders, such as psychotic experiences.

Several factors have been proposed to influence the risk of psychiatric conditions in individuals with intellectual impairment, including access to support services, poor physical health, and major life events (including but not limited to trauma)(Allan et al., Reference Allan, Cooper, Morrison, Smiley and Williamson2007; Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Smiley, Morrison, Allan, Williamson, Finlayson, Jackson and Mantry2007). The latter is particularly relevant in the case of psychotic disorders. Traumatic life events (such as physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, neglect, domestic violence, and bullying victimization) are among the most consistently identified risk factors for psychotic experiences and disorders in the general population (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson, Houtepen and Zammit2019; Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer, Read, Van Os and Bentall2012). Emerging evidence suggests high rates of exposure to traumatic life events in the population with intellectual impairment (Berg et al., Reference Berg, Shiu, Feinstein, Acharya, MeDrano and Msall2019; Wigham & Emerson, Reference Wigham and Emerson2015). There is an absence of studies investigating whether and to what extent traumatic life events mediate any associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic experiences and disorders.

Using prospectively collected questionnaire, interview, and health record linkage data in a population-based birth cohort in the UK, we assessed: (A) the risk of psychotic disorders and psychotic experiences during early adulthood in individuals with and without intellectual impairment, (B) the potential associations of intellectual impairment to longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences reflecting the persistence and frequency of psychotic experiences from age 12 to age 24 (C) the extent to which trauma experienced in childhood may mediate the links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and psychotic experiences, (D) the possible confounding influence of familial, socioeconomic and demographic factors in any identified links.

Methods

Study design and participants

A visual summary of the study’s aims and design can be found in Figure 1. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a population-based cohort study of children born to 14,541 pregnant mothers residing in the former county of Avon, United Kingdom, with an expected delivery date between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992. Of these initial pregnancies, there was a total of 14,676 fetuses, resulting in 14,062 live births and 13,988 children who were alive at 1 year of age. When the oldest children were approximately 7 years of age, eligible participants who did not join the study initially were contacted, and additional participants were recruited. This resulted in a total of 15,447 pregnancies and 15,658 fetuses, of which 14,901 were alive at 1 year of age.

Figure 1 Visual summary of the study aims and design. Solid black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences. Dotted black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the extent to which any links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences are mediated via traumatic experiences in childhood. Dashed black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the possible confounding influence of familial, socioeconomic, and demographic factors in the links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences. Please note that intellectual impairment in the context of the present study refers to borderline intellectual functioning and intellectual disabilities that are typically of neurodevelopmental origin and therefore in the present study they were assumed to precede the mediator and outcome.

Further information on the ALSPAC cohort is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac) and elsewhere (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson, Molloy, Ness, Ring and Davey Smith2013; Fraser et al., Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy and Ness2013; Northstone et al., Reference Northstone, Lewcock, Groom, Boyd, Macleod, Timpson and Wells2019). The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/). Some study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at the University of Bristol. REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based software platform designed to support data capture for research studies. (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Taylor, Thielke, Payne, Gonzalez and Conde2009, Reference Harris, Taylor, Minor, Elliott, Fernandez, O’Neal, McLeod, Delacqua, Delacqua and Kirby2019).

For data collected via questionnaires and clinics, informed consent was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees (NHS Haydock REC: 10/H1010/70).

Linkage to health and administrative records

When the index children reached legal adulthood (age 18), ALSPAC conducted a postal fair-processing campaign to re-enroll them into the study and to seek permission for linkage to health and administrative records. This was an ‘opt-out’ approach, meaning linkage was attempted for all participants, except those who objected and those who were not sent fair processing materials. Where ‘opt-in’ consent became practicable (e.g., when a participant attended a study assessment visit) then this was collected by a trained fieldworker. Details on the linkage process can be found elsewhere (Cornish et al., Reference Cornish, Macleod, Boyd and Tilling2021).

In the context of the present study, the eligible sample was defined based on the following criteria: (1). having available linkage data and not dissenting to their use, (2). having linked primary care data available from age 11 onwards (considering that we were interested in the adolescent and early adulthood period). A total of 9,680 participants were eligible.

Measures

Exposure: intellectual impairment

Intellectual impairment was identified in ALSPAC using a composite measure created in previously published work (Madley-Dowd et al., Reference Madley-Dowd, Thomas, Boyd, Zammit, Heron and Rai2022) based on information from six different sources: (i) IQ scores less than 85 measured at age 8 and age 15, (ii) free text fields from parent-reported questionnaires, (iii) school-reported provision of educational services for individuals with a statement of special educational needs for cognitive impairments, (iv) from relevant Read codes (Chisholm, Reference Chisholm1990) contained in GP records (Read codes are a comprehensive list of standardized clinical terms used by healthcare professionals within the UK National Health Service to record clinical information), (v) international classification of disease (ICD) diagnoses contained in hospital episode statistics and (vi) recorded interactions with mental health services for ID contained within the mental health services data set. The intellectual impairment measure is available on the UK Secure eResearch Platform (UKSeRP). Further details on the creation of the composite intellectual impairment measure can be found in the original publication (Madley-Dowd et al., Reference Madley-Dowd, Thomas, Boyd, Zammit, Heron and Rai2022).

Considering that our exposure definition captured a broad group of individuals with intellectual impairment (IQ < 85), we additionally used a stricter definition of intellectual disabilities. Specifically, intellectual disabilities were defined using the same measures and criteria described above, with the only difference in criteria being an IQ score less than 70, where IQ information was available.

Primary outcome: psychotic disorders

Read codes (V.2) from GP records relevant to the diagnosis and symptoms of psychotic disorders were extracted to identify the outcomes of interest in the eligible sample. GP records were available from 1990 to 2016 when the oldest participants were 25 years of age. The full list of reading codes used can be found in the project-dedicated repository: https://github.com/pmadleydowd/BailyThomas-IntellectualDisability-and-MentalHealth.

Primary outcome: psychotic experiences until early adulthood

Psychotic experiences were assessed at ages 18 and 24 using the semi-semi-structured Psychosis-Like Symptoms interview (PLIKSi), administered by trained psychologists, and scored according to criteria predefined by the World Health Organization (Organization, 1994). The PLIKSi consists of 12 core questions covering hallucinations, delusions, and thought interference. Participants were asked about experiences that had occurred since age 12 years. Psychotic experiences were considered present if, at ages 18 and/or 24 years, one or more of the experiences was rated by the interviewer as suspected or definitely present, and if this was not attributable to falling asleep or waking up or fever. We additionally examined psychotic experiences that had been distressing and/or frequent, since these experiences are more clinically relevant and predictive of psychotic disorder (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher, Holmans, Jones, Jones, Linden and Lewis2020).

Secondary outcome: longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences

Considering that psychotic experiences are, in most cases, transient in the general population (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher, Holmans, Jones, Jones, Linden and Lewis2020), and do not necessarily reflect liability to psychotic disorders later in life, we additionally used a measure reflecting the persistence and frequency of psychotic experiences across three time points in ALSPAC: ages 12, 18, and 24 years. Details on the measure can be found in the original publication (Rammos et al., Reference Rammos, Sullivan, Kounali, Jones, Hammerton, Hines, Lewis, Jones, Cannon, Thompson, Wolke, Heron and Zammit2021). Briefly, using information from the PLIKSi on current presence (over the past 6 months) and frequency of psychotic experiences (0: “Not present,” 1: “Low-frequency” – experiences occurring less than weekly, 2: “High-frequency” – experiences occurring weekly or daily), at each time point, an empirical composite measure was generated reflecting four longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences from ages 12–24: (A). No experiences: Individuals without a psychotic experience at any time point; (B). Transient: Individuals with a psychotic experience rated at only one time-point, regardless of frequency; (C). Low-frequency persistent: Individuals with low-frequency psychotic experiences at two or more time points, or with a low-frequency rating at one time point and a high-frequency rating at another; (D). High-frequency persistent: Individuals with high-frequency psychotic experiences rated at two or more time points. Following previous work we did not make an assumption on the potential severity ordering of the profiles (Rammos et al., Reference Rammos, Sullivan, Kounali, Jones, Hammerton, Hines, Lewis, Jones, Cannon, Thompson, Wolke, Heron and Zammit2021), particularly considering that the boundaries between the transient and persistent-low profiles might be difficult to define.

Mediator: traumatic experiences in childhood

The measures of childhood trauma and their associations with psychotic experiences have been described in detail elsewhere (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson, Houtepen and Zammit2019). In brief, we used a measure of childhood trauma between ages 5 and 11 based on responses to 57 questions from questionnaires and interviews about domestic violence (regular acts of physical violence taking place in the home), physical abuse (physical harm to the participant from caregivers or other adults), emotional abuse (emotional cruelty to the participant from caregivers or other adults), emotional neglect (caregivers not taking an interest in the participant’s life), sexual abuse (adults or older children forcing the participant into sexual activity, including attempts to do so), and bullying victimization (regular name-calling, blackmail, or assault by peers). Measures of sexual, physical, and emotional abuse, assessed contemporaneously by the participant and their caregivers between participant ages 5 to 11, were supplemented with data from a participant-completed questionnaire at age 22, as all data on sexual abuse, and most data on physical and emotional abuse prior to age 11, were based on parental report. Each type of trauma was coded as present or not, and a single trauma variable was created representing exposure to any type of trauma (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson, Houtepen and Zammit2019).

Covariates

Covariates in the present study were selected on the basis of their potential associations with the exposure, outcomes, and mediator. These included child sex (male/female), maternal parity (≤1 child versus ≥2 children), major financial problems in the family when the child was 8 months old (yes/no), maternal highest educational attainment (32 weeks gestation), maternal age (at delivery), maternal Crown-Crisp anxiety scores (Crown & Crisp, Reference Crown and Crisp1966) (18 weeks gestation), and maternal depression measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987) (EPDS; 18 weeks gestation scores ≥13). Moreover, in order to better capture socioeconomic position which has been consistently associated with intellectual impairment, we added in our association analysis models as covariates: car ownership status (owning/not owning, 8 weeks of gestation), maternal marital status (married/separated, 8 weeks of gestation) and home ownership status (owned/rented, 8 weeks of gestation). This decision was based not only on the information they could provide but also on their completeness in the eligible sample (ranging from 87–88%).

Statistical analyses

Association analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in StataSE version 18. We estimated descriptive statistics of participant characteristics for individuals with and without intellectual impairment, traumatic experiences, and psychotic experiences.

Using logistic regression, we estimated odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for the associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders as well as psychotic experiences in early adulthood. Using g-computation via Stata’s margins command, we further estimated the adjusted marginal risk (overall covariates) and risk difference of each outcome for participants with and without ID. Standard errors were calculated using the delta method. Using multinomial logistic regression, we estimated relative risk ratios (RRRs) and 95% CIs for the associations between intellectual impairment and the four longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences. Across all association analyses, we performed crude and covariate-adjusted models.

We additionally conducted association analyses using intellectual disabilities (including IQ < 70) as the exposure. It is worth noting that these analyses were expected to have substantially lower power than the ones using intellectual impairment as the exposure, but nevertheless informative on the potential associations between intellectual disabilities, psychotic disorders, and experiences.

Mediation analyses

We decided to conduct mediation analyses between intellectual impairment (exposure), trauma (mediator), and psychosis-related outcomes regardless of whether there was evidence of associations between intellectual impairment and the outcomes of interest. This decision was based on previous work suggesting that evidence of associations between exposure and outcome should not guide decisions for subsequent mediation analyses, particularly when the effect size is expected to be small or there may be suppression effects (when the direct and indirect effects of an exposure on an outcome have opposite directions)(O’Rourke & MacKinnon, Reference O’Rourke and MacKinnon2018; Shrout & Bolger, Reference Shrout and Bolger2002). Mediation analyses were performed using the g-formula package (Daniel et al., Reference Daniel, De Stavola and Cousens2011) in Stata. We used the parametric g-formula using 10,000 Monte Carlo simulations to estimate the natural direct effect (NDE) of intellectual impairment on psychotic experiences, and the natural indirect effect (NIE) that was mediated via traumatic experiences between ages 5 and 11. We performed crude as well as covariate-adjusted models. Corresponding 95% CIs were estimated using the standard errors from 1000 nonparametric bootstrap resamples.

Missing data

Considering previous work suggesting that individuals with intellectual impairment are more likely to have missing data in ALSPAC (particularly those with more severe ID)(Madley-Dowd et al., Reference Madley-Dowd, Thomas, Boyd, Zammit, Heron and Rai2022), we performed multiple imputation across all association and mediation analyses to mitigate potential bias from missing data (Little & Rubin, Reference Little and Rubin2019). For primary and secondary outcomes, we performed multiple imputations by chained equations, using Stata’s MI impute command. One hundred datasets were imputed with 25 burn-in iterations and estimates were combined across imputed datasets using Rubin’s rules, implemented via Stata’s MI estimate command. Details on the imputation models applied can be found in Supplementary Note 1. In the case of mediation analyses, we used the inbuilt g-formula imputation commands allowing simultaneous imputation of missing data and mediation analyses, entering in the models the same auxiliary variables we used for the association analyses. In the context of the present study, we present both complete records and imputed data analyses, although we consider as primary the imputed data analyses.

The role of the funding source

The funders of the study had no role in study design, data analysis, data interpretation, writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Results

The maximum sample size with data on exposure and at least one outcome measure was 9,407 (49.6% male; 3.6% ID; 0.3% psychotic disorder diagnosis). Full characteristics of our study sample, including covariates and outcome variables are listed in Supplementary Tables 1a (sample with intellectual impairment, IQ < 85) and 1b (sample with intellectual disabilities, IQ < 70). Those with intellectual impairment were more likely to have experienced trauma between ages 5–11. The mothers of those with intellectual impairment were less likely to have a university degree and had a greater prevalence of screening positive for depression. Approximately 65% of the sample had complete data on exposure, psychotic disorder diagnosis (complete for all participants in the eligible sample), and covariates, while 34% of the sample had complete data on exposure, psychotic experiences, and covariates. Participants with complete data were more likely to have a higher socioeconomic background than those with incomplete data (details on the identified patterns can be found in Supplementary Tables 2a2b).

Association analyses

There was some evidence suggesting an association between intellectual impairment and primary care diagnoses of psychotic disorders in crude and adjusted-for covariate models (adjusted OR = 4.84; 95%CI: 1.64, 14.29; Table 1). When considered on the risk difference scale, this odds ratio reflects a small absolute increase in risk among those with intellectual impairment (adjusted marginal risk difference = 1.14%; 95%CI: −0.28%, 2.05%; Table 1); we were unable to report the absolute risk according to the intellectual impairment group due to low counts with a diagnosis among individuals with intellectual impairment. There was also some evidence to support associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic experiences (adjusted OR = 1.59; 95%CI: 0.91, 2.77; adjusted marginal risk difference = 7.69%; 95%CI: −2.58%, 17.96%; Table 1) as well as distressing and/or frequent psychotic experiences (adjusted OR = 1.84; 95%CI: 0.96, 3.54; adjusted marginal risk difference = 7.97%; 95%CI: −2.16%, 19.16%; Table 1) although the confidence intervals of these results crossed the null. Association estimates in complete records analyses were of comparable magnitude, albeit less precise (Supplementary Table 3). In the case of the analyses using intellectual disabilities as the exposure (IQ < 70), there was some evidence of associations, particularly with psychotic disorder, but estimates were highly imprecise due to the small sample size of these analyses (Supplementary Table 4 for analyses using imputed data and Supplementary Table 5 for complete record analyses).

Table 1. Associations between intellectual impairment, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences in early adulthood from multiple imputation analyses

1 Odds ratio.

2 Confidence interval.

3 Adjusted for: child sex (male/female), maternal parity (≤1 child versus ≥2 children), major financial problems in the family when the child was 8 months old (yes/no), maternal highest educational attainment (32 weeks gestation), maternal age (at delivery), maternal Crown-Crisp anxiety scores (18 weeks gestation), maternal depression measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; 18 weeks gestation scores ≥13), car ownership status (owning/not owning, 8 weeks of gestation), maternal marital status (married/separated, 8 weeks of gestation), and home ownership status (owned/rented, 8 weeks of gestation).

4 Intellectual impairment.

5 Exact value omitted to avoid disclosure of cell counts <5 (1.5 = 100 × 5/336, where 336 is the number with ID).

There was little evidence to suggest that individuals with intellectual impairment may be more likely to present with persistent profiles of psychotic experiences. Although the relative risk ratios for the high-frequency persistent profiles were larger than those for the low-frequency persistent and transient profiles, the estimates were highly imprecise (Table 2 for imputed data analyses and Supplementary Table 6 for complete records analyses).

Table 2. Associations between ID and longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences from multiple imputation analyses

1 Relative risk ratio.

2 Confidence interval.

3 Adjusted for: child sex (male/female), maternal parity (≤1 child versus ≥2 children), major financial problems in the family when the child was 8 months old (yes/no), maternal highest educational attainment (32 weeks gestation), maternal age (at delivery), maternal crown-crisp anxiety scores (18 weeks gestation), maternal depression measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; 18 weeks gestation scores ≥13).

Mediation analyses

There was some evidence to suggest that childhood trauma may mediate the associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic experiences (effect of exposure on outcome via the mediator, NIE, adjusted OR = 1.09; 95%CI: 1.03–1.15), as well as distressing and/or frequent psychotic experiences (effect of exposure on outcome via the mediator, NIE, adjusted OR = 1.11; 95%CI: 1.03–1.20). Evidence was weaker in the case of psychotic disorders, where traumatic experiences did not appear to mediate the associations with intellectual impairment (Table 3 for imputed data analyses and Supplementary Table 7 for complete records analyses).

Table 3. Results of the mediation analyses with childhood traumatic experiences, for the associations between ID, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences using imputed data

1 Total effect.

2 Odds ratio.

3 Confidence intervals.

4 Natural direct effect.

5 Natural indirect effect.

6 Adjusted for: child sex (male/female), maternal parity (≤1 child versus ≥2 children), major financial problems in the family when the child was 8 months old (yes/no), maternal highest educational attainment (32 weeks gestation), maternal age (at delivery), maternal Crown-Crisp anxiety scores (18 weeks gestation), maternal depression measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; 18 weeks gestation scores ≥13).

Discussion

Using prospectively collected questionnaires, interviews, and health record linkage data in a population-based birth cohort, we examined the associations between intellectual impairment, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences in early adulthood and investigated the factors that may influence them. We found evidence suggesting that intellectual impairment may be associated with psychotic disorders. Although evidence was less consistent in the case of psychotic experiences, traumatic experiences in childhood appeared to mediate their associations with intellectual impairment. The identified relationships were unlikely to be explained by familial, socioeconomic, and demographic factors.

The relationships of intellectual impairment to psychotic disorders and experiences

Our findings are consistent with a growing body of evidence suggesting that people with intellectual impairment may be at higher risk of psychotic disorders than the general population. The latest and largest study in the field (N = 2,091) found that individuals with intellectual disabilities had a higher risk of psychosis not only compared to the general population but also compared to individuals with other neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism, ADHD)(Strålin & Hetta, Reference Strålin and Hetta2019). There is an ongoing discussion on the possibility of bias in the existing evidence due to confounding and/or measurement error (measurement error might arise for example, due to the application of diagnostic criteria and tools designed for the general population in individuals with intellectual impairment)(Aman et al., Reference Aman, Naeem, Farooq and Ayub2016). In our study, we attempted to overcome confounding bias by adjusting our models for several familial, socioeconomic, and demographic factors and found that they are unlikely to explain the identified links (although the possibility of residual confounding cannot be excluded – see Strengths and Limitations section). With regard to measurement error, previous work has indicated that primary care records may underestimate mental health conditions compared to population-based studies such as ALSPAC (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Willan, Prady, Dickerson, Santorelli, Tilling and Cornish2021). However, in the context of the present study, evidence of associations with psychotic disorder diagnoses was complemented with some evidence of associations between intellectual impairment and subclinical expressions of psychosis liability, and psychotic experiences. Although the evidence was relatively inconsistent across psychotic experience measures, the direction of the association estimates was consistent with the ones identified for psychotic disorders. Moreover, the strongest associations were identified in the case of psychotic experiences that were distressing and/or frequent, a phenotype that is considered to be more strongly related to the subsequent risk of psychotic disorders (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Kounali, Cannon, David, Fletcher, Holmans, Jones, Jones, Linden and Lewis2020). Nevertheless, it is important to acknowledge that psychotic experiences were assessed using an s semi-structured tool, PLIKSi, which is not intended for use in individuals with intellectual disabilities, leading therefore to potential under-ascertainment of psychotic experiences in the study sample.

The mediating role of traumatic experiences in childhood

Our study provides some evidence of the potentially mediating role of traumatic experiences in childhood in the associations between intellectual impairment and psychotic experiences. The evidence was weaker in the case of psychotic disorder and a number of reasons for this could be hypothesized. For example, lack of power may have influenced these analyses, particularly considering that only 0.3% of the total eligible sample (36 individuals) had a diagnosis of psychotic disorder. Moreover, traumatic experiences were measured with a combination of parent and self-reported questionnaires which could lead to underestimation of trauma in the eligible study sample (e.g., parents may underreport the occurrence of traumatic events in the child and/or participants at the age of 24 assessment of traumatic experiences are more likely to drop out).

Although our study is the first to apply a formal counterfactual mediation approach, previous work in a sample of 1,023 adults with intellectual disabilities found that major life events (including but not limited to trauma) were associated with psychiatric conditions in this population (Allan et al., Reference Allan, Cooper, Morrison, Smiley and Williamson2007). On this basis, interventions for trauma-related morbidity in this population may substantially improve mental health outcomes. Evidence on the effectiveness of trauma-focused interventions in people with intellectual impairment is promising, indicating that eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) as well as trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) may be effective in this population (McNally et al., Reference McNally, Taggart and Shevlin2021), with substantive trials underway (e.g., https://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN35167485). However, most of the evidence so far comes from case studies and therefore further work is necessary to appraise the appropriateness and effectiveness of these interventions in people with intellectual impairment.

Strengths and limitations

This is the first study to investigate the links between intellectual impairment, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences and assess the possible influence of traumatic experiences in childhood using prospectively collected data from a large population-based cohort. We also used linkage to health and administrative record data, which aided the identification of intellectual impairment and psychotic disorder cases and reduced the impact of attrition and therefore bias due to missing data.

Our study presents several limitations. First, our exposure definition has been broad, including cases of borderline intellectual functioning as well as mild and more severe cases of intellectual disabilities. Despite the fact that analyses using a strict intellectual disabilities definition supported the links with psychosis, the possibility that the associations may differ by the severity of intellectual impairment, cannot be excluded and needs to be investigated further. Second, although we used psychotic experiences as a phenotype reflecting psychotic disorder liability, this might not be the case, as psychotic experiences are associated with several adverse mental health outcomes such as depression and are not specific to psychotic disorders (Legge et al., Reference Legge, Jones, Kendall, Pardiñas, Menzies, Bracher-Smith, Escott-Price, Rees, Davis and Hotopf2019; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Wiles, Kounali, Lewis, Heron, Cannon, Mahedy, Jones, Stochl and Zammit2014). Third, our analyses may have been limited by lack of power; this is particularly true for the analyses using the strict intellectual disabilities definition as the exposure. Fourth, although we tried to mitigate the possibility of bias due to missing data using multiple imputations, some bias is still likely to influence the findings of the analyses using psychotic experiences as the outcome (association & and mediation analyses). This is because individuals with intellectual impairment and psychosis are less likely to participate in ALSPAC and the use of psychotic disorder diagnoses as an auxiliary for psychotic experience is unlikely to fully break the link between the outcome and the probability of missing data (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Heron, Sterne and Tilling2019). Fifth, although we adjusted our analyses for a number of potential familial, socioeconomic, and demographic factors, some level of residual confounding is still likely to be present. Sixth, the ALSPAC cohort is not representative of the whole UK population, characterized predominantly by socioeconomically advantaged individuals, and very little ethnic diversity (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson, Molloy, Ness, Ring and Davey Smith2013). Research using more diverse populations is necessary in order to further elucidate the links between intellectual impairments and psychosis.

Conclusions

Borderline intellectual functioning and intellectual disabilities are associated with psychotic disorders and experiences into young adulthood. Traumatic experiences in childhood may partially mediate the associations and further research in this area could shape current intervention strategies for psychotic disorders in this population.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724003556.

Data availability statement

Individual-level data from the ALSPAC birth cohort are not publicly available for reasons of clinical confidentiality. Data can be accessed after application to the ALSPAC Executive Team who will respond within 10 working days. Application instructions and data use agreements are available at http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/. The code used to conduct these analyses can be found in the repository: https://github.com/pmadleydowd/BailyThomas-IntellectualDisability-and-MentalHealth

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grant funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). This publication is the work of the authors and CD & PMD will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. CD is supported by a grant from the UK Medical Research Council (MRC) which forms part of the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit (IEU) at the University of Bristol (MC_UU_00032/06) and a postdoctoral fellowship from the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (2024078). DR and PMD are supported by the Baily Thomas Charitable Fund [TRUST/VC/AC/SG/6075-9277]. PMD also acknowledges support from the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit (IEU) at the University of Bristol (MC_UU_00032/02)). GMK acknowledges funding support from the MRC (MC_UU_00032/06; MR/W014416/1; MR/S037675/1; and MR/Z50354X/1), the Wellcome Trust (201486/Z/16/Z and 201486/B/16/Z), and the UK National Institute of Health and Care Research (NIHR) Bristol Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at the University Hospitals Bristol and Weston NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol (NIHR 203315). HJJ, DR, and PMD also acknowledge funding from the NIHR Bristol BRC (NIHR 203315). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. J.F.G.U. is supported by aGW4-CAT Clinical Doctoral Fellowship (222849/Z/21/Z) and an HEIW Welsh Clinical Academic Training Fellowship. LH is supported by an Early Career Fellowship from the Elizabeth Blackwell Institute (University of Bristol) & Rosetrees Trust.

Competing interest

None.

References

Allan, L., Cooper, S.-A., Morrison, J., Smiley, E., & Williamson, A. (2007). Mental ill-health in adults with intellectual disabilities: Prevalence and associated factors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(1), 2735. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.106.022483Google Scholar
Aman, H., Naeem, F., Farooq, S., & Ayub, M. (2016). Prevalence of nonaffective psychosis in intellectually disabled clients: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatric Genetics, 26(4), 145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, K. L., Shiu, C., Feinstein, R. T., Acharya, K., MeDrano, J., & Msall, M. E. (2019). Children with developmental disabilities experience higher levels of adversity. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 89, 105113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.03.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., Molloy, L., Ness, A., Ring, S., & Davey Smith, G. (2013). Cohort profile: The ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, N., Glasson, E. J., Chen, W., Epstein, A., Leonard, H., Skoss, R., Jacoby, P., Blackmore, A. M., Srinivasjois, R., Bourke, J., Sanders, R. J., & Downs, J. (2020). Prevalence estimates of mental health problems in children and adolescents with intellectual disability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 54(10), 970984. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867420924101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chisholm, J. (1990). The Read clinical classification. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 300(6732), 1092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, S.-A., Smiley, E., Morrison, J., Allan, L., Williamson, A., Finlayson, J., Jackson, A., & Mantry, D. (2007). Psychosis and adults with intellectual disabilities. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42(7), 530536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-007-0197-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornish, R. P., Macleod, J., Boyd, A., & Tilling, K. (2021). Factors associated with participation over time in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: A study using linked education and primary care data. International Journal of Epidemiology, 50(1), 293302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Edinburgh postnatal depression scale (EPDS). British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, J., Heron, J., Teufel, C., Cannon, M., Wolke, D., Thompson, A., Houtepen, L., & Zammit, S. (2019). Association of trauma type, age of exposure, and frequency in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(1), 7986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crown, S., & Crisp, A. H. (1966). A short clinical diagnostic self-rating scale for psychoneurotic patients: The Middlesex Hospital Questionnaire (MHQ). The British Journal of Psychiatry, 112(490), 917923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniel, R. M., De Stavola, B. L., & Cousens, S. N. (2011). gformula: Estimating causal effects in the presence of time-varying confounding or mediation using the g-computation formula. The Stata Journal, 11(4), 479517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, A., Macdonald-Wallis, C., Tilling, K., Boyd, A., Golding, J., Davey Smith, G., Henderson, J., Macleod, J., Molloy, L., & Ness, A. (2013). Cohort profile: The avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 97110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Minor, B. L., Elliott, V., Fernandez, M., O’Neal, L., McLeod, L., Delacqua, G., Delacqua, F., & Kirby, J. (2019). The REDCap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 95, 103208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009). Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 42(2), 377381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, R. A., Heron, J., Sterne, J. A. C., & Tilling, K. (2019). Accounting for missing data in statistical analyses: Multiple imputation is not always the answer. International Journal of Epidemiology, 48(4), 12941304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legge, S. E., Jones, H. J., Kendall, K. M., Pardiñas, A. F., Menzies, G., Bracher-Smith, M., Escott-Price, V., Rees, E., Davis, K. A. S., & Hotopf, M. (2019). Association of genetic liability to psychotic experiences with neuropsychotic disorders and traits. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(12), 12561265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (2019). Statistical analysis with missing data (Vol. 793). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Madley-Dowd, P., Thomas, R., Boyd, A., Zammit, S., Heron, J., & Rai, D. (2022). Intellectual disability in the children of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Wellcome Open Research, 7(172), 172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maes, B., Broekman, T. G., Došen, A., & Nauts, J. (2003). Caregiving burden of families looking after persons with intellectual disability and behavioural or psychiatric problems. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47(6), 447455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazza, M. G., Rossetti, A., Crespi, G., & Clerici, M. (2020). Prevalence of co-occurring psychiatric disorders in adults and adolescents with intellectual disability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 33(2), 126138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, P., Taggart, L., & Shevlin, M. (2021). Trauma experiences of people with an intellectual disability and their implications: A scoping review. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 34(4), 927949. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northstone, K., Lewcock, M., Groom, A., Boyd, A., Macleod, J., Timpson, N., & Wells, N. (2019). The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC): An update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Research, 4, 51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Rourke, H. P., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2018). Reasons for testing mediation in the absence of an intervention effect: A research imperative in prevention and intervention research. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 79(2), 171181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organization, W. H. (1994). Schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry: Version 2. World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Patel, D. R., Apple, R., Kanungo, S., & Akkal, A. (2018). Intellectual disability: Definitions, evaluation and principles of treatment. Pediatric Medicine, 1(11), 1021037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perälä, J., Suvisaari, J., Saarni, S. I., Kuoppasalmi, K., Isometsä, E., Pirkola, S., Partonen, T., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Hintikka, J., & Kieseppä, T. (2007). Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1), 1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rammos, A., Sullivan, S. A., Kounali, D., Jones, H. J., Hammerton, G., Hines, L. A., Lewis, G., Jones, P. B., Cannon, M., Thompson, A., Wolke, D., Heron, J., & Zammit, S. (2021). Precursors and correlates of transient and persistent longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences from late childhood through early adulthood. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 220(6), 19. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2021.145Google ScholarPubMed
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D., Willan, K., Prady, S. L., Dickerson, J., Santorelli, G., Tilling, K., & Cornish, R. P. (2021). Assessing and predicting adolescent and early adulthood common mental disorders using electronic primary care data: Analysis of a prospective cohort study (ALSPAC) in Southwest England. BMJ Open, 11(10), e053624. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2021-053624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steel, Z., Marnane, C., Iranpour, C., Chey, T., Jackson, J. W., Patel, V., & Silove, D. (2014). The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 476493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strålin, P., & Hetta, J. (2019). First episode psychosis and comorbid ADHD, autism and intellectual disability. European Psychiatry, 55, 1822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, S. A., Kounali, D., Cannon, M., David, A. S., Fletcher, P. C., Holmans, P., Jones, H., Jones, P. B., Linden, D. E. J., & Lewis, G. (2020). A population-based cohort study examining the incidence and impact of psychotic experiences from childhood to adulthood, and prediction of psychotic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(4), 308317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, S. A., Wiles, N., Kounali, D., Lewis, G., Heron, J., Cannon, M., Mahedy, L., Jones, P. B., Stochl, J., & Zammit, S. (2014). Longitudinal associations between adolescent psychotic experiences and depressive symptoms. PLOS ONE, 9(8), e105758. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomlinson, M., Yasamy, M. T., Emerson, E., Officer, A., Richler, D., & Saxena, S. (2014). Setting global research priorities for developmental disabilities, including intellectual disabilities and autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 58(12), 11211130. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., Read, J., Van Os, J., & Bentall, R. P. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective-and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 661671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigham, S., & Emerson, E. (2015). Trauma and life events in adults with intellectual disability. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2(2), 9399. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-015-0041-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Visual summary of the study aims and design. Solid black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences. Dotted black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the extent to which any links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences are mediated via traumatic experiences in childhood. Dashed black lines correspond to the analyses investigating the possible confounding influence of familial, socioeconomic, and demographic factors in the links between intellectual impairment and psychotic disorders and experiences. Please note that intellectual impairment in the context of the present study refers to borderline intellectual functioning and intellectual disabilities that are typically of neurodevelopmental origin and therefore in the present study they were assumed to precede the mediator and outcome.

Figure 1

Table 1. Associations between intellectual impairment, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences in early adulthood from multiple imputation analyses

Figure 2

Table 2. Associations between ID and longitudinal profiles of psychotic experiences from multiple imputation analyses

Figure 3

Table 3. Results of the mediation analyses with childhood traumatic experiences, for the associations between ID, psychotic disorders, and psychotic experiences using imputed data

Supplementary material: File

Dardani et al. supplementary material

Dardani et al. supplementary material
Download Dardani et al. supplementary material(File)
File 68.3 KB