Introduction
Working memory (WM) is a limited capacity cognitive system that functions to hold information in an active manner to facilitate the performance of complex cognitive tasks (Miyake and Shah, Reference Miyake and Shah1999). Such tasks include, for example, language comprehension, learning, abstract thinking (Twamley et al., Reference Twamley, Palmer, Jeste, Taylor and Heaton2006), problem-solving (Westen, Reference Westen2006), understanding the meaning of complex texts and planning verbal communications (Zihl and von Cramon, Reference Zihl and von Cramon1979). WM is limited in both capacity and duration and is often used synonymously, but inaccurately, with the term ‘short-term memory’ (Westen, Reference Westen2006). Baddeley and Hitch (Reference Baddeley, Hitch and Bower1974) expanded upon this WM concept and proposed a tripartite WM model that includes a central executive and two ‘slave systems’; the phonological loop and the visuospatial store. The visuospatial store is further broken down into (1) visual memory information that includes dimensions such as colour and shape and (2) spatial memory information that includes the capacity to understand, reason and to remember the spatial relations among objects or space. (Baddeley and Logie, Reference Baddeley, Logie, Miyake and Shah1999; Mammarella et al., Reference Mammarella, Pazzaglia and Cornoldi2008). There is accruing evidence that the two components of visuospatial memory are selectively engaged and/or processed by distinct brain regions and neuropsychological functions (Della Sala et al., Reference Della Sala, Gray, Baddeley, Allamano and Wilson1999; Passolunghi and Mammarella, Reference Passolunghi and Mammarella2010; Bormann et al., Reference Bormann, Seyboth, Umarova and Weiller2015; Eriksson et al., Reference Eriksson, Vogel, Lansner, Bergstrom and Nyberg2015).
There are a few brain imaging studies on visuospatial memory impairments among drug users. Kubler and colleagues reported that cocaine-dependent individuals were impaired in visuospatial WM. These were associated with prefrontal, cingulate and striatal regions (Kubler et al., Reference Kübler, Murphy and Garavan2005). In another study, opiate-dependent individuals were impaired in WM-related brain areas (Bach et al., Reference Bach, Vollstädt-Klein, Frischknecht, Hoerst, Kiefer, Mann, Ende and Hermann2012).
Hyman and colleagues have conceptualised the behavioural phenomena typically described as ‘addiction’ to a ‘pathological usurpation of the neural mechanisms of learning and memory that under normal circumstances serve to shape survival behaviours related to the pursuit of rewards and the cues that predict them’ (Hyman, Reference Hyman2005; Hyman et al., Reference Hyman, Malenka and Nestler2006).
In support of the potential centrality of learning and memory changes within drug addiction, two recent meta-analyses of observational studies suggested that chronic opioid exposure is associated with deficits across a range of different neuropsychological domains including attentional set-shifting, spatial planning and (Baldacchino et al., Reference Baldacchino, Balfour, Passetti, Humphris and Matthews2012, Reference Baldacchino, Armanyous, Balfour, Humphris and Matthews2017; Tolomeo et al., Reference Tolomeo, Gray, Matthews, Steele and Baldacchino2016, Reference Tolomeo, Matthews, Steele and Baldacchino2018). However, these meta-analyses also suggested that opioid-exposed groups with apparent WM impairments are a highly heterogenous group with mixed ages, educational attainment, gender and socio-economic status (Baldacchino et al., Reference Baldacchino, Balfour, Passetti, Humphris and Matthews2012, Reference Baldacchino, Armanyous, Balfour, Humphris and Matthews2017). Additionally, visuospatial memory impairments in opioid-exposed groups are confounded by, for example, comorbid personality disorders (Prosser et al., Reference Prosser, Eisenberg, Davey, Steinfeld, Cohen, London and Galynker2008), anxiety and/or depression (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Umbricht, Kleykamp, Vandrey, Strain, Bigelow and Mintzer2012), past and present medical conditions, neurological disorders and history of head trauma and non -fatal overdose (anoxic) episodes (Rounsaville et al., Reference Rounsaville, Jones, Novelly and Kleber1982; Specka et al., Reference Specka, Finkbeiner, Lodemann, Leifert, Kluwig and Gastpar2000; Prosser et al., Reference Prosser, Eisenberg, Davey, Steinfeld, Cohen, London and Galynker2008; Shmygalev et al., Reference Shmygalev, Damm, Weckbecker, Berghaus, Petzke and Sabatowski2011). Cognitive function may also be influenced by the global sedative effects of opioid drugs, sub-acute responses to the drugs or the presence of untreated withdrawal states (Baldacchino et al., Reference Baldacchino, Armanyous, Balfour, Humphris and Matthews2017) at time of testing. Table 1 summarises studies that have recorded significant impairments in visuospatial memory in chronic opioid using populations.
* = Opioid group compared with healthy controls unless otherwise stated; 1 = Abstinent group compared with methadone and/or buprenorphine group and NOT healthy controls.
p < 0.05; ↔ = no difference in neuropsychological performance; ↓ = neuropsychological impairment present; ↑ = improvement in neuropsychological performance when compared with healthy controls, d = Cohen's effect size defined as the difference between two means divided by a standard deviation for the data. Standardised effect sizes are reported regardless of the statistical significance (p value) of the results reported in the original studies.
BVRT, Benton Visual Retention Test; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; SOPT, Self-Ordered Pointing Test; WAIS-III, Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale- 3rd Edition; WMSR, Weschler Memory Scale Revised; CANTAB:DMS, Delayed Matching to Sample; PAL, Paired Associate Learning Task, PRM, Pattern Recognition Memory; SSP, Spatial Span.
The present study aimed to extend our understanding of neurocognitive performance in dependent and non-dependent opioid users, focusing on visuospatial memory function. Employing an ambispective cohort design, we tested representative samples of male participants exposed to illicit and therapeutic opioid drugs and matched, non-substance using, healthy controls. Specifically, the study aimed to determine if performance on tasks measuring visuospatial memory, especially delayed memory performance, which is very sensitive for the varying of ‘executive demands’, was affected by (1) the type of the opioid exposure (methadone v. heroin) at different stages of treatment seeking, (2) the context (opioids prescribed for pain control compared with illicit opioids) and (3) the presence or absence of syndromal opioid dependence (opioid dependent compared with non–opioid users) and (4) administration route – injection status (opioid dependent and injecting compared with dependent and non-injecting participants). We have previously identified and reported differential effects of heroin, methadone and prescribed analgesic medication on neurocognitive measures of impulsivity (Baldacchino et al., Reference Baldacchino, Balfour and Matthews2014) from the same study cohort.
Method
Participants
Ethical permission for the conduct of this study was provided by the East of Scotland Research Ethics Service (REC reference number: 06/S1401/32). A full description of the participants can be found in Baldacchino et al. (Reference Baldacchino, Balfour and Matthews2014). Male only participants were recruited from UK NHS substance misuse and pain management services. A control group of healthy participants was also recruited. All participants were screened to exclude lifetime or current histories of psychosis, PTSD, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, borderline or psychopathic personality disorders, head injuries; individuals with a lifetime history of non-fatal overdose episodes requiring medical attention (e.g. ambulance call out, CPR), co-occurring benzodiazepine, psychostimulant and alcohol dependence. All participants were screened by an experienced clinician (AB) for acute opioid and opioid withdrawal symptoms prior to the neuropsychological testing.
The Heroin group (H) (N = 24) were ‘first-time’ referrals to a structured Methadone Maintenance Treatment (MMT) programme. The Methadone group (M) (N = 29) were established and stable participants in a MMT programme with objective confirmation of the absence of illicit drug use for more than 6 months. Eighteen of the 29 MMT group participants who showed objective continuing clinical stability were retested 6 months after baseline testing. All recruits making up the H and MMT cohorts presented initially with opioid dependence and reported a history of more than 3 years of continuous and daily illicit opioid use.
Heroin participants (H) performed repeated neuropsychological testing during a single blinded procedure that permitted the objective observation of participants (a) 3–5 hours after their last illicit heroin administration to minimise the confounding cognitive effects of acute intoxication; (b) 10–15 hours after the last heroin dose in a state of controlled opioid withdrawal and subsequently (c) following more than 2 weeks on a stable dose of MMT. Clinically this is known as tolerance testing which is a single-blinded procedure that permitted the objective observation of individuals during stages of acute intoxication, withdrawal and subsequent stabilisation on a fixed dose methadone within a period of 7–14 days (Baldacchino, Reference Baldacchino2011).
The two opioid dependent groups (H and M groups) were matched for lifetime drug use history, morphine equivalent dosages and other drug use (including tobacco smoking) history 30 days prior to baseline testing. The CANTAB neuropsychological tests presented here refer to the standard tests selected from the batteries used at baseline testing. Where available, parallel versions of the tasks were used with the same participant to minimise practice effects.
This approach offered the opportunity to test whether any visuospatial memory measures that differed from those of control participants represented a stable phenomenon, or could be modified by differential opioid exposure and switch to an alternate opioid (MMT). A cohort of non-dependent participants prescribed opioids for chronic pain for more than 3 years (P) (N = 28) with no history of ‘illicit’ opioid use, or methadone treatment was also recruited. This group was prescribed tramadol, codeine, or both, for moderate chronic pain. Both P and HC groups were tested only once (Table 2).
† = tested; — = not tested.
Instruments
Clinical
All subjects were screened using the MINI Plus v. 5.0 (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998), Maudsley Addiction Profile (Marsden et al., Reference Marsden, Gossop, Stewart, Best, Farrell, Lehmann, Edwards and Strang1998), and Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (Fagerstrom and Schneider, Reference Fagerstrom and Schneider1989). Urine samples were collected from all participants to confirm their history of recent opioid intake and to confirm the absence of any other illicit drugs throughout the study period. The Clinical Opiate Withdrawal Scale (COWS) (Wesson & Ling, Reference Wesson and Ling2003), quantified the level of opioid withdrawal in the heroin group. A senior research nurse and an experienced clinician conducted the assessments. Both were clinically trained. No participants had HIV or AIDS or other medical comorbidities that could affect cognitive functions.
Cognitive
The neuropsychological tasks from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Robbins et al., Reference Robbins, James, Owen, Sahakian, McInnes and Rabbitt1994) were selected on the basis of their known sensitivity to detect impairments in (a) visual [Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS), Pattern Recognition Memory (PRM), Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM) and Paired Associate Learning (PAL)] and (b) spatial [Spatial Span Task (SSP) and Spatial WM (SWM) memory performance]. Pre-morbid general intelligence was assessed using the National Adult Reading Test (NART) (Nelson, Reference Nelson1982) (online Supplementary Table S1).
Data analysis
Data meeting assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance were analysed using ANOVA (Winer et al., Reference Winer, Brown and Michels1991). All other data were compared using appropriate non-parametric tests (e.g. Kruskal–Wallis and Mann–Whitney tests). Preliminary analysis of all the experimental and control groups separately indicated that the samples did not come from normally distributed populations with the same standard deviation. A planned (a priori) contrasts analysis was, therefore, run to test for significant differences between the four independent study groups. Mann Whitney U tests established that NART, age, morphine equivalent dosage and previous alcohol use all needed to be used as covariates for further analyses.
Mann Whitney U tests were also performed to examine: (a) sociodemographic characteristics for participants in the H group, comparing those who experienced the lowest (n = 8) and highest (n = 8) scores on the COWS. Similarly, the same test was used to determine if there were differences between the H group of participants who were tested at baseline and those who were followed up and tested in withdrawal and, subsequently, on methadone. (b) sociodemographic characteristics for participants in the MMT group, comparing those tested at baseline (n = 29) and those followed up after 6 months (n = 18). (c) sociodemographic characteristics for participants in the H and M groups comparing those with a lifetime subjective history of injecting illicit opioids (n = 41) and those with no history of injecting (n = 11). A high COWS score was defined as a score between 18 and 25; a low COWS score was defined as a score 8–14.
The data were first analysed using an omnibus test to determine if significant differences existed between the groups. If the test revealed significance, appropriate pair-wise comparisons were performed. In order to control for family-wise error, post hoc Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons was used (Field, Reference Field2009). P values <0.01 were considered significant. This minimised the effects of multiple comparisons, subgroup analyses and/or repeated measures as we were considering a family of statistical inferences simultaneously (Sainani, Reference Sainani2009). Those reported as between p < 0.05 and p > 0.01 are presented as non-significant trends when they are considered relevant to substantiate the interpretation of other significant results.
ANCOVA was used to test for group differences with respect to visuospatial memory performance measures. The PRM and SWM outcomes did not meet assumptions of normality and were square-root transformed prior to performing the ANCOVA. However, PAL outcomes were log10 transformed prior to performing the ANCOVA. For incremental levels of difficulty within the testing sessions, the within - subject factor DIFFICULTY was introduced, [e.g. SWM (between/within search errors], SSP (span length between 1–9), DMS (0, 4 and 12 s delays) and PAL (1, 2, 3, 6, or 8 shapes)). Homogeneity of variance was assessed using the Mauchly Sphericity Test. Where data sets significantly (p < 0.05) violated this requirement, the Greenhouse Geisser Epsilon (^ε) correction parameter for degrees of freedom was used to calculate a more conservative p value for each F ratio.
Further, a priori subgroup analyses were conducted using (1) a two-group factor reflecting DEPENDENCE status (H and M groups v. P and HC groups) and (2) a two-group factor reflecting INJECTING status (H and M injecting v. H and M never injecting groups) separately as between-subject factors. Importantly, we had specific a priori hypotheses about the impact of dependence on the H and M groups, however, we could not draw any particular conclusion about the exposure of the opiate use. In addition, we used DEPENDENCE as a proxy clinical measure of severity without any biological basis.
Similarly, repeated measures ANCOVA was used to evaluate all neuropsychological performance measures between the H group at baseline, in controlled opioid withdrawal and subsequently when stabilised on methadone with presumed opioid receptor occupancy state as a within-subjects factor. Similarly, repeated measures ANCOVA was performed for the M group at baseline and at 6 months follow up with duration as a within-subjects factor.
All analyses were conducted using SPSS for Windows (v.18, SPSS Inc. Chicago, Ill.).
Results
Demographic characteristics
A description of demographics, drug use and smoking variables for the four groups is presented in Table 3. The H and M groups differed from the P and HC groups with respect to several clinical characteristics. Opioid-dependent participants started to drink alcohol approximately 2 years earlier than the other groups. The mean morphine equivalent daily dose for the P group was significantly lower (59.1 mg) than the H and M groups (165.9 mg) (p < 0.001).
Sig¹ = significance at p < 0.01 two-tailed, 2Stable accommodation = own house + rented accommodation + living with parents (excluded hostel, student and homeless).
* = mean total scores (+/- standard deviation), * 1 = mean, *2 = Some participants prescribed Tramadol were also prescribed Codeine hence total number (31) higher than number recruited (n = 28).
n/a, no data is relevant as the Pain and HC groups did not present with illicit heroin use and/or dependence history; yrs, years; SIMD, Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation; NART, National Adult Reading Test; %, percentage; ns, not significant; N, Total number in group; yrs, years; n, number of individuals analysed; mg, milligrammes; ⁰Opioid equivalence: [Vieweg et al. (Reference Vieweg, Carlyle-Lipps and Fernandez2005)].
When comparing high against low scores for COWS in the H group, there were no differences between age (p = 0.88), SIMD score (p = 0.75), years in education (p = 0.38), years when starting using alcohol (p = 0.07), alcohol amount used in last month (p = 0.87) or current level of nicotine dependence (Fagerström scores) (p = 0.96).
Similarly, there were no group differences identified on these measures when comparing H group tested at baseline and those retested either through the tolerance testing protocol 6 months later when taking methadone. There were no significant differences with demographic and drug use characteristics between injecting participants (n = 43) and non-injecting participants (n = 10). However, NART scores were significantly higher (p < 0.01) in the injecting group.
Visual memory
Performance on DMS
There was a significant effect on the percentage of correct responses [GROUP F (3100) = 10.3, p < 0.001]. There were no significant performance differences between groups with respect to the simultaneous matching condition. Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons, however, showed participants from the H group made significantly more errors than (a) the HC group at the 0 (p < 0.005), 4 (p < 0.001) and 12 s (p < 0.001) delay stages, (b) the P group for the 4 (p < 0.01) and 12 s (p < 0.005) delay stages and (c) the M group for 0 (p < 0.005), 4 (p < 0.005) and 12 s (p < 0.001) delay stages (Fig. 1). In summary, the H group exhibited significant delay-dependent memory impairment when compared with the comparison and control groups.
Performance on PRM, SRM and PAL
There were no significant GROUP effects on the number of correct trials [F < 1] and mean response latencies [F < 1] on the PAL and PRM tests. There was a non-significant GROUP trend on the total number of correct trials [F (3102) = 3.6, p = 0.02] on the SRM only.
Spatial memory
Performance on SSP
There was a significant GROUP [F (3102) = 16.8, p < 0.001] effect for total errors. Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons showed that the participants from the H group significantly made more errors compared with the M (p < 0.001, d = 1.25) and HC (p < 0.005, d = 1.14) groups (Fig. 2). The total error score for the P group lay between those of the H, M and HC groups and did not differ significantly from any of the other three groups (p = 1.0).
There was also a significant GROUP [F (3101) = 3.7, p < 0.01] effect for span length with post hoc Bonferroni comparisons showing the M group was significantly less able to recall successfully the longest sequence compared with HC group (p < 0.01, d-1.17). The span length for the H (p = .41) and the P (p = .21) groups lay between those of the M and HC groups and did not differ significantly from any of the other groups (Fig. 2).
Performance on SWM
There was a non-significant GROUP trend for total mean errors [F(3102) = 3.2, p = 0.03] and strategy score [F(3102) = 2.9, p = 0.04] (Tables 4 and 5).
d = effect size, SQRT = square root transformation; log10 = logarithmic 10 transformation, Sig = significance.
* = p < 0.01, ** = p < 0.005, *** = p < 0.001, NS = no significant impairment in neuropsychological outcomes with p < 0.01, H = HEROIN Group, P = CHRONIC PAIN Group, M = METHADONE Group, C = HEALTHY CONTROL Group.
* = ANCOVA ‘between subject factor’ of GROUP, DEPENDENCE and INJECTING analysed separately; 1 = significant effects with p < 0.01, ↓ = significant neuropsychological impairments present, ↔ = no significant neuropsychological impairments present.
Chronic opioid dependence or injecting status and visuospatial memory performance
There were no significant effects for either of the factors DEPENDENCE or INJECTING STATUS on any of the DMS, PRM, SRM, and PAL outcome measures.
However, there were significant DEPENDENCE effects for total errors [F (3104) = 6.5, p < 0.01] on the SWM, but with no significant DEPENDENCE effects on the strategy score [F(1104) = 4.8,p = 0.03]. There was a significant DEPENDENCE status and task difficulty interaction on the SWM test for total errors [F (3133.75) = 6.2, p < 0.01]. Analysis using INJECTING status failed to reveal any significant effects or interactions on any SWM outcomes.
There was a significant effect of DEPENDENCE status [F (1103) = 7.1, p < 0.01] for span length on the SSP test, but not for total errors [F (1104) = 1.1, p = 0.29]. There was no significant effect on INJECTING status on SSP outcomes.
Type of opioid exposure at different stages of treatment and visuospatial memory performance
When the H group was tested during different states of opioid exposure (tolerance testing) there was a significant effect of on the DMS mean correct latency [F (2, 34.22) = 10.5, p < 0.001]. Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons showed a significant improvement at the 12 s delay stage (p < 0.001) but not the 0 s and 4 s delay conditions. These improvements were noted in comparison with the stable MMT, the ‘withdrawal’ stage (p < 0.005) and the illicit heroin stage (p < 0.001). There was no effect on PRM, SRM, PAL, SSP and SWM outcomes.
There was a trend (p < 0.05) for the M group to improve on DMS and SWM outcomes in selecting the right stimulus following prolonged exposure to a stable dose of methadone. There were no significant additional effects on all PRM, SRM, PAL, and SSP outcomes in the M group following prolonged exposure to a stable dose of methadone.
Discussion
This study identified the differential effects of chronic heroin and methadone exposures on neuropsychological measures of visuospatial memory that were independent of estimates of addiction severity (injecting behaviour, dependence status). The study also identified an improvement in DMS performance (specifically at longer delays) when the M group were compared with the H group and also when the H group was tolerance tested and then stabilised on methadone.
Interpretation
Although there are likely commonalities in the ways in which all opioids can affect cognitive performance, much can be learned from considering the distinctive features of each type of opioid and its effect on visuospatial memory. In this study, we have described significant differences in performance between the heroin, methadone and chronic pain groups. The H but not the M group differentially showed impairment in visual memory whereas both the H and M groups showed impairment in spatial memory. Importantly, the performance of the licit opioid-exposed group was broadly similar to that of the HC group. However, due to the significantly lower dose equivalence in the licit opioid group one needs to cautiously suggest that the impairments in visuospatial memory reported are evoked by chronic exposure to illicit opioids. Participants with potential confounders, such as impaired mood state (Jollant et al., Reference Jollant, Guillaume, Jaussent, Castelnau, Malafosse and Courtet2007), non-fatal overdoses, co-morbid personality disorders (Vassileva et al., Reference Vassileva, Petkova, Georgiev, Martin, Tersiyski, Raycheva, Velinov and Marinov2007) or a co-occurrence of polydrug dependence were excluded from the study. Thus, the impairments in visuospatial memory measures, seen in the participants who are opioid dependent, cannot be caused by these potential confounders.
Heroin users presented with significant delayed memory impairments when compared with either M or P groups using a cross-sectional comparison. These impairments diminished with a longer duration of stable methadone. Additionally, within-subject comparisons of participants who had used illicit heroin but had been transferred to a stable dose of methadone for only a few weeks also described a significant improvement in visuospatial impairments when stabilised on methadone. The poor performance of the H group compared with the M group supports previous findings of deficits in learning and memory that may be a function of damage from neurotoxicity to the hippocampal formation in the temporal lobe (Day et al., Reference Day, Langston and Morris2003) which is structurally altered by drug addictions (Robbins and Everitt, Reference Robbins and Everitt2002) and possibly reversed through administration of opioid replacement therapy such as methadone.
However, since DMS outcome impairment did increase significantly as a function of delay in the heroin group, the results are also suggesting that the impairment might also lie in higher order cognitive executive processes rather than solely as impairment in the memory storage process.
Additionally, tasks such as Paired Associates Learning (PAL) are associated with hippocampal function and may be highly sensitive to identify those with memory impairments.
Even though this study did not investigate the cognitive impairments observed in response to different opioids using molecular pharmacological techniques one still needs to be aware that heroin, methadone, codeine and tramadol interact with different μ opioid receptor subtypes exhibiting different activation profiles. This results in subtle pharmacological differences in potency, effectiveness, tolerability and neurotoxicity of the drugs (Pasternak, Reference Pasternak2012). These opioids also have variable agonist activity at both δ and κ opioid receptors (Pathan and Williams, Reference Pathan and Williams2012). Furthermore, the active metabolites of heroin and methadone display multimodal subunit-dependent antagonism of 5-HT3 receptors (Deeb et al., Reference Deeb, Sharp and Hales2009) and methadone, but not heroin, displays N-methyl- D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist properties (Davis and Inturrisi, Reference Davis and Inturrisi1999). The licit opioid users were prescribed either tramadol, codeine or both in much lower morphine equivalent doses. Tramadol, like methadone, is an opioid receptor agonist that, in addition to its MOP effects, also have activity at other non-opioid sites through the modulation of serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake (Pathan and Williams, Reference Pathan and Williams2012). These cellular and molecular variations might determine different neuropsychological impairments (Baldacchino et al., Reference Baldacchino, Balfour and Matthews2014).
The different neuropsychological impairments observed in the heroin and methadone cohorts might be linked to other factors that could selectively influence visuospatial processing. Human studies found impaired vigilance and slower reaction times in patients receiving high doses of methadone (Hepner et al., Reference Hepner, Homewood and Taylor2002). This suggests that there might be a trade-off between the intended effects of opioid agonists and the promotion of cognitive abilities. Current results suggest that SWM capacity is intact in opiate-dependent patients when treated with a moderate opioid dose. However, there may be individual patients (e.g. those treated with high opioid doses, using illicit heroin or using non-opioid drugs frequently) that show deficits in SWM. The strict methodology of our study attempted to minimise such effects.
Limitations
This study recruited treatment-seeking males and, thus, results may not generalise to non-treatment seeking, or female, populations (Ardila et al., Reference Ardila, Rosselli, Matute and Inozemtseva2011; McGivern et al., Reference McGivern, Adams, Handa and Pineda2012). It is important to appreciate the potential impact social deprivation and ageing may have on the neuropsychological performance in opioid dependence (Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, Farah and Meaney2010). Studies indicate that negative and stressful events during the early life period can persistently affect brain development and cognitive function such as learning and memory (Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Nacewicz, Sutterer, Cayo, Schaefer, Rudolph, Shirtcliff, Pollak and Davidson2015; Krugers et al., Reference Krugers, Arpa, Xiong, Kanatsou, Lesuis, Korosi, Joels and Lucassen2017). Drug use and risk factor histories of participants were, by necessity, based upon self-report, and no blood, hair or saliva samples taken to validate the accuracy of the information. Neuropsychological research has shown that consumption of alcohol, benzodiazepines and psychostimulants are potentially important confounding variables (Koob and Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010). The present study used stringent criteria to exclude regular and dependent users of most psychoactive substances. The exception to this was lack of nicotine use in the healthy controls. We could not control for the effects of this psychostimulant and this may have influenced our results due to its known effects on visuospatial memory (Richards et al., Reference Richards, Jarvis, Thompson and Wadsworth2003). This study also conducted urine drug screen analysis to confirm the absence of recent amphetamine, opioids, benzodiazepine and cocaine use prior to every session.
Opioid-dependent participants had a mean daily dose of 165 mg morphine equivalent. The P group, however, had a significant lower mean daily dose of 59.1 mg morphine equivalent. Opioids can cause measurable cognitive impairment even at low doses and equi-analgesic doses of different opioids may have nonlinear and non-equivalent adverse cognitive effects (Gagnon and Bruera, Reference Gagnon and Bruera1999). Opioid drug dose is often the only drug treatment variable that is included in the analyses of correlates of performance in visuospatial memory. Grevert et al. (Reference Grevert, Masover and Goldstein1977) reported a statistically significant correlation (0.37) between methadone dose and trials needed for correct visual recognition. However, when more rigorous statistical methods have been used (such as covariance or regression analyses), the relationships between methadone (Specka et al., Reference Specka, Finkbeiner, Lodemann, Leifert, Kluwig and Gastpar2000; Prosser et al., Reference Prosser, Eisenberg, Davey, Steinfeld, Cohen, London and Galynker2008; Soyka et al., Reference Soyka, Lieb, Kagerer, Zingg, Koller, Lehnert, Limmer, Kuefner and Henning-Fast2008; Yin et al., Reference Yin, Li, Pang, Zhu, Wang, Zhang, Tang and Dai2012) or buprenorphine (Lintzeris et al., Reference Lintzeris, Mitchell, Bond, Nestor and Strang2006; Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Kniest, Diehl, Mann and Croissant2008; Shmygalev et al., Reference Shmygalev, Damm, Weckbecker, Berghaus, Petzke and Sabatowski2011) doses and cognitive performance have turned out to be very low and statistically non-significant. In this study, we could not repeat cognitive testing in the healthy control and we could not recruit groups with similar socioeconomic status. It would be warranted for future studies as this will give a further confirmation of the cognitive improvement found in this study
Finally, we want to highlight that there is no literature to compare if any, dose-related cognitive effects between prescribed methadone, tramadol, codeine and/or combinations.
Clinical interpretation
This study identified opioid specific visuospatial memory impairments that need to be considered within the recovery-oriented treatment programmes for opioid-dependent populations (Ekhtiari et al., Reference Ekhtiari, Rezapour, Robin, Aupperle and Paulus2017). The visuospatial memory impairments will have implications for the successful outcomes of current non-pharmacological approaches, such as relapse prevention techniques and motivational enhancement therapies since all these interventions demand intact sophisticated encoding and retrieval strategies, visual processing and inhibition of irrelevant information. These approaches are reported to improve outcomes in individuals with opioid dependence when they are used to complement traditional therapeutic interventions (Ruiz-Sánchez de León et al., Reference Ruiz-Sánchez de León, Pedrero-Pérez, Rojo-Mota, Llanero-Luque and Puerta-García2011; Rezapour et al., Reference Rezapour, Hatami, Farhoudian, Sofuoglu, Noroozi, Daneshmand, Samiei and Ekhtiari2015). The aims of these novel clinical interventions are to improve the general cognitive functioning, in particular, executive and memory functioning, which the results of this study suggest may be compromised in opioid-dependent treatment-seeking populations.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003318
Acknowledgements
The authors thank NHS Fife Research and Development Department for assistance with the purchase of the CANTAB system. The authors also acknowledge the following services that helped to identify eligible participants. Fife NHS Addiction Services, Tayside Pain Services, Tayside Arrest Referral Team (NCH), Fife Drug Treatment and Testing Order Team, Frontline Fife, Fife Intensive Rehabilitation Support Team, Fife NHS Clinical Psychology Service and a multitude of General Practitioner Practices and NHS Primary Care Health Centres in the Fife and Tayside areas. Finally, to all individuals who gave up so much of their valuable time to participate in this study with the sole aim of helping in the better understanding of the ‘brain’ in addiction.
Declaration of interest
A.B. has received unrestricted educational grants from Schering Plough, Merck Serono, Lundbeck and Indivior. D.J.B. has received research support from Vifor Pharma and a BBSRC Case award in collaboration with MSD and an honorarium from the Society for Research on Nicotine & Tobacco as Editor-in-Chief the Society's research journal, Nicotine & Tobacco Research. K.M. has chaired advisory boards for studies of deep brain stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder sponsored by Medtronic. He has received educational grants from Cyberonics Inc. and Schering Plough, and he has received research project funding from Merck Serono, Lundbeck and Indivior and also from St Jude Medical for a multicentre clinical trial of deep brain stimulation for depression. He has received travel and accommodation support to attend meetings from Medtronic, St Jude Medical, the Focused Ultrasound Foundation and The Leksell Gamma Knife Society.