Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-b4m5d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-19T09:26:31.604Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Highly neurotic never-depressed students have negative biases in information processing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 May 2007

STELLA W. Y. CHAN
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK
GUY M. GOODWIN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
CATHERINE J. HARMER*
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr Catherine Harmer, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK. (Email: catherine.harmer@psy.ox.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cognitive theories associate depression with negative biases in information processing. Although negatively biased cognitions are well documented in depressed patients and to some extent in recovered patients, it remains unclear whether these abnormalities are present before the first depressive episode.

Method

High neuroticism (N) is a well-recognized risk factor for depression. The current study therefore compared different aspects of emotional processing in 33 high-N never-depressed and 32 low-N matched volunteers. Awakening salivary cortisol, which is often elevated in severely depressed patients, was measured to explore the neurobiological substrate of neuroticism.

Results

High-N volunteers showed increased processing of negative and/or decreased processing of positive information in emotional categorization and memory, facial expression recognition and emotion-potentiated startle (EPS), in the absence of global memory or executive deficits. By contrast, there was no evidence for effects of neuroticism on attentional bias (as measured with the dot-probe task), over-general autobiographical memory, or awakening cortisol levels.

Conclusions

These results suggest that certain negative processing biases precede depression rather than arising as a result of depressive experience per se and as such could in part mediate the vulnerability of high-N subjects to depression. Longitudinal studies are required to confirm that such cognitive vulnerabilities predict subsequent depression in individual subjects.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

INTRODUCTION

The aetiology of depression in community samples has been intensively investigated in twin studies that can broadly distinguish genetic from environmental factors. The existing literature suggests that the key vulnerability factors are neuroticism, family history of depression and early abuse/neglect or trauma, whereas the precipitating factor is often an adverse life event. Working with these variables, depressive episodes are moderately well predicted at the 12-month follow-up (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Kessler, Neale, Heath and Eaves1993, Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2002, Reference Kendler, Kuhn and Prescott2004, Reference Kendler, Gardner and Prescott2006).

While these findings are robust, the approach is essentially observational and thus insufficient to indicate the mechanisms whereby clinical depression emerges in high-risk individuals. The present study therefore set out to determine the cognitive and neurophysiological mechanisms of neuroticism, whereby adversity may lead to depression.

Cognitive theories of depression emphasize the role of negative biases in information processing in the aetiology and maintenance of the disorder (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979). Biases on the interpretation and memory for emotional information have been reported. For example, in facial expression recognition tasks, depressed patients show reduced recognition of positive expressions and/or increased perception of negative expressions (Gur et al. Reference Gur, Erwin, Gur, Zwil, Heimberg and Kraemer1992; Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Geerts and Gordijn1999; Surguladze et al. Reference Surguladze, Young, Senior, Brebion, Travis and Philips2004), that is a bias away from positive towards negative. Negative perceptual and memory biases have also been found in healthy volunteers following negative mood induction (Teasdale & Russell, Reference Teasdale and Russell1983; Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Bloem and Groothuis1995). Although attentional biases are less consistently found in depression, dot-probe tasks have revealed increased attention to negative stimuli in dysphoric patients and healthy volunteers undergoing a negative mood induction when longer stimulus durations are used (Bradley et al. Reference Bradley, Mogg and Lee1997).

Although the state of severe depression is evidently associated with cognitive abnormalities, there are fewer comparable studies conducted in recovered patients. In general, global impairments of executive function resolve following recovery (Peselow et al. Reference Peselow, Corwin, Fieve, Rotrosen and Cooper1991; Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001), but certain residual emotional biases remain and may provide the mediating mechanisms in subsequent relapse. Thus, facial expression recognition was found to be negatively biased in recovered depressed patients (Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Geerts and Gordijn1999; Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Cowen, Goodwin and Harmer2004; Hayward et al. Reference Hayward, Goodwin, Cowen and Harmer2005) and this was associated with subsequent relapse (Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Geerts and Gordijn1999).

In parallel with the cognitive findings are reports of dysfunction of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) system in depression. HPA dysfunction can be indicated by an elevated cortisol response to awakening, as demonstrated in both acutely depressed (Pruessner et al. Reference Pruessner, Wolf, Hellhammer, Buske-Kirschbaum, von Auer, Jobst, Kaspers and Kirschbaum1997) and recovered patients (Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Hafizi and Cowen2003). Longitudinal studies have provided evidence that cortisol level predicts subsequent depression onset in adult women (Harris et al. Reference Harris, Borsanyi, Messari, Stanford, Cleary, Shiers, Brown and Herbert2000) and adolescents (Goodyer et al. Reference Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin and Altham2000). Elevated cortisol levels may also contribute to the learning and memory impairments reported in depression (Young et al. Reference Young, Sahakian and Robbins1999).

Thus, measurable cognitive and HPA abnormality may be present in recovery from depression, but we cannot rule out a scar effect, so called because the residual biases and elevated cortisol may be a consequence of depression, rather than implying occurrence before the onset of the first episode. The present study therefore recruited young euthymic college students with high versus low scores for neuroticism (N), and without a history of depression. We hypothesized that high-N volunteers would display affective processing biases favouring negative versus positive information. These biases might also be accompanied by elevated morning cortisol levels, but global impairments of executive function were unlikely before a depressive episode.

METHOD

Volunteers and design

The study was approved by the local ethics committee. Seventy-two healthy college students with high or low N scores (see below) gave written informed consent to the study, and received payment for their participation. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV was used to screen for axis I disorders and seven volunteers were excluded from the study because of current or previous depression or anxiety disorders.

N scores for screening were derived from the 12-item neuroticism scale of the shortened Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck et al. Reference Eysenck, Eysenck and Barrett1985). Thirty-three (22 women) were in the high-N group (H: mean score=9·58, range=8–12) and 32 (18 women) in low-N group (L: mean score=1·25, range=0–3). The two groups were matched for age (18·82±0·98 v. 19·06±0·88), gender, verbal IQ (40·12±2·80 v. 38·16±5·54) and spatial IQ (26·19±11·37 v. 22·43±9·37) assessed with the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, Reference Nelson1982) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1981).

Characterization of state and trait variables

To assess mood, personality, family background and life experience, participants were interviewed with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1967) and filled in the following questionnaires: the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al. Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961), Befindlichkeits Scale of Mood and Energy (Bf-S; von Zerssen et al. Reference von Zerssen, Strian, Schwarz and Pichot1974), Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (FNE; Watson & Friend, Reference Watson and Friend1969), Buss–Durkee Hostility Inventory (Buss & Durkee, Reference Buss and Durkee1957), Social Adaptation Self-Evaluation Scale (SASS; Bosc et al. Reference Bosc, Dubini and Polin1997), Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS; Weissmann, Reference Weissmann1979, factors taken from Cane et al. Reference Cane, Olinger, Gotlib and Kuiper1986), ruminative items of the Response Styles Questionnaire (modified by Treynor et al. Reference Treynor, Gonzalez and Nolen-Hosksema2003), EPQ (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1975), Parental Bonding Inventory (PBI; Parker et al. Reference Parker, Tupling and Brown1979), stressful life events (adopted from Goodyer et al. Reference Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin, Secher and Pearson1997), and family history of psychiatric disorders. Two participants did not complete all the questionnaires.

Emotional categorization

Main task: personality characteristics categorization

Sixty personality characteristics chosen to be extremely desirable (e.g. honest) or undesirable (e.g. rude) (Anderson, Reference Anderson1968, matched on word length, frequency and meaningfulness) were presented on a computer screen for 500 ms. Participants were asked to categorize these traits as likeable or dislikeable by pressing the labelled key on the keyboard. To encourage self-referent judgement, participants were asked to imagine whether they would be pleased or displeased if they overheard someone describing them in this way.

Control task: animal attributes categorization

A similar task was carried out as a control, using 60 attribute words (30 per valence). This time participants were asked to classify each attribute as an ‘advantage’ (e.g. strong) or ‘disadvantage’ (e.g. weak) for a predatory animal. In both tasks classifications and reaction times for the correct identifications were recorded.

Emotional memory

A surprise memory task comprising recall and recognition (60 target words plus 60 distracters) was conducted 15 min after completing each of the categorization tasks. The number of correctly and incorrectly recalled words was counted. Recognition data were analysed using signal detection theory (Green & Swets, Reference Green and Swets1966; Grier, Reference Grier1971) to derive a measure of accuracy corrected for subjects' response tendency. The proportion of correctly recognized words (y) and the proportion of falsely recognized words (x) were entered into the following equations to give the sensitivity measure d′ and the response bias β: d′=0·5+[(yx)(1+yx)/4y(1−x)]; β=[y(1−y)−x(1−x)]/[y(1−y)+x(1−x)]. This allowed an assessment of accuracy (hits) unconfounded by the response criterion used by the volunteer.

Facial expression recognition

Stimuli and procedure

Pictures of faces representing six basic emotions (happiness, surprise, sadness, fear, anger, and disgust) were taken from the Pictures of Affect Series (Ekman & Friesen, Reference Ekman and Friesen1976). These were morphed between each full emotion (100%) and neutral (0%) in 10% steps (Young et al. Reference Young, Rowland, Calder, Etcoff, Seth and Perrett1997): four examples were given per intensity per emotion. Each face was also presented in a neutral expression, giving a total of 250 stimuli. Each stimulus flashed up on a computer screen for 500 ms followed by a blank screen. Participants were asked to recognize the emotion by pressing the appropriate key. Accuracy and reaction times for correct choices and misclassifications were recorded. Accuracy was defined by the threshold, that is the intensity level at which the participant gave three or more (i.e. ⩾75%) correct responses across three consecutive intensity levels.

Dot-probe task

Stimuli and procedure

The emotional stimuli included 60 social threatening negative words and 60 positive words, each of which was paired with a matched neutral word. Another 60 neutral–neutral word pairs were given as fillers. Preceded by a fixation cross (500 ms), a word pair was presented on the screen with one word above another. In the unmasked condition the word pair was presented for 500 ms, whereas in the masked condition the word pair appeared for 14 ms followed by the display (186 ms) of a mask. After that, a probe (one or two stars) appeared in the position of either preceding word, and participants were asked to indicate the number of stars. These 360 trials were presented in three blocks in random order. Reaction time and accuracy were recorded. Attentional vigilance scores were calculated for each participant by subtracting the mean score of ‘congruent trials’ (where the probe and emotional words appeared in the same position) from that of the ‘incongruent trials’ (where they appeared in opposite positions).

Emotion-potentiated startle (EPS)

Stimuli

Sixty-three pictures of three categories (pleasant, unpleasant, neutral) were taken from the International Affective Picture System (gender-specified, Larson et al. Reference Larson, Ruffalo, Nietert and Davidson2000). Each picture was presented for 13 s (mean inter-trial interval=13 s) on a computer screen. The pictures were presented in three blocks in a fixed order so that no two of the same category would appear successively.

Procedure and recording

The eye-blink component of the startle response was recorded from the orbicularis oculi using electromyography (EMG startle response system, San Diego Instruments, Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Acoustic probes were 50-ms, 95-dB bursts of white noise with a nearly instantaneous rise time (generated through the noise generator and amplifier of the EMG startle response system) and were delivered binaurally through headphones at 1·5, 4·5 or 7·5 s following picture onset. To minimize expectation, startle probes were skipped from two trials per valence per block, and three probes were given within the inter-trial interval. A practice session presenting nine neutral pictures and startle probes was used in the beginning to habituate participants to the startle probes.

EMG signals were filtered (low cut-off: 0·5 Hz; high cut-off: 100 Hz) and rectified. Eye-blink reflex magnitudes in μV were calculated by subtracting the amount of integrated EMG at reflex onset from the first peak amplitude of integrated EMG between 20 and 120 ms following probe onset. Trials with no traceable eye-blink reflex were assigned a magnitude of zero and included in the analysis. Eye-blink reflexes with an excessively noisy baseline (within 20 ms after the probe) were rejected. Four participants (two from each group) were excluded from the analysis because they displayed fewer than 25% blink responses. Magnitude and latency of the eye-blink reflex were recorded.

Subjective rating

After the recording, participants were asked to review the pictures and rate the valence and arousal levels of each picture on a 1–10 scale (from negative to positive, low arousal to high arousal).

Global executive functions

As global cognitive impairments (e.g. Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001; Elliot et al. Reference Elliott, Sahakian, McKay, Herrod, Robins and Paykel1996) and over-general autobiographical memory (for a recent review, see Williams et al. Reference Williams and Yiend2004) were widely demonstrated in depression, we included the following tasks to examine whether neuroticism has an effect on memory, learning and problem solving.

Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)

The AVLT (Rey, Reference Rey1964) was used to assess learning and memory. In the immediate recall phase, participants were read aloud a 15-item word list and asked to recall as many words as possible. This procedure was repeated five times. A distracter list was then presented to create a short delay, after which free recall of the first list was measured. Fifteen minutes later, participants were tested again with a free recall and recognition test (15 target words plus 35 distracters).

Tower of London (TOL)

In this task two sets of three coloured balls were presented on a touch-sensitive computer screen, with each set being arranged like snooker balls hanging in three pockets. Participants were asked to rearrange the balls in one set to match the other set under certain rules. The minimum number of moves (2, 3, 4 or 5) required for each trial was indicated. Participants were instructed to work out the whole solution in mind before making the first move. In addition, two control blocks were used to measure the time for actual movement. In these, the balls were constantly moved by the computer in one set and participants were asked to copy this movement on the other set. Task performance was assessed by three variables: number of problems solved with minimum moves, average number of moves, and thinking time. Thinking time was computed by subtracting the reaction time of the control trials from that of the main trials.

Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT)

In the AMT (Williams & Broadbent, Reference Williams and Broadbent1986), participants were presented with 18 cue words (nine positive and nine negative), each at a time, and asked to recall a memory of a specific event that the cue word reminded them of. Instructions defined a specific event as any event that took place on a particular day at a particular place and no more recent than a week ago, and that participants should not recall the same event for more than one cue word. Each word was shown on a card and read aloud by the experimenter. If a participant did not respond after 30 s, the next word would be presented. A practice trial with a neutral word (‘chicken’) was used in the beginning to check that the participant understood the instructions. Latency (duration between the presentation of a cue word and the start of the recall) and number of specific responses were recorded.

Awakening salivary cortisol

At the end of the experiment, participants were given instructions to collect five salivary samples at home in the following morning: the first sample was taken immediately upon waking (Time 1), and subsequent samples were taken at 15-min intervals for the next hour (Times 2, 3, 4 and 5 respectively). Participants were not allowed to eat or drink during the test and not to consume alcohol the night before. Nine subjects (seven from H) failed to return their samples. Cortisol was measured with an in-house double-antibody radioimmunoassay. Cortisol levels of the five samples (M1, M2, M3, M4, M5) were entered into the following formula to compute the area under the curve (Pruessner et al. Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer2003): [(M2+M1)+(M3+M2)+(M4+M3)+(M5+M4)]×15/2.

Statistics

Independent-samples t tests were used to reveal group differences in psychological characteristics, AVLT (delayed recognition), and TOL (number of trials solved in minimum moves) and salivary cortisol. Other data were analysed by using two-way (facial expression recognition, EPS, AMT, AVLT, TOL) or three-way (emotional categorization, emotional memory, dot probe) analyses of variance (ANOVAs), with between-Ss variable as group (H, L) and within-Ss variable(s) as emotion, trial and/or task conditions. Individual ANOVAs and t tests were run to clarify significant interactions.

RESULTS

Psychological characteristics

Using the full EPQ scale, the two groups were confirmed to have significant difference in neuroticism scores (17·91±2·84 v. 5·72±3·27, p<0·01). Although none of the volunteers had ever met criteria for DSM-IV depression, and mean scores on clinical symptom scales did not exceed usual levels for remission, H participants showed a significantly higher level of depressive mood, anxiety and hostility than L. They also reported more rumination, dysfunctional attitudes, parental over-protectiveness, and lower social adaptation. By contrast, the two groups did not differ in family history of psychiatric disorder, parental care, and stressful life experience (all p>0·10) (Table 1).

Table 1. Psychological characteristics of the two groups

N, Neuroticism; HAMD, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Bf-S, Befindlichkeits Scale of Mood and Energy; FNE, Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale; SASS, Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale; DAS, Dysfunctional Attitude Scale.

Values represent means (±standard deviations).

Asterisks represent significance of group comparisons: ** p⩽0·01.

Emotional categorization

There was a significant group×task×emotion interaction [F(1, 63)=4·73, p=0·03] for reaction time in the self-referent versus animal categorization tasks. Sensitivity analyses showed a significant group×emotion interaction in the emotional categorization [Fig. 1(a): F(1, 63)=3·88, p=0·05] but not in the control task [Fig. 1(b): F(1, 63)=0·51, p=0·48]. H volunteers were quicker at classifying negative versus positive personality characteristics than L. Furthermore, within the H group, the reaction time for positive items was significantly correlated with N scores [r(33)=0·37, p=0·04], so reaction time for positive items increased with neuroticism.

Fig. 1. Emotional categorization of self-referent personality characteristics (a) and animal attributes (b). Values represent mean difference scores of reaction time to identify positive minus negative words±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group comparisons (* p<0·05). ■, High-N; □, Low-N.

For accuracy data there was neither a group difference [F(1, 63)=0·45, p=0·51] nor a group×emotion×task interaction [F(1, 63)=0·51, p=0·48] but both groups achieved more than 90% accuracy in both categorization tasks, implying a potential ceiling effect.

Emotional memory

Recall

The two groups performed similarly in terms of correct recall [Fig. 2(a): group: F(1, 63)=0·47, p=0·50; task×emotion×group: F(1, 63)=0·87, p=0·35]. However, H volunteers produced fewer positive memory intrusions than L [Fig. 2(b): group×emotion: F(1, 63)=7·54, p=0·01] for self-referent information, but not in the control task [group×emotion: F(1, 63)=0·01, p=0·92].

Fig. 2. Emotional memory: (a) correct recall; (b) incorrect recall, of personality characteristics and animal attribute words. Values represent mean difference scores of number of words recalled for positive minus negative words ±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group comparisons (* p<0·05). ■, High-N; □, Low-N.

Recognition

The two groups had similar accuracy (d′) [group: F(1, 63)=0·07, p=0·79; task×emotion×group: F(1, 63)=3·09, p=0·09] and response bias (β) [group F(1, 63)=0·03, p=0·87; task×emotion×group F(1, 63)=0·50, p=0·48].

Facial expression recognition

There were no main effects on accuracy, reaction time or misclassifications in this task (all p values >0·10). However, differences in recognition of each emotion were further explored using independent t tests, given a strong a priori hypothesis for individual emotions. This revealed a significant group difference for accuracy of happy faces [Fig. 3: t(63)=2·05, p=0·04] but not in any other emotions (all p values >0·40). Specifically, H had a higher threshold in identifying happy faces than did L; that is, they needed higher intensity levels to be able to correctly identify happy facial expressions.

Fig. 3. Threshold of facial expression recognition in high-N (■) and low-N (□) volunteers. Values represent mean threshold levels (±standard error of the mean) required to correctly identify each emotion at a level of >75%. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group difference (* p<0·05).

Dot-probe task

There were no significant effects on the vigilance scores of reaction time [group: F(1, 63)=0·22, p=0·64; group×emotion×mask: F(1, 63)=0·03, p=0·87] or accuracy [group: F(1, 63)=1·92, p=0·17; group×emotion×mask: F(1, 63)=0·01, p=0·95]. This was also true when only the unmasked trials were considered: for reaction time [group: F(1, 63)=0·10, p=0·76, emotion×group: F(1, 63)=0·01, p=0·94] and for accuracy [group: F(1, 63)=0·49, p=0·49; emotion×group: F(1, 63)=0·49, p=0·49] (see Table 2, available online).

Table 2. Attentional bias as measured by dot-probe task under masked and unmasked conditions. Values represent means±standard deviations

EPS

Eye-blink magnitude

The z-transformed data revealed the expected potentiation effect in both groups [Fig. 4(a): overall effect of emotion: F(2, 118)=10·17, p=0·001], with volunteers showing a greater response to unpleasant than neutral or pleasant pictures. However, this was not affected by neuroticism [group: F(1, 59)=0·86, p=0·40; emotion×group: F(2, 118)=0·85, p=0·43]. Examination of raw startle amplitudes showed a similar pattern [emotion: F(2, 118)=7·19, p=0·001; group: F(1, 59)=1·33, p=0·25; emotion×group: F(2, 118)=0·36, p=0·70]. The average amplitudes were similar between H and L [mean=1416·20 v. 1674·83; t(59)=−1·15, p=0·25].

Fig. 4. (a) Amplitude of startle response (Z scores) and (b) average latency of startle response while viewing the three types of pictures. Values represent means±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of comparisons between startle responses during pleasant and unpleasant pictures within each group (* p<0·05)., Neutral; □, pleasant; ■, unpleasant.

Eye-blink latency

There was a significant group×emotion interaction [Fig. 4(b): F(2, 116)=3·08, p=0·05], but post-hoc comparisons failed to reveal group difference within each emotional category (all p>0·10). As shown by paired-samples t tests, the interaction was mostly driven by H responding more slowly to pleasant than unpleasant pictures [t(29)=2·04, p=0·05].

Subjective ratings

There was no group difference in terms of rating for valence [group: F(1, 58)=0·12, p=0·73; emotion×group: F(2, 116)=1·10, p=0·34] or arousal [group: F(1, 58)=0·43, p=0·52; emotion×group: F(2, 116)=1·14, p=0·32].

Non-emotional cognitive functions

In these tests, high-N subjects showed modest trends to improved performance compared with low-N (see Table 3, available online). In the AVLT immediate recall there was a significant group×trial interaction [F(4, 253)=3·27, p=0·01], with subsequent t tests suggesting that H outperformed L only in the first trial [t(63)=2·21, p=0·03]. For the short- versus long-delayed recalls there was no group difference [F(1, 63)=0·02, p=0·89] or interaction [F(1, 63)=1·77, p=0·19], nor was there any group difference on long-delayed recognition [hits: t(63)=−1·14, p=0·26; false alarms: t(63)=1·26, p=0·21]. In the TOL there was a significant group effect [F(1, 63)=5·75, p=0·02] and a group×trial interaction [F(3, 189)=3·32, p=0·02] in the number of moves required to solve the problem. Subsequent t tests found that H was better than L in the four-move problems [t(63)=−2·29, p=0·03]. H also solved more problems in a minimum number of moves than did L [t(63)=2·16, p=0·03]. However, among H volunteers, this measure was negatively correlated with N score [r(33)=−0·35, p=0·05], suggesting that the higher a participant scored in neuroticism the fewer problems s/he managed to solve in a minimum number of moves. There were no effects in terms of thinking times [group: F(1, 63)=0·55, p=0·46; interaction: F(3, 189)=0·48, p=0·70]. When the perfectionism score from the DAS was included as a covariate in these analyses, the differences were lost [AVLT Immediate Recall group×trial: F(4, 244)=1·33, p=0·26; TOL Number of Moves group effect: F(1, 61)=3·08, p=0·08; group×trial F(3, 183)=2·21, p=0·09].

Table 3. Memory and executive functions

Values represent means±standard deviations. Asterisks represent significance of group difference, * p<0·05.

In the AMT, there were no effects on latency [group: F(1, 63)=0·01, p=0·95; group×emotion: F(1, 63)=0·65, p=0·42] or number of specific memories produced [group: F(1, 63)=2·20, p=0·14; interaction: F(1, 63)=0·65, p=0·42] (see Table 3, available online).

Awakening salivary cortisol

The mean time of awakening did not differ between H and L participants (0838 v. 0813 h). The area under the curve of cortisol responses revealed no group difference (1375·80±552·23 v. 1568·22±593·85, p=0·23) (see Fig. 5, available online).

Fig. 5. Salivary cortisol level within the first hour of waking. ■, High-N; ▲, Low-N.

DISCUSSION

Our results show that biases in information processing are present in high-N students who are at risk of depression but have not been depressed. Decreased positive or increased negative processing was seen across a number of tasks including emotional categorization and memory, facial expression recognition and EPS, in the absence of global memory or executive deficits. However, there was no evidence for effects of neuroticism on attentional bias (measured with the dot-probe task), over-general autobiographical memory, or elevated morning cortisol levels.

As noted in the introduction, it has often been suggested that negative biases may be a trait vulnerability marker for depression. However, by eliminating subjects with previous depression, our results establish a direct association between cognitive abnormality and vulnerability to depression without the contamination of a scar effect.

Although the relationship between neuroticism and cognitive processing has been researched for decades (e.g. Lishman, Reference Lishman1974; Lloyd & Lishman, Reference Lloyd and Lishman1975; Martin et al. Reference Martin, Ward and Clark1983), these studies were mostly performed on non-selective samples with which correlations were examined between N and response (e.g. speed of memory recall). In contrast to this approach, our study selected participants from the extreme range of N (high versus low) and thus directly illustrated the influence of high neuroticism on emotional processing.

In the emotional categorization and memory tasks, H volunteers were faster to classify dislikeable self-referent personality characteristics and produced fewer positive memory intrusions. This bias away from the positive was similarly revealed in the perception of social information, as measured by the facial expression recognition task. H volunteers had a higher threshold for identifying happy faces than the control volunteers. A reduction in positive facial perception echoes previous experimental findings in depressed patients (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Sahakian, Rubinsztein, Michael, Rogers, Robbins and Paykel1999; Suslow et al. Reference Suslow, Junghanns and Arolt2001) and healthy volunteers undergoing a negative mood induction (Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Bloem and Groothuis1995). By contrast, recovered patients also tend to show an increased perception of negative expression such as fear, disgust and sadness (Bouhuys et al. Reference Bouhuys, Geerts and Gordijn1999; Bhagwagar et al. Reference Bhagwagar, Cowen, Goodwin and Harmer2004; Hayward et al. Reference Hayward, Goodwin, Cowen and Harmer2005). This suggests the hypothesis that risk for depression is largely manifest as reduced positive processing of emotional information, which is accompanied by increased negative processing only after the actual experience of depression.

EPS was used to give a physiological measure of reactivity to emotional information. Our sample, regardless of group membership, exhibited the expected EPS pattern with enhanced eye-blink during the presentation of aversive pictures relative to neutral or pleasant pictures, which is widely demonstrated in laboratory work (e.g. Bradley et al. Reference Bradley, Cuthbert and Lang1990; Cook et al. Reference Cook, Hawk, Davis and Stevenson1991). The two groups gave similar pleasantness ratings for the pictures. Unexpectedly, while amplitude was unaffected, there was an effect of neuroticism on blink latency. H volunteers showed a significant delay in their reflexive response to pleasant pictures. A reduced physiological reactivity to positive stimuli in the absence of differential subjective rating implies that neuroticism involves biases in mechanisms that are highly automatic and thus not influenced by self-report. The current result is compatible with the theory that low activity in appetitive emotional systems is the core deficit in depression (Fowles, Reference Fowles1988; Depue & Iacono, Reference Depue and Iacono1989; Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991), although latency variables are infrequently reported for the EPS task.

The negative findings in this study may indicate domains in which deficits are simply not associated with depression vulnerability or, instead, may arise solely from the experience of depression. Thus, the dot-probe task provides evidence that attentional biases play no role in neuroticism and, as noted in the introduction, attentional biases have never been reliably shown in depression either. By contrast, the negative results in autobiographical memory are notable because an over-general memory deficit has been robustly found across clinical samples. Among the few that examined over-general memory before depression (e.g. Mackinger et al. Reference Mackinger, Loschin and Leibetseder2000a, Reference Mackinger, Pachinger, Leibetseder and Fartacekb; von Minnen et al. Reference von Minnen, Wessel, Verhaak and Smeenk2005), the results were confounded to some extent by prior trauma experiences and/or unclear history of depression. A recent study (Gibbs & Rude, Reference Gibbs and Rude2004) found that over-general memory interacts with stressful life events in predicting subsequent depression symptoms in randomly selected students, but the experience of depression was not defined. We propose that over-general memory does not contribute to the vulnerability to depression shown by high-N subjects.

Our negative findings for awakening cortisol in high-N students contrast with the hyperactivity of the HPA axis reported previously in a substantially older sample (Portella et al. Reference Portella, Harmer, Flint, Cowen and Goodwin2005) and in an older subgroup of high-N subjects (Zobel et al. Reference Zobel, Barkpw, Schulze-Rauschenbach, von Widdern, Metten, Pfeiffer, Schnell, Wagner and Maier2004). Our data are comparable with those obtained in the high-N subjects of a similar age range (Zobel et al. Reference Zobel, Barkpw, Schulze-Rauschenbach, von Widdern, Metten, Pfeiffer, Schnell, Wagner and Maier2004). Therefore, it is possible that dysfunction of the HPA axis occurs with increasing age in vulnerable individuals, possibly as an interaction between risk and exposure to toxic life events. Alternatively, it cannot be ruled out from our data set that elevated cortisol responses may only be seen in a subpopulation of high neurotic subjects who will eventually develop depression. Longitudinal studies are required to test this hypothesis.

As anticipated, results from AVLT and TOL revealed no global cognitive impairment by neuroticism. These are consistent with the hypothesis that cognitive deficits are largely confined to periods of illness in depression rather than as a more general trait (Peselow et al. Reference Peselow, Corwin, Fieve, Rotrosen and Cooper1991; Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001). There was a tendency for H volunteers to perform better in these tasks, which appeared to be related to their enhanced drive or perfectionism. This enhanced cognitive function makes the specific reduction in positive processing also found in this group more noteworthy.

The current study has a number of limitations. Although high neuroticism is a robust risk factor for depression, the relatively low prevalence rates of depression imply that only a small proportion of the high-N population will go on to develop depression, thereby potentially diluting any effects that we may have seen. While this could account for the lack of effect in terms of cortisol responses and autobiographical memory performance, it could not account for the negative biases that were seen in the group as a whole. Longitudinal studies are required to assess the predictive power of negative biases for subsequent depression in a sample adequately powered for the detection of infrequent events. Finally, in the current study the experimenters were not blind to group membership. Although this is unlikely to have an influence on the results because responses were collected automatically by computer and task instructions were standardized across participants, future studies may want to assess negative bias using a blinded design to confirm these findings.

Conclusion

There has been an ongoing controversy whether biased cognition is a state or trait marker for depression. Negative cognitive biases in healthy volunteers after a depressive mood induction give partial support for the ‘state hypothesis’ (Mathews & Bradley, Reference Mathews and Bradley1983; Sutton et al. Reference Sutton, Teasdale and Broadbent1988). However, neuroticism in individuals without a history of depression was associated with reduced positive processing in our study, thus strongly suggesting that cognitive biases represent a trait vulnerability marker for depression. Furthermore, the processes that were found to be affected by neuroticism overlap with the processes that are affected by antidepressant drug treatment in healthy volunteers (Harmer et al. Reference Harmer, Hill, Taylor, Cowen and Goodwin2003, Reference Harmer, Shelley, Cowen and Goodwin2004). This overlap validates the earlier suggestion that antidepressants act on the key cognitive components of emotional processing related to the acquisition and maintenance of mood disorders. In the future, longitudinal studies are required to investigate whether, and to what extent, such cognitive vulnerability predicts subsequent depression. If so, this could pave the way for further studies evaluating the efficacy of early interventions targeting the dysfunctional cognitive styles of the high-risk population.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by the Medical Research Council.

NOTE

Supplementary information accompanies this paper on the Journal's website (http://journals.cambridge.org).

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

Catherine Harmer has acted as a consultant for Lundbeck, Merck Sharp & Dohme, and P1Vital. Guy Goodwin has received grants from Sanofi-Aventis, and Servier; honoraria from AstraZeneca, BMS, Eisai, Lundbeck, Sanofi-Aventis, and Servier; held paid positions with the University of Oxford, and Oxfordshire & Buckinghamshire NHS Mental Healthcare Trust and been on advisory boards of AstraZeneca, BMS, Lilly, Lundbeck, P1Vital, Sanofi-Aventis, Servier, and Wyeth.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likeableness rating of 555 personality trait words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 9, 272279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P. & Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. British Journal of Psychiatry 178, 200206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. Guilford: New York.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. M. & Harmer, C. J. (2004). Normalization of enhanced fear recognition by acute SSRI treatment in subjects with a previous history of depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 166168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhagwagar, Z., Hafizi, S. & Cowen, P. J. (2003). Increase in concentration of waking salivary cortisol in recovered patients with depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 18901891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosc, M., Dubini, A. & Polin, V. (1997). Development and validation of a social functioning scale, the Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale. European Neuropsychopharmacology 7 (Suppl. 1), S57S70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouhuys, A. L., Bloem, G. M. & Groothuis, T. G. G. (1995). Induction of depressed and elevated mood by music influences the perception of facial emotional expressions in healthy subjects. Journal of Affective Disorders 33, 215226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouhuys, A. L., Geerts, E. & Gordijn, M. C. (1999). Depressed patients' perceptions of facial emotions in depressed and remitted states are associated with relapse: a longitudinal study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 187, 595602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K. & Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy 35, 911927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N. & Lang, P. J. (1990). Startle reflex modification: attention or emotion? Psychophysiology 27, 513523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, A. H. & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology 21, 343349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cane, D. B., Olinger, L. J., Gotlib, I. H. & Kuiper, N. A. (1986). Factor structure of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale in a student population. Journal of Clinical Psychology 42, 307309.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A. & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100, 316336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, E. W., Hawk, L. W. Jr., Davis, T. L. & Stevenson, V. E. (1991). Affective individual differences and startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 11, 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depue, R. A. & Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology 40, 457492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., McKay, A. P., Herrod, J. J., Robins, T. W. & Paykel, E. S. (1996). Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychological Medicine 26, 975989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. J. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Hodder and Stoughton: London.Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J. & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences 6, 2129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1988). Psychophysiology and psychopathology: a motivational approach. Psychophysiology 25, 373391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, B. R. & Rude, S. S. (2004). Over-general autobiographical memory as depression vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research 28, 511526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., Tamplin, A. & Altham, P. M. (2000). Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry 177, 499504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., Tamplin, A., Secher, S. M. & Pearson, J. (1997). Short-term outcome of major depression: II. Life events, family dysfunction, and friendship difficulties as predictors of persistent disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 474480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. M. & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Wiley: London.Google Scholar
Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin 75, 424429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. C., Erwin, R. J., Gur, R. E., Zwil, A. S., Heimberg, C. & Kraemer, H. C. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioural findings in depression. Psychiatry Research 42, 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 6, 278296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, C. J., Hill, S. A., Taylor, M. J., Cowen, P. J. & Goodwin, G. M. (2003). Toward a neuropsychological theory of antidepressant drug action: increase in positive emotional bias after potentiation of norepinephrine activity. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 990–902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmer, C. J., Shelley, N. C., Cowen, P. J. & Goodwin, G. M. (2004). Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 12561263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, T. O., Borsanyi, S., Messari, S., Stanford, K., Cleary, S. E., Shiers, H. M., Brown, G. W. & Herbert, J. (2000). Morning cortisol as a risk factor for subsequent major depressive disorder in adult women. British Journal of Psychiatry 177, 505550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayward, G., Goodwin, G. M., Cowen, P. J. & Harmer, C. J. (2005). Low-dose tryptophan depletion in recovered depressed patients induces changes in cognitive processing without depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry 57, 517554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O. & Prescott, C. A. (2002). Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 11331145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A. (2006). Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in men. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 115124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Neale, N. C., Heath, A. C. & Eaves, L. J. (1993). The prediction of major depression in women: toward an integrated etiologic model. American Journal of Psychiatry 150, 11391141.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Kuhn, J. & Prescott, C. A. (2004). The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 631636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, C. L., Ruffalo, D., Nietert, J. Y. & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Temporal stability of the emotion-modulated startle response. Psychophysiology 37, 92101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lishman, W. A. (1974). The speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine 4, 212218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, G. G. & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effect of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine 5, 173180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackinger, H. F., Loschin, G. G. & Leibetseder, M. M. (2000 a). Prediction of postnatal affective changes by autobiographical memories. European Psychologist 5, 5261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackinger, H. F., Pachinger, M. M., Leibetseder, M. M. & Fartacek, R. R. (2000 b). Autobiographical memories in women remitted from major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 331334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, M., Ward, J. C. & Clark, D. M. (1983). Neuroticism and the recall of positive and negative personality information. Behaviour Research and Therapy 21, 495503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A. & Bradley, B. (1983). Mood and the self-reference bias in recall. Behaviour Research and Therapy 21, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, F. C., Sahakian, B. J., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W. & Paykel, E. S. (1999). Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine 29, 13071321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Parker, G., Tupling, H. & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology 52, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peselow, E. D., Corwin, J., Fieve, R. R., Rotrosen, J. & Cooper, T. B. (1991). Disappearance of memory deficits in outpatient depressives responding to imipramine. Journal of Affective Disorders 21, 173183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portella, M. J., Harmer, C. J., Flint, J., Cowen, P. & Goodwin, G. M. (2005). Enhanced early morning salivary cortisol in neuroticism. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 807809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G. & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Wolf, O. T., Hellhammer, D. H., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., von Auer, K., Jobst, S., Kaspers, F. & Kirschbaum, C. (1997). Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Science 61, 25392549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, A. (1964). The Clinical Examination in Psychology. Presses Universitaires de France: Paris.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L. & Lushene, R. D. (1970). STAI Manual. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Surguladze, S. A., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brebion, G., Travis, M. J. & Philips, M. L. (2004). Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. Neuropsychology 18, 212218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suslow, T., Junghanns, K. & Arolt, V. (2001). Detection of facial expressions of emotions in depression. Perceptual and Motor Skills 92, 857868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, L. J., Teasdale, J. D. & Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Negative self-schema: the effects of induced depressed mood. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 27, 188190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D. & Russell, M. L. (1983). Differential effects of induced mood on the recall of positive, negative and neutral words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 22, 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R. & Nolen-Hosksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 247259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Minnen, A., Wessel, I., Verhaak, C. & Smeenk, J. (2005). The relationship between autobiographical memory specificity and depressed mood following a stressful life event: a prospective study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44, 405415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Zerssen, D., Strian, F. & Schwarz, D. (1974). Evaluation of depressive states, especially in longitudinal studies. In Psychological Measurements in Psychopharmacology (ed. Pichot, P.), pp. 189202. Karger: Basel, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Watson, D. & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 33, 448457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS-R). Psychological Corporation: New York.Google Scholar
Weissmann, A. (1979). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: A Validation Study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G. (2004). Experimental cognitive psychology and clinical practice: autobiographical memory as a paradigm case. In Cognition, Emotion and Psychopathology (ed. Yiend, J.), pp. 251269. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G. & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95, 144149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wust, S., Federenko, I., Hellhammer, D. H. & Kirschbaum, C. (2000). Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 25, 707720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. H., Sahakian, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. (1999). The effects of chronic administration of hydrocortisone on cognitive function in normal male volunteers. Psychopharmacology 145, 260266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Calder, A. J., Etcoff, N. L., Seth, A. & Perrett, D. I. (1997). Facial expression megamix: tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition 63, 271313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zobel, A., Barkpw, K., Schulze-Rauschenbach, S., von Widdern, O., Metten, M., Pfeiffer, U., Schnell, S., Wagner, M. & Maier, W. (2004). High neuroticism and depressive temperament are associated with dysfunctional regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical system in health volunteers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 109, 393399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Psychological characteristics of the two groups

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Emotional categorization of self-referent personality characteristics (a) and animal attributes (b). Values represent mean difference scores of reaction time to identify positive minus negative words±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group comparisons (* p<0·05). ■, High-N; □, Low-N.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Emotional memory: (a) correct recall; (b) incorrect recall, of personality characteristics and animal attribute words. Values represent mean difference scores of number of words recalled for positive minus negative words ±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group comparisons (* p<0·05). ■, High-N; □, Low-N.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Threshold of facial expression recognition in high-N (■) and low-N (□) volunteers. Values represent mean threshold levels (±standard error of the mean) required to correctly identify each emotion at a level of >75%. Asterisks represent statistical significance of group difference (* p<0·05).

Figure 4

Table 2. Attentional bias as measured by dot-probe task under masked and unmasked conditions. Values represent means±standard deviations

Figure 5

Fig. 4. (a) Amplitude of startle response (Z scores) and (b) average latency of startle response while viewing the three types of pictures. Values represent means±standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent statistical significance of comparisons between startle responses during pleasant and unpleasant pictures within each group (* p<0·05)., Neutral; □, pleasant; ■, unpleasant.

Figure 6

Table 3. Memory and executive functions

Figure 7

Fig. 5. Salivary cortisol level within the first hour of waking. ■, High-N; ▲, Low-N.

Supplementary material: PDF

Chan Supplementary Material

Tables.pdf

Download Chan Supplementary Material(PDF)
PDF 44.2 KB