Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:49:43.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Are we making mountains out of molehills?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 May 2017

S. Moritz*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
J. P. Klein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Lübeck University, Ratzeburger Allee 160, 23538 Lübeck, Germany
T. Desler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
H. Lill
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
J. Gallinat
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
B. C. Schneider
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: S. Moritz, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistraße 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany. (Email: moritz@uke.de)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Most original studies and all meta-analyses conducted to date converge on the conclusion that patients with schizophrenia display rather generalized neurocognitive deficits. For the present study, we reopen this seemingly closed chapter and examine whether important influences, such as lack of motivation and negative attitudes towards cognitive assessment, result in poorer secondary neuropsychological performance.

Method

A sample of 50 patients with an established diagnosis of schizophrenia were tested for routine neurocognitive assessment and compared to 60 nonclinical volunteers. Before and after the assessment, subjective momentary influences were examined (e.g. motivation, concerns about assessment, fear about poor outcome) for their impact on performance using a new questionnaire called the Momentary Influences, Attitudes and Motivation Impact (MIAMI) on Cognitive Performance Scale.

Results

As expected, patients performed significantly worse than controls on all neurocognitive domains tested (large effect size, on average). However, patients also displayed more subjective momentary impairment, as well as more fears about the outcome and less motivation than controls. Mediation analyses indicated that these influences contributed to (secondary) poorer neurocognitive performance. Differences in neurocognitive scores shrank to a medium effect size, on average, when MIAMI scores were accounted for.

Conclusions

The data argue that performance on measures of neurocognition in schizophrenia are to a considerable extent due to secondary factors. Poor motivation, fears and momentary impairments distinguished patients from controls and these variables heavily impacted performance. Before concluding that neurocognitive deficits in psychiatric patients are present, clinicians should take these confounding influences into account. Although patients with schizophrenia achieved, on average, worse test scores than controls, a large subgroup displayed spared performance.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Neurocognitive deficits, such as impairments of memory and attention, are neither core criteria for the diagnosis of schizophrenia (APA, 2013) nor pathognomonic (Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Heeren, Andresen and Krausz2001, Reference Moritz, Birkner, Kloss, Jahn, Hand, Haasen and Krausz2002; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis2009; Palmer et al. Reference Palmer, Dawes and Heaton2009). Yet, this research domain has attracted much interest since the times of Kraepelin (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1899), who claimed that the disorder (then called dementia praecox) is characterized by neurocognitive decline ultimately manifesting as dementia. The vast majority of original studies, narrative reviews and meta-analyses arrive at the conclusion that patients with schizophrenia have major neurocognitive deficits (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998; Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Silva, Perkins and Lieberman1999; Reichenberg, Reference Reichenberg2010; Schaefer et al. Reference Schaefer, Giangrande, Weinberger and Dickinson2013; Fatouros-Bergman et al. Reference Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman and Farde2014). As a rule of thumb, deficits among individuals with schizophrenia are estimated to lie approximately 1 standard deviation below the norm. However, unlike Kraepelin's assumption, these deficits are rather stable and there is no evidence for progressive neurodegenerative decline subsequent to the first episode (Mesholam-Gately et al. Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009; Bora & Murray, Reference Bora and Murray2014).

Neurocognitive deficits, which have been often detected in comparison to samples of demographically matched controls, are mainly attributed to the primary disorder (i.e. schizophrenia). They are hypothesized to act as a risk factor for psychosis, particularly the negative and/or disorganized syndrome, and a more severe course of illness (Brewer et al. Reference Brewer, Wood, Phillips, Francey, Pantelis, Yung, Cornblatt and McGorry2006; Carlsson et al. Reference Carlsson, Nyman, Ganse and Cullberg2006). Since the mid-1990s, a number of studies (Green, Reference Green1996; Green et al. Reference Green, Kern, Braff and Mintz2000, Reference Green, Kern and Heaton2004) have suggested that objective neurocognitive deficits, in conjunction with (and sometimes overshadowed by) impairments in social cognition (Fett et al. Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011), predict poor functional outcome and may also compromise treatment outcome for some types of intervention (Kurtz, Reference Kurtz2011). Consequently, efforts to develop trainings (Wykes et al. Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011; Cella et al. Reference Cella, Reeder and Wykes2015) and medications (López-Muñoz & Álamo, Reference López-Muñoz and Álamo2011) that augment cognitive functioning (i.e. cognitive enhancers) are on the rise.

In this article, we will argue that the literature has neglected important influences that may contribute to neurocognitive test deficits, and thus, prohibit a fair comparison between patients and controls. These factors have only recently been addressed systematically (Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Zakzanis, Foussias, Graff-Guerrero, Agid and Remington2014; Strauss et al. Reference Strauss, Morra, Sullivan and Gold2015). For example, most patients with schizophrenia are prescribed medication, particularly antipsychotic agents, which – with the possible exception of clozapine – may induce Parkinsonian symptoms. Such symptoms will compromise performance in any motor test, such as the Trail Making Test (Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Agid, Takeuchi, Lee, Foussias, Zakzanis, Graff-Guerrero and Remington2015), whether or not cognitive faculties are affected. Additionally, patients are also often given tranquilizers and anticholinergic drugs known to decrease memory and attention (Vinogradov et al. Reference Vinogradov, Fisher, Warm, Holland, Kirshner and Pollock2009). Although it is implicitly claimed that neurocognitive deficits (as manifestation of brain-related dysfunction) underlie negative symptoms (Hovington & Lepage, Reference Hovington and Lepage2012), such as avolition and poor motivation, the causality may in fact be the reverse.

A number of studies suggest that patients with schizophrenia are less willing to engage to the best of their abilities (Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Zakzanis, Foussias, Graff-Guerrero, Agid and Remington2014), which makes it almost impossible to gain a true picture of their neurocognitive potential. Unlike psychophysiological or laboratory measures (e.g. blood tests), scores on neurocognitive tests heavily rely on the cooperation of the patient. In addition, patients may be distracted by rumination, unusual ideas and/or hallucinations during testing, which could direct attention away from the task at hand. Matching samples for age, gender and education is thus not fully sufficient. Finally, a restricted non-arousing hospital environment itself may lead to a decrement in performance, as has been shown in non-psychiatric populations (Wilson et al. Reference Wilson, Hebert, Scherr, Dong, Leurgens and Evans2012; Mathews et al. Reference Mathews, Arnold and Epperson2014). In patients with psychosis, the relationship between hospitalization and cognition is complex and perhaps mediated by more severe symptomatology (Harvey et al. Reference Harvey, Loewenstein and Czaja2013).

We have previously investigated secondary influences on neuropsychological functioning in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Hottenrott, Jelinek, Brooks and Scheurich2012), for whom neurocognitive deficits have also been described (Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Lee, Kim and Kwon2014) – albeit to a lesser degree than in individuals with schizophrenia. Poor motivation and checking compulsions were significantly associated with cognitive performance deficits. We concluded that while the study does not rule out that neurocognitive deficits may play a role in the formation of OCD symptoms, the reverse relationship should be considered as well. Likewise, another study found that in depression, cognitive deficits are also aggravated by motivational deficits (Scheurich et al. Reference Scheurich, Fellgiebel, Schermuly, Bauer, Wölfges and Müller2008).

In the present study, we examined whether the rationale of the aforementioned studies on OCD and depression can also be applied to patients with schizophrenia and compared a sizable sample of patients with schizophrenia to healthy controls. We assessed all individuals both before and after comprehensive neurocognitive testing with a newly developed self-report scale aimed at uncovering several subjective influences on cognitive performance, including effort, motivation and other factors (i.e. concerns about assessment, fear about poor outcome, poor performance due to momentary influences). Assessing patients before the assessment was deemed important as post-assessment data may be biased by tactical responses (e.g. a self-serving bias to blame poor performance on low motivation). We expected that patients would perform worse on neurocognitive tests than controls as a group but that differences would be (partially) mediated by other group differences, particularly on motivation and fears about the outcome of testing. While we expected that the nonclinical controls would be highly motivated to produce good results, we hypothesized that patients with schizophrenia would show less ambition.

Methods

The clinical sample was comprised of 50 inpatients consecutively referred to the clinical neuropsychology unit of the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy at the University Medical Center in Hamburg-Eppendorf (Germany). All patients met diagnostic criteria for a schizophrenia spectrum disorder as verified by a psychiatrist. All patients were prescribed psychotropic medication, mainly atypical antipsychotics. The total score of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Overall & Gorham, Reference Overall and Gorham1962) was 41.29 (18.25), which corresponds to ‘moderately ill’ according to the Clinical Global Impression (CGI) scale (Leucht et al. Reference Leucht, Kane, Kissling, Hamann, Etschel and Engel2005). On average, patients had 1.51 (1.13) prior inpatient admissions (range: 0–7). A group of 60 people served as nonclinical controls, for whom a psychiatric diagnosis had been excluded by means of a short psychiatric screening for mental disorders based on the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview, which had been adapted to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (APA, 2013) criteria by the authors (MINI) (Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). Control participants were sought from various sources via word-of-mouth and advertisements. General exclusion criteria were major neurological disorders and age younger than 18 or older than 65 years.

All participants underwent a battery of neuropsychological tests, which took approximately 2–3 h depending on participants’ speed and requests for breaks. Prior to the assessment and afterwards, participants were asked to complete a short questionnaire asking for subjective momentary attitudes, momentary impairments and motivation (see below).

Characteristics of patients and controls are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Differences between schizophrenia and healthy individuals on demographic characteristics and scores on the MIAMI subscales

Note. MIAMI = Momentary Influences, Attitudes and Motivation Impact (MIAMI) on Cognitive Performance Scale, higher scores designate more negative thoughts and feelings associated with the assessment.

Participants completed the following battery of neurocognitive tasks:

Trail Making Test (TMT) (Reitan, Reference Reitan1992)

Psychomotor speed was assessed with the TMT Part A (adult version). The individual was asked to connect numbers in ascending order as quickly as possible. Part B assesses set-shifting and requires alternating between numbers and letters, again in ascending order. Age-adapted standard scores were applied (Tombaugh, Reference Tombaugh2004).

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Heaton, Reference Heaton1981)

Participants completed a computerized version of the WCST (Loong, Reference Loong1990), which followed the procedure of the original paper test. Participants were shown a maximum of 128 cards, which had to be matched according to three varying principles (i.e. number of items, color, shape). These matching principles were not disclosed to the individual. Via a tone (high v. low) and a corresponding verbal cue, it was indicated whether a match was correct or not. Categories completed and perseverative errors served as dependent variables.

Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995)

Memory and learning were assessed with the German version of the AVLT (Helmstaedter et al. Reference Helmstaedter, Lendt and Lux2001). A list of 15 words (List A) was read to the participants five times. After each trial, the individual had to repeat as many words as possible in no particular order. Afterwards, the individual had to memorize items from a separate inference list (List B). Following this, words from List A had to be recalled without renewed presentation. Thirty minutes later, the individual was asked to repeat words from List A only. The AVLT allows for the computation of several memory parameters (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995). Learning was indexed by the sum of correctly recalled words on Trials 1 to 5. The number of correctly reproduced words after the 30-min delay served as an index for long-term memory. Normative scores for the German version of the task are available for different age ranges (Helmstaedter et al. Reference Helmstaedter, Lendt and Lux2001).

Selective attention Test d2 (Brickenkamp, Reference Brickenkamp1962)

Test d2 is a letter cancellation test that taps selective attention. The individual had to cross out the letter d whenever it was presented with two small lines; d’s with more or less than two lines, or any stimuli containing the character p, served as distracters. Following a practice trial, 14 rows containing target and distracter stimuli were presented. Participants had to complete each row within 20 s. The test is scored for errors and number of correctly crossed out stimuli within the allotted time. Normative scores for the parameter concentration performance (Konzentrationsleistung) served as the primary parameter. Age-adjusted normative scores were derived from a large population sample (Schmidt-Atzert, Reference Schmidt-Atzert2004).

Divided attention subtest from the Test for Attentional Performance (TAP) (Zimmermann & Fimm, Reference Zimmermann and Fimm1995)

For divided attention, participants had to perform two different tasks concurrently on a computer. The space bar had to be pressed whenever asterisks formed a rectangle on a 4 × 4 dot matrix (optical target), or whenever two tones of the same frequency were repeated (acoustic target). One hundred optical and 200 acoustic trials were presented. There were 16 targets for each modality. Norm values derived from a large sample of participants are available for median reaction times and number of omissions (Zimmermann & Fimm, Reference Zimmermann and Fimm1995).

Selective attention subtest (Go-Nogo) from the Test for Attentional Performance (TAP) (Zimmermann & Fimm, Reference Zimmermann and Fimm1995)

Participants were presented with either an ‘x’ (target, ‘go’) or a ‘+’ (distracter, ‘nogo’) in this simple computerized task of selective attention. A button had to be pressed whenever a target stimulus appeared. Norm scores are available for speed variables (Zimmermann & Fimm, Reference Zimmermann and Fimm1995).

Story Recall from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) – Revised Edition (Woodard & Axelrod, Reference Woodard and Axelrod1987)

Two short stories were read and the participant was asked to repeat as much of the story as they could remember immediately after the completion of each story. Thirty minutes later, participants were again asked to recall as much of the story as possible. No other tests of verbal memory were presented during the 30-min follow-up time. Scoring followed instructions in the manual; German norm values were applied (Härting et al. Reference Härting, Markowitsch, Neufeld, Calabrese, Deisinger and Kessler2000).

Similarities from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th edition (Jacobs & Petermann, Reference Jacobs and Petermann2007; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008)

In this verbal reasoning task, the individual has to deduce what two words have in common. Scoring followed the original manual. Scaled scores exist for a large German population (Jacobs & Petermann, Reference Jacobs and Petermann2007).

Matrix Reasoning from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th edition (Jacobs & Petermann, Reference Jacobs and Petermann2007; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008)

The matrix subtest measures nonverbal reasoning. The individual is presented with a pattern sequence and must select the item that completes the sequence from five response choices. Scaled scores exist for a large German population.

Zoo Test from the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) (Wilson et al. Reference Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie and Evans2003)

The zoo test captures planning ability. The individual had to plan a route to visit six of 12 possible locations in a zoo. The test has two phases (i.e. first, a demanding situation with little external structure was given; then, a concrete, externally imposed strategy had to be followed).

Block Design from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 4th edition (Jacobs & Petermann, Reference Jacobs and Petermann2007; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008)

In this visuospatial performance test, participants must re-create two-dimensional patterns using colored blocks. Task demands become increasingly difficult over time. Scoring is made according to accuracy and time following instructions in the manual. Scaled scores exist for a large German population.

In keeping with prior studies, we expected no clear profile of impairment across domains. Therefore, we composed two new cognitive subscores, which aggregated either speed or performance variables, expressed as z-values, rather than proceeding with individual domains (if higher scores designated better performance, as in the memory tests, parameters were reversed).

Assessment of subjective influences on cognitive performance

Prior and subsequent to the cognitive assessment, participants received either the baseline or post-assessment version of a questionnaire called the Momentary Influences, Attitudes and Motivation Impact (MIAMI) on Cognitive Performance Scale. Each version consists of 18 items. The baseline version covers four domainsFootnote Footnote 1 : (1) poor motivation [e.g. ‘I am not motivated at all to take part in the assessment.’; ‘I am willing to do my best.’ (reverse)], (2) concerns about the assessment [e.g. ‘I experience the situation as very unpleasant and would like to leave.’; ‘I know what I can expect from the assessment.’ (reverse)] (3) fear about poor outcome [e.g. ‘I worry that the tasks will be too difficult for me.’; ‘In general, I feel confident before and after assessments. I am aware of my cognitive strengths and am able to show them.’ (reverse)] (4) negative momentary influences [e.g. ‘Right now, I feel very tired and exhausted.’; ‘I feel fit and capable.’ (reverse)]. Items were rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 – fully agree, 2 – rather agree, 3 – rather disagree, 4 – disagree). Scores were inverted for all subscales; higher scores designate more negative thoughts and feelings associated with the assessment.

Following the neuropsychological assessment, participants completed the post version of the MIAMI questionnaire, which again consisted of 18 items that were rated using the same Likert scale as before. The retrospective ratings measure the same domains albeit with different items: (1) poor motivation [e.g. ‘I was not motivated and accordingly did not achieve my best performance.’; ‘I pushed myself and gave my best.’ (reverse)], (2) concerns about the assessment [e.g. ‘I felt under pressure by the test situation.’; ‘The testing situation was not as bad as I had expected.’ (reverse)] (3) fear about poor outcome (e.g. ‘I am fearful about the results.’) (4) negative momentary influences [e.g. ‘I had a headache during the assessment.’; ‘I could sufficiently concentrate during the assessment’ (reverse)]. Subscale scores were inverted; higher scores designate more negative thoughts and feelings associated with the assessment. In an unrelated study (Moritz et al. submitted) in 30 patients with OCD, the MIAMI total score modestly correlated (r = 0.42, p < 0.01) with an expert rating (the assessor was asked to determine the patient's motivation and distraction by symptoms among other indexes).

Strategy of data analysis

We first compared the two groups using t-tests for independent samples on all neurocognitive tasks. In order to assess whether momentary influences, assessment-related fears and concerns, as well as motivation, as tapped by the MIAMI scale, mediate group differences on neuropsychological functioning, we adopted a two-fold strategy. For the main analyses, we calculated mediation analyses using Hayes’ process procedure (model 4; 5000 bootstrap samples) with group as the independent variable (x), neuropsychological functioning as the dependent variable (y) and MIAMI total scores as the mediator (M). Analyses were run separately for the two neuropsychological indexes (speed, performance) and two MIAMI (pre, post) total scores. Second, we recalculated mean comparisons by entering MIAMI total scores (pre and post separately) as covariates to explore whether effect sizes would be attenuated substantially. Please note that the latter analyses are meant to aid in the interpretation of the mediation analyses and represent no proper test of whether the mediator/covariate exerts a significant effect.

Results

Both samples were very similar with respect to age, gender and education. Patients differed from controls on all MIAMI subscores and total scores, except for the motivation subscale at baseline. Cronbach's alphas for the MIAMI total score were 0.70 (baseline) and 0.78 (post). As expected, patients performed worse on most cognitive test parameters (16 out of 18 scores) relative to controls (see Table 2). On average, between group-effect sizes were large (M d  = 0.78). Almost two out of three patients (64%) performed at least one standard deviation below the mean on two or more tests (controls: 32%)Footnote 2 . When stricter criteria were applied (at least two standard deviations below the mean on two or more tests, for a critical discussion of thresholds see Abramovitch & Schweiger, Reference Abramovitch and Schweiger2015) the percentage dropped to 14% in patients v. 0% in controls.

Table 2. Differences in neurocognitive functioning between schizophrenia and healthy individuals

Notes. T = T-scores (M = 50), P = percentile (M = 50), S = scaled score (M = 10).

As shown in Table 3, correlational analyses indicated significant associations between aggregated neuropsychological and MIAMI scores in 17 of 20 correlations (2 aggregated neuropsychological scores x 2 (pre, post) × 5 MIAMI (sub)scales); the average absolute correlation was r = 0.30. Only motivation prior to testing, as well as concerns about the assessment (post), were uncorrelated with neurocognitive functioning.

Table 3. Correlations between MIAMI subscales with the aggregated neuropsychological scores (full sample)

+p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.005, ****p < 0.001.

Figure 1 shows that, with the exception of mediation B (i.e. the effect of group on speed, mediator: MIAMI post), the significant total relationship between group and neurocognition was largely mediated by MIAMI total scores, as evidenced by a significant Sobel test (in addition, confidence intervals of the indirect effect did not cross zero), even though all direct effects remained significant. Results remained unchanged when the subscale Negative Momentary Influences was removed from the total score, as one may argue that this subscale may partly reflect the subjective perception of one's actual cognitive impairment.

Fig. 1. Results from the mediation analyses using Hayes’ process macro. Group status predicted MIAMI scores (=a) and these in turn predicted neuropsychological measures (=b). Except for analysis B, the indirect effect was significant (i.e. confidence intervals did not cross 0) indicating that momentary influences, concerns and fears as well as motivation (MIAMI subscales) largely contribute to group differences (0 = healthy, 1 = patients) on test results.

The result of the mediation analysis was corroborated by the ANCOVA analyses displayed in the last columns of Table 2, showing that effect sizes decreased substantially if MIAMI scores were accounted for (for ANCOVAs using the MIAMI baseline score, the average effect size dropped to d = 0.53, for the post version it was d = 0.51; uncorrected average effect size: d = 0.78, see above). The uncorrected group comparisons yielded twelve large (d ⩾ 0.8) and 5 medium effects (d ⩾ 0.5; 18 subscores and 2 aggregated scores). When the MIAMI baseline total score was accounted for, only 3 comparisons showed large effects and 9 effects were in the medium range. For the MIAMI post score (i.e. retrospective assessment of the test situation), a similar picture emerged (i.e. 3 large effects, 10 medium effects).

Discussion

At first glance, our results corroborate a plethora of studies suggesting that patient with schizophrenia have severe neurocognitive deficits (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998; Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Silva, Perkins and Lieberman1999; Schaefer et al. Reference Schaefer, Giangrande, Weinberger and Dickinson2013; Fatouros-Bergman et al. Reference Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman and Farde2014). On average, we found large effect sizes indicating better neurocognitive performance among controls. However, upon closer inspection, some caution is warranted when interpreting these findings.

First, only a minority of patients in our study had neurocognitive abnormalities according to standard criteria (Abramovitch & Schweiger, Reference Abramovitch and Schweiger2015); 14% of patients scored at least two standard deviations below the mean on two or more tests. This rose to 64% if the threshold is set very liberally (at least one standard deviation below the norm on two or more tests). This result is clearly not novel or surprising. Clinicians trained in statistics know that group differences eclipse outliers to both ends of the performance distribution. Yet, when such findings are communicated to peers and the public, researchers often fail to highlight that results cannot be generalized to every single individual, thereby possibly increasing stigma and fears (e.g. about dementia). Of note, even Kraepelin (Reference Kraepelin1899) described a substantial subgroup of patients who recovered without residual symptoms, casting doubt on his general claim about premature dementia, which continues to fuel the interest in cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.

Secondly, patients markedly differed from controls regarding their endorsements of subjective influences on cognitive performance before and after assessment, particularly regarding fears about the outcome of the test, distracting symptoms (e.g. headache, tiredness), and performance motivationFootnote 3 (see also Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Zakzanis, Foussias, Graff-Guerrero, Agid and Remington2014). Indeed, as predicted, a number of these variables heavily impacted performance. If taken into account statistically, group differences in neurocognitive functioning were still detectable (= molehill) but not as huge (= mountain) as before – after correcting for subjective influences on cognitive performance, differences declined from large effects to medium. Differences in subjective influences on cognitive performance found before testing are especially meaningful, as these suggest that differences were real and not just ad-hoc explanations for poor performance. Again, while neurocognitive deficits were detectable among patients with schizophrenia, even if contextual factors are accounted for, this says little about individual performance. Momentary influences, negative attitudes and fear about the assessment may shift performance of an unimpaired participant to impaired or push poor performances in the range of one standard deviation below the norm to two standard deviations (which is often considered diagnostically relevant and may thus lead to additional diagnoses).

We argue that the investigation has a number of important implications. As researchers, we might need to adjust our language. While it is circumstantial to talk about ‘a subgroup of patients’ rather than patients (in general), the latter formulation enhances stigma as not all patients have neurocognitive deficits. In light of the high prevalence of depression, suicidal ideation and low self-esteem in those afflicted with schizophrenia, we must be careful not to insult patients when confronting them with test results or to induce further fears that may propagate self-stigma (e.g. giving a comorbid diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment), particularly when transient secondary influences, like medication and poor motivation cannot be ruled out. As shown, due to internal (e.g. poor motivation due to a self-fulfilling prophecy), external (e.g. medication) or symptom-related (e.g. rumination, voice-hearing that distracts the individual) factors, many patients do not achieve their best performance. We need to pay closer attention to these factors before making serious recommendations; for example, whether to further academic education or career decisions. As mentioned before, matching control samples for demographic variables is clearly not enough in this regard.

We would also like to highlight a number of limitations. First, results need to be replicated with larger samples and psychiatric control groups, for whom similar findings have been reported (Scheurich et al. Reference Scheurich, Fellgiebel, Schermuly, Bauer, Wölfges and Müller2008; Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Hottenrott, Jelinek, Brooks and Scheurich2012). One may also object that our samples were equated for background characteristics, such as education, which is usually lower in patients than controls. We also excluded patients with dependence disorders or neurological problems, which are also frequently observed in patients with schizophrenia. We did this so that we could attribute any differences on cognitive test results to diagnostic status and not to secondary factors other than MIAMI scores. This strategy may have contributed to a slight overestimation of patients’ performance. Our results thus need replication in more severe samples. Second, motivation, attitudes and momentary symptoms were measured with self-report scales. Subjective poor performance may prompt a self-serving bias and a tendency to blame performance on context, motivation or other factors. Indeed, a tendency to externalize blame has been frequently observed in patients with psychosis (Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, Kinderman and Kaney1994; Kinderman & Bentall, Reference Kinderman and Bentall1996). In order to minimize such biases, we administered the MIAMI questionnaire both before and after assessment. Subsequent trials may also try to collect objective data (e.g. number of yawns during assessment, psychophysiological recording) and explore the impact of specific symptoms (e.g. distraction due to voice-hearing, delusional pre-occupations etc.). In addition, with the exception of one study (see methods section), the MIAMI scale has not been validated against other measures of motivation or performance anxiety. We could also not control for medication in our analysis as all patients were medicated; chlorpromazine equivalent or percentage of maximum dosage algorithms cannot fully address this problem (Strauss et al. Reference Strauss, Morra, Sullivan and Gold2015).

For the future, it seems important to examine if secondary impairments may also depend on how investigations are framed. For example, some investigators explicitly tell patients that tests are carried out to investigate impaired cognition, which is introduced as an important factor for the formation of the illness. Such information may elicit stereotype threat (Pennington et al. Reference Pennington, Heim, Levy and Larkin2016), frustrate patients and impede their full engagement (Kit et al. Reference Kit, Tuokko and Mateer2008). Unfortunately, studies often report little information beyond which specific tests were administered. From studies with women and older people (Spencer et al. Reference Spencer, Steele and Quinn1999), we already know that even subtle formulations (framing) that potentially trigger stigma can impair performance.

To conclude, bold claims that ‘neurocognitive impairment is present in most, if not all, persons with schizophrenia’ (Palmer et al. Reference Palmer, Dawes and Heaton2009) (p. 365) may need to be revised in light of our findings. We should refrain from monocausal inferences when low test scores are detected in patients. While brain-related deficits may undoubtedly play a vital role, they are not the only source of impairment.

Footnotes

SM and JPK have equally contributed to the manuscript and share first authorship.

The notes appear after the main text.

1 For a copy of the MIAMI questionnaires, please write to

2 We would like to thank one anonymous referee for suggesting this algorithm.

3 Interestingly, differences in motivation emerged for the post but not the baseline assessment. Based on clinical experience, we speculate that many patients are initially as motivated as controls but lose interest and motivation in the course of the assessment.

References

Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS, Mittelman, A (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 11631171.Google Scholar
Abramovitch, A, Schweiger, A (2015). Misuse of cognitive neuropsychology in psychiatry research: the intoxicating appeal of neo-reductionism. Behavior Therapist 38, 187191.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA.Google Scholar
Bentall, RP, Kinderman, P, Kaney, S (1994). The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy 32, 331341.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Murray, RM (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in ultra-high risk to psychosis and first-episode psychosis: do the cognitive deficits progress over, or after, the onset of psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 744755.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Yucel, M, Pantelis, C (2009). Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. British Journal of Psychiatry 195, 475482.Google Scholar
Brewer, WJ, Wood, SJ, Phillips, LJ, Francey, SM, Pantelis, C, Yung, AR, Cornblatt, B, McGorry, PD (2006). Generalized and specific cognitive performance in clinical high-risk cohorts: a review highlighting potential vulnerability markers for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 538555.Google Scholar
Brickenkamp, R (1962). Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test Handanweisung d-2 [Attention-Pressure-Test Manual d2]. Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Carlsson, R, Nyman, H, Ganse, G, Cullberg, J (2006). Neuropsychological functions predict 1- and 3-year outcome in first-episode psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 113, 102111.Google Scholar
Cella, M, Reeder, C, Wykes, T (2015). Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia-now it is really getting personal. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 4, 147151.Google Scholar
Fatouros-Bergman, H, Cervenka, S, Flyckt, L, Edman, G, Farde, L (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in drug-naïve patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 158, 156162.Google Scholar
Fervaha, G, Agid, O, Takeuchi, H, Lee, J, Foussias, G, Zakzanis, KK, Graff-Guerrero, A, Remington, G (2015). Extrapyramidal symptoms and cognitive test performance in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 161, 351356.Google Scholar
Fervaha, G, Zakzanis, KK, Foussias, G, Graff-Guerrero, A, Agid, O, & Remington, G (2014). Motivational deficits and cognitive test performance in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 10581065.Google Scholar
Fett, AKJ, Viechtbauer, W, Dominguez, MDG, Penn, DL, van Os, J, Krabbendam, L (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35, 573588.Google Scholar
Green, MF (1996). What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry 153, 321330.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Kern, RS, Braff, DL, Mintz, J (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the ‘right stuff’? Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 119136.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Kern, RS, Heaton, RK (2004). Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophrenia Research 72, 4151.Google Scholar
Härting, C, Markowitsch, HJ, Neufeld, H, Calabrese, P, Deisinger, K, Kessler, J (2000). Wechsler Gedächtnistest - Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R) [Wechsler Memory Scale - revised version (WMS-R)] . Hans Huber: Bern.Google Scholar
Harvey, PD, Loewenstein, DA, Czaja, SJ (2013). Hospitalization and psychosis: Influences on the course of cognition and everyday functioning in people with schizophrenia. Neurobiology of Disease 53, 1825.Google Scholar
Heaton, RK (1981). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, RW, Zakzanis, KK (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426445.Google Scholar
Helmstaedter, C, Lendt, M, Lux, S (2001). VLMT Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest [VLMT Verbal Learning and Memory Test]. Belz Test: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Hovington, CL, Lepage, M (2012). Neurocognition and neuroimaging of persistent negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 12, 5369.Google Scholar
Jacobs, C, Petermann, F (2007). Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (WIE) [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults]. Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, Psychologie und Psychotherapie 55, 205208.Google Scholar
Keefe, RS, Silva, SG, Perkins, DO, Lieberman, JA (1999). The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25, 201222.Google Scholar
Kinderman, P, Bentall, RP (1996). A new measure of causal locus: the internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences 20, 261264.Google Scholar
Kit, KA, Tuokko, HA, Mateer, CA (2008). A review of the stereotype threat literature and its application in a neurological population. Neuropsychology Review 18, 132148.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E (1899). Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Aerzte [Psychiatry. A textbook for students and physicians]. J. A. Barth: Leipzig.Google Scholar
Kurtz, MM (2011). Neurocognition as a predictor of response to evidence-based psychosocial interventions in schizophrenia: what is the state of the evidence? Clinical Psychology Review 31, 663672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leucht, S, Kane, JM, Kissling, W, Hamann, J, Etschel, E, Engel, R (2005). Clinical implications of Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale scores. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 366371.Google Scholar
Lezak, MD (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd ed. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Loong, JWK (1990). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (IBM version) . Wang Neuropsychological Laboratory: San Luis Obispo, CA.Google Scholar
López-Muñoz, F, Álamo, C (2011). Neurobiological background for the development of new drugs in schizophrenia. Clinical Neuropharmacology 34, 111126.Google Scholar
Mathews, SB, Arnold, SE, Epperson, CN (2014). Hospitalization and cognitive decline: can the nature of the relationship be deciphered? American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22, 465480.Google Scholar
Mesholam-Gately, RI, Giuliano, AJ, Goff, KP, Faraone, SV, Seidman, LJ (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 23, 315336.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Birkner, C, Kloss, M, Jahn, H, Hand, I, Haasen, C, Krausz, M (2002). Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17, 477483.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Heeren, D, Andresen, B, Krausz, M (2001). An analysis of the specificity and the syndromal correlates of verbal memory impairments in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 101, 2331.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Hottenrott, B, Jelinek, L, Brooks, AM, Scheurich, A (2012). Effects of obsessive-compulsive symptoms on neuropsychological test performance: complicating an already complicated story. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 26, 3144.Google Scholar
Overall, JE, Gorham, DR (1962). The brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychological Reports 10, 799812.Google Scholar
Palmer, BW, Dawes, SE, Heaton, RK (2009). What do we know about neuropsychological aspects of schizophrenia? Neuropsychology Review 19, 365384.Google Scholar
Pennington, CR, Heim, D, Levy, AR, Larkin, DT (2016). Twenty years of stereotype threat research: a review of psychological mediators. PLoS ONE 11, e0146487.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A (2010). The assessment of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12, 383392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, RM (1992). Trail Making Test. Manual of Administration and Scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory: Tucson, AZ, USA.Google Scholar
Schaefer, J, Giangrande, E, Weinberger, DR, Dickinson, D (2013). The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophrenia Research 150, 4250.Google Scholar
Scheurich, A, Fellgiebel, A, Schermuly, I, Bauer, S, Wölfges, R, Müller, MJ (2008). Experimental evidence for a motivational origin of cognitive impairment in major depression. Psychological Medicine 38, 237246.Google Scholar
Schmidt-Atzert, L (2004). Test d2: Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test [Test d2: attention-strain-test]. Diagnostik von Konzentration und Aufmerksamkeit 6th edn. Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R, Dunbar, GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59, 2233.Google Scholar
Shin, NY, Lee, TY, Kim, E, Kwon, JS (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 11211130.Google Scholar
Spencer, SJ, Steele, CM, Quinn, DM (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35, 428.Google Scholar
Strauss, GP, Morra, LF, Sullivan, SK, Gold, JM (2015). The role of low cognitive effort and negative symptoms in neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 29, 282291.Google Scholar
Tombaugh, TN (2004). Trail making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19, 203214.Google Scholar
Vinogradov, S, Fisher, M, Warm, H, Holland, C, Kirshner, MA, Pollock, BG (2009). The cognitive cost of anticholinergic burden: decreased response to cognitive training in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 10551062.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) . Pearson: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Wilson, B, Alderman, N, Burgess, P, Emslie, H, Evans, J (2003). Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome (BADS). Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability 5, 3337.Google Scholar
Wilson, RS, Hebert, LE, Scherr, PA, Dong, X, Leurgens, SE, Evans, DA (2012). Cognitive decline after hospitalization in a community population of older persons. Neurology 78, 950956.Google Scholar
Woodard, JL, Axelrod, BN (1987). Wechsler memory scale - revised. Psychological Assessment 7, 445449. ST–Parsimonious prediction of Wechsler.Google Scholar
Wykes, T, Huddy, V, Cellard, C, McGurk, SR, Czobor, P (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 472485.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P, Fimm, B (1995). Test for Attentional Performance (TAP) . PsyTest: Herzogenrath.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Differences between schizophrenia and healthy individuals on demographic characteristics and scores on the MIAMI subscales

Figure 1

Table 2. Differences in neurocognitive functioning between schizophrenia and healthy individuals

Figure 2

Table 3. Correlations between MIAMI subscales with the aggregated neuropsychological scores (full sample)

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Results from the mediation analyses using Hayes’ process macro. Group status predicted MIAMI scores (=a) and these in turn predicted neuropsychological measures (=b). Except for analysis B, the indirect effect was significant (i.e. confidence intervals did not cross 0) indicating that momentary influences, concerns and fears as well as motivation (MIAMI subscales) largely contribute to group differences (0 = healthy, 1 = patients) on test results.