Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:24:11.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Linguistic alignment and theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia patients' dialogic interactions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 September 2019

Karen Dwyer*
Affiliation:
Department of English Language and Literature, University College London, London, UK Department of Psychology, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK
Anthony S. David
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK Institute of Mental Health, University College London, London, UK
Rosaleen McCarthy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Wessex Neurological Centre, Southampton General Hospital, Southampton University Hospital Trust, Tremona Road, Southampton, UK
Peter McKenna
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Research Foundation, Germanes Hospitalàries, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Madrid , Spain
Emmanuelle Peters
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, Bethlem Royal Hospital, Monks Orchard Road, Beckenham, Kent, BR3 3BX, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Karen Dwyer, E-mail: k.dwyer@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Impairments of contextual processing and theory of mind (ToM) have both been offered as accounts of the deviant language characterising formal thought disorder (FTD) in schizophrenia. This study investigated these processes in patients' dialogue. We predicted that FTD patients would show a decrement in linguistic alignment, associated with impaired ToM in dialogue.

Methods

Speech samples were elicited via participation in an interactive computer-based task and a semi-structured interview to assess contextual processing abilities and ToM skills in dialogue, respectively, and from an interactive card-sorting task to measure syntactic alignment. Degree of alignment in dialogue and the syntactic task, and evidence of ToM in (i) dialogue and (ii) a traditional ToM task were compared across schizophrenia patients with FTD (n = 21), non-FTD patients (n = 22) and healthy controls (n = 21).

Results

FTD patients showed less alignment than the other two groups in dialogue, and than healthy controls on the syntactic task. FTD patients showed poorer performance on the ToM task than the other two groups, but only compared to the healthy controls in dialogue. The FTD group's degree of alignment in dialogue was correlated with ToM performance in dialogue but not with the traditional ToM task or with syntactic alignment.

Conclusions

In dialogue, FTD patients demonstrate an impairment in employing available contextual information to facilitate their own subsequent production, which is associated with a ToM deficit. These findings indicate that a contextual processing deficit impacts on exploiting representations via the production system impoverishing the ability to make predictions about upcoming utterances in dialogue.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Approximately 16% of patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia exhibit formal thought disorder (FTD) (Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1979), a symptom representing problems of language and communication (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). FTD is, however, a notoriously heterogeneous symptom, where large variations in the efficiency of communicative behaviours through speech are observed (Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1982).

FTD as a linguistic impairment

One account of FTD proposes that it is the result of hyperactivity in the semantic network (Manschreck et al., Reference Manschreck, Maher, Milavetz, Ames, Weisstein and Schneyer1988; Spitzer et al., Reference Spitzer, Braun, Hermle and Maier1993). Automatic semantic priming appears to be stronger in FTD, specifically in the earlier or automatic stages of semantic activation (see Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Oh, Laws and McKenna2008 for a meta-analysis). Alternatively, FTD may be a result of abnormalities in the building up and use of context (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, Reference Cohen and Servan-Schreiber1992). Abnormalities in sensitivity to linguistic context have been documented on both behavioural measures (Truscott, Reference Truscott1970; Kuperberg et al., Reference Kuperberg, McGuire and David1998, Reference Kuperberg, McGuire and David2000, Reference Kuperberg, Krecher, Goff, McGuire and David2006a; Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, David, McCarthy, McKenna and Peters2014a, Reference Dwyer, Peters, McKenna, David and McCarthy2014b) and in event related potentials (ERP) studies (see Wang et al., Reference Wang, Cheung, Gong and Chan2011 for a meta-analysis). People with schizophrenia do not demonstrate the reduction in the amplitude of the N400 that is reliably observed in healthy controls when a word is preceded by a semantically supportive context (see Kutas and Federmeier, Reference Kutas and Federmeier2011, for a review).

However, these may not be incompatible accounts of FTD. Evidence of a processing bias for semantic associations driving impairments in contextual processing in schizophrenia (both non-FTD and FTD patients) has been found in studies investigating the resolution of lexical ambiguity in congruous, incongruous or biasing contexts (Titone et al., Reference Titone, Levy and Holzman2000; Sitnikova et al., Reference Sitnikova, Salisbury, Kuperberg and Holcomb2002; see Kuperberg et al., Reference Kuperberg, Kreher, Sitnikova, Caplan and Holcomb2007 for a review). These findings suggest an impairment in suppressing contextually inappropriate meanings and a dependence on lexical semantic associations together compromising the building of the whole sentence context.

An integrated theory of language production and comprehension

The relationship between contextual processing impairments during comprehension and the deviant language production seen in FTD remains unclear. Research into FTD has focussed primarily on these as unrelated independent processes. However, there is considerable evidence of the inter-related nature of these acts, including their recruitment of strongly overlapping neural circuits (Scott and Johnsrude, Reference Scott and Johnsrude2003; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Saygin, Sereno and Iacoboni2004), engagement of the production system during speech perception (e.g. Scott et al., Reference Scott, McGettigan and Eisner2009; Pulvermüller and Fadiga, Reference Pulvermüller and Fadiga2010; Silbert et al., Reference Silbert, Honey, Simony, Poeppel and Hasson2014), and evidence that prediction during comprehension engages production processes (e.g. Federmeier, Reference Federmeier2007; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Branzi and Bar2018).

Pickering and Garrod (Reference Pickering and Garrod2013) invoke a theory of forward modelling in action and action perception (e.g. Wolpert, Reference Wolpert1997; Davidson and Wolpert, Reference Davidson and Wolpert2005) to explain the relatedness of these processes. In this prediction-based account, the listener uses the same mechanisms used in production to make predictions about the speakers' upcoming utterances. Firstly, the listener exploits contextual information to determine the speaker's intention and then to predict what she would say. The listener imitates the speaker's utterance as it unfolds and then uses the inverse model and context to derive the production command that the listener would use if she were to produce the speaker's utterance. These representations are then exploited to make predictions about upcoming utterances that the speaker will make, and so on. In constructing a representation corresponding to the speaker's utterance, the listener aligns her linguistic representations with the speaker along with a representation corresponding to what she predicts the speaker will say next. Such alignment increases the likelihood that those representations will be used in the listeners' subsequent utterances since it is more efficient to re-employ previously activated elements. If the speaker and the listener are successful, they will produce similar predictions about the speaker's upcoming utterance, leading to well-coordinated communication. Thus, alignment is facilitated by emulation and prediction during comprehension using the production system (Pickering and Garrod, Reference Pickering and Garrod2013). This mirroring is widely observed in dialogue at various levels of structure (see Pickering and Garrod, Reference Pickering and Garrod2013 for an overview).

An imbalance in the mechanisms involved in the building of the whole sentence context would be expected to result in poverty of alignment in patients' output. We carried out a study of alignment in FTD, non-FTD patients' and healthy controls' contributions in dialogue. We hypothesized that the FTD group would produce poor alignment, demonstrating failure to utilise the available linguistic context and update their interpretation of the discussion via the production system.

Theory of mind in schizophrenia

Theory of mind (ToM) is the capacity to attribute and understand others' mental states. Impairment of ToM is a consistent finding within schizophrenia research (see Sprong et al., Reference Sprong, Schothorst, Vos, Hox and van Engeland2007 for a meta-analysis and Brüne, Reference Brüne2005; Harrington et al., Reference Harrington, Siegert and McClure2005 for systematic reviews) at both the level of a ‘state’ and ‘trait’ (see Bora et al., Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis2009; Bora and Pantelis, Reference Bora and Pantelis2013 for meta-analyses). Psychotic speech may reflect a specific problem of ToM (Frith, Reference Frith1992; Hardy-Bayle, Reference Hardy-Baylé1994). Impoverished contextual processing could also impair the ability to collate the contextual information necessary to build up an interpretation of the interlocutor's mental state (Schenkel et al., Reference Schenkel, Spaulding and Silverstein2005).

The only study of alignment in schizophrenia to date found evidence of preserved alignment in FTD in the presence of impaired ToM (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Corcoran and Drake2008). These authors proposed that alignment facilitates patients' successful conversation, obviating intensive modelling of the interlocutor's mental state. Yet, studies analysing ToM in dialogue have demonstrated preserved ToM abilities in schizophrenia (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Leudar and Antaki2004; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Corcoran and Drake2009). The inconsistency in findings across ToM studies could be attributed to a difference between explicit reasoning skills and implicit skills required in dialogue (Frith, Reference Frith2004). Explicit reasoning skills as measured by traditional ToM tasks require processing complex sentential structure (e.g. Mary thinks that Billy thinks that…) (De Villars, Reference de Villiers, Astington and Baird2005). Problematic is that explicit reasoning places demands upon a range of executive functions known to be impaired in schizophrenia, e.g. working memory, sustained attention and sequencing (e.g. Docherty and Gordinier, Reference Docherty and Gordinier1999; Docherty, Reference Docherty2005; Lee and Park, Reference Lee and Park2005), which have been found to be strongly associated with FTD (Kerns and Berenbaum, Reference Kerns and Berenbaum2002).

We compared patients' ToM performance on a traditional ToM task and in natural dialogue. We hypothesised that FTD patients would show a relative decrement in ToM in natural dialogue as a result of contextual processing problems. We further hypothesised this would result in a correlation between poverty of alignment and ToM performance.

Methods

Participants

Forty-three patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) were recruited from inpatient wards in London and Cambridge. All clinical participants were chronic patients who were stable on typical and atypical antipsychotic medication. Twenty-one healthy controls were recruited from a local Job Centre, and from non-academic posts at University College London. Inclusion criteria for all participants were: age between 18 and 65 years, no self-reported history of brain injury, substance abuse, or neurological illness, and native speaker level of English. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the relevant clinical research ethical committees (Ref: 06/Q0706/86).

The Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1984) was administered to clinical participants. Recordings from SAPS interviews were used to derive a positive FTD score with the version of Andreasen's Thought Language and Communication (TLC) scale in the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH; Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, Flaum and Arndt1992). Description of tests and the procedure used to calculate FTD scores are in the online supplementary material 1. The two patient groups did not differ on global scores for delusions or hallucinations (see Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the three groups [means; standard deviations (s.d.s) in parentheses]

LSD: least significant difference test; a: FTD patients v. non-FTD patients; b: FTD patients v. healthy controls; c: non-FTD patients v. healthy controls; (*p < 0.05).

The three groups differed in terms of current and pre-morbid IQ, and working memory function. The two patient groups only differed on premorbid IQ, and all other differences were between the patients and healthy controls. Description of tests used to calculate the various variables are in the online supplementary material 1. Participants' demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Materials and procedures

Alignment in dialogue (the maze task)

Garrod and Anderson's (Reference Garrod and Anderson1987) maze game was reproduced with permission [see Garrod and Anderson (Reference Garrod and Anderson1987) for a full description of the task]. An illustration of the maze can be found in the online supplementary material 2. In this study, the experimenter (K.D.) and participant were each seated in front of a laptop displaying a maze configuration. Each player had different starting points and different destinations. The aim of the task was for each player to get to their destination by taking turns to move through the maze one box at a time. Each maze had locked gates, which were controlled by the other player. This required cooperation between the two players. Previous studies have shown that players achieve this by aligning in their descriptions of the mazes (Garrod and Anderson, Reference Garrod and Anderson1987; Garrod and Clark, Reference Garrod and Clark1993; Garrod and Doherty, Reference Garrod and Doherty1994).

Each participant played 3 × 10 min games against the experimenter. Unbeknownst to the participants, the experimenter used a confederate script technique to ensure consistent descriptions of the maze were provided to participants across the three groups. The experimenter provided location descriptions if requested by the participant, or when cooperating with the participant to negotiate unlocking gates. The experimenter requested the participant's location if the participant requested her cooperation, or if the experimenter required assistance because she was otherwise unable to move. The dialogue between the two players was recorded to allow for scoring of alignment.

Syntactic alignment

This syntactic priming task was adapted from Branigan et al. (Reference Branigan, Pickering and Cleland2000) with permission. The task comprised 12 experimental items cards and 36 filler cards. Each card depicted a scene involving a finite set of recurring characters e.g. doctor, cowboy, as agents performing an action towards either an inanimate object or another character (see online supplementary material 3). The naïve participant and the confederate each had two sets of cards; a description set, and a duplicate of their partner's description set. Both the experimenter and the participant took it in turns to describe their pictures, while the other matched the picture that was being described from a further set of cards arranged on the table in an 8 × 6 grid formation. Participants were asked to provide a description of their illustrations using the verb indicated on the bottom of the card.

Each experimental item consisted of a pair of picture cards (the card prime described by the experimenter and the participant's experimental card). Each of the experimental item cards was a ditransitive verb (e.g. The cowboy handing the ballerina a cake) and involved an agent, patient and beneficiary. These cards depicted the ditransitive verbsFootnote 1Footnote give, hand, offer, sell, show and throw a total of twice. These pictures could be described according to two possible word order rules in English depending on the order of the complements following the verb, e.g. ‘the soldier gave [the ballerinaNP] [a gunNP] or ‘the soldier gave [a gunNP] [to the ballerinaPP]. The filler cards depicted 18 transitive verbs a total of twice each. The target verb was printed on the bottom of each card.

The participants were unaware that the confederate had a script of descriptions for the prime cards. The confederate's prime preceded the participant's prime card for each of the 12 experimental cards. There were two different orders of cards, according to the structure of the complements described by the experimenter (i.e. ‘[the ballerinaNP] [a gunNP] or ‘[a gunNP] [to the ballerinaPP]). These were counterbalanced across participants. While the verbs on the prime cards and the target cards differed, they were always ditransitive verbs. This is because we were interested in the syntactic structure that the participants' would use in their descriptions of target items. We calculated the number of times the participants' description of the ditransitive target consisted of the same syntactic structure (i.e. either NP,NP or NP,PP after the verb) as that used by the experimenter in her immediate preceding turn describing the ditransitive prime.

Theory of mind in dialogue

Participants were interviewed after each of the three maze games to elicit a speech sample of their reflections of their own and the experimenter's thinking during participation in the task. Interviews consisted of questions about their views on both players' performance during the maze game, e.g. ‘Did we make any mistakes?’,Do you think I understood what you were trying to do to?’ To seek participants' full justification for their answers, responses were probed further with scripted WH-questions (who, what, where, why, which) (e.g. ‘What mistakes did we make?’, ‘What was I trying to do?’). All interviews were recorded and later transcribed verbatim for analysis (see online supplementary material 4 for examples illustrating how these were scored).

Theory of mind stories

A set of six stories (Frith and Corcoran, Reference Frith and Corcoran1996) was used to assess ToM ability involving explicit reasoning skills assessed in a traditional ToM task. These stories contained first- and second-order deception questions and the other three contained first- and second-order false belief questions. Stories were read aloud to participants, who were also shown cartoons simultaneously to facilitate comprehension. After each story, participants were asked one memory/reality question (concerning an event in the story) and one question that depended on the ability to infer the mental state of one of the characters. The memory control question is not a measure of ToM, but serves as a control for understanding and memory of the story, from which the participant makes a ToM judgment. Each question type occurred three times across the six stories, making a total of 12 questions across the four types.

Scoring

Alignment in dialogue (maze task)

A full description of the system of measuring alignment is reported elsewhere (Garrod and Doherty, Reference Garrod and Doherty1994) plus a discussion in the online supplementary material 5. In brief, there was a total of six description types: Path, matrix, line, figural, comment, goal, and non-descriptions. Alignment was scored as the degree that the participants' descriptions were influenced by the experimenters' descriptions in the previous exchange N−1 (see Garrod and Clark, Reference Garrod and Clark1993). We calculated the number of participants' descriptions in each dialogue that matched that of the experimenter's preceding description in the same game. This number was then divided by the total number of transitions in the game where the experimenter had provided a preceding description to create an alignment score as a percentage.

Due to the relatively low number of comments, non-description and goal type descriptions, scores for these were collapsed into one category labelled ‘other’. A Kruskal–Wallis test showed no significant difference in the mean number of each description type across the three mazes provided by the experimenter for each group; path [H(2) = 4.29, p = 0.12], line [H(2) = 0.87, p = 0.65], figure [H(2) = 3.86, p = 0.15], matrix [H(2) = 1.04, p = 0.6] and other [H(2) = 0.9, p = 0.64]. A one-way ANOVA revealed there was no significance difference between the groups' mean total recording times (F (2,63) = 3.55, p = 0.13).

Theory of mind in dialogue

Transcripts were analysed for evidence of ToM through patients' demonstration of representation of their own or others mental states either spontaneously or in response to a question (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Leudar and Antaki2004). Interviews were coded by two raters, one blind to group membership. Responses from participants that simply provided yes/no answers were not counted as evidence of ToM unless justification was provided. The number of references to own and other's beliefs used by participants was measured according to lexical indices of ToM e.g. ‘think’, ‘believe’, ‘want’, ‘try’, ‘aim’ and ‘realise’ (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Corcoran and Drake2009).

Inter-rater reliability between the two scorers on 15 interview scripts showed almost perfect agreement for reference to own beliefs (intra-class correlations (ICC): 0.99) and for reference to others' beliefs (ICC = 0.99).

Analyses

Due to differences between the two patient groups on the NART (pre-morbid IQ), scores for this measure were entered as covariates in all analyses, as a sensitive and conservative strategy. Because a working memory deficit is well established to be a core feature of schizophrenia (see Lee and Park, Reference Lee and Park2005 for a meta-analysis), and is found to be strongly correlated with both language comprehension (Bagner et al., Reference Bagner, Melinder and Barch2003) and language production deficits (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Barch, Carter and Servan-Schreiber1999; Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Rakfeldt, Hawkins, Hoffman, Quinlan and Sledge1996), scaled scores for Letter Number Sequencing span (working memory), arguably the strongest measure of working memory, were also entered on all analyses.

To test for differences on degree of alignment in the maze tasks, a 3 (group: FTD v. non-FTD v. healthy control) × 3 (maze: three levels) ANCOVA was carried out. The dependent variable (DV) was the total score on alignment for each of the three mazes. In the syntactic alignment task, only the first responses that contained target verbs were included. Data were entered into a 3 (group: FTD, non-FTD, HC) × 2 (word order: direct object, indirect object v. indirect object, direct object) ANCOVA, with syntactic alignment scores as the DV. To test for performance on the traditional ToM task, two separate two-way ANCOVAs for deception and false belief mentalising with a group as the between-participant factor and order as the within-participant factor were carried out with scores from the memory question and NART entered as covariates. To test for differences on ToM performance in the interviews, a 3 (group) × 2 (own belief v. experimenter's belief) ANCOVA was carried out. The DV was a total score on references to own and other's beliefs indicating ToM. Individual group differences were investigated using LSD post-hoc tests. To test for the relationship between alignment and ToM, Pearson's correlations were calculated between total alignment scores (separately for the maze and syntax tasks) and ToM scores on own, and others', beliefs, and on ToM stories, within each group separately.

Results

Alignment

Alignment in dialogue (the maze task)

A one-way ANOVA revealed that there was a significant group difference on participants' success on the maze task as measured by the number of successful attempts at reaching their destination (F (2,61) = 4.4, p < 0.05). LSD post hoc analysis showed that there was no difference in success rates between the two patient groups (p = 0.6), but both FTD and non-FTD patients were less successful at task completion than healthy controls (FTD: p < 0.01; non-FTD: p < 0.05). Pearson's correlations showed there was a significant relationship between task success and degree of alignment in the FTD group (r = 0.45, p < 0.05), but not in either the non-FTD group or the healthy controls (r = −0.08, p = 0.73) and (r = −0.35, p = 0.12) respectively.

In the alignment analysis, as predicted, the main effect of group (F (2,59) = 19.33, p < 0.001) was found (see Fig. 1). LSD post-hoc analysis revealed significant differences in all comparisons: the FTD group produced less alignment than both the non-FTD group (p < 0.01) and the healthy controls (p < 0.001), while the non-FTD group also produced less alignment than healthy controls (p < 0.01). There was no main effect of a maze (F (2,118) = 0.11, p = 0.90) or interaction between maze and group (F (4,118) = 0.46, p = 0.76). There was no effect of working memory (F (1,59) = 0.88, p = 0.35) or NART pre-morbid IQ (F (1,59) = 0.84, p = 0.36).

Fig. 1. Mean percentage scores for each of the three groups in establishing alignment in the dialogue task based on the N−1 scores. Error bars represent standard errors.

Cohen's d between-participant effect sizes demonstrated that on the degree of alignment, there were medium effect sizes between the FTD and non-FTD group (d = 0.91) and the non-FTD group and healthy controls (d = 1.06), and a very large effect size between the FTD group and healthy controls (d = 2.25).

Syntactic alignment

As predicted, the two-way ANCOVA for the syntactic alignment task revealed a main effect of group (F (2,59) = 4.38, p < 0.05) (see Fig. 2). LSD post hoc tests revealed a significant difference between the FTD group and healthy controls (p < 0.01) with the FTD patients showing less alignment. The difference between the FTD group and non-FTD group was not significant (p = 0.1) and there was no significant difference between the non-FTD patients and healthy controls (p < 0.16). There was a trend for an effect of word order (F (1,59) = 3.67, p = 0.06), but no interaction between word order and group (F (2,59) = 0.84, p = 0.44). There was an effect of working memory (F (2,59) = 8.7, p < 0.01), but no effect of NART (F (2,59) = 0.6, p = 0.46).

Fig. 2. Total mean scores for each of the three groups on the syntactic alignment task for double object and prepositional object forms.

Theory of mind tasks

Theory of mind in dialogue

One-way ANOVAs revealed no significant differences between the groups' mean total recording times (F (2,61) = 0.01, p = 0.99) or mean number of utterances (F (2,61) = 0.42, p = 0.66). As predicted, the 2-way ANCOVA revealed a main effect of group (F (2,59) = 4.48, p < 0.05) on overall ToM scores, but there was no effect of own v. experimenter belief reference (F (1,59) = 1.93, p = 0.17) (see Fig. 3). Post-hoc LSD tests showed that the healthy controls produced more belief references than the FTD group (p < 0.01), and, at trend level, than the non-FTD group (p = 0.06). Unexpectedly, there was no significant difference between the two patient groups (p = 0.24). There was no interaction between belief reference and group (F (2,59) = 1.17, p = 0.32). There were no effects of working memory (F (1,59) = 2.34, p = 0.13) or pre-morbid IQ (F (1,59) = 0.24, p = 0.63). Pearson's correlations showed there was no significant relationship between task success and degree of ToM in any of the three groups (FTD: r = 0.01, p = 0.96; non-FTD: r = 0.18, p = 0.42; healthy controls: r = −0.18, p = 0.42).

Fig. 3. Ratio scores for the occurrence of own belief and experimenter's belief references made by the three groups in the post maze interview. Error bars represent standard errors.

Theory of mind stories

A one-way ANOVA revealed a group difference in the memory control task (F (2,61) = 4.4, p < 0.01). Tukey's LSD post hoc tests revealed no significant difference between the two patient groups (p = 0.6), who both scored lower than the healthy controls (FTD: p < 0.01; non-FTD group: p = 0.03).

For deception questions, there was a significant main effect of group (F (2,59) = 7.65, p = 0.001) while there was no effect of memory questions (F (1,59) = 0.33, p = 0.57) and no effect of (pre-morbid) IQ (F (1,59) = 0.03, p = 0.86). LSD post hoc analysis revealed that the FTD group produced fewer accurate responses than both the non-FTD group (p < 0.05) and healthy controls (p < 0.001), as did the non-FTD compared to the healthy controls (p < 0.05). There was no effect of order (F (1,59) = 0.13, p = 0.72) and no interaction between order and group (F (2,59) = 1.13, p = 0.33).

In the false belief stories there was again a main effect of group (F (2,59) = 13.02, p < 0.001) and a significant main effect of the memory questions (F (1,59) = 7.16, p = 0.01) but no effect of (pre-morbid) IQ (F (1,59) = 2.34, p = 0.13). LSD post hoc analysis revealed that the FTD group performed worse than both the non-FTD group (p < 0.05) and healthy controls (p < 0.001), as did the non-FTD group compared to the healthy controls (p < 0.01). There was no main effect of order (F (1,59) = 0.04, p = 0.85) or interaction between order and group (F (2,59) = 1.12, p = 0.33).

Relationship between alignment and theory of mind

In the FTD group, there were significant relationships between alignment in dialogue and both own beliefs (r = 0.48, p < 0.05) and experimenter's belief (r = 0.44, p < 0.05) utterances on the maze task, and in the healthy control group for alignment and own belief (r = 0.45, p < 0.05). No other significant correlations for total alignment scores (separately for the maze and syntax tasks) and ToM (separately for dialogue or stories) were found. All correlations are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Correlations between alignment and theory of mind

Discussion

Alignment

As predicted, we found that FTD patients, compared to both healthy controls and non-FTD patients, displayed significantly less alignment with a conversational partner on a common lexical and semantic system in their dialogic contributions, and lower level syntactic alignment than healthy controls. Differences across groups were independent of pre-morbid IQ and there was no effect of working memory, apart from on the syntactic alignment task. Referential communication impairments are correlated with poor performance on specific neuropsychological measures, including working memory (Docherty and Gordinier, Reference Docherty and Gordinier1999; Doherty, Reference Docherty2005) and thus the distinction here may represent differing working memory demands for the two tasks.

Poverty of alignment observed in both patient groups relative to the healthy control group can be explained as a failure to utilise the available linguistic context and update their own interpretation of the evolving discussion via the production system. This would impoverish the ability to make predictions about upcoming structures, as observed in ERP studies of contextual processing in schizophrenia (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Cheung, Gong and Chan2011). Moreover, the healthy listener's employment of the production system in comprehension suggests a disruption in underlying mechanisms for both input and output processes in FTD.

While the non-FTD patients also showed a decrement in producing a shared semantic and conceptual representation of the maze in comparison to the healthy controls, they did not show reduced syntactic alignment. It thus seems unlikely that poverty in alignment at lower levels failing to percolate up to the situational model can account for the reduced alignment in non-FTD patients in an interactive dialogue.

The non-FTD group's reduced alignment in dialogue relative to healthy controls may reveal subtle weaknesses in the mechanisms underpinning alignment in schizophrenia, which are more prominent in FTD. This would support the idea of a continuum of context processing deficits in schizophrenia (Kuperberg et al., Reference Kuperberg, Kreher and Ditman2010; Tan et al., Reference Tan, Neill and Rossell2015). Problems of priming at low levels of activation in schizophrenia (see Doughty and Done, Reference Doughty and Done2009 for a review), may create a tipping point for the manifestation of clinical FTD in the context of an imbalance between algorithmic and semantic associative streams of processing (Kuperberg et al., Reference Kuperberg, Kreher and Ditman2010).

Poor alignment found here may result from difficulty in creating an efference copy that feeds into the forward production model. This would result in difficulty in generating predictions, as observed in schizophrenia (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Cheung, Gong and Chan2011), and potentially in processing new input as it unfolds (Kuperberg and Jaeger, Reference Kuperberg and Jaeger2016). Alternatively, a problem of monitoring the current utterance and the predicted utterance percept would be consistent with the suggestion that underlying schizophrenia is a failure to monitor one's own representations (Frith, Reference Frith1992), and more specifically, of FTD as a failure in self- or error-monitoring (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Scheldt, Hengtsberger and Dark1997; Laws et al., Reference Laws, Kondel and McKenna1999; Kircher and David, Reference Kircher, David, Kircher and David2003).

These findings are also consistent with the proposal of FTD as a breakdown in generative circuitry within a hierarchical generative framework of language processing (Brown and Kuperberg, Reference Brown and Kuperberg2015). These authors argue that a tendency to discount the precision of predictions prior to encountering bottom up information leads to an overweighting of prediction error along with an over-dependence on bottom-up activity, resulting in an over-reliance on semantic associations to establish the global structure. This would result in reduced alignment as patients' activation of a much broader set of semantic neighbours when a new input is encountered.

Theory of mind: main findings

Our second hypothesis was that FTD patients would display reduced implicit ToM in online communication relative to non-FTD patients and healthy controls. Both patient groups demonstrated a decrement in performance on ToM in dialogue relative to healthy controls but the difference between the two patient groups did not reach significance. Neither clinical group demonstrated an absence of ToM reasoning in dialogue but they did demonstrate a relative impairment (see also Langdon et al., Reference Langdon, Michie, Ward, McConaghy, Catts and Coltheart1997; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Reynaud, Herba, Morris and Corcoran2006; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Corcoran and Drake2009). In traditional ToM tasks, however, FTD patients showed significantly poorer performance than the other two groups. The main finding here is that, as predicted, FTD patients display both impoverished explicit and implicit ToM performance, while the non-FTD group displayed more impoverished ToM in dialogue only.

Importantly the two tasks test distinct skills. It is plausible that impoverished contextual processing could impact on FTD patients' ToM abilities in dialogue specifically. An inability to collate the contextual information necessary to build up an interpretation of the mental states of others (Schenkel et al., Reference Schenkel, Spaulding and Silverstein2005) may create difficulty in exploiting linguistic context to construct a representation corresponding to what the speaker has said. This in turn may impoverish alignment with the speaker and cause difficulty with tailoring communication appropriately to the listener in dialogue.

Theory of mind and alignment

As predicted, a correlation between alignment and ToM in dialogue was robust in the FTD group, but not in the non-FTD group, and was only evident for own beliefs in the healthy control group. This finding supports the hypothesis that reduced alignment representing problems of contextual processing has implications for mentalising. Failure to align linguistic representations to converge on a common situational model might create a decrement in the contextual scaffolding that facilitates establishing the interlocutor's mental state in dialogue. There was no correlation between ToM as measured by traditional tasks or between syntactic alignment and either measure of ToM, potentially reflecting a distinction of mediated and unmediated alignment.

Mediated accounts conceptualize alignment as a more strategic process, whereby imitation in conversation is deployed to facilitate communicative success. In contrast to syntactic alignment, lexical and semantic alignment observed in the maze task might be supported by additional communicative strategies, i.e. beliefs about the audience. Additionally, the FTD group's performance on ToM in dialogue correlated with alignment in dialogue but not with syntactic alignment. Collectively, this suggests that both measures of performance in dialogue might capture a more conscious element or ‘communicative design’. However, a more mediated role might be expected to be related with working memory abilities (Kaschak and Glenberg, Reference Kaschak and Glenberg2004), widely reported to be impaired in schizophrenia (Lee and Park, Reference Lee and Park2005), yet there was no effect of working memory in either the alignment in dialogue task or in the ToM in dialogue task.

This pattern of processing in alignment and ToM in FTD might indicate a more generalised impairment of contextual processing underlying schizophrenia (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, Reference Cohen and Servan-Schreiber1992). This relationship between abnormalities of alignment and ToM can also be captured within the hierarchical generative framework (Brown and Kuperberg, Reference Brown and Kuperberg2015). A failure to take the listener's feedback into account may, to a large extent, also be implicated in abnormal monitoring at other levels of the system in FTD (e.g. McGrath, Reference McGrath1991). Impaired monitoring of feedback and detection of miscommunication, could at least contribute to the failure of communication that characterizes thought disorder.

Limitations

The researcher's role meant that the dialogue in the task did not allow entirely natural alignment with the participants. For the purpose of achieving experimental control, it was, however, important to use similar input across the three groups. This study has used a categorical approach, distinguishing between patients with and without FTD, rather than treating FTD as a continuous variable preferred by some researchers. As a strength, we recruited a sufficient number of FTD patients with well-matched non-FTD patients and ensured thorough clinical and cognitive assessment; moreover, only patients with marked or severe FTD were included in the final analysis, which offsets the relatively small sample size. As a consequence, however, it was not possible to match all participants on all estimates of IQ.

Conclusion

This is the first study to investigate contextual processing in FTD in dialogic interactions. The findings (the key tasks show a decrement in performance) show that an impairment in sensitivity to contextual influences may underlie patients' disruption in spontaneous speech, given that mechanisms of alignment in the dialogue are underpinned by the production system (although patient groups performed similarly on syntactic alignment). The non-FTD group's relative impairment supports the continuum view of language functioning in schizophrenia. The association between ToM and alignment performance in FTD may indicate a more generalised deficit in integrating multiple sources of information into a contextual whole or a consequence of failure to build up a representation of the speaker's utterance. This indicates a potential area for future work that may allow the teasing apart of these different possibilities.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002289

Acknowledgements

We thank Professor Simon Garrod and Professor Martin Pickering for providing permission to recreate the alignment tasks and Professor Rhiannon Corcoran for her permission to use the ToM stories. All authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to this study.

Financial support

This work was supported by the British Academy Small Research Grants Fund (Grant number FW: SG-44292) [EP]; and by CIBERSAM, Spain [PMcK].

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Verbs which take an Indirect Object and a Direct Object are known as ditransitive verbs. Intransitive verbs take only a direct object after the verb.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC (1979) Thought, language and communication disorders: II. Diagnostic significance. Archives of General Psychiatry 36, 13221325.10.1001/archpsyc.1979.01780120055007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, NC (1982) The scale for the assessment of Thought Language and Communication (TLC). Schizophrenia Bulletin 12, 473482.10.1093/schbul/12.3.473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, NC (1984) The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City: University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC, Flaum, M and Arndt, S (1992) The comprehensive assessment of symptoms and history. An instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology, CASH. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 615623.10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080023004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagner, DM, Melinder, MRD and Barch, DM (2003) Language comprehension, working memory and working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 60, 299309.10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00280-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bora, E and Pantelis, C (2013) Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 144, 3136.10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bora, E, Yucel, M and Pantelis, C (2009) Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 109, 19.10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, HP, Pickering, MJ and Cleland, AA (2000) Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition 75, B13B25.10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00081-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, M and Kuperberg, GR (2015) A hierarchical generative framework of language processing: linking language perception, interpretation, and production abnormalities in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 643.10.3389/fnhum.2015.00643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brüne, M (2005) “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31, 2142.10.1093/schbul/sbi002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, JD and Servan-Schreiber, D (1992) Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behaviour and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review 99, 45.10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, JD, Barch, DM, Carter, CS and Servan-Schreiber, D (1999) Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108, 120133.10.1037/0021-843X.108.1.120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, PR and Wolpert, DM (2005) Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. Journal of Neural Engineering 2, S313.10.1088/1741-2560/2/3/S11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, PA (2005) The role of language in theory of mind development: what deaf children tell us. In Astington, JW and Baird, J (eds), Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press, pp. 266297.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, NM (2005) Cognitive impairment and disordered speech in schizophrenia: thought disorder, disorganisation, and communication failure perspectives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 269278.10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, NM and Gordinier, SW (1999) Immediate memory, attention, and communication disturbances in schizophrenia patients and their relatives. Psychological Medicine 29, 189197.10.1017/S0033291798007843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, NM, Rakfeldt, J, Hawkins, KA, Hoffman, RE, Quinlan, DM and Sledge, WH (1996) Working memory, attention, and communication disturbances in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 212219.10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, OJ and Done, DJ (2009) Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 14, 473509.10.1080/13546800903073291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, K, David, A, McCarthy, R, McKenna, P and Peters, E (2014 a) Higher-order semantic processing in formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 216, 168176.10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, K, Peters, E, McKenna, P, David, A and McCarthy, R (2014 b) Verbatim recall in schizophrenia: a study of contextual influences. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 19, 337358.10.1080/13546805.2013.870069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, KD (2007) Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology 44, 491505.10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00531.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, CD (1992) The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Frith, CD and Corcoran, R (1996) Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 26, 521530.10.1017/S0033291700035601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, CD (2004) Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological medicine 34, 385389.10.1017/S0033291703001326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S and Anderson, A (1987) Saying what you mean in dialogue: a study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition 27, 181218.10.1016/0010-0277(87)90018-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S and Clark, A (1993) The development of dialogue co-ordination skills in schoolchildren. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 101126.10.1080/01690969308406950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S and Doherty, G (1994) Conversation, coordination and convention: an empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition 53, 181215.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90048-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardy-Baylé, MC (1994) Organisation de l'action, phénomènes de conscience et représentation mentale de l'action chez des schizophrènes. Actualités psychiatriques 24, 918.Google Scholar
Harrington, L, Siegert, R and McClure, J (2005) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 10, 249286.10.1080/13546800444000056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M and Glenberg, G (2004) Comment in M.J. Pickering and S. Garrod towards a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 27, 169226.Google Scholar
Kerns, JG and Berenbaum, H (2002) Cognitive impairments associated with formal thought disorder in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111, 211.10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kircher, TTJ and David, A (2003) Self consciousness: an integrative approach from philosophy, psychopathology and the neurosciences. In Kircher, TTJ and David, A (eds), The Self in Neuroscience and Psychiatry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 445473.10.1017/CBO9780511543708.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR and Jaeger, TF (2016) What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31, 3259.10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR, McGuire, PK and David, AS (1998) Reduced sensitivity to linguistic context in schizophrenic thought disorder. Evidence from on-line monitoring for words in linguistically anomalous sentences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 423434.10.1037/0021-843X.107.3.423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR, McGuire, PK and David, AS (2000) Sensitivity to linguistic anomalies in spoken sentences: a case study approach to understanding thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 30, 345357.10.1017/S0033291700001744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR, Krecher, DA, Goff, D, McGuire, PK and David, AS (2006 a) Building up linguistic context in schizophrenia: evidence from self-paced reading. Neuropsychology 20, 442452.10.1037/0894-4105.20.4.442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR, Kreher, DA, Sitnikova, T, Caplan, DN and Holcomb, PJ (2007) The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sentences: evidence from event-related potentials. Brain and language 100, 223237.10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, GR, Kreher, DA and Ditman, T (2010) What can event-related potentials tell us about language, and perhaps even thought, in schizophrenia? International Journal of Psychophysiology 75, 6676.10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M and Federmeier, KD (2011) Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology 62, 621.10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langdon, R, Michie, PT, Ward, PB, McConaghy, N, Catts, S and Coltheart, M (1997) Defective self and/or other mentalizing in schizophrenia: a cognitive neuropsychological approach. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 2, 167193.10.1080/135468097396324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laws, KR, Kondel, TK and McKenna, PJ (1999) Receptive language deficits in schizophrenic thought disorder: evidence for impaired semantic access and monitoring. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 4, 89105.10.1080/135468099395981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J and Park, S (2005) Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 599611.10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manschreck, TC, Maher, BA, Milavetz, JJ, Ames, D, Weisstein, CC and Schneyer, ML (1988) Semantic priming in thought disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research 1, 6166.10.1016/0920-9964(88)90041-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, CD, Branzi, FM and Bar, M (2018) Prediction is production: the missing link between language production and comprehension. Scientific Reports 8, 1079.10.1038/s41598-018-19499-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, R, Leudar, I and Antaki, C (2004) Do people with schizophrenia display theory of mind deficits in clinical interactions? Psychological Medicine 34, 401411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, J (1991) Ordering thoughts on thought disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 158, 307316.10.1192/bjp.158.3.307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, J, Scheldt, S, Hengtsberger, P and Dark, F (1997) Thought disorder and executive ability. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 2, 303314.10.1080/135468097396306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, HE (1982) The National Adult Reading Test (NART). Windsor: NFER.Google Scholar
Pickering, MJ and Garrod, S (2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioural Brain Science 36, 329392.10.1017/S0140525X12001495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomarol-Clotet, E, Oh, TMSS, Laws, KR and McKenna, PJ (2008) Semantic priming in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry 192, 9297.10.1192/bjp.bp.106.032102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F and Fadiga, L (2010) Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, 351.10.1038/nrn2811CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, T, Reynaud, E, Herba, C, Morris, R and Corcoran, R (2006) Do you see what I see? Interpretations of intentional movement in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 81, 101111.10.1016/j.schres.2005.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenkel, LS, Spaulding, WD and Silverstein, SM (2005) Poor premorbid social functioning and theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: evidence of reduced context processing? Journal of Psychiatric Research 39, 499508.10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, SK and Johnsrude, IS (2003) The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception. Trends in Neurosciences 26, 100107.10.1016/S0166-2236(02)00037-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, SK, McGettigan, C and Eisner, F (2009) A little more conversation, a little less action—candidate roles for the motor cortex in speech perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 295.10.1038/nrn2603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silbert, LJ, Honey, CJ, Simony, E, Poeppel, D and Hasson, U (2014) Coupled neural systems underlie the production and comprehension of naturalistic narrative speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111, E4687E4696.10.1073/pnas.1323812111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sitnikova, T, Salisbury, DF, Kuperberg, G and Holcomb, PI (2002) Electrophysiological insights into language processing in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology 39, 851860.10.1111/1469-8986.3960851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, M, Braun, U, Hermle, L and Maier, S (1993) Associative network dysfunction in thought disordered schizophrenic patients: direct evidence from indirect priming. Biological Psychiatry 34, 864877.10.1016/0006-3223(93)90054-HCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sprong, M, Schothorst, P, Vos, E, Hox, J and van Engeland, H (2007) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 191, 513.10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, SLK, Corcoran, R and Drake, RJ (2008) Alignment and theory of mind in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 13, 431448.10.1080/13546800802405610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, SLK, Corcoran, R and Drake, RJ (2009) Mental states in psychosis: a pilot study of prompted implicit mentalizing during dialogue and its relationship to social functioning. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 14, 5375.10.1080/13546800902743449CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, EJ, Neill, E and Rossell, SL (2015) Assessing the relationship between semantic processing and thought disorder symptoms in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 21, 629638.10.1017/S1355617715000648CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D, Levy, DL and Holzman, PS (2000) Contextual insensitivity in schizophrenic language processing: evidence from lexical ambiguity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 761767.10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, IP (1970) Contextual constraint and schizophrenic language. Journal of Consult Clinical Psychology 35, 189194.10.1037/h0030122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, K, Cheung, EF, Gong, QY and Chan, RC (2011) Semantic processing disturbance in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the N400 component. PLoS One 6, e25435.10.1371/journal.pone.0025435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997) WAIS-III Manual; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wilson, SM, Saygin, AP, Sereno, MI and Iacoboni, M (2004) Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience 7, 701.10.1038/nn1263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolpert, DM (1997) Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1, 209216.10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01070-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the three groups [means; standard deviations (s.d.s) in parentheses]

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Mean percentage scores for each of the three groups in establishing alignment in the dialogue task based on the N−1 scores. Error bars represent standard errors.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Total mean scores for each of the three groups on the syntactic alignment task for double object and prepositional object forms.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Ratio scores for the occurrence of own belief and experimenter's belief references made by the three groups in the post maze interview. Error bars represent standard errors.

Figure 4

Table 2. Correlations between alignment and theory of mind

Supplementary material: File

Dwyer et al. supplementary material

Dwyer et al. supplementary material

Download Dwyer et al. supplementary material(File)
File 149 KB