Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:08:17.131Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Profiles of psychosocial and clinical functioning in adolescence and risk for later depression and other outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2019

Thomas M. Olino*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Temple University, Weiss Hall, Philadelphia, PA19122, USA
Daniel N. Klein
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
John R. Seeley
Affiliation:
University of Oregon & Oregon Research Institute, Eugene, OR, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Thomas M. Olino, E-mail: thomas.olino@temple.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Most studies examining predictors of the onset of depression focus on variable centered regression methods that focus on the effects of multiple predictors. In contrast, person-centered approaches develop profiles of factors and these profiles can be examined as predictors of onset. Here, we developed profiles of adolescent psychosocial and clinical functioning among adolescents without a history of major depression.

Methods

Data come from a subsample of participants from the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project who completed self-report measures of functioning in adolescence and completed diagnostic and self-report measures at follow-up assessments up to approximately 15 years after baseline.

Results

We identified four profiles of psychosocial and clinical functioning: Thriving; Average Functioning; Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress and Internalizing Vulnerability at the baseline assessment of participants without a history of depression at the initial assessment in mid-adolescence. Classes differed in the likelihood of onset and course of depressive disorders, experience of later anxiety and substance use disorders, and psychosocial functioning in adulthood. Moreover, the predictive utility of these classes was maintained when controlling for multiple other established risk factors for depressive disorders.

Conclusions

This work highlights the utility of examining multiple factors simultaneously to understand risk for depression.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is among the most common forms of psychopathology (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005) and accounts for a large proportion of lost productivity in youth and adults (World Health Organization, 2002; Lynch and Clarke, Reference Lynch and Clarke2006). MDD is etiologically heterogeneous (Goodman and Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib2002; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2002) which results in significant challenges in identifying mechanisms leading to the development of, and conversely, preventing, the onset of the disorder. However, the predominant approach for examining the prediction of MDD onset has focused on the influence of specific variables using multiple regression (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Glenn, Kosty, Seeley, Rohde and Lewinsohn2013) or structural equation (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2002) models. These have frequently been termed variable centered approaches (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2000).

Work relying on variable centered methods has identified some well-replicated predictors of MDD onset. Female sex is strongly associated with MDD, particularly after the commencement of puberty (Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Abramson, Moffitt, Silva, Mcgee and Angell1998; Cyranowski et al., Reference Cyranowski, Frank, Young and Shear2000; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Mezulis and Abramson2008). Maternal history of depression is also associated with MDD onset (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley and Olino2005; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011). Beyond these factors, a number of studies have reported associations between MDD and negative cognitive style, stress, subthreshold internalizing problems, externalizing symptoms and disorders, peer and family support and family conflict (e.g. Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Hettema, Butera, Gardner and Prescott2003; Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., Reference Vrshek-Schallhorn, Stroud, Mineka, Hammen, Zinbarg, Wolitzky-Taylor and Craske2015). However, variable centered studies assume that associations hold across all individuals in a given population, ignoring the likely possibility that there are subpopulations of individuals with different etiological pathways toward depression.

In contrast, person-centered approaches do not presume that risk processes are consistent across all individuals in a population (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2000). In a person-centered framework, subgroups of individuals may develop psychopathology through qualitatively different processes (Hankin, Reference Hankin2012; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Haeffel, Hankin, Maxwell and Perera2014). Person-centered methods, such as latent profile analysis (LPA), can identify relatively homogenous subgroups of individuals (i.e. classes) that differ based on profiles of multiple within-person characteristics (Hallquist and Wright, Reference Hallquist and Wright2014). This is similar to cluster analytic methods, but LPA provides additional indices of model fit and is able to quantify precision of assignment to classes and differences between classes on outcomes. In one of the few examples of using LPA to predict future depression, St Clair et al. (Reference St Clair, Croudace, Dunn, Jones, Herbert and Goodyer2015) identified different classes of childhood adversity, including maltreatment and abuse, normative variation in parenting styles, family dissolution, family stress and parental history of psychopathology and examined their relationships with emerging symptoms of depression in adolescents. The authors found that classes characterized by greater dysfunction were associated with higher levels of depression and that some of these associations differed by sex. However, this study was limited to predictors related to familial processes.

The present study examines profiles of adolescent psychosocial and clinical characteristics and later outcomes of psychopathology. We chose to focus on adolescent psychosocial and clinical constructs and use fixed demographic risk factors (i.e. sex), family history and outcomes as validators. Indices of psychosocial functioning include a wide array of risk factors previously examined as variable centered predictors of depressive disorders, including major and minor stressors (Hammen, Reference Hammen2006), cognitive style (Alloy et al., Reference Alloy, Abramson, Hogan, Whitehouse, Rose, Robinson, Kim and Lapkin2000), self-esteem (Sowislo and Orth, Reference Sowislo and Orth2013), future aspirations (Hirsch et al., Reference Hirsch, Duberstein, Conner, Heisel, Beckman, Franus and Conwell2007), peer and family support (Stice et al., Reference Stice, Ragan and Randall2004) and internalizing and externalizing psychopathology (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shankman, Lewinsohn and Seeley2009; Groenman et al., Reference Groenman, Janssen and Oosterlaan2017). Although our analyses are largely exploratory, we hypothesized that classes with higher levels of constructs previously shown to be linked with depression (e.g. negative cognitive styles, poorer social support, greater experience of stress) would be associated with greater risk for the onset of depressive disorders. We also examined associations between class profiles and depressive morbidity, including a number of depressive episodes experienced and a total length of illness. We expected that classes characterized by higher levels of internalizing vulnerability factors, such as negative cognitive style and stress in adolescence, will have a poorer course of depression.

Female sex (Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Abramson, Moffitt, Silva, Mcgee and Angell1998; Cyranowski et al., Reference Cyranowski, Frank, Young and Shear2000; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Mezulis and Abramson2008), anxiety and substance use disorders (Kim-Cohen et al., Reference Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne and Poulton2003; Bittner et al., Reference Bittner, Egger, Erkanli, Jane Costello, Foley and Angold2007) and parental history of psychopathology are also well-established risk factors for depression (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne, Moreau and Olfson1997; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley and Olino2005; Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad, Verdeli, Skipper and Talati2016). We utilized these constructs in two ways. First, we examined whether classes differed on these characteristics. Second, as a conservative test, we examined whether class differences in risk for later depression were still present when controlling for these well-established risk factors. Thus, we test whether classes based on psychosocial constructs incrementally predict risk for depression beyond thoroughly established risk factors for depression. Third, we examined the prediction of later anxiety and substance use disorders (SUDs) as a means of evaluating the specificity of the class utility. Finally, in addition to the emphasis on psychopathological outcomes, we also examined class differences on functional outcomes and life satisfaction as a means of evaluating positive developmental outcomes (Rottenberg et al., Reference Rottenberg, Devendorf, Kashdan and Disabato2018).

Methods

Participants

The present study uses data from the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project (OADP) (Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Hops, Roberts, Seeley and Andrews1993), a longitudinal study of a large cohort of high school students who were assessed twice during adolescence, a third time when the average age was 24 and a fourth time when the average age was 30. For this report, we examined baseline factors that predicted the onset of psychopathology throughout all follow-up assessments. Thus, we only included adolescents who completed the age 30 assessment so that the follow-up duration would be consistent for all participants (total n = 816), which would avoid biases in examining total morbidity of depressive illness by including adolescents with partial follow-up data. Participants with a lifetime history of psychosis or bipolar spectrum disorders were excluded (n = 34). Finally, as the focus of the study was on the prediction of MDD, adolescents with a history of MDD and/or dysthymia at study entry were excluded (n = 215). Thus, the final included sample included 567 participants. Participants were randomly selected from nine high schools in western Oregon. A total of 1709 adolescents (ages 14–18; mean age 16.6, s.d. = 1.2) completed the initial (T 1) assessments between 1987 and 1989. The participation rate at T 1 was 61%. All youth provided informed consent before completing research procedures. Retention across assessment waves was good, with modest differences between participants who did and did not fail to complete follow-ups (Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Hops, Roberts, Seeley and Andrews1993; Olino et al., Reference Olino, Klein, Lewinsohn, Rohde and Seeley2008).

Measures

Proband diagnostic measures

At T 1, T 2 and T 3 probands were interviewed with a version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (K-SADS; Orvaschel et al., Reference Orvaschel, Puig-Antich, Chambers, Tabrizi and Johnson1982), which combined features of the Epidemiologic and Present Episode versions, and included additional items to derive Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition revised (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnoses. Follow-up assessments at T 2 and T 3 were jointly administered with the Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation (LIFE; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Lavori, Friedman, Nielsen, Endicott, Mcdonald-Scott and Andreasen1987). The K-SADS/LIFE procedure provided information regarding the onset and course of disorders since the previous interview. The T 4 interview consisted of a joint administration of the LIFE and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996) to probe for new or continuing episodes since T 3. Diagnoses were based on DSM-III-R criteria for T 1 and T 2 and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for T 3 and T 4. Interviews at T 3 and T 4, were conducted by telephone, which generally yields comparable results to face-to-face interviews (Sobin et al., Reference Sobin, Weissman, Goldstein, Adams, Wickramaratne, Warner and Lish1993; Rohde et al., Reference Rohde, Lewinsohn and Seeley1997). Most interviewers had advanced degrees in a mental health field and several years of clinical experience.

A subset of interviews from each wave was rated from audio or videotapes by a second interviewer for reliability purposes: T 1 = 263, T 2 = 162, T 3 = 190 and T 4 = 124 interviews. Diagnostic agreement among raters was indexed by kappa. To avoid potential inflation, deflation and/or unreliability of the kappa statistic, reliability was calculated only for categories diagnosed 10 or more times by both raters combined. Fleiss (Reference Fleiss1981) provides guidelines for the interpretation of kappa, whereby values ⩾0.75 denote excellent agreement beyond chance, those between 0.75 and 0.40 are indicative of good to fair agreement, and coefficients <0.40 reflect poor agreement. Across the four assessment waves, inter-rater diagnostic reliability was good to excellent for all disorders that occurred with sufficient frequency to be evaluated (Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Seeley, Kosty and Lewinsohn2009; Seeley et al., Reference Seeley, Kosty, Farmer and Lewinsohn2011)

Parental psychopathology

First-degree family members of OADP participants were interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, non-patient version (SCID-NP; First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996) at the time of the T 3 assessment. In addition, family history data were collected from the original OADP participants and at least one other family member using a modified version of the Family Informant Schedule and Criteria (FISC; Mannuzza and Fyer, Reference Mannuzza and Fyer1990), supplemented with items necessary to derive DSM-IV diagnoses. Interviews were conducted without the knowledge of the offspring's diagnoses. All family member participants provided written informed consent before completing research procedures. Of the 568 probands included in this report, diagnostic information was available for 478 mothers (84.1%) and 471 (82.9%) fathers. Direct interviews were available for 365 mothers and 231 fathers (76.4% and 49.0%, respectively, of mothers and fathers with diagnostic information).

As multiple data sources were available for most parents, we derived lifetime best-estimate DSM-IV diagnoses from all available information (Leckman et al., Reference Leckman, Sholomskas, Thompson, Belanger and Weissman1982). Two diagnosticians, from a team of four senior clinicians, independently derived best-estimate diagnoses without knowledge of offspring diagnoses. Disagreements were resolved by consensus. Interrater reliability of the independently derived best-estimate diagnoses prior to the resolution of discrepancies was excellent for MDD (κ = 0.91), any anxiety disorder (κ = 0.94), AUD (κ = 0.97) and SUD (κ = 0.96).

Psychosocial constructs

An extensive battery of psychosocial measures was administered to all participants at T 1 (Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Clarke, Seeley and Rohde1994; Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein and Gotlib2003). Variables were constructed such that higher scores indicated greater impairment or severity. A full description of these self-report measures is presented in the online Supplementary Materials. The target constructs included as indicators of latent profiles were depression, other internalizing problems, externalizing problems, hypomania, minor hassles, major stressors, self-consciousness, negative cognitions, attributional style, self-esteem, social competence, emotional reliance, coping skills, future aspirations in academic, occupational and family domains, family support, peer support and conflict with parents.

At the T 4 assessment, participants completed single-item self-report measures of their highest grade completed and annual household income with nine income ranges. Participants also completed measures of social adjustment and life satisfaction. Fifty-four items from the Social Adjustment Scale, spanning multiple family, social and occupational domains, (Weissman and Bothwell, Reference Weissman and Bothwell1976) were used to assess social adjustment during the two weeks preceding the T 4 interview. Higher scores indicated poorer adjustment. This measure had a coefficient alpha of 0.70 in the current sample and yields similar results to those obtained by the interview format of the instrument (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Prusoff, Thompson, Harding and Myers1978). Fifteen items related to general feelings of happiness and contentment (Andrews and Withey, Reference Andrews and Withey1976; Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Converse and Rodgers1976) were used to assess life satisfaction at T 4. Higher scores indicated poorer life satisfaction. This measure had a coefficient alpha of 0.87 in the current sample.

Data analysis

Latent profile analysis (LPA) models were estimated using Mplus 8.2 (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2018). Missing data at the T 1 assessment were considered missing at random and accommodated using FIML estimation methods. Empirical comparisons of models were based on the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC), corrected AIC (AICC), Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), sample-size adjusted BIC (aBIC), the Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test (LMR-LRT) and the bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT). Lower information criteria values indicate better fit. The LMR-LRT is a comparison of fit between the k and k − 1 class solutions. A significant difference indicates that the k class solution provides a significantly better fit than the k – 1 class solution. Simulation work (Nylund et al., Reference Nylund, Asparouhov and Muthén2007) found that the BIC performed best of the information criteria. Thus, this criterion is weighted most strongly in empirical comparisons within model sets. All models were estimated with a sufficient number of random starts to yield a replicated log-likelihood value. The BLRT indicated that all differences between k and k – 1 classes were significant. As this was not informative, we do not present these results (Table 1).

Table 1. Model fit statistics

Class comparisons on outcomes were implemented using the manual three-step approach recommended by Asparouhov and Muthén (Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2014). We relied on this approach to compare classes as we were interested in class differences on outcomes when including covariates in the model. This approach estimates class differences on outcomes with a pseudo-class draw using posterior probabilities. When there was evidence that there was an omnibus difference in outcomes, we examined pairwise comparisons on outcomes across classes. This method provided a consistent means to examine unadjusted class differences in outcomes, as well as class differences when including covariates.

Results

For complete reporting, we include a full correlation matrix among our key study variables in online Supplementary Materials. All variables were standardized in the full T 1 sample (n = 1709) so that variability in variable values is comparable and can be interpreted with respect to the sample means. We conducted Little's Test of Missing Completely at Random for the indicator variables in the LPA and found that this was supported (χ2(34) = 29.49, p = 0.91).

Latent class model estimation

We included 19 indicator variables in our LPA and estimated up to nine classes. All information criteria demonstrated reductions in values when estimating models with increasing numbers of classes. The LMR-LRT was non-significant for all model comparisons. Thus, statistical indices provided little guidance for a preferred model. Model selection was informed by patterns of variability across class solutions. There was an increasing differentiation of classes in all solutions. This class differentiation was substantial through the four class solution. Beyond the four class solution, there was a subdivision of classes within one of the classes, raising questions about the meaningfulness of the subsequent classes. Specifically, the classes became trivially small, suggesting their limited utility and robustness. Thus, we identified the four class solution as our preferred solution. Class means and standard errors for indicators are presented in online Supplementary Materials and a figure depicting class characteristics is presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. For full description of class indicators, see the online supplement. Dep Sev, depression severity; Other Int, other internalizing problems; Min Str, stressors: daily hassles; Maj Str, stress: major stressors; Pessimism, negative cognitions; Emot Rel, emotional reliance; Attributions, attributional style; Self-Consc, self consciousness; Acad Asp, academic aspirations; Fam Asp, family aspirations; Occ Asp, occupational aspirations; Family Sup, family support; Peer Sup, peer support; Parent Conf, parental conflict; Ext Sev, externalizing problems severity.

Class 1 (31.5%) included participants scoring, on average, 0.57 standard deviations below the mean (s.d. = 0.26) on class indicators. Thus, individuals in this class were functioning very well on most measures. This class is referred to as the ‘Thriving Functioning’ class. Class 2 (45.7%) was the largest class and included participants scoring, on average, 0.08 standard deviations below the mean (s.d. = 0.10) on class indicators. Thus, individuals in this class were functioning within the average range on most measures. This class is referred to as the ‘Average Functioning’ class. Class 3 (4.9%) was the smallest class and included participants scoring, on average, 0.47 standard deviations above the mean (s.d. = 0.64) on class indicators. Their mean level of externalizing problems was very high and they also had scores greater than 0.70 s.d.s above the mean on major stressors, (lower) academic aspirations, poorer family support and more family conflict. Thus, this class is referred to as the ‘Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress’ class. Finally, Class 4 (17.8%) included participants scoring, on average, 0.48 standard deviations above the mean (s.d. = 0.36) on class indicators. Individuals in this class had scores greater than 0.70 s.d.s above the mean on depressive symptomatology, internalizing problems, minor stressors, (reversed) self-esteem and negative cognitive style. Thus, this class is referred to as the ‘Internalizing Vulnerability’ class.

Class comparisons

We first examined participant sex, parental educational attainment (i.e. whether at least one biological parent earned a 4 year college degree), adolescent anxiety and SUDs at study entry and maternal and paternal history of psychopathology as class correlates. In these analyses (Table 2), we found that there were class differences in sex, parental education, adolescent anxiety and SUD and paternal history of MDD and SUD. Group differences in maternal psychopathology and paternal anxiety disorder were not significant. We found a greater proportion of males in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class relative to the other three classes. A higher proportion of youth in the Thriving class had a parent with a college degree than any other class. The proportion of participants with a parent with a college degree in the Average functioning class was higher than that in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class. Youth in the Internalizing Vulnerability class had the greatest proportion of anxiety disorders at study entry, which was significantly greater than that in the Thriving and Average Functioning classes. Youth in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class had a higher proportion of SUDs than any other class. We also found that paternal history of MDD was higher in the Average Functioning, Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress and Internalizing Vulnerability classes than the Thriving class. Finally, paternal SUD was significantly higher in the Internalizing Vulnerability class relative to the Thriving and Average Functioning classes. The Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class did not differ from any of the other classes on paternal SUD.

Table 2. Class comparisons on proband sex, parental education and psychopathology by the first assessment and parent psychopathology

Note: Percentages are model-based estimates taking into account imprecision of class membership. χ2 statistic is computed based on the adjusted differences between log-likelihood values between the model with constrained thresholds v. freely estimated thresholds across classes. Class 1: Thriving Class (31.9%); Class 2: Average Functioning (45.5%); Class 3: Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress (4.9%); Class 4: Internalizing Vulnerability (17.7%). Different superscripts indicate significant pairwise differences at p < 0.05. MDD, major depressive disorder; ANX, anxiety disorder; SUD, substance use disorder

Next, we examined relationships between class membership and later psychopathological outcomes (Table 3). Initial models examined unadjusted class differences and follow-up analyses examined class differences when controlling for better-established risk factors, including sex, adolescent anxiety and substance use disorders, and maternal and paternal depressive, anxiety and substance use disorders.

Table 3. Comparisons of classes on later psychopathology and psychosocial functioning

Note: Percentages are model-based estimates taking into account imprecision of class membership. χ2 statistic is computed based on the adjusted differences between log-likelihood values between the model with constrained thresholds v. freely estimated thresholds across classes. Class 1: Thriving Class (31.9%); Class 2: Average Functioning (45.5%); Class 3: Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress (4.9%); Class 4: Internalizing Vulnerability (17.7%). MDD, major depressive disorder; ANX, anxiety disorder; SUD, substance use disorder; MDE, major depressive episode; MDD, duration in months; PT1, post-T1 assessment; Highest Grade, highest grade level completed; Household Income, mean of income ranges (1 = no income; 2 ⩽ $5000; 3 = $5000–$9999; 4 = $10 000–14 999; 5 = $15 000–$19 999; 6 = $20 000–$29 999; 7 = $30 000–$39 999; 8 = $40 000–$49 999; 9 = $50 000 or more).

First, we estimated class differences in time until the first onset of major depressive episode using survival models and presented the proportion of individuals within each class experiencing MDD. We found that individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability class were significantly more likely to develop MDD than individuals in all other classes, none of which differed from one another. In addition, individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability class had more episodes of MDD than individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress and Thriving classes. Moreover, individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class had significantly fewer episodes than individuals in the Average Functioning class. We also found that individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability class had longer total MDD durations than individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class. There were no other significant group differences.

Classes were compared on the prediction of later anxiety disorders and SUDs by examining proportions of disorders post-T 1, regardless of whether onsets were first episodes or recurrences. The Internalizing Vulnerability class was more likely to develop an anxiety disorder than any other classes. In addition, individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability and Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress classes were more likely to develop SUDs than the Average Functioning and Thriving classes.

Finally, we compared educational attainment, household income, life satisfaction and social adjustment across classes. Classes differed in average levels of education attained – individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress had the lowest levels of education, individuals in the Average Functioning and Internalizing Vulnerability classes had an intermediate level, and individuals in the Thriving class achieved the highest level. Classes also differed in average levels of household income, with individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress reporting the lowest household income, individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability having an intermediate level, and individuals in the Thriving class having the highest levels. Household income of the Average Functioning class did not differ from that of the Thriving or Internalizing Vulnerability classes.

Levels of life satisfaction for individuals in the Average Functioning, Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress, and Internalizing Vulnerability classes were significantly lower than that for individuals in the Thriving class. In addition, individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability class had lower levels of life satisfaction than that in the Average Functioning class. The Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class did not differ from individuals in the Average Functioning or Internalizing Vulnerability classes on life satisfaction. Finally, individuals in the Thriving and Average Functioning class had significantly better overall social adjustment than individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress and the Internalizing Vulnerability classes.

Despite adjusting for sex, adolescents’ anxiety disorders and SUDs, and parental history of MDD, anxiety, and SUDs, class differences were generally consistent with the unadjusted results (online Supplementary Table 1) and differences in findings follow. However, in these adjusted analyses, there were no significant class differences in household income. We also found that individuals in the Thriving Class had a shorter duration of MDD than those in the Internalizing Vulnerability class. Individuals in the Thriving and Average Functioning classes had significantly better overall social adjustment than individuals in the Internalizing Vulnerability class. Social adjustment of individuals in the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Stress class did not differ from that of those in any other classes.

Discussion

There are numerous established risk factors for MDD. Previous studies have focused on variable-centered approaches to risk factors of the onset of depression (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2002; Hankin, Reference Hankin2012; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Glenn, Kosty, Seeley, Rohde and Lewinsohn2013; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Haeffel, Hankin, Maxwell and Perera2014). These methods presume that risk factors will be similarly predictive for all individuals from a population. However, person-centered approaches (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2000) circumvent this assumption by permitting tests of qualitatively different pathways to an outcome for subgroups who share profiles of functioning. In the present study, we examined how profiles of psychosocial and clinical functioning in adolescence are associated with future psychopathology and adaptive functioning. We identified four classes, labeled as Thriving, Average, Externalizing Vulnerability with Family Stress and Internalizing Vulnerability. These classes were associated with different patterns of pathological outcomes as well as markers of adaptive functioning in adulthood. Moreover, most class differences persisted when controlling for better-established risk factors for psychopathology.

Two of our identified classes, labeled as Thriving and Average functioning, reflect superior and average levels of psychosocial and clinical functioning. The Average class included the largest proportion of individuals in the sample (45.5%) and had values near the mean on most class indicators. Thus, the label Average captures the statistical characteristics of this class well. The Thriving class was also sizeable (31.9%) and was characterized by highly adaptive functioning across multiple domains, with many class indicators having means well above the sample average. Though these two class profiles were quantitatively distinct with regard to their indicators, they did not differ significantly from one another on any psychopathological outcome examined. This suggests that a wide range of adaptive functioning is associated with buffering against the experience of psychopathology. However, these classes differed in levels of educational attainment, life satisfaction and social adjustment (in our conservative analyses) in adulthood, with the Thriving class having higher levels than the Average functioning class. Schaefer et al. (Reference Schaefer, Caspi, Belsky, Harrington, Houts, Horwood, Hussong, Ramrakha, Poulton and Moffitt2017) examined differences between individuals from the Dunedin cohort who never experienced mental health problems and those who had mental health problems on only 1–2 assessments throughout the study. The authors found that there were no differences in multiple risk factor domains, including family history of psychopathology, but there were differences in life satisfaction and relationship quality. Thus, across parallel conceptualizations of functioning, associations with well-being are similar.

The other two classes had similar overall levels of indicators, but differed qualitatively on which specific indicators were elevated. The Internalizing Vulnerability class had elevations on many internalizing correlates, whereas the Externalizing Vulnerability and Family Conflict class had elevations on those domains. The Externalizing Vulnerability with Family Stress class had a higher proportion of males than all other classes and higher rates of SUDs than the Thriving and Average classes. This is consistent with evidence that males (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Chou, Huang, Stinson, Dawson, Saha, Smith, Pulay and Pickering2009) and early externalizing problems (Groenman et al., Reference Groenman, Janssen and Oosterlaan2017) are risk factors for SUDs. However, relative to the Thriving and Average classes, the Externalizing Vulnerability with Family Stress class did not differ on depressive morbidity or risk for anxiety disorders. Thus, this class had specific risk for SUDs. Moreover, they had fewer episodes of depression than the Average group, providing further evidence of qualitative, rather than just severity, differences. Thus, externalizing problems and heightened family conflict appeared to be associated with reduced depressive morbidity.

The Internalizing Vulnerability class had the greatest psychiatric morbidity relative to other classes. It was characterized by elevations on multiple indices of cognitive vulnerability to depression, which have previously been shown to be potent predictors of depressive disorders (Alloy et al., Reference Alloy, Abramson, Hogan, Whitehouse, Rose, Robinson, Kim and Lapkin2000). Moreover, it exhibited a higher risk for anxiety disorders and SUDs than the Thriving and Average classes. This suggests that the collection of elevated indicators in the Internalizing Vulnerability class reflects transdiagnostic risk (Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins2011; Hong and Cheung, Reference Hong and Cheung2015). When controlling for additional well-established clinical and demographic risk factors, the Internalizing Vulnerability class continued to have a higher risk for MDD than the Thriving and Average classes, as well as a greater number of MDD episodes and total duration of illness than the Thriving and Externalizing Vulnerability with Family Stress classes. Thus, these profiles continued to provide additional explanatory utility beyond established risk factors.

The Internalizing Vulnerability class was also associated with an increase in paternal, but not maternal, history of depression. This parallels earlier work in this dataset showing paternal depression was associated with lower adolescent social competence (Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Olino and Klein2005). The non-significant association for maternal depression raises important questions about the processes that give rise to these classes. Maternal depression is an established risk factor for depression (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley and Olino2005), but did not discriminate between classes defined by other psychosocial risk factors. Thus, these psychosocial functioning classes do not appear to mediate the relationship between maternal and offspring depression. However, maternal depression is associated with other risk indicators that were not included here (Goodman and Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib2002), for example personality or temperamental characteristics, and biological processes such as neural response (Olino, Reference Olino2016).

In the broader work on the aggregation of psychopathology in the population, there is evidence that a majority of incident psychopathology (Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Kosty, Seeley, Olino and Lewinsohn2013) and adverse physical and psychosocial outcomes (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Harrington, Hogan, Ramrakha, Poulton and Moffitt2017) condensed within a small proportion of the population. Our class indicators focused on both psychosocial and clinical functioning and paralleled the epidemiological results focusing on clinical outcomes. Thus, there is an apparent parallel between vulnerability and clinical outcomes.

The LPA models demonstrated utility relative to traditional regression-based methods. The identified profiles showed constellations of multiple constructs and found that they were associated with different psychopathological outcomes. To identify similar patterns using regression methods, many more models would need to be estimated. Moreover, as we found some non-linear patterns within our classes, particularly with respect to the presence of heightened externalizing problems, these would have required estimating additional interaction effects. This would lead to many tests and increase the likelihood of type-I error.

Our study benefits from a wide array of measures of risk for psychopathology assessed on a large cohort of youth who were carefully assessed for multiple forms of psychopathology for up to 15 years. However, these strengths must be weighed against several limitations. First, we relied solely on self-report measures to examine psychosocial and clinical functioning. Other types of measures and variables (e.g. neuroimaging, behavior, personality traits) may add value in predicting psychopathology. Second, identifying pathways to emergence of psychopathology in a mechanistic fashion requires a longitudinal assessment of risk factors to identify how these change over time (Hankin, Reference Hankin2012; Olino, Reference Olino2016).

The results of this work suggest that empirically-derived profiles of clinical and psychosocial risk factors have prognostic value for predicting onset and course of depression, as well as adaptive function. Moreover, these associations are independent of, and account for additional variance, over and above better-established clinical and demographic risk factors for depression.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002186.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants R01MH66023 (Dr Klein), R01 MH40501, R01 MH50522, R01 MH52858 and R01 DA012951 (Dr Lewinsohn), and R01 MH107495 (Dr Olino). We thank Peter M. Lewinsohn for his long-term support of the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project.

References

Alloy, LB, Abramson, LY, Hogan, ME, Whitehouse, WG, Rose, DT, Robinson, MS, Kim, RS and Lapkin, JB (2000) The Temple-Wisconsin cognitive vulnerability to depression project: lifetime history of axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 403418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd Edn, rev. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn, rev. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Andrews, FM and Withey, SB (1976) Social Indicators of Well-Being: Americans’ Perceptions of Life Quality. New York, NY: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asparouhov, T and Muthén, B (2014) Auxiliary Variables in Mixture Modeling: Using the BCH Method in Mplus to Estimate a Distal Outcome Model and an Arbitrary Secondary Model. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA.Google Scholar
Bittner, A, Egger, HL, Erkanli, A, Jane Costello, E, Foley, DL and Angold, A (2007) What do childhood anxiety disorders predict? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 11741183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, A, Converse, PE and Rodgers, WL (1976) The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW, Harrington, H, Hogan, S, Ramrakha, S, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2017) Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nature Human Behaviour 1, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cyranowski, JM, Frank, E, Young, E and Shear, MK (2000) Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: a theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, RF, Kosty, DB, Seeley, JR, Olino, TM and Lewinsohn, PM (2013) Aggregation of lifetime Axis I psychiatric disorders through age 30: Incidence, predictors, and associated psychosocial outcomes. Journal of abnormal psychology 122, 573586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, RF, Seeley, JR, Kosty, DB and Lewinsohn, PM (2009) Refinements in the hierarchical structure of externalizing psychiatric disorders: patterns of lifetime liability from mid-adolescence through early adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 118, 699710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M and Williams, JBW (1996) The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Non-patient Edn. New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Fleiss, JL (1981) The measurement of interrater agreement Statistical methods for rates and proportions, (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley. pp. 212236.Google Scholar
Goodman, SH and Gotlib, IH (eds) (2002) Children of Depressed Parents: Mechanisms of Risk and Implications for Treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, SH, Rouse, MH, Connell, AM, Broth, MR, Hall, CM and Heyward, D (2011) Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 14, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, BF, Goldstein, RB, Chou, SP, Huang, B, Stinson, FS, Dawson, DA, Saha, TD, Smith, SM, Pulay, AJ and Pickering, RP (2009) Sociodemographic and psychopathologic predictors of first incidence of DSM-IV substance use, mood and anxiety disorders: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 10511066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groenman, AP, Janssen, TW and Oosterlaan, J (2017) Childhood psychiatric disorders as risk factor for subsequent substance abuse: a meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 56, 556569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallquist, MN and Wright, AG (2014) Mixture modeling methods for the assessment of normal and abnormal personality, Part I: Cross-sectional models. Journal of Personality Assessment 96, 256268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C (2006) Stress generation in depression: reflections on origins, research, and future directions. Journal of Clinical Psychology 62, 10651082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, BL (2012) Future directions in vulnerability to depression among youth: integrating risk factors and processes across multiple levels of analysis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 41, 695718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, BL, Abramson, LY, Moffitt, TE, Silva, PA, Mcgee, R and Angell, KE (1998) Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 128140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, JK, Duberstein, PR, Conner, KR, Heisel, MJ, Beckman, A, Franus, N and Conwell, Y (2007) Future orientation moderates the relationship between functional status and suicide ideation in depressed adults. Depression and Anxiety 24, 196201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, RY and Cheung, MW-L (2015) The structure of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression and anxiety: evidence for a common core etiologic process based on a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychological Science 3, 892912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, JS, Mezulis, AH and Abramson, LY (2008) The ABCs of depression: integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychological Review 115, 291313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, MB, Lavori, PW, Friedman, B, Nielsen, E, Endicott, J, Mcdonald-Scott, P and Andreasen, NC (1987) The longitudinal interval follow-up evaluation – a comprehensive method for assessing outcome in prospective longitudinal-studies. Archives of General Psychiatry 44, 540548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Gardner, CO and Prescott, CA (2002) Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 11331145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, KS, Hettema, JM, Butera, F, Gardner, CO and Prescott, CA (2003) Life event dimensions of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger in the prediction of onsets of major depression and generalized anxiety. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 789796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Chiu, WT, Demler, O and Walters, EE (2005) Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 617627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim-Cohen, J, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Harrington, HL, Milne, BJ and Poulton, R (2003) Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 709717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, DN, Lewinsohn, PM, Rohde, P, Seeley, JR and Olino, TM (2005) Psychopathology in the adolescent and young adult offspring of a community sample of mothers and fathers with major depression. Psychological Medicine 35, 353365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, DN, Shankman, SA, Lewinsohn, PM and Seeley, JR (2009) Subthreshold depressive disorder in adolescents: predictors of escalation to full-syndrome depressive disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48, 703710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, DN, Glenn, CR, Kosty, DB, Seeley, JR, Rohde, P and Lewinsohn, PM (2013) Predictors of first lifetime onset of major depressive disorder in young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leckman, JF, Sholomskas, D, Thompson, WD, Belanger, A and Weissman, MM (1982) Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric-diagnosis – a methodological study. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 879883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, PM, Hops, H, Roberts, RE, Seeley, JR and Andrews, JA (1993) Adolescent psychopathology: I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM-III–R disorders in high school students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102, 133144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, PM, Clarke, GN, Seeley, JR and Rohde, P (1994) Major depression in community adolescents: age at onset, episode duration, and time to recurrence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 33, 809818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, PM, Rohde, P, Seeley, JR, Klein, DN and Gotlib, IH (2003) Psychosocial functioning of young adults who have experienced and recovered from major depressive disorder during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112, 353363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, PM, Olino, TM and Klein, DN (2005) Psychosocial impairment in offspring of depressed parents. Psychological Medicine 35, 14931503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, FL and Clarke, GN (2006) Estimating the economic burden of depression in children and adolescents. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 31, 143151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannuzza, S and Fyer, AJ (1990) Family informant schedule and criteria (FISC), July 1990 revision.Google Scholar
Muthén, BO and Muthén, LK (2000) Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24, 882891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, LK and Muthén, BO (1998–2018) Mplus User's Guide, Eighth Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S and Watkins, ER (2011) A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6, 589609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylund, KL, Asparouhov, T and Muthén, BO (2007) Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling 14, 535569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olino, TM (2016) Future research directions in the positive valence systems: measurement, development, and implications for Youth Unipolar Depression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 45, 681705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olino, TM, Klein, DN, Lewinsohn, PM, Rohde, P and Seeley, JR (2008) Longitudinal associations between depressive and anxiety disorders: a comparison of two trait models. Psychological Medicine 38, 353363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orvaschel, H, Puig-Antich, J, Chambers, WJ, Tabrizi, MA and Johnson, R (1982) Retrospective assessment of prepubertal major depression with the Kiddie-SADS-E. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 21, 392397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohde, P, Lewinsohn, PM and Seeley, JR (1997) Comparability of telephone and face-to-face interviews in assessing axis I and II disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 15931598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rottenberg, J, Devendorf, AR, Kashdan, TB and Disabato, DJ (2018) The curious neglect of high functioning after psychopathology: the case of depression. Perspectives on Psychological Science 13, 549566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, A, Haeffel, GJ, Hankin, BL, Maxwell, SE and Perera, RA (2014) Moving beyond main effects: a data analytic strategy for testing complex theories of clinical phenomena. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 21, 385397.Google Scholar
Schaefer, JD, Caspi, A, Belsky, DW, Harrington, H, Houts, R, Horwood, LJ, Hussong, A, Ramrakha, S, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2017) Enduring mental health: prevalence and prediction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126, 212224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, JR, Kosty, DB, Farmer, RF and Lewinsohn, PM (2011) The modeling of internalizing disorders on the basis of patterns of lifetime comorbidity: associations with psychosocial functioning and psychiatric disorders among first-degree relatives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120, 308321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobin, C, Weissman, MM, Goldstein, RB, Adams, P, Wickramaratne, P, Warner, V and Lish, JD (1993) Diagnostic interviewing for family studies – comparing telephone and face-to-face methods for the diagnosis of lifetime psychiatric-disorders. Psychiatric Genetics 3, 227233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sowislo, JF and Orth, U (2013) Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin 139, 213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St Clair, MC, Croudace, T, Dunn, VJ, Jones, PB, Herbert, J and Goodyer, IM (2015) Childhood adversity subtypes and depressive symptoms in early and late adolescence. Development and Psychopathology 27, 885899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stice, E, Ragan, J and Randall, P (2004) Prospective relations between social support and depression: differential direction of effects for parent and peer support? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113, 155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrshek-Schallhorn, S, Stroud, CB, Mineka, S, Hammen, C, Zinbarg, RE, Wolitzky-Taylor, K and Craske, MG (2015) Chronic and episodic interpersonal stress as statistically unique predictors of depression in two samples of emerging adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 124, 918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, MM and Bothwell, S (1976) Assessment of social adjustment by patient self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry 33, 11111115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, MM, Prusoff, BA, Thompson, WD, Harding, PS and Myers, JK (1978) Social adjustment by self-report in a community sample and in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 166, 317326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, MM, Warner, V, Wickramaratne, P, Moreau, D and Olfson, M (1997) Offspring of depressed parents, 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 932940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, MM, Wickramaratne, P, Gameroff, MJ, Warner, V, Pilowsky, D, Kohad, RG, Verdeli, H, Skipper, J and Talati, A (2016) Offspring of depressed parents: 30 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 10241032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2002) The World Health Report 2002-Reducing Risks, Promoting Healthy Life. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Model fit statistics

Figure 1

Fig. 1. For full description of class indicators, see the online supplement. Dep Sev, depression severity; Other Int, other internalizing problems; Min Str, stressors: daily hassles; Maj Str, stress: major stressors; Pessimism, negative cognitions; Emot Rel, emotional reliance; Attributions, attributional style; Self-Consc, self consciousness; Acad Asp, academic aspirations; Fam Asp, family aspirations; Occ Asp, occupational aspirations; Family Sup, family support; Peer Sup, peer support; Parent Conf, parental conflict; Ext Sev, externalizing problems severity.

Figure 2

Table 2. Class comparisons on proband sex, parental education and psychopathology by the first assessment and parent psychopathology

Figure 3

Table 3. Comparisons of classes on later psychopathology and psychosocial functioning

Supplementary material: File

Olino et al. supplementary material

Olino et al. supplementary material 1

Download Olino et al. supplementary material(File)
File 38.6 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Olino et al. supplementary material

Olino et al. supplementary material 2

Download Olino et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 8.3 KB