Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-956mj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-19T04:45:46.044Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2007

JAMES B. KIRKBRIDE*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
CRAIG MORGAN
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College, London, UK
PAUL FEARON
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College, London, UK
PAOLA DAZZAN
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College, London, UK
ROBIN M. MURRAY
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College, London, UK
PETER B. JONES
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: James B. Kirkbride, Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Box 189, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK. (Email: jbk25@cam.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The incidence of schizophrenia varies by individual-level characteristics and neighbourhood-level attributes. Few specific socio-environmental risk factors (SERFs) have been identified at the neighbourhood level. Cross-level interactions are poorly understood. We investigated these issues using data from the Aetiology and Ethnicity in Schizophrenia and Other Psychoses (AESOP) study.

Method

All incidence cases of ICD-10 schizophrenia (F20) and other non-affective psychoses (F21–29), aged 16–64 years, across 33 wards in Southeast London were identified over a 2-year period (1997–1999). Census data provided the denominator for each ward. Multilevel Poisson regression simultaneously modelled individual- and neighbourhood-level SERFs, including socio-economic deprivation, voter turnout (proxy for social capital), ethnic fragmentation (segregation) and ethnic density.

Results

A total of 218 subjects were identified during 565 576 person-years at risk. Twenty-three per cent of variance in incidence of schizophrenia across wards could be attributed to neighbourhood-level risk factors [95% confidence interval (CI) 9·9–42·2]. Thus, 1% increases in voter turnout [incidence rate ratio (IRR) 0·95, 95% CI 0·92–0·99] and ethnic segregation (IRR 0·95, 95% CI 0·92–0·99) were both independently associated with a reduced incidence of 5%, independent of age, sex, ethnicity, deprivation and population density. This was similar for other non-affective psychoses. There was some evidence that ethnic minority individuals were at greater risk of schizophrenia in areas with smaller proportions of minority groups (p=0·07).

Conclusion

SERFs at individual and neighbourhood levels were implicated in the aetiology of psychosis, but we were unable to determine whether these associations were causal. Individual risk may be mediated by social capital, which could operate as a protective factor, perhaps moderating social stress in the onset of psychoses.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

INTRODUCTION

In 1939, Faris and Dunham demonstrated variation in the incidence of schizophrenia across the city of Chicago, with the highest rates in the most socially disorganized tracts. In stark contrast, the affective psychoses showed no such variation. Subsequently, social class was proposed as a candidate explanation for these findings (Hollingshead & Redlich, Reference Hollingshead and Redlich1958), but methodological limitations of the ecological approach, combined with difficulties disentangling social drift from causation, contributed to a disengagement of research into psychosis from the social sciences (Pilgrim & Roger, Reference Pilgrim and Roger2005). Ecological research was superseded by an individualistic approach to disease causation, and a belief that place played little or no role in causation (Jablensky et al. Reference Jablensky, Sartorius, Ernberg, Anker, Korten, Cooper, Day and Bertelsen1992; Crow, Reference Crow2000).

This paradigm prevailed throughout the 1990s, despite evidence of a dose–response relationship between urban birth and upbringing and adult onset of schizophrenia (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, David, Andreasson and Allebeck1992; Takei et al. Reference Takei, O'Callaghan and Sham1992; Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Pedersen, Westergaard, Wohlfahrt, Ewald, Mors, Andersen and Melbye1999; Pedersen & Mortensen, Reference Pedersen and Mortensen2001), implying that social causation, and not social drift, was the most parsimonious explanation of urban–rural differences in the incidence of psychosis. Despite this, few specific environmental correlates of risk for non-affective psychoses have been unequivocally identified. More recently, neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors (SERFs) have been associated with psychoses, including deprivation (Croudace et al. Reference Croudace, Kayne, Jones and Harrison2000), social mobility (Silver et al. Reference Silver, Mulvey and Swanson2002) and social capital (Allardyce et al. Reference Allardyce, Gilmour, Atkinson, Rapson, Bishop and McCreadie2005). Social capital can be considered as a group of features related to the social organization of a neighbourhood that, collectively, ‘facilitate coordination and cooperation for mutual benefit’ (Putnam, Reference Putnam1993, p. 36). Despite these findings, two recent reviews have highlighted the paucity of empirical evidence linking social capital to psychoses (McKenzie et al. Reference McKenzie, Whitley and Weich2002; De Silva et al. Reference De Silva, McKenzie, Harpham and Huttly2005).

There is some evidence that individual-level risk may be conditional upon neighbourhood characteristics (van Os et al. Reference van Os, Driessen, Gunther and Delespaul2000). For example, Boydell et al. (Reference Boydell, van Os, McKenzie, Allardyce, Goel, McCreadie and Murray2001) observed that the risk of schizophrenia for black and minority ethnic (BME) individuals increased in neighbourhoods with smaller proportions of BME residents (ethnic density hypothesis). This finding has been replicated in The Hague (Veling et al. Reference Veling, Selten, Van Hoeken, Hoek and Mackenbach2006), but was not found in an earlier study in the UK (Cochrane & Bal, Reference Cochrane and Bal1988).

Recent findings from the Aetiology and Ethnicity in Schizophrenia and Other Psychoses (AESOP) study also support variation in the incidence of schizophrenia – both between and within cities – beyond that which can be explained by individual-level differences in age, sex and ethnicity (Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Murray and Jones2006, in press). These findings do not exclude the possibility that SERFs at the neighbourhood level may be aetiologically relevant to the onset of psychosis, perhaps involving gene–environment or other interactions (Krabbendam & van Os, Reference Krabbendam and van Os2005). By contrast, we found no evidence to support corresponding variance for the affective psychoses, either between or within cities, after adjustment for individual-level confounders.

In this study, we attempted to quantify the proportion of variation in incidence rates of non-affective psychoses that could be attributed to neighbourhood-level factors using data from the AESOP study. Having controlled for individual-level age, sex and ethnicity, we then attempted to explain any variation at the neighbourhood level by modelling potentially relevant SERFs, including markers of social capital, socio-economic deprivation, ethnic density and the interaction between ethnic density and individual-level ethnic status.

METHOD

The AESOP study is a large, population-based first-episode psychosis study conducted over 2 years in three UK centres: Southeast London, Nottingham and Bristol. We have previously provided a detailed methodology of the AESOP study as well as descriptive epidemiology of the dataset and between-centre differences in rates of psychoses (Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Murray and Jones2006). In the current study, we considered within-centre differences in the incidence of non-affective psychoses and potential individual- and neighbourhood-level effects that may have explained this variation in Southeast London. We did not include data from Nottingham or Bristol because our sample sizes were underpowered to test for neighbourhood effects using multilevel models in these centres.

Population at risk

The population ‘at risk’ was estimated from the 2001 Census of Great Britain; the temporally closest data to our AESOP study period (September 1997–August 1999). All people aged 16–64 years residing in our Southeast London centre at the time of the census (29 April 2001) were included. This area comprised 33 Census Area Statistic (CAS) wards covering the Local Authorities (LA) of Lambeth and the southern two-thirds of Southwark. CAS wards (mean size: 5880 people) were introduced in the 2001 census to supersede electoral wards. We used CAS wards to define our neighbourhoods because a large amount of neighbourhood-level information was available at this level from routine data sources. The census population was doubled to account for the 2-year study period.

Case ascertainment

We identified everyone aged 16–64 years living in Southeast London who made contact with mental health services because of a first episode of any probable psychosis. Initial inclusion criteria were based upon those used in the World Health Organization (WHO) ten-country study (Jablensky et al. Reference Jablensky, Sartorius, Ernberg, Anker, Korten, Cooper, Day and Bertelsen1992). For these analyses, we also excluded cases if an address at first presentation was not obtained.

All potential cases presenting to psychiatric services were screened. We contacted health service bases weekly to identify all potential contacts. A leakage study, based on the methods of Cooper et al. (Reference Cooper, Goodhead, Craig, Harris, Howat and Korer1987), was conducted after the survey period to identify subjects missed by the screening process. Our case-finding procedure thus included as many potential service bases as possible in order to estimate the true incidence of psychoses in our study area (Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Murray and Jones2006).

Subjects who passed the screen underwent the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) and a modified Personal and Psychiatric History Schedule. We completed the SCAN Item Group Checklist for all subjects who declined an interview, based upon case-notes and information from clinical staff. ICD-10 diagnoses were made by consensus agreement from a panel of clinicians, including a principal investigator and the clinical researcher who conducted the individual assessments. Inter-rater reliability was high (Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Murray and Jones2006).

We obtained sociodemographic data for our cases from a schedule developed for the AESOP study (Mallett, Reference Mallett1997). Case ethnicity was ascribed using self-ascription, place of birth and parental place of birth, rated independently by three researchers (J.B.K., C.M. and P.F.). Discrepant classifications were resolved by consensus with a fourth researcher (P.B.J.). Inter-rater reliability was high (κ=0·92).

Socio-environmental risk factors (SERFs)

Individual-level SERFs

We included three individual-level risk factors in our study: age, sex and ethnicity. For each CAS ward, we stratified our numerator and denominator by age (16–19, 20–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–64 years), sex and ethnicity; the latter ascribed to one of 16 categories defined in the 2001 census. For our analyses, we collapsed ethnicity into a seven-category variable: White British, Black Caribbean (including Black Other groups), Black African, Asian (Indian, Pakistani, Bangladeshi), Mixed ethnicity, White Other (White Irish and White Other) and Other ethnic groups. A dichotomous ethnicity variable (BME versus White British) was also used to test the ethnic density hypothesis (discussed later).

Neighbourhood-level SERFs

We obtained data on neighbourhood-level SERFs that we hypothesized may be associated with psychoses. Our choice was guided by the previous literature. These variables were predominantly obtained from nationally collected, routine sources. We used the 2001 census to measure population density (people per hectare, pph), ethnic density (BME population as a proportion of total population) and ethnic fragmentation, using the Index of Dissimilarity (ID), in each ward.

The ID is a geographical measure of the extent to which two groups are evenly distributed (or segregated) across a neighbourhood (ward), frequently used in the social sciences (Peach, Reference Peach, Jackson and Smith1981, Reference Peach1996). Here, we interpreted the ID as a measure of ethnic segregation between White British and BME populations. ID scores may vary between 0 (no segregation) and 100 (total segregation). To obtain an ID score for each ward, it was necessary to know the distribution of the White British and BME populations within each ward. This was determined by considering the proportion of White British and BME groups within Output Areas (OAs) in each ward. OAs are nested within CAS wards and provide the finest spatial scale at which the census is enumerated. In Southeast London the mean number of OAs per ward was 42·6, with a mean population size of 286 people per OA. The ID was calculated using the following formula:

{\rm ID} \equals {1 \over 2}\sum\limits_{i \equals \setnum{1}}^{N} {\left\vert {{{b_{i} } \over B} \minus \left. {{{w_{i} } \over W}} \right\vert} \right.}\times {\rm 100\comma}

where b i is the BME population of the ith OA, B is the total BME population in the CAS ward for which the ID is being calculated, w i is the White British population of the ith OA, and W is the total White British population in the CAS ward for which the ID is being calculated.

ID can be interpreted as the proportion of one population needed to move to a different OA (in the same ward) to achieve an even distribution of the two populations across the ward. More segregated wards would require a larger proportion of either population to move between OA to achieve an even population distribution. As segregation increases, each ethnic group becomes less fragmented. Here, we interpret high ID scores as markers of less ethnic fragmentation within the White British and BME groups.

We used the Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) (2004) to measure deprivation within each CAS ward. The IMD incorporates six domains (unemployment, education, health, barriers to housing and services, living unit environment, crime) drawing on a range of nationally collected datasets, predominantly collected between 1997 and 2001 (ODPM, 2004). The IMD was released at Super Output Area (SOA) and LA level, but not CAS ward level directly, which is nested between these two levels of geography. To obtain a ward-level measure of deprivation, we therefore calculated the mean SOA IMD score for each CAS ward, weighting each SOA IMD score by its relative population contribution to the total CAS ward population. As IMD scores are not directly interpretable, we standardized them to have a mean of zero and standard deviation of one.

We obtained data on voter turnout at local government elections as a proxy for social capital. We considered that these data provided a proxy measure of social capital, representing the extent to which people within each ward were motivated by local-level issues. Local elections are held to determine political control of the LA, with each ward electing a candidate to represent their constituency. We obtained voter turnout data at the 2002 local elections for each ward within our study area (Rawlings & Thrasher, Reference Rawlings and Thrasher2002); temporally closest to our study period using CAS ward geography.

Statistical analyses

We used multilevel Poisson regression with random effects to model the incidence of schizophrenia (F20) and other non-affective psychoses (F21–29) between CAS wards. By fitting random effects at ward level we were able to quantify the variation in the incidence of each outcome between neighbourhoods. We entered potential individual- and neighbourhood-level SERFs as fixed effects and included the logarithm of our denominator as an offset, to test whether there was an association between neighbourhood-level SERFs and the incidence of non-affective psychoses.

All potential SERFs were entered into a univariate analysis as fixed effects. We monitored change in residual variance (variance in incidence at the neighbourhood level not explained by fixed effects) from the null model (without fixed effects) after fitting each SERF. We then entered all individual-level variables into a multivariate model, followed by significant neighbourhood-level variables, entered according to the magnitude in change of residual variance identified in the univariate analysis. We re-entered non-significant variables to ensure that they did not improve our final model. We tested the ethnic density hypothesis by considering an a priori interaction between individual-level ethnic status and neighbourhood-level ethnic density. For both outcomes, we tested this interaction in two models containing identical covariates, but differing with respect to the individual-level ethnicity variable used. The first model contained our seven-category ethnicity variable, while the second model was fitted with our dichotomous ethnicity variable, in line with the previous literature (Boydell et al. Reference Boydell, van Os, McKenzie, Allardyce, Goel, McCreadie and Murray2001; Veling et al. Reference Veling, Selten, Van Hoeken, Hoek and Mackenbach2006). Significance testing of fixed effects and their interactions was assessed by likelihood ratio tests (LRTs). We reported the level of residual variation at the neighbourhood level and incidence rate ratios (IRRs) for all fixed effects. Modelling was conducted using xtpoisson in stata version 9 (StataCorp, 2005).

RESULTS

We identified 228 cases of non-affective disorder in Southeast London during 565 000 person-years of follow-up. Ten cases (4·4%) were excluded because an address at first presentation was not obtained, leaving 218 cases. Sixty-eight per cent received a diagnosis of schizophrenia (n=148) (ICD-10 F20), the remaining subjects having other non-affective psychoses (n=70). Fifty-seven per cent of our sample were men, 50% under 30 years of age, and 75% from a BME group (Table 1). In comparison, men comprised 49% of our denominator, 35% of the denominator were under 30 years of age, and 36% were from a BME group.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample and denominator population

BME, Black and minority ethnic.

Values are given as n (%).

Neighbourhood-level SERFs were normally distributed with little evidence of skewness (data available on request). We observed considerable heterogeneity by ward in terms of ethnic density (24·8–74·3%) and population density (31·8–154·5 pph). The range of values was narrower for ethnic fragmentation (11·1–32·9%) and voter turnout (17·1–41·6%). The IMD was normally distributed across the wards in our study region. However, when we compared the rank position of our SOA IMD scores with all SOAs in England, there was evidence that our study area was relatively deprived; 95% of the 268 SOAs in our CAS wards were in the most deprived 50% of SOAs in England. We observed significant correlations between several neighbourhood-level SERFs (Table 2). Notably, socio-economic deprivation was highly positively correlated with the proportion of BME residents at the neighbourhood level (ethnic density) (r 2=0·84, p<0·01) and with population density (r 2=0·49, p<0·01). Voter turnout was negatively correlated with socio-economic deprivation (r 2=−0·43, p<0·05) and the proportion of BME residents at the neighbourhood level (r 2=−0·43, p<0·05).

Table 2. Correlation matrix for neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors (SERFs)

* Significant at p<0·05.

** Significant at p<0·01.

Schizophrenia

We observed considerable neighbourhood-level variance for schizophrenia in our null multilevel model [intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) 0·36, 95% CI 0·15–0·82, p<0·01], representing 26·5% [0·36/(1+0·36)] of total variance (95% CI 13·0–45·1) (Table 3). Adjustment for individual age, sex and ethnicity led to a reduction in the proportion of variance attributable to the neighbourhood level, but this variation remained statistically significant (22·5%, 95% CI 9·9–42·2). There was evidence from univariate analyses that deprivation, ethnic fragmentation and voter turnout were associated with the incidence of schizophrenia. These variables were entered into our multivariate model.

Table 3. Modelling of individual- and neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors for schizophrenia

SERFs, Socio-environmental risk factors; IRR, incidence rate ratio; LRT, likelihood ratio test; df, degrees of freedom; CI, confidence interval.

a Individual- and neighbourhood-level variables included in final model.

b IRR reports change in risk of schizophrenia associated with a 1% increase in the neighbourhood-level variable.

Voter turnout and ethnic fragmentation were significantly associated with schizophrenia at the neighbourhood level after adjustment for individual-level age, sex and ethnicity and neighbourhood-level deprivation (multivariate model, Table 3). Thus, a 1% increase in voter turnout was associated with a reduction in the incidence of schizophrenia of 5% (IRR 0·95, 95% CI 0·92–0·99). Similarly, as wards became less ethnically fragmented, the incidence of schizophrenia fell (IRR 0·95, 95% CI 0·92–0·99). These findings were independent of deprivation, which did not significantly improve the model (LRT p=0·12). Population density and ethnic density were not associated with schizophrenia and were removed from the final model. After fitting the final multivariate model, there was no evidence of significant residual variance at the neighbourhood level (χ2p=0·30). IRRs for several ethnic groups were raised, as reported previously (Fearon et al. Reference Fearon, Kirkbride, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Lloyd, Hutchinson, Tarrant, Lun, Holloway, Mallett, Harrison, Leff, Jones and Murray2006), and were not significantly attenuated by adjustment for neighbourhood-level SERFs.

There was no evidence that the association between schizophrenia and ethnicity (seven-category) was modified by ethnic density in our final multivariate model (LRT p=0·18) (Table 3). However, when we tested this interaction using our dichotomous ethnicity variable, retaining the above variables in our final model (age, sex, deprivation, voter turnout and ethnic fragmentation), we observed some evidence (LRT p=0·07) that the risk of schizophrenia in the BME group was conditional upon the ethnic density of the neighbourhood. Thus, the risk of schizophrenia for BME individuals was highest in the third of wards with the smallest proportion of BME residents (IRR 6·51, 95% CI 3·01–14·08). This risk appeared to be lower for BME residents living in more ethnically dense neighbourhoods, despite some imprecision in our estimates (Table 4).

Table 4. Interaction between individual-level BME status (dichotomous) and ethnic density for schizophrenia

IRR, Incidence rate ratio; BME, black and minority ethnic; CI, confidence interval.

a After adjustment for age, sex, deprivation, ethnic fragmentation and voter turnout. Model identical to final model presented in Table 3, except a dichotomous ethnicity variable (BME versus White British) was fitted instead of the seven-category ethnicity variable to test the interaction with ethnic density.

Other non-affective psychoses

We observed significant neighbourhood-level variance for other non-affective psychoses in our null model (23·1%, 95% CI 6·5–53·9, p=0·02) (Table 5). Adjustment for age, sex and ethnicity at the individual level led to some attenuation of this figure, with resulting imprecision in the estimate of variance attributable to neighbourhood-level characteristics (15·3%, 95% CI 2·9–55·0, p=0·09). Nevertheless, neighbourhood-level SERFs were significantly associated with the incidence of other non-affective psychoses in our multivariate model. As for schizophrenia, increased voter turnout was associated with a lower incidence of other non-affective psychoses (IRR 0·93, 95% CI 0·88–0·98), independent of age, sex, ethnicity, population density (IRR 0·98, 95% CI 0·97–0·99) and ethnic density (IRR 1·04, 95% CI 1·01–1·08). Ethnic fragmentation (LRT p=0·70) and deprivation (LRT p=0·99) were not significantly associated with other non-affective psychoses and were removed from the final multivariate model. There was no evidence that ethnic density interacted with individual-level ethnicity, using either the model fitted with our seven-category (LRT p=0·43) or the dichotomous ethnicity variable (LRT p=0·43). As for schizophrenia, the elevated incidence of other non-affective psychoses in BME groups was not attenuated by inclusion of neighbourhood-level characteristics (multivariate model, Table 5). After fitting our final model, there was no evidence of significant residual variance at the neighbourhood level.

Table 5. Modelling of individual- and neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors for other non-affective psychoses

SERF, Socio-environmental risk factors; IRR, incidence rate ratio; LRT, likelihood ratio test; df, degrees of freedom; CI, confidence interval.

a Individual- and neighbourhood-level variables included in final model.

b IRR reports change in risk of schizophrenia associated with a 1% increase in the neighbourhood-level variable.

c IRR reports change in risk of schizophrenia associated with 1 standard deviation increase in socio-economic deprivation at the neighbourhood-level variable.

DISCUSSION

Principal findings

The incidence of schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses varied at both the individual and neighbourhood levels within Southeast London. Approximately 23% and 15% of variance of each disorder respectively could be attributed to neighbourhood-level risk factors after adjustment for age, sex and ethnicity.

Social capital, as measured by voter turnout, was a significant predictor of both outcomes, independent of age, sex, ethnicity and socio-economic deprivation. The incidence of non-affective psychoses was higher in wards with lower voter turnout at local elections. We also observed a significant association between ethnic fragmentation and schizophrenia, suggesting that the incidence of schizophrenia was lower in wards where White British and BME individuals lived in more cohesive, less fragmented groups. We found no evidence of person–environment interaction between individual-level ethnic status and neighbourhood proportion of BME groups when we considered a broad ethnicity variable. However, when we used a dichotomous variable, we observed some evidence of an interaction, such that BME individuals were at greatest risk of schizophrenia in wards where they constituted a smaller proportion of the total population. These findings suggest that ethnic density is likely to interact with individual-level ethnicity in a more complex way than previously considered with regard to psychoses. Our results raise the possibility that contextual, neighbourhood-level factors operate somewhere along the causal pathway for both schizophrenia and other non-affective psychoses.

Methodological considerations

Denominator population and individual-level risk factors

We have previously discussed potential limitations of using the 2001 census to estimate our denominator population (Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Murray and Jones2006). We acknowledge that the true, dynamic, population at risk over the survey period may have varied slightly, but we do not believe we have introduced systematic bias.

We may not have adequately controlled for all individual-level risk, which could have led to an overestimation of the effect of the neighbourhood. Specifically, we did not have data on family history of psychosis, the most important individual-level risk factor for disorder (Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Pedersen, Westergaard, Wohlfahrt, Ewald, Mors, Andersen and Melbye1999). This could have explained a degree of clustering of risk in some neighbourhoods, although the relationship between urbanicity and schizophrenia appears to persist independently of family history (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, David, Andreasson and Allebeck1992; Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Pedersen, Westergaard, Wohlfahrt, Ewald, Mors, Andersen and Melbye1999). To minimize this problem in our study, we used incidence rather than prevalence data over a short study period. Furthermore, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to exclude any subsequent cases with the same postcode (as a potential marker for genetic clustering of cases within the same family). This did not change our results (data available on request).

We recognize that individual-level social class may have accounted for some of the variation we observed at the neighbourhood level (Silver et al. Reference Silver, Mulvey and Swanson2002). Unfortunately, we could not obtain denominator data stratified by a fourth variable (social class) at the ward level because of confidentiality issues in the 2001 census. We included its neighbourhood-level equivalent, socio-economic deprivation, given it is unclear at which level social class may operate (Diez-Roux et al. Reference Diez-Roux, Nieto, Muntaner, Tyroler, Comstock, Shahar, Cooper, Watson and Szklo1997). Furthermore, social class may only present an indicator of risk for other underlying SERFs (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Bebbington, Foerster, Lewis, Murray, Russell, Sham, Toone and Wilkins1993; Cooper, Reference Cooper2005). Our data are consistent with that view.

Our findings could have been explained by downward social drift of psychotic individuals into poorer, less cohesive neighbourhoods. However, social causation appears to present a more parsimonious explanation of the ‘urbanicity’ effect than social drift (van Os, Reference van Os2004). We measured place of residence at onset of psychosis rather than at birth or during upbringing (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, David, Andreasson and Allebeck1992; Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Pedersen, Westergaard, Wohlfahrt, Ewald, Mors, Andersen and Melbye1999). It is plausible, however, that neighbourhood setting close to onset is relevant to the aetiology of psychoses. If prodromal or At-Risk Mental State (ARMS) individuals drift downwards (socially and geographically), neighbourhood risk factors (close to onset) may be sufficient to induce (or fail to ‘buffer’ against) psychotic disorders. Thus, downward social drift would operate synergistically with causal effects at the neighbourhood level. We would therefore expect to observe significant associations between the incidence of psychoses and neighbourhood-level effects close to the onset of disorder. Longitudinal studies are clearly required to elucidate the crucial exposure period(s) to ‘urbanicity’.

Neighbourhood-level SERFs

We obtained neighbourhood-level SERFs from a variety of routinely collected national datasets. Ethnic density and fragmentation were calculated from the 2001 census. Our ethnic density variable differed slightly from a previously used measure (Boydell et al. Reference Boydell, van Os, McKenzie, Allardyce, Goel, McCreadie and Murray2001) because we included the non-British white population in the BME group. We took this decision because a recent study has demonstrated that non-British white migrants may be at increased risk of schizophrenia compared to their British-born counterparts (Fearon et al. Reference Fearon, Kirkbride, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Lloyd, Hutchinson, Tarrant, Lun, Holloway, Mallett, Harrison, Leff, Jones and Murray2006).

The ID is a validated measure of segregation (Peach, Reference Peach, Jackson and Smith1981, Reference Peach1996), to our knowledge applied here to psychiatric disorders for the first time. As absolute levels of segregation in Southeast London were low, and because the term segregation has negative connotations, it was helpful to consider this variable as a measure of residential fragmentation within the White British and BME groups in the same neighbourhood. Ethnic fragmentation and density appear to have distinct effects, providing some justification for inclusion of the ID. The ID measures the degree of residential segregation in an area, rather than the spatial structure of this patterning. A measure of ethnic fragmentation that takes into account the spatial clustering of residential patterns may provide further insights into the association between ethnic fragmentation and schizophrenia.

We recognize that voter turnout provides an imperfect measure of social capital. It may be influenced by a range of sociopolitical, sociocultural and sociodemographic factors (Smith & Dorling, Reference Smith and Dorling1996), not restricted to local-level issues. We attempted to minimize this by using data from local elections in an election year where national elections were not being held concurrently. In addition, voter turnout may have measured some factors that should have been specified at the individual level, such as social class or education. However, although voter turnout was our primary measure of social capital, ethnic fragmentation, which may also present a marker for social capital, was independently associated with the incidence of schizophrenia. Voter turnout is a reliable, routinely collected measure, but more detailed measures of social capital, such as the scales proposed by Sampson et al. (Reference Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls1997) or McCulloch (Reference McCulloch2001), may provide further aetiological clues to this potential association.

Voter turnout data were obtained from local elections in 2002 for our study area. This was the closest election to our study period based on the new census geography used in this study. We acknowledge that some of our neighbourhood SERF variables have been measured subsequent to our case ascertainment period, but we have attempted to minimize any error by using the temporally closest data available.

We consider that the IMD provided a suitable measure of deprivation. It incorporates a number of domains, meaning that it is unlikely that our findings could be explained by residual confounding, However, separate consideration of these domains may further our aetiological understanding of the role of socio-economic deprivation. Although released in 2004, the IMD used data collected close to, or during, the AESOP study period (ODPM, 2004). IMD was not significant in our multivariate model for other non-affective psychoses, but this is most likely to be due to its high positive correlation with ethnic density.

We chose to analyse neighbourhoods at the ward level. Wards are, however, unlikely to be conterminous with neighbourhoods as perceived by the people who live in them. If we could measure more ‘ecological’ neighbourhoods we would be better placed to understand the association between neighbourhood-level SERFs and psychoses. Here, however, our primary concern was to obtain reliable neighbourhood-level measures from routine sources. Inspection of ward boundaries suggested that they were not entirely arbitrary; several followed major roads, railways or parks, which may have some ecological validity.

Meaning of findings

The above caveats notwithstanding, it remains important to discuss the potential causal pathways that may underlie the associations we have observed here, if such associations were genuinely causal.

Stress is associated with dopamine dysregulation (Howes et al. Reference Howes, McDonald, Cannon, Arseneault, Boydell and Murray2004), which may be important in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. If city living increases exposure to social stressors, this may be a potential mechanism through which residency in urban areas induces psychotic symptoms, particularly in genetically vulnerable individuals or where there is no ‘buffering’ by external factors. It is plausible that social stress is mediated – or buffered – by social capital, such that in neighbourhoods with high social capital, individuals are able to dissipate social stress through access to actual or perceived support networks. By contrast, less cohesive neighbourhoods may foster paranoid attributional styles that, in some susceptible individuals, may eventually manifest as psychotic symptoms (Garety et al. Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; van Os et al. Reference van Os, Hanssen, Bijl and Vollebergh2001). Having said this, we acknowledge the potential for reverse causality; a paranoid attributional style could lead to the formation of less cohesive neighbourhoods.

We have observed differential findings regarding the ethnic density hypothesis. When we operationalized the hypothesis as previously tested (interaction between dichotomous ethnicity variable and proportion of BME groups in a neighbourhood), we found weak evidence that the risk of schizophrenia for BME individuals was greatest when they constituted a smaller proportion of the overall neighbourhood population. This finding supports two recent studies that have observed similar interactions (Boydell et al. Reference Boydell, van Os, McKenzie, Allardyce, Goel, McCreadie and Murray2001; Veling et al. Reference Veling, Selten, Van Hoeken, Hoek and Mackenbach2006), lending weight to the ‘social capital as mediator’ hypothesis outlined earlier. It appears that the risk of schizophrenia for BME individuals is greatest when they are more isolated from others who may share similar beliefs, cultures, values, attitudes or experiences of city life. Thus, there may be less opportunity for these individuals to mediate social stress by accessing social capital. This finding complements the independent association we observed between risk of schizophrenia and increasing ethnic fragmentation. It is possible that White British and BME groups that have formed more cohesive groups confer protection from psychotic illness by gaining increased access to social support.

When, however, we operationalized the ethnic density hypothesis using our seven-category ethnicity variable, we did not observe evidence of a significant interaction with ethnic density, for either outcome. We suggest three possible explanations for this absence of association. First, it may be genuine. Second, our study is likely to be underpowered to test cross-level interactions, particularly using a broad ethnicity variable. Third, our ethnic density variable may not be valid for different ethnic groups. As investigated here, ethnic density measured the proportion of White British residents in each ward. Conceivably, this may only be a relevant risk factor for psychoses in the White British group. The relevant ‘ethnic density’ variable for the Black Caribbean group may be the density of Black Caribbean residents in a neighbourhood, compared with all other groups. The relevant ‘ethnic density’ variable for non-British white migrants may be the density of non-British white, or more specifically Polish, Turkish, Italian or Portuguese migrants (et cetera) at the neighbourhood level. To investigate these pertinent issues, ethnic density needs to be tested for specific ethnic groups in larger samples.

Comparison with previous studies

Multilevel studies investigating the respective roles of individual- and neighbourhood-level risk factors for schizophrenia remain sparse. However, one previous study, undertaken in Maastricht, reported that 12% of the variation in the incidence of schizophrenia could be attributed to neighbourhood-level factors (van Os et al. Reference van Os, Driessen, Gunther and Delespaul2000). This figure was half that observed in our study, but both findings may be valid given the level of urbanization in Southeast London compared with Maastricht. Of note, neighbourhood variation in Maastricht does not appear to be associated with social capital (Drukker et al. Reference Drukker, Krabbendam, Driessen and van Os2006). However, Silver et al. (Reference Silver, Mulvey and Swanson2002) found evidence to suggest the level of residential mobility was associated with the risk of schizophrenia, and a complementary finding has been observed by Allardyce et al. (Reference Allardyce, Gilmour, Atkinson, Rapson, Bishop and McCreadie2005) in Scotland with respect to social fragmentation. Our findings on voter turnout and ethnic fragmentation support the hypothesis that social capital mediates the relationship between exposure to stress and later psychoses (Myin-Germeys et al. Reference Myin-Germeys, van Os, Schwartz, Stone and Delespaul2001; McKenzie et al. Reference McKenzie, Whitley and Weich2002; Krabbendam & van Os, Reference Krabbendam and van Os2005).

It appears that the relationship between schizophrenia and social class is complicated, and factors allied to social class, such as social capital, may be more aetiologically relevant. This idea is not new; Faris & Dunham (Reference Faris and Dunham1939) hypothesized that it was the most socially disorganized neighbourhoods, not the most socio-economically deprived neighbourhoods per se, that had the highest incidence of schizophrenia. Our findings support this hypothesis.

CONCLUSIONS

Variation in the incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in Southeast London cannot solely be explained by individual-level differences in age, sex and ethnicity. Neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors appear to explain approximately 23% and 15% respectively of this variation. Voter turnout, ethnic density, ethnic fragmentation and socio-economic deprivation were significant predictors of risk at the neighbourhood level. Social capital is one mechanism that potentially binds these associations together, perhaps acting to mediate social stress. These findings support and extend Faris and Dunham's hypothesis that the highest rates of schizophrenia occurred in the most disorganized tracts of the city, not necessarily the poorest.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We acknowledge the contributions of the entire AESOP study team, listed online at www.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/aesop. This study was funded by grants from the UK Medical Research Council and the Stanley Medical Research Institute. Neither body was involved in the design and conduct of the study; the collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; or the preparation, review or approval of the manuscript. We thank the staff in the mental health services who helped in the case ascertainment and we thank the research subjects for their cooperation. We gratefully acknowledge advice from the late R. E. Kendell regarding the design of the study.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

Robin M. Murray is an editor on the board of Psychological Medicine.

References

REFERENCES

Allardyce, J., Gilmour, H., Atkinson, J., Rapson, T., Bishop, J. & McCreadie, R. G. (2005). Social fragmentation, deprivation and urbanicity: relation to first-admission rates for psychoses. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 401406.Google Scholar
Boydell, J., van Os, J., McKenzie, K., Allardyce, J., Goel, R., McCreadie, R. G. & Murray, R. M. (2001). Incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities in London: ecological study into interactions with environment. British Medical Journal 323, 13361338.Google Scholar
Cochrane, R. & Bal, S. S. (1988). Ethnic density is unrelated to incidence of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 153, 363366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, B. (2005). Immigration and schizophrenia: the social causation hypothesis revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry 186, 361363.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. E., Goodhead, D., Craig, T., Harris, M., Howat, J. & Korer, J. (1987). The incidence of schizophrenia in Nottingham. British Journal of Psychiatry 151, 619626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croudace, T. J., Kayne, R., Jones, P. B. & Harrison, G. L. (2000). Non-linear relationship between an index of social deprivation, psychiatric admission prevalence and the incidence of psychosis. Psychological Medicine 30, 177185.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (2000). Schizophrenia as the price that homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews 31, 118129.Google Scholar
De Silva, M. J., McKenzie, K., Harpham, T. & Huttly, S. R. (2005). Social capital and mental illness: a systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diez-Roux, A. V., Nieto, F. J., Muntaner, C., Tyroler, H. A., Comstock, G. W., Shahar, E., Cooper, L. S., Watson, R. L. & Szklo, M. (1997). Neighborhood environments and coronary heart disease: a multilevel analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology 146, 4863.Google Scholar
Drukker, M., Krabbendam, L., Driessen, G. & van Os, J. (2006). Social disadvantage and schizophrenia: a combined neighbourhood- and individual-level analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 41, 595604.Google Scholar
Faris, R. E. L. & Dunham, H. W. (1939). Mental Disorders in Urban Areas. University of Chicago Press: Chicago.Google Scholar
Fearon, P., Kirkbride, J. B., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Morgan, K., Lloyd, T., Hutchinson, G., Tarrant, J., Lun, Alan Fung W., Holloway, J., Mallett, R., Harrison, G., Leff, J., Jones, P. B. & Murray, R. M. (2006). Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic minority groups: results from the MRC AESOP study. Psychological Medicine 36, 15411550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D. & Bebbington, P. E. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine 31, 189195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B. & Redlich, F. C. (1958). Social Class and Mental Illness. Wiley: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howes, O. D., McDonald, C., Cannon, M., Arseneault, L., Boydell, J. & Murray, R. M. (2004). Pathways to schizophrenia: the impact of environmental factors. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 7 (Suppl. 1), S7S13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablensky, A., Sartorius, N., Ernberg, G., Anker, M., Korten, A., Cooper, J. E., Day, R. & Bertelsen, A. (1992). Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures. A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychological Medicine. Monograph Supplement 20, 197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P. B., Bebbington, P., Foerster, A., Lewis, S. W., Murray, R. M., Russell, A., Sham, P. C., Toone, B. K. & Wilkins, S. (1993). Premorbid social underachievement in schizophrenia. Results from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 6571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkbride, J. B., Fearon, P., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Morgan, K., Murray, R. M. & Jones, P. B. (in press). Neighbourhood variation in the incidence of psychotic disorders in Southeast London. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.Google Scholar
Kirkbride, J. B., Fearon, P., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Morgan, K., Tarrant, J., Lloyd, T., Holloway, J., Hutchinson, G., Leff, J. P., Mallett, R. M., Harrison, G. L., Murray, R. M. & Jones, P. B. (2006). Heterogeneity in incidence rates of schizophrenia and other psychotic syndromes: findings from the 3-center ÆSOP Study. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 250258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krabbendam, L. & van Os, J. (2005). Schizophrenia and urbanicity: a major environmental influence – conditional on genetic risk. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31, 795799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, G., David, A., Andreasson, S. & Allebeck, P. (1992). Schizophrenia and city life. Lancet 340, 137140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallett, M. R. (1997). MRC Sociodemographic Schedule. Institute of Psychiatry: London.Google Scholar
McCulloch, A. (2001). Social environments and health: cross-sectional national survey. British Medical Journal 323, 208209.Google Scholar
McKenzie, K., Whitley, R. & Weich, S. (2002). Social capital and mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry 181, 280283.Google Scholar
Mortensen, P. B., Pedersen, C. B., Westergaard, T., Wohlfahrt, J., Ewald, H., Mors, O., Andersen, P. K. & Melbye, M. (1999). Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine 340, 603608.Google Scholar
Myin-Germeys, I., van Os, J., Schwartz, J. E., Stone, A. A. & Delespaul, P. A. (2001). Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 11371144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ODPM (2004). The English Indices of Deprivation 2004. Office of the Deputy Prime Minister: Wetherby (www.communities.gov.uk/index.asp?id=1128440) Accessed 19 October 2006.Google Scholar
Peach, C. (1981). Conflicting interpretations of segregation. In Social Interaction and Ethnic Segregation, vol. 12 (ed. Jackson, P. and Smith, S.), pp. 1933. Institute of British Geographers (IBG) Special Publication. Academic Press: London.Google Scholar
Peach, C. (1996). Does Britain have ghettos? Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers 21, 216235.Google Scholar
Pedersen, C. B. & Mortensen, P. B. (2001). Evidence of a dose–response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 10391046.Google Scholar
Pilgrim, D. & Roger, A. (2005). Social psychiatry and sociology. Journal of Mental Health 14, 317320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, R. (1993). Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy. Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Rawlings, C. & Thrasher, M. (2002). The Local Elections Handbook. Local Government Chronicle Elections Centre: Plymouth.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W. & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277, 918924.Google Scholar
Silver, E., Mulvey, E. P. & Swanson, J. W. (2002). Neighborhood structural characteristics and mental disorder: Faris and Dunham revisited. Social Science and Medicine 55, 14571470.Google Scholar
Smith, G. D. & Dorling, D. (1996). ‘I'm all right, John’: voting patterns and mortality in England and Wales, 1981–92. British Medical Journal 313, 15731577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp (2005). STATA Statistical Software. Stata Corporation: College Station, TX.Google Scholar
Takei, N., O'Callaghan, E. & Sham, P. (1992). Winter birth excess in schizophrenia: its relationship to place of birth. Schizophrenia Research 6, 102108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Os, J. (2004). Does the urban environment cause psychosis? British Journal of Psychiatry 184, 287288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J., Driessen, G., Gunther, N. & Delespaul, P. (2000). Neighbourhood variation in incidence of schizophrenia. Evidence for person–environment interaction. British Journal of Psychiatry 176, 243248.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V. & Vollebergh, W. (2001). Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: an urban–rural comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 663668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W. A., Selten, J. P., Van Hoeken, D., Hoek, H. W. & Mackenbach, J. P. (2006). Ethnic density and incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities in the Netherlands. Schizophrenia Research 81, 174174.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample and denominator population

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlation matrix for neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors (SERFs)

Figure 2

Table 3. Modelling of individual- and neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors for schizophrenia

Figure 3

Table 4. Interaction between individual-level BME status (dichotomous) and ethnic density for schizophrenia

Figure 4

Table 5. Modelling of individual- and neighbourhood-level socio-environmental risk factors for other non-affective psychoses