Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:55:55.248Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psycho-educational interventions designed to prevent deployment-related psychological ill-health in Armed Forces personnel: a review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2010

K. Mulligan*
Affiliation:
Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health (ACDMH), King's College London, UK
N. T. Fear
Affiliation:
Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health (ACDMH), King's College London, UK
N. Jones
Affiliation:
Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health (ACDMH), King's College London, UK
S. Wessely
Affiliation:
Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health (ACDMH), King's College London, UK
N. Greenberg
Affiliation:
Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health (ACDMH), King's College London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr K. Mulligan, ACDMH, King's College London, Weston Education Centre, Cutcombe Road, London SE5 9RJ, UK. (Email: kathleen.mulligan@kcl.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Employers such as the Armed Forces (AF) and emergency services, who predictably expose their staff to potentially traumatic events (PTEs), often provide psycho-educational briefings in an attempt to mitigate possible adverse psychological sequelae. Within the military, psycho-educational briefings are widely used, particularly following exposure to PTEs on operations. The aim of this review was to evaluate the efficacy of these interventions and make appropriate recommendations.

Method

A search of Medline, PsycINFO and EMBASE was conducted, bibliographies of retrieved articles were searched and experts in the field were consulted.

Results

Two surveys and seven intervention studies were identified for inclusion in the review. Only three studies were randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Overall, the review found some evidence of benefit of psycho-educational interventions but it was not consistent across studies or outcomes and effects were small. However, there was also little evidence to suggest that they caused harm. There was some evidence that the beneficial effects may be greater for those who have been exposed to a higher number of PTEs.

Conclusions

Given the high operational tempo currently faced by coalition forces personnel, there remains a pressing need to identify the most effective way of minimizing the impact of exposure to potentially traumatic deployment incidents. To date, few psycho-educational interventions designed to prevent deployment-related psychological ill-health have been evaluated systematically in methodologically robust studies. The review recommends that future interventions are theoretically based and evaluated in cluster RCTs that examine both process and outcome variables.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Introduction

Numerous occupations, including the Armed Forces (AF), emergency services and media organizations, inevitably expose staff to potentially traumatic events (PTEs). Although it is unrealistic to assume that exposure to PTEs can be eliminated from such occupations (see, for example, McGeorge et al. Reference McGeorge, Hacker Hughes and Wessely2006), it remains incumbent on employers to take what reasonable steps they can to minimize the risk of problems occurring as a consequence of that exposure. For example, UK government guidance on workplace stress states that, ‘Managers should make active attempts to minimize or prevent stress in the workforce’ (Health and Safety Executive, 2007). One such approach is the provision of psycho-education.

Although there is no clear definition of what constitutes psycho-education, interventions typically include information about common symptoms experienced following trauma, self-help techniques, and also information about where to get help if symptoms persist. There is some debate about equating psycho-education with psychological debriefing sessions, in which participants are encouraged to go through detailed recollection and emotional processing of the traumatic event experienced (Krupnick & Green, Reference Krupnick and Green2008). However, debriefing sessions do incorporate psycho-education in that they include information about possible symptoms for example, and some authors have concluded that debriefing is a form of education (Wessely et al. Reference Wessely, Bryant, Greenberg, Earnshaw, Sharpley and Hacker Hughes2008). For the purposes of this review, we use the term psycho-education to include debriefing.

Within the AF, psycho-education could be delivered at several stages throughout the deployment cycle, for instance before, during and after deployment, and also when a military unit is reconstituting in preparation for future deployment. The interventions might be expected to vary somewhat across stages. For example, pre-deployment emphasis is likely to be on potential operational stressors and how to manage them. Interventions delivered during deployment may focus on specific incidents. At the end of the deployment, interventions are likely to focus on leaving the operational environment and coping with returning home, and also managing grief, where appropriate.

The single identified paper that evaluated the use of psycho-educational briefings delivered before operational deployment (Sharpley et al. Reference Sharpley, Fear, Greenberg, Jones and Wessely2008) found no evidence of effectiveness in terms of common mental health disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or alcohol misuse some 2–3 years after personnel had returned home, although it should be noted that this study was a natural experiment rather than a randomized controlled trial (RCT).

Psycho-education is also widely implemented following exposure to PTEs, either shortly after an incident or at the end of the deployment period. For instance, of 16 nations who participated in a military leader's survey on occupational stress, 11 reported some type of psychological support, which could include defusing or debriefing sessions, being available during deployment, often in response to a specific traumatic event. Fourteen nations reported that some type of post-deployment psychological support was offered, among which were briefs on homecoming and debriefings (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Cawkill, van den Berg, Arvers, Puente and Cuvelier2008 b). Despite their frequent use in the military in many countries, few studies have evaluated these interventions (Litz et al. Reference Litz, Gray, Bryant and Adler2002). Evaluation of intervention efficacy is important to ensure that they do not cause harm, that they are acceptable to participants and that they provide a beneficial use of resources. Evaluation is especially pertinent given the generally disappointing results that have been obtained from studies that have evaluated both single session (Rose et al. Reference Rose, Bisson, Churchill and Wessely2002; van-Emmerik et al. Reference van-Emmerik, Kamphuis, Hulsbosch and Emmelkamp2002) and multiple session psychological interventions (Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Kitchiner, Kenardy and Bisson2009) more generally. It is noteworthy, however, that the reviewed studies mostly evaluated interventions for individual victims of trauma rather than high-risk occupational groups (see Regel, Reference Regel2007). Furthermore, the reviews of single session interventions each included only one study that examined a military population whereas none of the multiple session intervention studies included the military.

The aim of this review was therefore to examine the evidence for efficacy of psycho-educational interventions for military personnel delivered following operational deployment, where exposure to PTEs is commonplace.

Method

Search strategy

A literature search of Medline, PsycINFO and EMBASE from 1979 to March 2009 was conducted using the terms: (‘military’ or ‘armed forces’ or ‘soldier*’ or ‘army’ or ‘navy’ or ‘air force’ or ‘peacekeeper*’ or ‘combat’) and (‘psychoeducation’ or ‘debrief*’ or ‘stress education’).

Papers were eligible if: (i) they included an evaluation of a psycho-educational intervention with military personnel delivered following exposure to PTEs (this could include a specific deployment incident or the end of a period of deployment) and (ii) they were reported in English. This review is concerned with interventions aimed at prevention rather than treatment of psychological ill-health, therefore papers that reported evaluations of treatment of personnel with diagnosed psychological illness were excluded.

The titles of all retrieved articles were screened. If the study seemed to relate to a psycho-educational intervention for AF personnel, the abstract was reviewed and if the study met the inclusion criteria, the full article was examined. In addition, the bibliographies of retrieved articles were searched and experts in the field were consulted.

Results

Design of included studies

The search identified two distinct types of studies that examined psycho-educational interventions. First, surveys of health and well-being (Tables 1 and 3) did not evaluate interventions directly but asked respondents if they had received a psycho-educational intervention and examined whether receipt of an intervention was predictive of psychosocial outcomes. The second type of study reported the results of intervention trials, of which seven studies were identified (Tables 2 and 4). The robustness of design of these studies varied, with only three studies conducting cluster RCTs (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a; Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Langston, Everitt, Iversen, Fear, Jones and Wesselyin press). Although Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000) included a comparison group, they acknowledge that the study was not an RCT. Most studies that included a comparison group compared the intervention of interest with a no-treatment control, except in the study by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a), in which, for ethical reasons, this was no longer considered appropriate; the comparison group therefore received stress education. The timing of the follow-up assessment varied from immediately post-intervention to 12 months. There was no consistent finding between studies in the duration of follow-up post-intervention when benefits were found.

Table 1. Surveys

Table 2. Intervention studies

RCT, Randomized controlled trial; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; CISD, Critical Incident Stress Debriefing; TRiM, Trauma Risk Management; n.a., not applicable.

Table 3. Study outcomes: surveys

PTSD, Post-traumatic stress disorder; PCL-C, Post-traumatic Stress Disorder Checklist – Civilian version (Blanchard et al. Reference Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley and Forneris1996); BSI, Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis & Spencer, Reference Derogatis and Spencer1982); OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.

Table 4. Study outcomes: intervention studies

AUDIT, Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (Babor et al. Reference Babor, Higgins-Biddle, Saunders and Monteiro2001); CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies – Depression scale (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977); GHQ, General Health Questionnaire (Goldberg & Williams, Reference Goldberg and Williams1988); HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Zigmond & Snaith, Reference Zigmond and Snaith1983); IES, Impact of Events Scale (Horowitz et al. Reference Horowitz, Wilner and Alvarez1979); PCL-C, Post-traumatic Stress Disorder Checklist – Civilian version (Blanchard et al. Reference Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley and Forneris1996); PHQ-D, Patient Health Questionnaire for Depression (Spitzer et al. Reference Spitzer, Kroenke and Williams1999); POS, Perceived Organizational Support scale (Lynch et al. Reference Lynch, Eisenberger and Armeli1999); PTSS-10, Post-Traumatic Symptom Scale (Holen et al. Reference Holen, Sund and Weisaeth1983); SCL-90, Symptom Checklist (Derogatis, Reference Derogatis1983).

a Group differences in this variable at baseline were not controlled for in the analysis.

Study participants

Participants were recruited from the AF of Israel, Sweden, the UK and the USA. Sample sizes ranged from 3461 to 4762 in the surveys and from 41 to 2297 in the intervention studies. Study participation rates were fairly high, but loss to follow-up was also high. All studies were not explicit in reporting inclusion and exclusion criteria.

The types of deployment that participants were involved in included operational warships, peace-keeping missions in Bosnia, Kosovo and Somalia and combat missions in Iraq and the Israel/Lebanon border. The study samples also varied in the extent of their exposure to PTEs. For example, three of the earlier studies, which had small sample sizes (Deahl et al. Reference Deahl, Gillham, Thomas, Searle and Srinivasan1994; Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Peri and Rogel-Fuchs1998; Larsson et al. Reference Larsson, Michel and Lundin2000), included only those who were known to have experienced PTEs. Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) did not limit their population in this way but the participants, who had completed a combat tour in Iraq, were all found to have experienced PTEs. By contrast, 57% of the sample recruited by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a) had been exposed to at least one event. The extent of combat exposure was found to be a significant moderator of outcome in both studies by Adler et al. (see below) but this relationship was not examined in the other studies in the review.

Interventions evaluated

The studies included in this review evaluated a variety of different interventions, which are described below.

Iversen et al. (Reference Iversen, Fear, Ehlers, Hacker, Hull, Earnshaw, Greenberg, Rona, Wessely and Hotopf2008) asked service personnel if they had received a homecoming brief, but the exact nature of the intervention was not known.

Historical Group Debriefing (HGD)

HGD, evaluated by Shalev et al. (Reference Shalev, Peri and Rogel-Fuchs1998), was developed during World War II by Brigadier General Marshall to obtain comprehensive historical overviews of combat events. This method involves asking all members of a team who have experienced the event to describe it in detail in a strict chronological path. Events, thoughts and feelings are of equal importance. Marshall considered that sharing combat stories in this way had the effect of helping to build morale (Shalev, Reference Shalev, Raphael and Wilson2000), albeit without any evidence to support his conclusion. It is also fair to say that his methods have been challenged (Chambers, Reference Chambers2003). Although not primarily a psycho-educational intervention, the process allows participants to be educated about other people's experience of trauma and their responses.

Critical Incident Stress Debriefing (CISD)

CISD was developed for emergency service personnel by Mitchell (Reference Mitchell1983) as part of a system of Critical Incident Stress Management (CISM). Mitchell has described CISD as ‘a psycho-educational small group process’ (Mitchell, Reference Mitchell2009). CISD aims to reduce the risk of post-incident psychological problems by promoting detailed recollection and emotional processing of the event using a structured format. Some adaptations of this model also exist (e.g. Dyregrov, Reference Dyregrov1989; Everly & Mitchell, Reference Everly and Mitchell2000). Variations of this approach were assessed by Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Gillham, Thomas, Searle and Srinivasan1994, Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000) and Larsson et al. (Reference Larsson, Michel and Lundin2000) and a version for multiple incidents was assessed by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a). Orsillo et al. (Reference Orsillo, Roemer, Litz, Ehlich and Friedman1998) also evaluated a debriefing session, although a description of the type of debriefing was not given. A defusing session, as used by Larsson et al. (Reference Larsson, Michel and Lundin2000), is a briefer, less structured version of CISD, which is conducted on the day of the event.

Stress education

Stress education is widely used in military organizations. In the studies by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a), the stress education package included information on identification of stressors, symptoms associated with stress and adaptive coping behaviours.

Trauma Risk Management (TRiM)

TRiM (Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Langston, Everitt, Iversen, Fear, Jones and Wesselyin press) is a peer-group model of psychological risk assessment developed in collaboration with the Royal Marines. TRiM personnel are trained to use a structured interview model to identify those who might be at substantial risk of developing post-incident psychological disorders. These high-risk personnel are encouraged to access appropriate help or support. TRiM attendees are also provided with a booklet or an interactive briefing containing information about stress reactions and how to cope with them. In contrast to the debriefing models described above, TRiM purposely avoids excessive exploration of emotions (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Roberts and Greenberg2003).

Battlemind

Battlemind training (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) is a cognitive and skills-based group approach developed by the US Army. Battlemind aims to normalize reactions to operational stress, build resilience and help participants to recognize difficulties in oneself and one's colleagues and to seek help promptly. Battlemind training focuses on 10 core skills or strengths that helped people cope in the combat environment and highlights how they should be adapted for the home environment.

Battlemind debriefing (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a, Reference Adler, Castro and McGurkb) differs from other debriefing models described above in that it de-emphasizes the recounting of specific events and reactions to them, so as to avoid the risk of re-exposing participants, and instead focuses on normalizing reactions. This is achieved through group discussion that reinforces the concepts included in Battlemind training.

Delivery of interventions

All interventions, with the exception of TRiM, were delivered in a single session. The interventions were all fairly brief, lasting from under 1 h (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) to 2½ h (Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Peri and Rogel-Fuchs1998). This lack of variation meant that we were unable to consider the possible impact of intervention duration on outcomes.

Most interventions were delivered in small groups, with the exception of Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a), in which large-group Battlemind and Stress Education had group sizes of up to 225 and 257 respectively. Only Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) evaluated the effect of group size, and did not find a consistent effect.

There was some variation between studies in the time at which interventions were delivered; this could be after exposure to a PTE (Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Peri and Rogel-Fuchs1998; Larsson et al. Reference Larsson, Michel and Lundin2000; Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Langston, Everitt, Iversen, Fear, Jones and Wesselyin press), at the end of deployment (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a) or soon after returning from deployment (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a; Deahl et al. Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000). Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Gillham, Thomas, Searle and Srinivasan1994) delivered the intervention as soon as possible, either in theatre or on return to the UK. No consistent relationship was identified between time of delivery and intervention outcomes.

Study findings

The findings of the survey and intervention studies are summarized in Tables 3 and 4 respectively. The surveys each reported the effect of the intervention on a single outcome. Orsillo et al. (Reference Orsillo, Roemer, Litz, Ehlich and Friedman1998) found that debriefing was not a significant predictor of psychiatric symptomatology. However, Iversen et al. (Reference Iversen, Fear, Ehlers, Hacker, Hull, Earnshaw, Greenberg, Rona, Wessely and Hotopf2008) found that receipt of a homecoming brief was associated with reduced reporting of PTSD symptoms, although the association became non-significant after adjusting for pre-deployment (childhood adversity) and deployment related (e.g. morale, perceived danger to self) factors.

In the intervention studies, the main outcomes assessed were measures of mental health, the most commonly measured being symptoms of PTSD. Although the interventions were mostly ineffective in terms of PTSD symptoms, two studies by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) both found an interaction between study condition and degree of combat exposure; those in the intervention groups who had experienced high levels of combat reported greater reduction over time in symptoms than those who received stress education. However, it should be noted that effect sizes were small. Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a) also found that, at higher levels of combat exposure, the control group showed greater reduction in symptoms than those who received stress education, although the effect size was small.

Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) also found an interaction effect for sleep problems, which might be expected as sleep problems are substantial in PTSD. At high levels of combat exposure, those who received Battlemind debriefing or small-group Battlemind training reported fewer sleep problems than those who received stress education.

General psychiatric morbidity was assessed in four intervention studies. A beneficial effect of debriefing was found by Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000) at one assessment time and Larsson et al. (Reference Larsson, Michel and Lundin2000) found peer support plus a defusing session to have some benefits over peer support alone. Other interventions did not have a significant impact on general psychiatric morbidity.

Other outcomes were evaluated in very few studies and findings were generally inconsistent across studies. A significant beneficial effect on depression was found with Battlemind training (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) but not CISD (Deahl et al. Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000; Adler et al. Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a). However, personnel who had received CISD reported lower levels of anxiety than controls (Deahl et al. Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000).

Behavioural outcomes were assessed in only two studies, both of which examined alcohol misuse. Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Thomas, Neblett and Jolly2000) found debriefing to be better than a no-treatment control in reducing alcohol misuse whereas Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a) found stress education to be better than CISD. The former intervention included specific advice about limiting alcohol (Jones, personal communication), but it is not known if such advice was included by Adler et al.

Only one study (Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Langston, Everitt, Iversen, Fear, Jones and Wesselyin press) supplemented self-report measures of well-being with data on occupational functioning, by using personnel data on disciplinary offences collected in the year preceding and the year following the introduction of TRiM. Although the authors qualified their findings, commenting that confounding factors made comparison of the groups difficult, the crews who received TRiM reported significantly fewer offences than the control crews at follow-up.

The two studies that assessed stigma related to seeking help for mental health problems did not find an effect of study condition. However, at higher levels of combat exposure, Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) found that those who received large-group Battlemind reported less stigmatizing beliefs than those who received Stress Education.

Where participant feedback about the interventions was obtained (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a; Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Langston, Everitt, Iversen, Fear, Jones and Wesselyin press), interventions were considered acceptable, with modified CISD and Battlemind training being rated more positively than Stress Education.

Discussion

Although psycho-educational interventions are widely implemented in the AF (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Cawkill, van den Berg, Arvers, Puente and Cuvelier2008 b), few have undergone systematic evaluation. Overall, this review found evidence of some benefit; however, there is variation in the composition of the psycho-educational interventions studied and the outcomes where benefit was found. The effects, where found, are modest.

It might be argued that the largely non-statistically significant findings of this review indicate that psycho-education for military personnel is ineffective in preventing deployment-related psychological ill-health and therefore unnecessary. However, such a decision may be premature. The studies by Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) indicate that the effects are greater in those most at risk (albeit with small effect sizes). Given the high operational tempo currently being experienced by AF deployed in Afghanistan, the need for interventions that support mental health, such as psycho-education, may be increasing and result in significant health benefits if appropriate interventions are delivered.

However, it does not follow that interventions should only target those who have experienced high levels of PTEs; in practice this is neither feasible nor desirable. Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) stress the importance of including all unit members, as participants are encouraged to watch out for and support each other and the inclusion of personnel who are at low risk of developing problems may benefit those at higher risk. They also argue that selecting out some personnel could potentially increase stigma and also be more complex logistically. As few well-designed studies of interventions have been conducted to date, it would be premature to restrict them to subsamples of the deployed population at this stage.

The review highlights several areas in which research in this field can develop, as follows.

Study design

Because of the paucity of RCTs of psycho-educational interventions in this population, we accepted other levels of evidence in this review, including surveys, non-randomized controlled and single-group repeated-measures designs. Of these, the RCT is commonly considered to provide the best level of evidence for establishing treatment efficacy (e.g. Rawlins, Reference Rawlins2008; Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine, 2009). Controlled studies that do not use random allocation are more susceptible to selection bias. Cross-sectional surveys provide evidence of association but not causality and potential confounding variables may not be distributed evenly between intervention recipients and non-recipients. Studies using single-group repeated-measures designs provide very limited evidence as improvement over time could reflect natural improvement, regression to the mean or a placebo effect.

It is acknowledged that conducting good quality RCTs with military populations is difficult, but not impossible, although smaller nations in particular may be limited by the substantial costs involved. However, as the risk of bias is higher in other study designs, good quality RCTs are essential to provide reliable evaluations (Pocock & Elbourne, Reference Pocock and Elbourne2000). Indeed, the structure of the AF lends itself more to randomization by cluster rather than by individual, where the cluster consists of personnel belonging to the same team who therefore work closely together. Cluster randomization is essential to prevent ‘contamination’ between study arms. Analysis of study findings in a cluster RCT also takes account of the similarities that exist between members of the same team. It is encouraging to note that the most recent intervention studies in this review did use the cluster RCT design.

Another aspect of design that needs careful consideration is the nature of the control group. Studies in this review used mostly no-treatment controls. As many AF now provide some form of psycho-education as ‘standard care’, it is likely that future studies will need to compare any new intervention with the pre-existing intervention, such as in Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a), where stress education was the control condition.

Studies that compare more than one intervention, where a single component is varied, would also help to identify which components are most important.

Intervention content

Medical Research Council (MRC) guidance on developing and evaluating complex interventions (Craig et al. Reference Craig, Dieppe, Macintyre, Michie, Nazareth and Petticrew2008) recommends that interventions have a coherent theoretical basis, drawn from evidence on what factors are related to outcomes and the likely process of change. Intervention content should be informed by evidence on potentially modifiable factors that are related to poor psychological ill-health in AF populations (e.g. Britt et al. Reference Britt, Davison, Bliese and Castro2004; Hoge et al. Reference Hoge, Castro, Messer, McGurk, Cotting and Koffman2004; Iversen et al. Reference Iversen, Fear, Ehlers, Hacker, Hull, Earnshaw, Greenberg, Rona, Wessely and Hotopf2008). This approach has been adopted in Battlemind training, for example, which aims to address factors such as stigma, which is a barrier to help-seeking (French et al. Reference French, Rona, Jones and Wessely2004; Hoge et al. Reference Hoge, Castro, Messer, McGurk, Cotting and Koffman2004), and social support, which is related to better outcomes.

Process of change

The likely process of change also needs to be examined more closely. For example, if improvement in psychological health is theorized to arise through building resilience, then approaches that are thought to build resilience should be made explicit and incorporated into the intervention. Change in these process variables should also be measured.

Moderators of effect

The findings of Adler et al. (Reference Adler, Litz, Castro, Suvak, Thomas, Burrell, McGurk, Wright and Bliese2008 a, Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a) suggest that the extent of combat exposure may moderate the effect of post-deployment interventions. Further study is required to identify other potential moderator variables to ascertain which type of intervention is likely to be of most benefit to which participants and on which outcomes. When Sijbrandij et al. (Reference Sijbrandij, Olff, Reitsma, Carlier and Gersons2006) conducted a ‘dismantling trial’ comparing emotional ventilation debriefing, educational debriefing and no debriefing in 236 adult survivors of trauma, they found that participants in the emotional debriefing who had a high baseline hyperarousal score had more PTSD symptoms than those in the control group at 6 weeks. These findings highlight the need to consider the interaction between the population and the nature of the intervention when evaluating effectiveness, and further examination of these factors may help in the development of more beneficial interventions.

Study evaluation

In addition to health outcomes, acceptability of the intervention is also important. Greenberg et al. (Reference Greenberg, Langston, Fear, Jones and Wessely2009), asked Royal Navy personnel if they had received stress education during their service. They found that receipt of stress education was not a significant predictor of PTSD for the full sample. When participants were categorized as to whether or not they thought the brief useful, those who found it useful were less likely to be a PTSD case than those who had not received education. However, those who did not find it useful were no more or less likely to be a case than those who had not received a brief. Obtaining participant ratings of the intervention is therefore important.

An assessment of intervention fidelity, that is whether it was delivered as intended (Bellg et al. Reference Bellg, Borrelli, Resnick, Hecht, Minicucci, Ory, Ogedegbe, Orwig, Ernst and Czajkowski2004), is also recommended.

Intervention efficacy should be assessed based on what the intervention was designed to target. Studies need to consider the most relevant outcomes. This review demonstrates that a wide range of outcome measures have been used to evaluate military psycho-educational interventions; however, several have been assessed in too few studies to draw meaningful conclusions about the impact upon them. The question about the most appropriate outcomes, raised by Deahl et al. (Reference Deahl, Srinivasan, Jones, Neblett and Jolly2001), remains important. Although the presence of PTSD (either caseness or symptoms) is often the focus of interventions, symptoms of PTSD are not the only consequence of traumatic experiences; in fact depressive symptoms and alcohol misuse are more common (Rona et al. Reference Rona, Jones, French, Hooper and Wessely2004; Fear et al. Reference Fear, Jones, Murphy, Hull, Iversen, Coker, Machell, Sundin, Woodhead, Jones, Greenberg, Landau, Dandeker, Rona, Hotopf and Wessely2010), whereas aggression and violence might have the most adverse consequences. In addition to symptoms, assessment of occupational and social functioning is also important. Studies in this review have mostly measured adverse outcomes; however, if the aim of the intervention is to enhance well-being, then an assessment of positive psychological health is also merited.

Limitations of the reviewed studies

The reviewed studies had several limitations that should be taken into account when interpreting their findings. The findings of studies that did not use RCT methodology are susceptible to bias. Several studies did not report inclusion and exclusion criteria, and therefore the extent to which their results can be generalized must be treated with caution. Future studies must be more explicit about their population selection, which should be aided by journals requesting that reports meet Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines (Begg et al. Reference Begg, Cho, Eastwood, Horton, Moher, Olkin, Pitkin, Rennie, Schulz, Simel and Stroup1996).

In many studies loss to follow-up was high, although this may result from difficulty in locating a mobile population. Tate et al. (Reference Tate, Jones, Hull, Fear, Rona, Wessely and Hotopf2007) found that non-response in a military population was related to younger age, lower rank, male gender, non-white ethnicity and reservist enlistment but not to health. Given that the nature of military populations is unlikely to change, future authors should give more thought to establishing that loss to follow-up is at least not biased by outcome (see, for example, the assessment of bias by Adler et al. Reference Adler, Bliese, McGurk, Hoge and Castro2009 a).

Limitations of the review

Limitations of this review must also be considered. Inclusion criteria for studies in the review were not limited to RCTs and therefore the quality of some of the included studies is suboptimal. However, as this is the first published review of psycho-educational interventions in this population, and to date few studies have been conducted, it was considered important to be as inclusive as possible to provide an overview of work conducted in the area. The limitations in the design of included studies have been reported in the review.

The type of interventions evaluated in the review form an eclectic mix and therefore the findings of one type of psycho-education are not necessarily applicable to psycho-education as a whole. However, combining these interventions in a single review is justified to gain a broader understanding of interventions that aim to mitigate the potential adverse effects of deployment. Furthermore, our recommendations for how to progress research in this field apply across the variety of interventions.

The summary of the findings was made discursively rather than by using meta-analysis. This method was chosen because meta-analysis requires homogeneity in the interventions evaluated and in the outcomes assessed. Given the heterogeneity of the included studies in terms of design and intervention evaluated, meta-analysis was not considered to be an appropriate methodology.

The review is limited to published studies and therefore may not be an exhaustive summary of the area. However, we did ask experts in the field if they were aware of any other studies that we had not identified. As none were reported to us, it is probably unlikely that any unpublished studies consist of RCTs.

Conclusions

Although widely implemented by military forces in many countries, few psycho-educational interventions have been evaluated systematically in methodologically robust studies. This review found some evidence of benefit for the various psycho-educational interventions that military forces have used, but the effect seems to be small. Some results suggested that the beneficial effects may be greater for those who have been exposed to a greater number of combat events. Given the considerable exposure to traumatic situations currently faced by AF personnel operating in the Middle East, with repeated and/or prolonged operational tours and considerable combat exposure for many, we suggest there remains a pressing need to identify the most useful and effective way of minimizing the impact of exposure to potentially traumatic deployment incidents.

Declaration of Interest

N.G. and N.J. are both AF employees based at the Academic Centre for Defence Mental Health. S.W. is an Honorary Civilian Consultant Advisor in Psychiatry to the British Army (unpaid).

References

Adler, AB, Bliese, PD, McGurk, D, Hoge, CW, Castro, CA (2009 a). Battlemind debriefing and battlemind training as early interventions with soldiers returning from Iraq: randomization by platoon. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 77, 928940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adler, AB, Litz, BT, Castro, CA, Suvak, M, Thomas, JL, Burrell, L, McGurk, D, Wright, KM, Bliese, PD (2008 a). A group randomized trial of critical incident stress debriefing provided to U.S. peacekeepers. Journal of Traumatic Stress 21, 253263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adler, AB, Castro, CA, McGurk, D (2009 b). Time-driven Battlemind Psychological Debriefing: a group-level early intervention in combat. Military Medicine 174, 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adler, AB, Cawkill, P, van den Berg, C, Arvers, P, Puente, J, Cuvelier, Y (2008 b). International military leaders survey on operational stress. Military Medicine 173, 1016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babor, TF, Higgins-Biddle, JC, Saunders, JB, Monteiro, MG (2001). AUDIT. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Department of Mental Health and Substance Dependence, World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Begg, C, Cho, M, Eastwood, S, Horton, R, Moher, D, Olkin, I, Pitkin, R, Rennie, D, Schulz, KF, Simel, D, Stroup, DF (1996). Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. Journal of the American Medical Association 276, 637639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellg, AJ, Borrelli, B, Resnick, B, Hecht, J, Minicucci, DS, Ory, M, Ogedegbe, G, Orwig, D, Ernst, D, Czajkowski, S (2004). Enhancing treatment fidelity in health behavior change studies: best practices and recommendations from the NIH Behavior Change Consortium. Health Psychology 23, 443451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, EB, Jones-Alexander, J, Buckley, TC, Forneris, CA (1996). Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist. Behaviour Research and Therapy 34, 669673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Britt, TW, Davison, J, Bliese, PD, Castro, C (2004). How leaders can influence the impact that stressors have on soldiers. Military Medicine 169, 541545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, JW II (2003). S. L. A. Marshall's Men Against Fire: new evidence regarding fire ratios. Parameters 33, 113121.Google Scholar
Craig, P, Dieppe, P, Macintyre, S, Michie, S, Nazareth, I, Petticrew, M (2008). Developing and evaluating complex interventions: the new Medical Research Council guidance. British Medical Journal 337, a1655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deahl, M, Srinivasan, M, Jones, N, Thomas, J, Neblett, C, Jolly, A (2000). Preventing psychological trauma in soldiers: the role of operational stress training and psychological debriefing. British Journal of Medical Psychology 73, 7785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deahl, MP, Gillham, AB, Thomas, J, Searle, MM, Srinivasan, M (1994). Psychological sequelae following the Gulf War. Factors associated with subsequent morbidity and the effectiveness of psychological debriefing. British Journal of Psychiatry 165, 6065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deahl, MP, Srinivasan, M, Jones, N, Neblett, C, Jolly, A (2001). Evaluating psychological debriefing: are we measuring the right outcomes? Journal of Traumatic Stress 14, 527529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, LR (1983). SCL-90-R Manual II. Clinical Psychometric Research: Towson, MD.Google Scholar
Derogatis, LR, Spencer, PM (1982). The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): Administration, and Procedures Manual – I. Clinical Psychometric Research: Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Dyregrov, A (1989). Caring for helpers in disaster situations: psychological debriefing. Disaster Management 2, 2530.Google Scholar
Everly, GS, Mitchell, JT (2000). Critical Incident Stress Management: Advanced Group Crisis Interventions: A Workbook. International Critical Incident Stress Foundation: Ellicot City, MD.Google Scholar
Fear, NT, Jones, M, Murphy, D, Hull, L, Iversen, A, Coker, B, Machell, L, Sundin, J, Woodhead, C, Jones, N, Greenberg, N, Landau, S, Dandeker, C, Rona, RJ, Hotopf, M, Wessely, S (2010). The consequences of deployment to Iraq and Afghanistan on the mental health of the UK armed forces: a cohort study. Lancet. Published online: 13 May 2010. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60672-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, C, Rona, RJ, Jones, M, Wessely, S (2004). Screening for physical and psychological illness in the British Armed Forces: II: Barriers to screening – learning from the opinions of Service personnel. Journal of Medical Screening 11, 153157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D, Williams, P (1988). A User's Guide to the General Health Questionnaire. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Greenberg, N, Langston, V, Everitt, B, Iversen, A, Fear, NT, Jones, N, Wessely, S (in press). A cluster randomised controlled trial to determine the efficacy of TRiM (Trauma Risk Management) in a military population. Journal of Traumatic Stress.Google Scholar
Greenberg, N, Langston, V, Fear, NT, Jones, M, Wessely, S (2009). An evaluation of stress education in the Royal Navy. Occupational Medicine 59, 2024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Health and Safety Executive (2007). Managing the Causes of Work-related Stress: A Step-by-step Approach using the Management Standards. HSE Books: London.Google Scholar
Hoge, CW, Castro, CA, Messer, SC, McGurk, D, Cotting, DI, Koffman, RL (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of Medicine 351, 1322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holen, A, Sund, A, Weisaeth, L (1983). PTSS-10: Questionnaire for screening disaster victims. In The Alexander L. Kielland Disaster March 27, 1980: Psychological Reactions Among the Survivors [in Norwegian]. Division for Disaster Psychiatry, University of Oslo: Norway.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M, Wilner, N, Alvarez, W (1979). Impact of events scale: a measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine 41, 209218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iversen, AC, Fear, NT, Ehlers, A, Hacker, HJ, Hull, L, Earnshaw, M, Greenberg, N, Rona, R, Wessely, S, Hotopf, M (2008). Risk factors for post-traumatic stress disorder among UK Armed Forces personnel. Psychological Medicine 38, 511522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, N, Roberts, P, Greenberg, N (2003). Peer-group risk assessment: a post-traumatic management strategy for hierarchical organizations. Occupational Medicine 53, 469475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupnick, JL, Green, BL (2008). Psychoeducation to prevent PTSD: a paucity of evidence. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 71, 329331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, G, Michel, P-O, Lundin, T (2000). Systematic assessment of mental health following various types of posttrauma support. Military Psychology 12, 121135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litz, BT, Gray, MT, Bryant, RA, Adler, A (2002). Early intervention for trauma: current status and future directions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 9, 112134.Google Scholar
Lynch, P, Eisenberger, R, Armeli, R (1999). Perceived organizational support: inferior versus superior performance by wary employees. Journal of Applied Psychology 84, 467483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeorge, T, Hacker Hughes, J, Wessely, S (2006). The MOD PTSD decision: a psychiatric perspective. Occupational Health Review 122, 2128.Google Scholar
Mitchell, JT (1983). When disaster strikes: the critical incident stress debriefing process. Journal of Emergency Medical Services 8, 3639.Google ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, JT (2009). Critical Incident Stress Debriefing (CISD) (www.info-trauma.org/flash/media-e/mitchellCriticalIncidentStressDebriefing.pdf). Accessed 18 April 2010.Google Scholar
Morin, CM (1993). Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Orsillo, SM, Roemer, L, Litz, BT, Ehlich, P, Friedman, MJ (1998). Psychiatric symptomatology associated with contemporary peacekeeping: an examination of post-mission functioning among peacekeepers in Somalia. Journal of Traumatic Stress 11, 611625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine (2009). Levels of Evidence (March 2009) (www.cebm.net/index.aspx?o=1025). Accessed 18 April 2010.Google Scholar
Pocock, SJ, Elbourne, DR (2000). Randomized trials or observational tribulations? New England Journal of Medicine 342, 19071909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, LS (1977). A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawlins, MD (2008). De Testimonio: on the evidence for decisions about the use of therapeutic interventions. The Harveian Oration. Royal College of Physicians: London.Google Scholar
Regel, S (2007). Post-trauma support in the workplace: the current status and practice of critical incident stress management (CISM) and psychological debriefing (PD) within organizations in the UK. Occupational Medicine 57, 411416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, NP, Kitchiner, NJ, Kenardy, J, Bisson, J (2009). Multiple session early psychological interventions for the prevention of post-traumatic stress disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 3, CD006869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rona, RJ, Jones, M, French, C, Hooper, R, Wessely, S (2004). Screening for physical and psychological illness in the British Armed Forces. I: The acceptability of the programme. Journal of Medical Screening 11, 148152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S, Bisson, J, Churchill, R, Wessely, S (2002). Psychological debriefing for preventing post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2, CD000560.Google Scholar
Shalev, AY (2000). Stress management and debriefing: historical concepts and present patterns. In Psychological Debriefing: Theory, Practice and Evidence (ed. Raphael, B. and Wilson, J.), pp. 1731. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shalev, AY, Peri, T, Rogel-Fuchs, Y (1998). Historical group debriefing after combat exposure. Military Medicine 163, 494498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpley, JG, Fear, NT, Greenberg, N, Jones, M, Wessely, S (2008). Pre-deployment stress briefing: does it have an effect? Occupational Medicine 58, 3034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sijbrandij, M, Olff, M, Reitsma, JB, Carlier, IV, Gersons, BP (2006). Emotional or educational debriefing after psychological trauma. Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 189, 150155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, Z, Weisenberg, M, Schwarnvald, J, Mikulincer, M (1988). Combat stress reaction and posttraumatic stress disorder as determinants of perceived self-efficacy in battle. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 6, 356370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, RL, Kroenke, K, Williams, JBW (1999). Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. Journal of the American Medical Association 282, 17371744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tate, AR, Jones, M, Hull, L, Fear, NT, Rona, R, Wessely, S, Hotopf, M (2007). How many mailouts? Could attempts to increase the response rate in the Iraq war cohort study be counterproductive? BMC Medical Research Methodology 7, 51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van-Emmerik, AA, Kamphuis, JH, Hulsbosch, AM, Emmelkamp, PM (2002). Single session debriefing after psychological trauma: a meta-analysis. Lancet 360, 766771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessely, S, Bryant, RA, Greenberg, N, Earnshaw, M, Sharpley, J, Hacker Hughes, J (2008). Does psychoeducation help prevent post traumatic psychological distress? Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 71, 287302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zigmond, AS, Snaith, RP (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 67, 361370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Surveys

Figure 1

Table 2. Intervention studies

Figure 2

Table 3. Study outcomes: surveys

Figure 3

Table 4. Study outcomes: intervention studies